Abstract
In this paper the augmentation of worked examples with animations for teaching problem-solving skills in mathematics is advocated as an effective instructional method. First, in a cognitive task analysis different knowledge prerequisites are identified for solving mathematical word problems. Second, it is argued that so called hybrid animations would be most effective for acquiring these prerequisites, because they show the continuous transition from a concrete, but superficial problem representation to a more abstract, mathematical problem model that forms a basis for solving a problem. An experiment was conducted, where N = 32 pupils from a German high school studied either only text-based worked examples explaining different problem categories from the domain of algebra or worked examples augmented with hybrid animations. Learners with hybrid animations showed superior problem-solving performance for problems of different transfer distance relative to those in the text-only condition.



Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
Cohen’s f is reported as a measure of effect size, where .10, .25, and .40 correspond to small, medium and large effect sizes, respectively (Cohen 1988).
References
Ainsworth, S. E. (1999). A functional taxonomy of multiple representations. Computers & Education, 33, 131–152.
Alessandrini, K. L. (1987). Computer graphics in learning and instruction. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Eds.), The psychology of illustration (Vol. 2, pp. 159–188). New York: Springer.
Atkinson, R. K., Derry, S. A., Renkl, A., & Wortham, D. (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181–214.
Baek, Y. K., & Layne, B. H. (1988). Color, graphics, and animation in a computer-assisted learning tutorial lesson. Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 15, 131–135.
Bétrancourt, M., & Tversky, B. (2000). Effect of computer animation on users’ performance: A review. Le Travail Humain, 63, 311–329.
Byrne, M. D., Catrambone, R., & Stasko, J. T. (1999). Evaluating animations as student aids in learning computer algorithms. Computers & Education, 33, 253–278.
Catrambone, R. (1998). The subgoal learning model: Creating better examples so that students can solve novel problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 355–376.
Catrambone, R., & Seay, F. A. (2002). Using animations to help students learn computer algorithms. Human Factors, 44, 495–511.
ChanLin, L.-J. (2001). Formats and prior knowledge on learning in a computer-based lesson. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 17, 409–419.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 68–82.
Cummins, D. D., Kintsch, W., Reusser, K., & Weimer, R. (1988). The role of understanding in solving word problems. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 405–438.
Dixon, J. A., & Bangert, A. (2004). On the spontaneous discovery of a mathematical relation during problem solving. Cognitive Science, 28, 433–449.
Gerjets, P., Scheiter, K., & Catrambone, R. (2004). Designing instructional examples to reduce intrinsic cognitive load: Molar versus modular presentation of solution procedures. Instructional Science, 32, 33–58.
Ginns, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). When imagining information is effective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 229–251.
Goldstone, R. L., & Sakamoto, Y. (2003). The transfer of abstract principles governing complex adaptive systems. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 414–466.
Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 69–110.
Hansen, S., Narayanan, H., & Hegarty, M. (2002). Designing educationally effective algorithm animations. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 13, 291–317.
Höffler, T. N., & Leutner, D. (2007). Instructional animation versus static pictures: A meta-analysis. Learning and Instruction, 17, 722–738.
Huk, T., & Steinke, M. (2007). Learning cell biology with close-up views or connecting lines: Evidence for the structure mapping effect. Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 1089–1104.
Jonassen, D. H. (2003). Designing research-based instruction for story problems. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 267–296.
Knowlton, J. Q. (1966). On the definition of “picture”. AV Communication Review, 14, 157–183.
Koedinger, K. R., & Nathan, M. J. (2004). The real story behind story problems: Effects of representations on quantitative reasoning. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13, 129–164.
LeFevre, J.-A., & Dixon, P. (1986). Do written instructions need examples? Cognition & Instruction, 3, 1–30.
Lowe, R. K. (1999). Extracting information from an animation during complex visual learning. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14, 225–244.
Lucangeli, D., Tressoldi, P., & Cendron, M. (1998). Cognitive and metacognitive abilities involved in the solution of mathematical problem solving: Validation of a comprehensive model. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 257–275.
Magill, R. A., & Hall, K. G. (1991). A review of the contextual interference effect in motor skill acquisition. In R. B. Wilberg (Ed.), The learning, memory, and perception of perceptual-motor skills (pp. 35–83). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Mayer, R. E., & Anderson, R. B. (1991). Animations need narrations: An experimental test of a dual-coding hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 484–490.
Narayanan, H., & Hegarty, M. (2002). Multimedia design for communication of dynamic information. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57, 279–315.
Nathan, M. J., Kintsch, W., & Young, E. (1992). A theory of algebra-word-problem comprehension and its implications for the design of learning environments. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 329–389.
Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Paas, F. G. W. C., & Van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122–133.
Quilici, J. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1996). Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 144–161.
Reed, S. K. (1999). Word problems: Research and curriculum reform. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Reed, S. K. (2005). From research to practice and back: The animation tutor project. Educational Psychology Review, 17, 55–82.
Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, 1–29.
Renkl, A. (2002). Learning from worked-out examples: Instructional explanations supplement self-explanations. Learning & Instruction, 12, 529–556.
Reusser, K. (1993). Tutoring system and pedagogical theory: Representational tools for understanding, planning, and reflection in problem solving. In S. P. Lajoie & S. Derry (Eds.), Computers as cognitive tools (pp. 143–177). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rieber, L. P. (1990). Animation in computer-based instruction. Educational Technology Research and Development, 38, 77–86.
Riley, M. G., & Greeno, J. G. (1988). Developmental analysis of understanding language about quantities and of problem solving. Cognition and Instruction, 5, 49–101.
Scheiter, K., & Gerjets, P. (2005). When less is sometimes more: Optimal learning conditions are required for schema acquisition from multiple examples. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1943–1948). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Catrambone, R. (2006). Making the abstract concrete: Visualizing mathematical solution procedures. Computers in Human Behavior, 22, 9–26.
Schwartz, D. L. (1995). Reasoning about the referent of a picture versus reasoning about the picture as a referent: An effect of visual realism. Memory & Cognition, 23, 709–722.
Schwartz, D. L., & Black, J. B. (1996). Shuttling between depictive models and abstract rules: Induction and fallback. Cognitive Science, 20, 457–497.
Schwartz, B., & Dreyfus, T. (1993). Measuring integration of information in multi-representational software. Interactive Learning Environments, 3, 177–198.
Simon, H. A. (1977). Models of discovery and other topics in the methods of science. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition & Instruction, 2, 59–89.
Tversky, B., Bauer Morrison, J., & Bétrancourt, M. (2002). Animation: Can it facilitate? International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57, 247–262.
Van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science, 32, 83–98.
Van Lehn, K. (1996). Cognitive skill acquisition. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 513–539.
Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., Schuurman, J. G., De Croock, M. B. M., & Paas, F. G. W. C. (2002). Redirecting learners attention during training: Effects on cognitive load, transfer test performance and training efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 12, 11–37.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P. & Schuh, J. The acquisition of problem-solving skills in mathematics: How animations can aid understanding of structural problem features and solution procedures. Instr Sci 38, 487–502 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-009-9114-9
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-009-9114-9