Abstract
Five groups of children defined by presence or absence of syntactic deficits and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) took vocabulary tests and provided sentences, definitions, and word associations. Children with ASD who were free of syntactic deficits demonstrated age-appropriate word knowledge. Children with ASD plus concomitant syntactic language impairments (ASDLI) performed similarly to peers with specific language impairment (SLI) and both demonstrated sparse lexicons characterized by partial word knowledge and immature knowledge of word-to-word relationships. This behavioral overlap speaks to the robustness of the syntax–lexicon interface and points to a similarity in the ASDLI and SLI phenotypes.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.References
Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77–117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
ASHA. (1990). Guidelines for screening for hearing impairments and middle ear disorders. ASHA, 32, 17–24.
Baldwin, D. A. (2000). Interpersonal understanding fuels knowledge acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 40–45.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 285–297.
Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (1998). Specific language impairment and grammatical morphology: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1185–1192.
Bishop, D. V. M. (2000). Pragmatic language impairment: A correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part of the autistic continuum? In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 99–113). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: Phenomimicry or shared etiology? Behavioral Genetics, 40, 618–629.
Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F. (2002). Exploring the borderlands of autistic disorder and specific language impairment: A study using standardised diagnostic instruments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 917–929.
Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 108, 896–904.
Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. (1992). Understanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 1169–1182.
Condouris, K., Meyer, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). The relationship between standardized measures of language and measures of spontaneous speech in children with autism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 12, 349–358.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 741–748.
Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N. (2006). The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 621–628.
Cronin, V. S. (2002). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift and reading development. Journal of Child Language, 29, 189–204.
Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development, 74, 394–412.
Dockrell, J. E., & Messer, D. (2004). Lexical acquisition in the early school years. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 35–52). Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Eigsti, I.-M., Bennetto, L., & Dadlani, M. B. (2007). Beyond pragmatics: Morphosyntactic development in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1007–1023.
Eskes, G. A., Bryson, S. E., & McCormick, T. A. (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 61–73.
Fenson, L., Dale, P., Reznick, J., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (Serial No. 242, Vol. 59, No. 5).
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L., & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition, 73(2), 135–176.
Gray, S., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Henrichsen, M. (1999). The diagnostic accuracy of four vocabulary tests administered to preschool-age children. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 30, 196–206.
Groen, W. B., Zwiers, M. P., van der Gaag, R.-J., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2008). The phenotype and neural correlates of language in autism: An integrative review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1416–1425.
Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1989). Emotion-related and abstract concepts in autistic people: Evidence from the British picture vocabulary scale. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 601–623.
Johnston, J. R., & Kamhi, A. G. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30(1), 65–85.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1998). Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(10), 389–398.
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). The Kaufman brief intelligence test-2. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.
King, G., & Fletcher, P. (1993). Grammatical problems in school-age children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 7, 339–352.
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16(2–3), 287–308.
Leonard, L. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Lewy, A. L., & Dawson, G. (1992). Social stimulation and joint attention in young autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20(6), 555–566.
Leyfer, O. T., Tager-Flusberg, H., Dowd, M., Tomblin, J. B., & Folstein, S. E. (2008). Overlap between autism and specific language impairment: Comparison of autism diagnostic interview and autism diagnostic observation schedule scores. Autism Research, 1, 284–296.
Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule—WPS edition. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Marchman, V., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 21(2), 339–366.
Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorders: A comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(5), 547–559.
McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R., & Capone, N. C. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 45(5), 998–1014.
Miller, C. A., Leonard, L. B., & Finneran, D. (2008). Grammaticality judgements in adolescents with and without language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 43, 346–360.
Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 17(2), 357–374.
Nelson, K. (1977). The syntagmatic-paradigmatic shift revisited: A review of research and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 93–116.
Norbury, C. F. (2005). Barking up the wrong tree? Lexical ambiguity resolution in children with language impairments and autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90(2), 142–171.
Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Conti-Ramsden, G., Falcaro, M., Simkin, A., Charman, T., et al. (2009). Loss of language in early development of autism and specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 843–852.
Rice, M., Warren, S., & Betz, S. (2005). Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 7–27.
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1412–1431.
Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(1), 23–36.
Roberts, J. A., Rice, M. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429–448.
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social communication questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Schoonen, R., & Verhallen, M. (2008). The assessment of deep word knowledge in young first and second language learners. Language Testing, 25, 211–236.
Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—fourth edition (CELF-4). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. K. (2010). Lexical-semantic organization in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 146–159.
Simmonds, L., Messer, D., & Dockrell, J. (2005, July). Exploring semantic deficits in children with WFDs. In Paper presented at the 10th international congress for the study of child language, Berlin, Germany.
Stahl, S. A. (1998). Four questions about vocabulary knowledge and reading and some answers. In C. R. Hynd, S. A. Stahl, M. Carr, & S. M. Glynn (Eds.), Learning from text across conceptual domains (pp. 73–94). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32(7), 1123–1134.
Tomasello, M. (2000). The item-based nature of children’s early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 156–160.
Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (1999). Language patterns and etiology in children with specific language impairment. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders (pp. 361–382). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.
Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1193–1208.
Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X., Catts, H., Ellis-Weismer, S., & Weiss, A. (2004). Dimensions of individual differences in communication skills among primary grade children. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 53–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Verhallen, M., & Schoonen, R. (1993). Word definitions of monolingual and bilingual children. Applied Linguistics, 14(4), 344–365.
Vermeer, A. (2001). Breadth and depth of vocabulary in relation to L1/L2 acquisition and frequency of input. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 217–234.
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 319–336.
Williams, K. T. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.
Williams, D., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.
Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. New York: Touchstone Applied Science Associated Inc.
Acknowledgments
We thank the participants and their families for their generosity. The first author gratefully acknowledges the support of NIH-NIDCD 2 R01 DC003698 together with an augmentation award from Autism Speaks. Some of the data in this paper were presented on May 2, 2008 at the Iowa Center for Developmental and Learning Sciences Workshop on Word Learning, Iowa City, IA.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Appendix
Appendix
Sentence Formulation Procedures
Task Instructions, Models, and Prompts
I’m going to say a word and your job is to put it in a sentence. Like if I said “dog” you might say, “The spotted dog hunted cats in the neighborhood,” or if I said “memorize” you could say, “The student memorized the answers on the test.” Do you have the idea of what to do? OK, the first word is…
Examiner’s Note: Prompt for the child to think of a different type of the word if they respond with the wrong word class.
Scoring Guidelines with Examples
Sentences were scored according to number of clauses. A clause was defined by the occurrence of a main verb. Each sentence was analyzed if it included a verb and the target word used as the targeted word class.
Example sentence in response to purpose with verbs underlined:
“The mom that picks up (her) the kids says “What’s the purpose of running away from school without getting your food”? = 5 clauses
Definition Procedures
Task Instructions, Models, and Prompts
I’m going to say some words. Your job is to tell me what each word means. For example, if I said “dog” you might say, “it’s an animal, it has fur and a tail and four legs, and for example a poodle is a dog.” If I said “memorize” you might say, “when you memorize something you try to remember it forever, like you might memorize your phone number.” You have the idea, right? OK, your first word is….. (Note: Prompt for the child to think of a different type of the word if they respond to the wrong word class.)
Scoring Guidelines with Examples
-
0-
No correct information.
-
Worship: It’s when you sell something, like hand-me-downs.
-
Origin: The state “origin”.
-
-
1-
Information is at least partially correct but definition is not conventional.
-
Believe: Would faith be kinda like believing? You can believe in different religions, and different kinds of stuff like deodorant.
-
Mystery: Like the mystery of the disappearing sock, like my disappearing sock that I found in my sock drawer under my underwear.
-
-
2-
Information is correct and conventional but minimal.
-
Stretch: To make longer.
-
Chair: Chairs are things that people sit on.
-
-
3-
Information is correct, conventional, and well elaborated.
-
Worship: Praise something, like the Lord, or football, or statues.
-
Stretch: Pulling something so that it expands; you can stretch a rubber band.
-
Word Association Procedures
Task Instructions, Models, and Prompts
I’m going to say some words. As soon as I say each word, I want you to say the first word you think of, and your answer can only be one word. For example, what is the first word you think of when I say “dog” (praise answer) Yes, you could say “bone, or barks, or cat,” all of those things come to mind when we hear dog, right? What about “memorize”? You could say “answer, hard, or test,” because all of those things come to mind when we hear memorize. Do you have the idea of what to do? OK, the first word is… (Note: If the child responds with rhymes, direct him/her away, “In this activity, we aren’t interested in rhyming so you don’t need to make rhymes,” and if the child gives the same word or near inflection prompt for another word. Also, prompt for one word responses and responses to the correct word class.)
Scoring Guidelines with Examples
-
0-
No discernable relationship.
-
chair: toy
-
consider: done
-
-
1-
The relationship is minimal in that it is idiosyncratic, syntactic, or derivational.
-
love: you
-
health: healthy
-
-
2-
The relationship is thematic.
-
table: dinner
-
love: family
-
-
3-
The relationship is paradigmatic-taxonomic.
-
coin: money
-
love: hate
-
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
McGregor, K.K., Berns, A.J., Owen, A.J. et al. Associations Between Syntax and the Lexicon Among Children With or Without ASD and Language Impairment. J Autism Dev Disord 42, 35–47 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1210-4