Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Skip to main content

Costly Signaling Theory in Archaeology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Evolutionary Research in Archaeology
  • 76k Accesses

Abstract

Why do people engage in seemingly wasteful behaviors and invest in extravagant material displays? Since its introduction into anthropological archaeology two decades ago, costly signaling theory (CST) has been used to provide an answer to this question. With broad origins in biology and social theory, costly signaling theory seeks to provide an evolutionary explanation for why humans engage in seemingly wasteful behaviors. In this chapter, I take stock of costly signaling theory in archaeology by (1) tracing its theoretical origins and history of adoption into anthropological archaeology, (2) highlighting key issues that archaeologists have been wrestling with in order to make CST applicable to the past, (3) discussing the breadth of uses of CST in the recent archaeological literature, and (4) presenting an analytical framework that can make CST more rigorous and in the future. Despite persistent doubts about the explanatory utility of CST, the study of signaling from an evolutionary perspective remains a key aspect of evolutionary archaeology.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ames, K. M. (2010). On the evolution of the human capacity for inequality and/or egalitarianism. In T. D. Price & G. Feinman (Eds.), Pathways to power: New perspectives on the emergence of social inequality (pp. 15–44). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Appadurai, A. (Ed.). (1986). The social life of things: Commodities in cultural perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L. (1991). Hunter-gatherers: Archaeological and evolutionary theory. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Binford, L. R. (1962). Archaeology as anthropology. American Antiquity, 28(2), 217–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., Bliege Bird, R. L., & Codding, B. F. (2009). In pursuit of mobile prey: Martu hunting strategies and archaeofaunal interpretation. American Antiquity, 74, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., Codding, B. F., Bliege Bird, R. L., Zeanah, D. W., & Taylor, C. J. (2013). Megafauna in a continent of small game: Archaeological implications of Martu camel hunting in Australia’s Western Desert. Quaternary International, 297, 155–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bird, D. W., & O’Connell, J. F. (2012). Human behavioral ecology. In I. Hodder (Ed.), Archaeological theory today (2nd ed., pp. 37–61). Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanton, D. B. (2016). Evolution of a ritual: Pipes and smoking in Etowah’s Realm. In E. A. Bollwerk & S. Tushingham (Eds.), Perspectives on the archaeology of pipes, tobacco and other smoke plants in the ancient Americas (pp. 93–108). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Blanton, R. E., & Fargher, L. F. (2013). Reconsidering Darwinian anthropology: With suggestions for a revised agenda for cooperation research. In D. Carballo (Ed.), Cooperation and collective action (pp. 93–127). Boulder: University Press of Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R. L., Bird, D. W., Smith, E. A., & Kushnick, G. C. (2002). Risk and reciprocity in Meriam food sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 297–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R. L., & Smith, E. A. (2005). Signaling theory, strategic interaction and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology, 46(2), 221–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bliege Bird, R. L., Smith, E. A., & Bird, D. W. (2001). The hunting handicap: Costly signaling in male foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 9–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bollwerk, E. A. (2016). An examination of the social dynamics behind native smoking pipe variation in the late woodland and early contact period middle Atlantic Region. In E. A. Bollwerk & S. Tushingham (Eds.), Perspectives on the archaeology of pipes, tobacco and other smoke plants in the ancient Americas (pp. 51–76). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boone, J. L. (1998). The evolution of magnanimity: When is it better to give than to receive? Human Nature, 9(1), 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boone, J. L. (2000). Status signaling, social power, and lineage survival. In M. Diehl (Ed.), Hierarchies in action: Qui Bono? (pp. 84–110). Carbondale, IL: Center for Archaeological Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bray, T. L. (Ed.). (2003). The archaeology and politics of food and feasting in early states and empires. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., & Bayham, F. E. (2003). Showing off, foraging models, and the ascendance of large-game hunting in the California Middle Archaic. American Antiquity, 68, 783–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., Cannon, M. D., Bayham, F. E., & Byers, D. A. (2011). Prey body size and ranking in zooarchaeology: Theory, empirical evidence, and applications from the Northern Great Basin. American Antiquity, 76, 403–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byers, D. A., & Broughton, J. M. (2004). Holocene environmental change, artiodactyl abundances, and human hunting strategies in the Great Basin. American Antiquity, 69, 235–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carballo, D. M. (2013). Cultural and evolutionary dynamics of cooperation in archaeological perspective. In D. M. Carballo (Ed.), Cooperation and collective action: Archaeological perspectives (pp. 3–33). Boulder, CO: University Press of Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, M. (2012). Florentine palaces, costly signaling, and lineage survival. PhD thesis, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Codding, B. F., & Bird, D. W. (2015). Behavioral ecology and the future of archaeological science. Journal of Archaeological Science, 56, 9–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Codding, B. F., & Jones, T. L. (2007). Man the showoff? Or the ascendance of a just-so-story: A comment on recent applications of costly signaling theory in American archaeology. American Antiquity, 72(2), 349–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Codding, B. F., Porcasi, J. F., & Jones, T. L. (2010). Explaining prehistoric variation in the abundance of large prey: A zooarchaeological analysis of deer and rabbit hunting along the Pecho Coast of Central California. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 29, 47–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conolly, J. (2017). Costly signaling in archaeology: Origins, relevance, challenges, and prospects. World Archaeology, 49(4), 435–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (1991). Human behavioral ecology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 20, 25–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (2005). The application of animal signaling theory to human phenomena: Some thoughts and clarifications. Social Science Information, 44(4), 603–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Čučković, Z. (2017). Claiming the sea: Bronze Age fortified sites of the North-Eastern Adriatic Sea (Cres and Lošinj Islands, Croatia). World Archaeology, 49(4), 526–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Vanhaeren, M., & van Niekerk, K. (2005). Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: Evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. Journal of Human Evolution, 48(1), 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: Information or manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (1st ed., pp. 282–309). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeMarrais, E., & Earle, T. (2017). Collective action theory and the dynamics of complex societies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 46, 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiNapoli, R. J., Morrison, A. E., Lipo, C. P., Hunt, T. L., & Lane, B. G. (2017). East Polynesian Islands as models of cultural divergence: The case of Rapa Nui and Rapa Iti. The Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology, 13(2), 206–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell, R. C. (1989). Aspects of the application of evolutionary theory in archaeology. In C. C. Lamberg-Karlovsky (Ed.), Archaeological thought in America (pp. 35–99). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Finlayson, C., Brown, K., Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Negro, J. J., Bortolotti, G. R., et al. (2012). Birds of a feather: Neanderthal exploitation of raptors and corvids. PLoS One, 7(9), 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. L. (2015). Faunal quantification and the ascendance of hunting debate: Reevaluation of the data from Southeastern California. American Antiquity, 80(4), 767–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galle, J. E. (2010). Costly signaling and gendered social strategies among slaves in the eighteenth-century Chesapeake: An archaeological perspective. American Antiquity, 75(1), 19–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis, H., Smith, E. A., & Bowles, S. L. (2001). Cooperation and costly signaling. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 213, 103–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gittins, R., & Pettitt, P. (2017). Is Palaeolithic cave art consistent with costly signaling theory? Lascaux as a test case. World Archaeology, 49(4), 466–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glatz, C., & Plourde, A. M. (2011). Landscape monuments and political competition in Late Bronze Age Anatolia: An investigation of costly signaling theory. Bulletin of the American Schools of Oriental Research, 361, 33–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, N., & Andrefsky Jr., W. (Eds.). (2015). Lithic technological systems and evolutionary theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, N., Jones, G. T., & Beck, C. (2011). Natural selection and material culture. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 4, 427–434.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144(4), 517–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graves, M. W., & Ladefoged, T. N. (1995). The evolutionary significance of ceremonial architecture in Polynesia. In P. Teltser (Ed.), Evolutionary archaeology: Methodological issues (pp. 149–174). Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimstead, D. N. (2010). Ethnographic and modeled costs of long-distance, big-game hunting. American Antiquity, 75, 61–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grose, J. (2011). Modelling and the fall and rise of the handicap principle. Biology and Philosophy, 26, 677–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halstead, P., & O’Shea, J. (Eds.). (1989). Bad year economics: Cultural responses to risk and uncertainty. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, O. (1994). Cheating signals. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 167, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1990). Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky strategies. In E. Cashdan (Ed.), Risk and uncertainty in tribal and peasant economies (pp. 145–166). Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1991). Showing off: Tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 12, 29–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. (1993). Why hunter-gatherers work: An ancient version of the problem of public goods. Current Anthropology, 34, 341–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., & Bliege Bird, R. L. (2002). Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11(2), 58–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, B. (1995). Pathways to power: Principles for creating socioeconomic inequalities. In T. D. Price & G. Feinman (Eds.), Foundations of social inequality (pp. 15–85). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, B. (2014). The power of feasts: From prehistory to the present. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, B., & Villeneuve, S. (2011). A century of feasting studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 40, 433–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2007). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Inferentially potent displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Papers on Economics and Evolution, 721, 1–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation, and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 244–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., van Niekerk, K. L., Coquinot, Y., Jacobs, Z., Lauritzen, S.-E., et al. (2011). A 100,000-year-old ochre-processing workshop at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Science, 334, 219–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., & Watts, I. (2009). Engraved ochres from the middle stone age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higham, J. P. (2014). How does honest costly signaling work? Behavioural Ecology, 25(1), 8–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrandt, W. R., & McGuire, K. R. (2002). The ascendance of hunting during the California Middle Archaic: An evolutionary perspective. American Antiquity, 67, 231–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hildebrandt, W. R., & McGuire, K. R. (2003). Large-game hunting, gender-differentiated work organization, and the role of evolutionary ecology in California and Great Basin prehistory: A reply to Broughton and Bayham. American Antiquity, 68, 790–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hockett, B. (2005). Middle and Late Holocene hunting in the Great Basin: A critical review of the debate and future prospects. American Antiquity, 70, 713–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodder, I. (2012). Entangled: An archaeology of the relationships between humans and things. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. (2017). Costly signalling, the arts, archaeology and human behaviour. World Archaeology, 49(4), 446–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, T. L., & Lipo, C. P. (2011). The statues that walked. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2013). The reindeerman’s lasso. In P. Lemonnier (Ed.), Technological choices: Transformation in material cultures since the neolithic (2nd ed., pp. 108–125). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, R. A. (1997). The evolution of animal signals. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 155–178). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, T. L., & Codding, B. F. (2010). Historical contingencies, issues of scale, and flightless hypotheses: A response to Hildebrandt et al. American Antiquity, 75(3), 689–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, P., & Zollman, K. J. S. (2015). An evolutionary comparison of the handicap principle and hybrid equilibrium theories of signaling. PLoS One, 10(9), S.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantner, J. (2010). Implications of human behavioral ecology for understanding complex human behavior. Journal of Anthropological Research, 66(2), 231–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantner, J., & Vaughn, K. J. (2012). Pilgrimage as costly signal: Religiously motivated cooperation in Chaco and Nasca. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 31, 66–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keane, W. (2003). Semiotics and the social analysis of material things. Language & Communication, 23, 409–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knight, C., & Lewis, J. (2017). Wild voices: Mimicry, reversal, metaphor, and the emergence of language. Current Anthropology, 58(4), 435–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach (2nd ed., pp. 380–402). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L. (2014). Signaling theory and technologies of communication in the paleolithic. Biological Theory, 9(1), 42–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupo, K. D., & Schmitt, D. N. (2016). When bigger is not better: The economics of hunting megafauna and its implications for Plio-Pleistocene hunter-gatherers. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 44, 185–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, M., Lipo, C., & Cannon, M. (1999). Fitness and reproductive trade-offs in uncertain environments: Explaining the evolution of cultural elaboration. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 18, 251–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mauss, M. (1924). The gift: Forms and functions of exchange in archaic societies. London: Cohen and West.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. G. C. (1995). Animal signals: Models and terminology. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 177, 305–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. G. C. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, K. R., & Hildebrandt, W. R. (2005). Re-thinking Great Basin foragers: Prestige hunting and costly signaling during the Middle Archaic period. American Antiquity, 70(4), 695–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGuire, K. R., Hildebrandt, W. R., & Carpenter, K. L. (2007). Costly signaling and the ascendance of no-can-do archaeology: A reply to Codding and Jones. American Antiquity, 72(2), 358–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. (2015). Is it intensification yet? Current archaeological perspectives on the evolution of hunter-gatherer economies. Journal of Archaeological Research, 23, 163–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munson, J., Amati, V., Collard, M., & Macri, M. J. (2014). Classic Maya bloodletting and the cultural evolution of religious rituals: Quantifying patterns of variation in hieroglyphic texts. PLoS One, 9(9), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neff, H. (2014). Pots as signals: Explaining the enigma of long-distance ceramic exchange. In M. Martinón-Torres (Ed.), Craft and science: International perspectives on archaeological ceramics (pp. 1–12). Doha: Bloomsbury Qatar Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neiman, F. (1997). Conspicuous consumption as wasteful social advertising: A Darwinian perspective on spatial patterns in Classic Maya terminal monument dates. In G. Clarke & M. Barton (Eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary theory in archaeological explanation (pp. 267–290). Arlington, VA: Archaeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, K. C., & Howard, S. P. (2010). Using evolutionary archaeology and evolutionary ecology to explain cultural elaboration: The case of Middle Ohio Valley Woodland period ceremonial subsistence. North American Archaeologist, 31(2), 119–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., Lupo, K. D., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (2002). Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. Journal of Human Evolution, 43(6), 831–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Driscoll, J. (2017). Hillforts in prehistoric Ireland: A costly display of power? World Archaeology, 49(4), 506–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, D. E. (2017). Finding class in the glass: Obsidian source as a costly signal. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 48, 217–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plourde, A. (2008). The origins of prestige goods as honest signals of skill and knowledge. Human Nature, 19, 374–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentiss, A. M. (2011). Introduction to this special issue on the evolution of material culture. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 4, 374–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prentiss, A. M., Skelton, R. R., Eldredge, N., & Quinn, C. P. (2011). Get rad! The evolution of the skateboard deck. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 4, 379–389.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, C. P. (2006a). Exotics, exchange, and elites: Exploring mechanisms of movement of prestige goods in the Interior Northwest. Journal of Northwest Anthropology, 41, 207–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, C.P. (2006b). Vital signs: Costly signaling and personal adornment in the Near Eastern Neolithic. M.A. thesis, Washington State University, Pullman, WA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, C.P. (2015). Signals in stone: Exploring the role of social information exchange, conspicuous consumption, and costly signaling theory in lithic analysis. In N. Goodale & W. Andrefsky Jr. (Eds.), Lithic technological systems and evolutionary theory (pp. 198–222). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Radovčić, D., Sršen, A. O., Radovčić, J., & Frayer, D. W. (2015). Evidence for Neanderthal Jewelry: Modified White-Tailed Eagle Claws at Krapina. PLoS One, 10(3), 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Vidal, J., d’Errico, F., Giles Pacheco, F., Blasco, R., Rosell, J., Jennings, R. P., et al. (2014). A rock engraving made by Neanderthals in Gibraltar. PNAS, 111(37), 13301–13306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roscoe, P. (2009). Social signaling and the organization of small-scale society: The case of contact-era New Guinea. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 16(2), 69–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, J. R. (1982). Approaches to style in lithic archaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 1, 59–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, J. R. (1985). Style and ethnicity in the Kalahari: A reply to Wiessner. American Antiquity, 50(1), 154–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Safi, K.N. (2015). Costly signaling among Great Houses on the Chaco Periphery. PhD thesis, Washington State University, Pullman, WA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1987). Formation processes of the archaeological record. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy, W., & Nowicki, S. (2005). The evolution of animal communication: Reliability and deception in signaling systems. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silvestri, L., Achino, K. F., Gatta, M., & Rolfo, M. F. (2017). Bioarchaeological remains as indicators of costly signalling: Two case studies from the Middle Bronze Age of Central Italy. World Archaeology, 49(4), 491–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simms, S. R., O’Connell, J. F., & Jones, K. T. (2014). Some thoughts on evolution, ecology, and archaeology in the Great Basin. In N. Parezo & J. Janetski (Eds.), Archaeology in the Great Basin and Southwest (pp. 177–188). Salt Lake City, UT: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2000). Three styles in the evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In K. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons (Eds.), Adaptation and human behavior (pp. 27–46). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Human Nature, 15(4), 343–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2010). Communication and collective action: Language and the evolution of human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 231–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., & Bliege Bird, R. L. (2000). Turtle hunting and tombstone opening: Public generosity as costly signaling. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 245–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A., Bliege Bird, R. L., & Bird, D. W. (2003). The benefits of costly signaling: Meriam turtle hunters. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 116–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2000). Costly signaling and torch fishing on Ifaluk Atoll. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 223–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2003). Why aren’t we all hutterites? Costly signaling theory and religious behavior. Human Nature, 14(2), 91–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. (2003). Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 211–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Speth, J. D. (2010). The paleoanthropology and archaeology of big-game hunting: Protein, fat, or politics. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Számadó, S. (2011). The cost of honesty and the fallacy of the handicap principle. Animal Behaviour, 81, 3–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Számadó, S. (2012). The rise and fall of handicap principle: A commentary on the “Modelling and the fall and rise of the handicap principle”. Biology and Philosophy, 27(2), 279–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veblen, T. (1994 [1899]). The theory of the leisure class. New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waguespack, N. M., Surovell, T. A., Denoyer, A., Dallow, A., Savage, A., Hyneman, J., et al. (2009). Making a point: Wood- versus stone-tipped projectiles. Antiquity, 83, 786–800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wandsnider, L. (2013). Public buildings and civic benefactions in Western Rough Cilicia: Insights from signaling theory. In M. Hoff & R. Townsend (Eds.), Rough cilicia: New archaeological and historical approaches (pp. 170–181). Oxford: Oxbow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wandsnider, L. (2015). “Fighting over a shadow?”: Hellenistic Greek cities and Greco-Roman cities as for a and media for multi-level social signaling. In A. M. Kemezis (Ed.), Urban dreams and realities in antiquity: Remains and representations of the ancient city (pp. 69–98). Boston: Brill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. T., & Phelps, D. O. (2016). Violence and perimortem signaling among early irrigation communities in the Sonoran Desert. Current Anthropology, 57(5), 586–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. American Antiquity, 48(2), 253–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (1985). Style or isochrestic variation? A reply to Sackett. American Antiquity, 50(1), 160–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B., Lu, F., & Tucker, B. (1999). Risk-sensitive adaptive tactics: Models and evidence from subsistence studies in biology and anthropology. Journal of Archaeological Research, 7(4), 301–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9(2), 51–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wobst, M. (1977). Stylistic behavior and information exchange. Anthropological Papers of the University of Michigan, 61, 317–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, J. (2017). The honest labour of stone mounds: Monuments of Bronze and Iron Age Mongolia as costly signals. World Archaeology, 49(4), 547–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. M. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53(1), 205–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. M., & Zahavi, A. V. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s Puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank William Andrefsky, Jr., Lacey Carpenter, Nathan Goodale, Ian Kuijt, John Speth, and two anonymous reviewers for productive conversations, critical comments, and helpful suggestions that have significantly improved this chapter. Any remaining errors and misunderstandings are my own.

Data Sharing Statement Data Sharing Statement Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Data Sharing Statement

Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Colin P. Quinn .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Quinn, C.P. (2019). Costly Signaling Theory in Archaeology. In: Prentiss, A. (eds) Handbook of Evolutionary Research in Archaeology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-11117-5_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-11117-5_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-11116-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-11117-5

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics