Abstract
Cognitive load theory (CLT) has been successful in identifying instructional formats that are more effective and efficient than conventional problem solving in the initial, novice phase of skill acquisition. However, recent findings regarding the “expertise reversal effect” have begun to stimulate cognitive load theorists to broaden their horizon to the question of how instructional design should be altered as a learner's knowledge increases. To answer this question, it is important to understand how expertise is acquired and what fosters its development. Expert performance research, and, in particular, the theoretical framework of deliberate practice have given us a better understanding of the principles and activities that are essential in order to excel in a domain. This article explores how these activities and principles can be used to design instructional formats based on CLT for higher levels of skills mastery. The value of these formats for e-learning environments in which learning tasks can be adaptively selected on the basis of online assessments of the learner's level of expertise is discussed.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Camp, G., Paas, F., Rikers, R.M.J.P., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2001). Dynamic problem selection in air traffic control training: A comparison between performance, mental effort and mental efficiency.Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 575–595.
Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1991). Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction.Cognition and Instruction, 8, 293–332.
Charness, N., Reingold, E.M., Pomplun, M., & Stampe, D.M. (2001). The perceptual aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements.Memory & Cognition, 29, 1146–1152.
Chi, M.T.H., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Eds.), (1988).The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 68–82.
Deakin, J.M., & Cobley, S. (2003). A search for deliberate practice: An examination of the practice environment in figure skating and volleyball. In J. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.),Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 115–132). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ericsson, K.A. (2002). Attaining excellence through deliberate practice: Insights from the study of expert performance. In M. Ferrari (Ed.),The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 21–55). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ericsson, K.A. (2003). How the expert-performance approach differs from traditional approaches to expertise in sports: In search of a shared theoretical framework for studying expert performance. In J. Starkes & K.A. Ericsson (Eds.),Expert performance in sport: Recent advances in research on sport expertise (pp. 371–401). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ericsson, K.A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory.Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.
Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance.Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Ericsson, K.A., & Lehmann, A.C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence for maximal adaptation to task constraints.Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.
Ericsson, K.A., & Simon, H.A. (1993).Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (Rev. ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fitts, P.M., & Posner, M.I. (1967).Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Gerjets, P., & Scheiter, K. (2003). Goal configurations and processing strategies as moderators between instructional design and cognitive load: Evidence from hypertext-based instruction.Educational Psychologist, 38, 33–41.
Glaser, R. (1996). Changing the agency for learning: Acquiring expert performance. In K.A. Ericsson (Ed.),The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 303–311). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect.Educational Psychologist, 38, 23–31.
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design.Human Factors, 40, 1–17.
Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2005). Rapid dynamic assessment of expertise to improve the efficiency of adaptive e-learning. [This special issue].Educational Technology, Research and Development 53(3), 83–93.
Paas, F. (1992). Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive load approach.Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 429–434.
Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments.Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4.
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Darabi, A. (2005). A motivational perspective on the relation between mental effort and performance: Optimizing learners' involvement in instructional conditions. [This special issue].Educational Technology, Research and Development, 53(3), 25–33.
Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: instructional explanations support learning by self-explanations.Learning and Instruction, 12, 529–556.
Rikers, R.M.J.P., Schmidt, H.G., & Boshuizen, H.P.A. (2002). On the constraints of encapsulated knowledge: Clinical case representations by medical experts and subexperts.Cognition and Instruction, 20, 27–45.
Rikers, R.M.J.P., van Gerven, P.W.M., & Schmidt, H.G. (2004). Cognitive load theory as a tool for expertise development.Instructional Science, 32, 173–182.
Salden, R.J.C.M., Paas, F., Broers, N.J., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Mental effort and performance as determinants for the dynamic selection of learning tasks in air traffic control training.Instructional Science, 32, 153–172.
Shute, V., & Towle, B. (2003). Adaptive e-learningEducational Psychologist, 38, 105–114.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning.Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285.
Sweller, J. (2004). Instructional design consequences of an analogy between evolution by natural selection and human cognitive architecture.Instructional Science, 32, 9–31.
Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J.J.G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design.Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–295.
van Gog, T., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (2004). Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding.Instructional Science, 32, 83–98.
Zhu, X., & Simon, H.A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing.Cognition and Instruction, 4, 137–166.
Zimmerman, B.J. (2002). Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective. In M. Ferrari (Ed.),The pursuit of excellence through education (pp. 85–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
In their preparation of this article, the first and last authors were supported by an Internationalization grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO, The Hague, project number 411-01-010).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
van Gog, T., Ericsson, K.A., Rikers, R.M.J.P. et al. Instructional design for advanced learners: Establishing connections between the theoretical frameworks of cognitive load and deliberate practice. ETR&D 53, 73–81 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02504799
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02504799