Abstract
This chapter is intended to be both theoretical and a little speculative. It draws upon psychological, neuro-dynamic and philosophical sources to create an account of what happens when we experience presence, that is, when we become aware that we are present. This chapter also offers an alternate treatment of the work of Riva and his colleagues with respect to their bio-cultural approach to presence.
Rather than appealing to evolution, biology or technology, this account starts like this: affect precedes cognition. We feel before we think. These feelings (or affective responses) are primarily evaluative and effectively prime our cognition for the world (real or digital) we find ourselves in. All of the apparatus of sense making, reasoning and so forth follow fairly quickly but they are not the first responders. Our affective response is very fast – much faster than our cognition.
In answering the question, how do we find the world, that is, just what is this affective response in response to, we must switch from psychology to philosophy.
Heidegger tells us that we encounter the world as available. Psychology and sense making follow this. We connect with this available world by what Merleau-Ponty calls an intentional arc or intentional threads which “anchor us” to it. This is not simply philosophical discourse as Freeman is able to explain the neuro-dynamics of this arc by invoking the operation of the limbic system – that is, those parts of the brain responsible for our emotional response to the world.
Access this chapter
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Similar content being viewed by others
Notes
- 1.
It should be noted that in their more recent treatments of presence, Riva and Waterworth have modified and significantly extended their position. For example, they treat extended presence as, “related to the emergence of the extended self: the intuitive perception of successfully acting in the external world towards a possible object” (Riva and Waterworth 2013, p. 207).
From this short quotation we can see that they propose that presence is intuitive and is concerned with goal-directed behaviour. By intuitive, they mean that the experience of presence, as a product of our cognition, does not require conscious deliberation while the term object carrying with it the suggestion of objective, motivation and, possibly, intentionality. Here presence is seen to have an evaluative role, providing the organism with feedback on how it is doing with regard to achieving its object or goals. Riva and Waterworth have also suggested a neurological mechanism for this involving a particular form of simulation theory (cf. Blackemore and Decety 2001).
- 2.
To illustrate the importance of recognizing presence as intentional it is worth contrasting it with a non-intentional state. One such non-intentional state is anxiety. Anxiety is, by definition, a sense or state of nervousness or unease about something uncertain or ill-defined, for example, “I am anxious about the state of the world”. Presence is about feeling one is somewhere or engaged with something in particular.
References
Ballew, C. C., II, & Todorov, A. (2007). Predicting political elections from rapid and unreflective face judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(46), 17948–17953.
Basso, A., Goldberg, D., Greenspan, S., & Weimer, D. (2001). First impressions: Emotional and cognitive factors underlying judgments of trust in e-commerce. In Proceedings of the 3rd ACM conference on electronic commerce (pp. 137–143). New York: ACM Press.
Berry, D. S. (2000). Attractiveness, attraction, and sexual selection: Evolutionary perspectives on the form and function of physical attractiveness. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 273–342). San Diego: Academic.
Blackemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567.
Borkenau, P., Brecke, S., Möttig, C., & Paelecke, M. (2009). Extraversion is accurately perceived after a 50-ms exposure to a face. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 703–706.
Bornstein, R. F. (1992). Subliminal mere exposure effects. In R. F. Bornstein & T. S. Pittman (Eds.), Perception without awareness. New York: Guilford Press.
Bouchard, S., Robillard, G., Marchand, A., Renaud, P., & Riva, G. (2005). Presence and the bond between patients and their psychotherapists in the cognitive-behavior therapy of panic disorder with agoraphobia delivered in videoconference. In 2007 10th Annual International Workshop on Presence, October 25–27, 2007 Barcelona, Spain (pp. 265–276).
Brentano, F. C. (1874). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell & L. McAlister, Trans.). London: Routledge, 1993.
Brentano, F. (1995). Psychology from an empirical standpoint (A. C. Rancurello, D. B. Terrell & L. L. McAlister, Trans.). London: Routledge.
Carlin, A. S., Hoffman, H. G., & Weghorst, S. (1997). Virtual reality and tactile augmentation in the treatment of spider phobia: A case study. Behavior Research and Therapy, 35, 153–158.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 40, pp. 61–149). New York: Academic.
Cunningham, M. R. (1986). Measuring the physical in physical attractiveness: Quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of female facial beauty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 925–935.
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace and Co.
Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. London: William Heinemann.
Dreyfus, H. L. (1991). Being-in-the-world: A commentary on Heidegger’s being and time, Division 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Dreyfus, H. L., & Wrathall, M. A. (2005). Introduction. In H. L. Dreyfus & M. A. Wrathall (Eds.), A companion to Heidegger. Malden: Blackwell Publishing.
Freeman, W. J. (1995). Societies of brains. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Freeman, W. J. (1999). Consciousness, intentionality and causality. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 143–172.
Garcia-Palacios, A., Hoffman, H. G., Kwong See, S., Tsai, A., & Botella, C. (1998). Redefining therapeutic success with virtual reality exposure therapy. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 4(3), 341–348.
Gawronski, B., Rydell, R. J., Vervliet, B., & De Houwer, J. (2010). Generalization versus contextualization in automatic evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 683.
Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Goldie, P. (2002). The emotions: A philosophical exploration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2002). Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study. Neuropsychologia, 40, 212–222.
Harman, G. (2002). Tool being: Heidegger and the metaphysics of objects. Chicago: Open Court.
Heidegger, M. (1927/1962). Being and time (J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson, Trans.). New York: Harper Collins.
LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Lindgaard, G., Fernandes, G., Dudek, C., & Brown, J. (2006). Attention web designers: You have 50 milliseconds to make a good first impression! Behaviour & Information Technology, 25(2), 115–126.
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945/1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London: Routledge Classics.
Norman, D. A. (1988). Psychology of everyday things. New York: Basic Books.
Norman, D. A. (2004). Emotional design. New York: Basic Books.
Oatley, K. (1992). Best laid schemes: The psychology of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive account of emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 1, 29–50.
Piaget, J. (1963). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton.
Piaget, J. (1973). The child’s conception of the world. London: Palladin.
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child’s conception of space. London: Routledge Kegan Paul.
Riva, G., & Waterworth, J. A. (2013). Being present in a virtual world. In M. Grimshaw (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of virtuality (pp. 205–221). New York: Oxford University Press.
Riva, G., Waterworth, J. A., & Waterworth, E. L. (2004). The layers of presence: A bio-cultural approach to understanding presence in natural and mediated environments. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 7(4), 405–419.
Rothbaum, B. O., Hodges, L., Watson, B. A., Kessler, G. D., & Opdyke, D. (1996). Virtual reality exposure therapy in the treatment of fear of flying: A case report. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 477–481.
Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). Brief exposures: Male sexual orientation is accurately perceived at 50 ms. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1100–1105.
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69(5), 378–399.
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Turvey, M. T. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: An outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology, 4(3), 173–187.
Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703.
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions making up your mind after a 100-Ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592–598.
Wundt, W. M. (1897). Outlines of psychology (C. Hubbard Judd, Trans.). Available from http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Wundt/Outlines/sec15.htm. Last retrieved December 29, 2013.
Zahorik, P., & Jenison, R. L. (1998). Presence as being-in-the-world. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7, 78–89.
Zajonc, R. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Turner, P. (2015). Affect, Availability and Presence. In: Lombard, M., Biocca, F., Freeman, J., IJsselsteijn, W., Schaevitz, R. (eds) Immersed in Media. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10190-3_4
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10190-3_4
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-319-10189-7
Online ISBN: 978-3-319-10190-3
eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)