Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Skip to main content

Associations Between Syntax and the Lexicon Among Children With or Without ASD and Language Impairment

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Five groups of children defined by presence or absence of syntactic deficits and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) took vocabulary tests and provided sentences, definitions, and word associations. Children with ASD who were free of syntactic deficits demonstrated age-appropriate word knowledge. Children with ASD plus concomitant syntactic language impairments (ASDLI) performed similarly to peers with specific language impairment (SLI) and both demonstrated sparse lexicons characterized by partial word knowledge and immature knowledge of word-to-word relationships. This behavioral overlap speaks to the robustness of the syntax–lexicon interface and points to a similarity in the ASDLI and SLI phenotypes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Explore related subjects

Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.

References

  • Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77–117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • ASHA. (1990). Guidelines for screening for hearing impairments and middle ear disorders. ASHA, 32, 17–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D. A. (2000). Interpersonal understanding fuels knowledge acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 40–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1185–1192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2000). Pragmatic language impairment: A correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part of the autistic continuum? In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 99–113). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: Phenomimicry or shared etiology? Behavioral Genetics, 40, 618–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 917–929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1992). Understanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1169–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 741–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 621–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, V. S. (2002). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift and reading development. Journal of Child Language, 29, 189–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development, 74, 394–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dockrell, J. E., & Messer, D. (2004). Lexical acquisition in the early school years. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 35–52). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eigsti, I.-M., Bennetto, L., & Dadlani, M. B. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1007–1023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eskes, G. A., Bryson, S. E., & McCormick, T. A. (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 61–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Serial No. 242, Vol. 59, No. 5).

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73(2), 135–176.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, S., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Henrichsen, M. (1999). The diagnostic accuracy of four vocabulary tests administered to preschool-age children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 196–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groen, W. B., Zwiers, M. P., van der Gaag, R.-J., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2008). The phenotype and neural correlates of language in autism: An integrative review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1416–1425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1989). Emotion-related and abstract concepts in autistic people: Evidence from the British picture vocabulary scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 601–623.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. R., & Kamhi, A. G. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30(1), 65–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 389–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). The Kaufman brief intelligence test-2. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, G., & Fletcher, P. (1993). Grammatical problems in school-age children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 7, 339–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 287–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewy, A. L., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social stimulation and joint attention in young autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20(6), 555–566.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leyfer, O. T., Tager-Flusberg, H., Dowd, M., Tomblin, J. B., & Folstein, S. E. (2008). Overlap between autism and specific language impairment: Comparison of autism diagnostic interview and autism diagnostic observation schedule scores. Autism Research, 1, 284–296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule—WPS edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchman, V., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21(2), 339–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorders: A comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(5), 547–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45(5), 998–1014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Finneran, D. (2008). Grammaticality judgements in adolescents with and without language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 346–360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17(2), 357–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1977). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: A review of research and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 93–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(2), 142–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Conti-Ramsden, G., Falcaro, M., Simkin, A., Charman, T., et al. (2009). Loss of language in early development of autism and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 843–852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M., Warren, S., & Betz, S. (2005). Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 7–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1412–1431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(1), 23–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J. A., Rice, M. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoonen, R., & Verhallen, M. (2008). The assessment of deep word knowledge in young first and second language learners. Language Testing, 25, 211–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—fourth edition (CELF-4). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. K. (2010). Lexical-semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 146–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmonds, L., Messer, D., & Dockrell, J. (2005, July). Exploring semantic deficits in children with WFDs. In Paper presented at the 10th international congress for the study of child language, Berlin, Germany.

  • Stahl, S. A. (1998). Four questions about vocabulary knowledge and reading and some answers. In C. R. Hynd, S. A. Stahl, M. Carr, & S. M. Glynn (Eds.), Learning from text across conceptual domains (pp. 73–94). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1123–1134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 156–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (1999). Language patterns and etiology in children with specific language impairment. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 361–382). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1193–1208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Catts, H., Ellis-Weismer, S., & Weiss, A. (2004). Dimensions of individual differences in communication skills among primary grade children. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 53–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhallen, M., & Schoonen, R. (1993). Word definitions of monolingual and bilingual children. Applied Linguistics, 14(4), 344–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeer, A. (2001). Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 217–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 319–336.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, D., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. New York: Touchstone Applied Science Associated Inc.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank the participants and their families for their generosity. The first author gratefully acknowledges the support of NIH-NIDCD 2 R01 DC003698 together with an augmentation award from Autism Speaks. Some of the data in this paper were presented on May 2, 2008 at the Iowa Center for Developmental and Learning Sciences Workshop on Word Learning, Iowa City, IA.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karla K. McGregor.

Appendix

Appendix

Sentence Formulation Procedures

Task Instructions, Models, and Prompts

I’m going to say a word and your job is to put it in a sentence. Like if I said “dog” you might say, “The spotted dog hunted cats in the neighborhood,” or if I said “memorize” you could say, “The student memorized the answers on the test.” Do you have the idea of what to do? OK, the first word is…

Examiner’s Note: Prompt for the child to think of a different type of the word if they respond with the wrong word class.

Scoring Guidelines with Examples

Sentences were scored according to number of clauses. A clause was defined by the occurrence of a main verb. Each sentence was analyzed if it included a verb and the target word used as the targeted word class.

Example sentence in response to purpose with verbs underlined:

“The mom that picks up (her) the kids says “What’s the purpose of running away from school without getting your food”? = 5 clauses

Definition Procedures

Task Instructions, Models, and Prompts

I’m going to say some words. Your job is to tell me what each word means. For example, if I said “dog” you might say, “it’s an animal, it has fur and a tail and four legs, and for example a poodle is a dog.” If I said “memorize” you might say, “when you memorize something you try to remember it forever, like you might memorize your phone number.” You have the idea, right? OK, your first word is….. (Note: Prompt for the child to think of a different type of the word if they respond to the wrong word class.)

Scoring Guidelines with Examples

  1. 0-

    No correct information.

    • Worship: It’s when you sell something, like hand-me-downs.

    • Origin: The state “origin”.

  2. 1-

    Information is at least partially correct but definition is not conventional.

    • Believe: Would faith be kinda like believing? You can believe in different religions, and different kinds of stuff like deodorant.

    • Mystery: Like the mystery of the disappearing sock, like my disappearing sock that I found in my sock drawer under my underwear.

  3. 2-

    Information is correct and conventional but minimal.

    • Stretch: To make longer.

    • Chair: Chairs are things that people sit on.

  4. 3-

    Information is correct, conventional, and well elaborated.

    • Worship: Praise something, like the Lord, or football, or statues.

    • Stretch: Pulling something so that it expands; you can stretch a rubber band.

Word Association Procedures

Task Instructions, Models, and Prompts

I’m going to say some words. As soon as I say each word, I want you to say the first word you think of, and your answer can only be one word. For example, what is the first word you think of when I say “dog” (praise answer) Yes, you could say “bone, or barks, or cat,” all of those things come to mind when we hear dog, right? What about “memorize”? You could say “answer, hard, or test,” because all of those things come to mind when we hear memorize. Do you have the idea of what to do? OK, the first word is… (Note: If the child responds with rhymes, direct him/her away, “In this activity, we aren’t interested in rhyming so you don’t need to make rhymes,” and if the child gives the same word or near inflection prompt for another word. Also, prompt for one word responses and responses to the correct word class.)

Scoring Guidelines with Examples

  1. 0-

    No discernable relationship.

    • chair: toy

    • consider: done

  2. 1-

    The relationship is minimal in that it is idiosyncratic, syntactic, or derivational.

    • love: you

    • health: healthy

  3. 2-

    The relationship is thematic.

    • table: dinner

    • love: family

  4. 3-

    The relationship is paradigmatic-taxonomic.

    • coin: money

    • love: hate

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

McGregor, K.K., Berns, A.J., Owen, A.J. et al. Associations Between Syntax and the Lexicon Among Children With or Without ASD and Language Impairment. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 35–47 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4

Keywords