Ethology Ecology & Evolution
ISSN: 0394-9370 (Print) 1828-7131 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/teee20
Rodents in the arena: a critical evaluation of
methods measuring personality traits
Maria Vittoria Mazzamuto, Giacomo Cremonesi, Francesca Santicchia,
Damiano Preatoni, Adriano Martinoli & Lucas A. Wauters
To cite this article: Maria Vittoria Mazzamuto, Giacomo Cremonesi, Francesca Santicchia,
Damiano Preatoni, Adriano Martinoli & Lucas A. Wauters (2018): Rodents in the arena: a
critical evaluation of methods measuring personality traits, Ethology Ecology & Evolution, DOI:
10.1080/03949370.2018.1488768
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2018.1488768
View supplementary material
Published online: 13 Jul 2018.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 4
View Crossmark data
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=teee20
Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 2018
https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2018.1488768
Rodents in the arena: a critical evaluation of methods
measuring personality traits
MARIA VITTORIA MAZZAMUTO1,*, GIACOMO CREMONESI1, FRANCESCA SANTICCHIA1,
DAMIANO PREATONI1, ADRIANO MARTINOLI1 and LUCAS A. WAUTERS1,2
1
Environment Analysis and Management Unit, Guido Tosi Research Group, Department
of Theoretical and Applied Sciences, University of Insubria, Via J.H. Dunant 3, I-21100
Varese, Italy
2
Evolutionary Ecology Group, Department of Biology, University of Antwerp, B-2610
Wilrijk, Belgium
Received 22 December 2017, accepted 12 May 2018
The Open Field Test (OFT) and Mirror Image Stimulation (MIS) are used to
measure behaviours related to an individual’s personality. These tests, carried out in a
same novel arena, have been used for different taxa, but only a few papers underline
the importance of method validation. Here we investigate how Eurasian red squirrels
(Sciurus vulgaris) and Eastern grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) behave during
OFT and MIS. Next, we compare the performance between three analytical methods:
the Principal Component Analysis (PCA), the Factor Analysis (FA) and an expertbased (EB) method. The EB approach classifies behaviours in groups relating on
researchers’ knowledge and returns personality-trait values for each individual facilitating comparisons over studies and/or with new datasets. The comparison between
the three methods gave similar results and high repeatabilities in some expert-based
personality traits as well as PCA components and FA factors, showing that all three
methods were valid to measure activity using OFT (both species) and sociability using
MIS (grey squirrel). Repeatabilities of the other traits were less strong. Proportion of
time spent in different behaviours did not differ with test duration, since shorter tests
yielded valid measures of individual differences in personality. Shorter tests reduce
operator time in the field, and are likely to reduce stress and arena-habituation of the
animals. Test sequence affected the outcome of OFT: squirrels tested for the first time
were more active than squirrels tested a second time. For the two squirrel species
investigated, we recommend an OFT of 4 min and a MIS test of three and suggest to
test an individual no more than 2 times per season with at least 2 months between
repetitions.
KEY WORDS :
boldness, Eurasian red squirrel, Eastern grey squirrel, exploration, mirror image stimulation, open field test, rodents.
*
Corresponding author: Maria Vittoria Mazzamuto, Dipartimento di Scienze Teoriche e Applicate,
Università degli Studi dell’Insubria, Via J.H. Dunant 3, I-21100 Varese, Italia (E‑mail: maria.mazzamuto@
uninsubria.it).
© 2018 Dipartimento di Biologia, Università di Firenze, Italia
2
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
INTRODUCTION
A growing number of studies on many animal taxa has shown that individual
animals exhibit differences in behaviour that persist over time and across contexts (Sih
et al. 2004; Réale et al. 2007). This phenomenon is known as animal personality, also
temperament or coping style. Behaviours that are part of an individual’s personality,
called personality traits, show within-individual consistency (repeatability) (Réale et al.
2000; Dingemanse et al. 2002; Bell et al. 2009) and are, to some extent, heritable (Drent
et al. 2003; Bester-Meredith & Marler 2007; Brown et al. 2007). Personality traits are
often classified into five axes proposed in a review by Réale et al. (2007): (1) the shy–
boldness axis (response to risky situations), (2) exploration–avoidance (response to a
new situation), (3) activity (general moving activity), (4) aggressiveness (tendency to
respond with agonistic behaviours towards conspecifics), and (5) sociability (any nonagonistic responses to conspecifics).
Differences in personality among individuals have been observed throughout a
variety of taxa (Sih et al. 2012): invertebrates (e.g. Sinn et al. 2006; Kortet & Hedrick
2007), fish (Budaev et al. 1999; Bierbach et al. 2015), reptiles (e.g. López et al. 2005;
Cote & Clobert 2007), birds (e.g. Carere & van Oers 2004; Both et al. 2005; Dingemanse
et al. 2012) and mammals (e.g. Svartberg et al. 2005; Dochtermann & Jenkins 2007).
Personality can have consequences for space use (Wilson & McLaughlin 2007; Martin &
Réale 2008), dispersal (Fraser et al. 2001; Dingemanse et al. 2003; Cote et al. 2010),
invasiveness (Rehage & Sih 2004; Malange et al. 2016), mating and/or reproductive
success (Réale et al. 2000; Both et al. 2005), parental care (Budaev et al. 1999; Both
et al. 2005), survival (Boon et al. 2008; Haage et al. 2017), and thus an individual’s
fitness (Sinn et al. 2006; Réale et al. 2007; Smith & Blumstein 2008). Moreover, animal
personality may also be relevant in applied wildlife conservation research since taking
into consideration the individual behavioural profile may lead to a more effective
management, conservation, and recovery of populations (Haage et al. 2017; Merrick
& Koprowski 2017).
To advance the study of animal personality the methods used to measure an
individual’s personality are of overriding importance. In behavioural ecology, two of
the most used tests for a direct measure of personality traits are the open field test
(OFT) (Walsh & Cummins 1976) and mirror image stimulation test (MIS) (Svendsen &
Armitage 1973). The first is applied to quantify activity, exploration and responses
linked to stress in a novel environment; the second to assess aggressiveness and sociability towards conspecifics. Both tests have been used for different animal taxa (e.g.
Armitage 1986; Dammhahn 2012; Bierbach et al. 2015; Haage et al. 2017) but only a few
papers underline the importance of analysing the test validity (Martin & Réale 2008;
Montiglio et al. 2010; Carter et al. 2013). Carter et al. (2013) reviewed the definitions
and methods used in personality studies in behavioural ecology, underlining the risk of
misclassifying traits without a strict and detailed test validation. It is essential that each
personality trait can be operationally defined and measured by a set of correlated
behaviours (Réale et al. 2007). Such a set of behaviours can be derived from statistical
inference or from an expert-based approach; the latter is a classification of behaviours
into groups, each of which reflects a personality trait, based on researchers’ previous
knowledge (experience and literature). Hence, to obtain reliable estimates of personality
traits, not only the validity of the OFT and/or MIS should be explored, but also the
different data analysis methods should be validated (Réale et al. 2007; Carter et al.
2013). The majority of studies on rodent personality have used Principal Component
Analysis (PCA) to reduce the measured behavioural variables defined in ethograms to a
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
3
smaller number of synthetic personality traits. Carter et al. (2013) suggested that a
factor analytic approach (Factor Analysis, FA) may be more appropriate in behavioural
ecology to both establish independent factors/axes of correlated personality traits and
investigate how these orthogonal axes are related to the ecological factors investigated
(see also Budaev 2010). Moreover, a factorial approach would also have the benefit of
reducing the problems associated with so-called jingle-jangle fallacies (Carter et al.
2013). Nevertheless, none of the studies on wild rodents’ personality carried out after
2013 used that approach (see Table 1). Other than behavioural data analysis, the arena
test protocol itself might affect personality measures. The repetition of the arena test on
the same individual can lead to a decrease in the intensity of activity/exploration
behaviours over trails as found in many studies on birds and small mammals (Archer
1973; Dingemanse et al. 2002; Boon et al. 2007, 2008; Martin & Réale 2008; Boyer et al.
2010; Montiglio et al. 2010; Taylor et al. 2012). Moreover, long tests may also result in
habituation to the arena, hence test duration, other than number of test repetitions, is
another important factor that can affect the measure of personality (Montiglio et al.
2010).
In this study we explore the performance of direct methods to measure individual
variation in personality traits in free-living rodents and compare the performance of
both PCA and FA to classify behaviours into personality traits, that are then compared
to our expert-based classification. We used data collected with the open field test (OFT)
and mirror image stimulation test (MIS) on Eurasian red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris)
and Eastern grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) to explore the following hypotheses.
(1) Our direct indices of traits are consistent over time (significant repeatability) for the
individual squirrel, indicating they reflect its personality. (2) Different techniques for
the analysis of the behaviours measured during arena tests (Principal Component
Analysis, Factor Analysis, expert-based method) result in comparable discrimination
of personality traits. (3) They produce consistent patterns of variation among individuals; in other words, the values of personality trait indices (scores for an individual
squirrel from a given arena test using either PCA, FA or expert-based method) should be
positively correlated. Finally, we discuss how factors related to novelty (first versus
subsequent capture of an animal), repetition and duration of the arena test can affect its
results in measuring personality.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Study system
Squirrels show marked individual differences in behaviour and in personality that can affect
important ecological interactions (e.g. host–parasite interactions, Boyer et al. 2010) and influence
individual variation in fitness (e.g. Boon et al. 2008; Le Cœur et al. 2015). Arena tests have been
commonly used to measure personality traits and study these relationships in squirrels (Table 1),
but often without a critical consideration of test parameters (e.g. duration, number of repeated
tests). Therefore they make good models for a personality method validation.
Red squirrels were studied in three study sites in the Stelvio National Park (Lombardy, Italy;
for details see Salmaso et al. 2009; Rodrigues et al. 2010), and grey squirrels in two sites in the Poplain in Piedmont. Red squirrels were monitored during two capture sessions per site for at least
5 days, one in May–June 2016 and the other one in September–October 2016. Grey squirrels were
monitored for at least 5 days over three capture sessions per site: December 2015–April 2016 in one
site and November 2016–January 2017 in the other.
4
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Trapping and handling squirrels
Single-capture traps (model 202, Tomahawk Live Trap Co., Tomahawk, WI, USA) were used
and fixed on tree trunks or on the ground. Prebaiting started 1 week before each trapping session
using some hazelnuts placed inside the blocked traps. Activated traps were checked 2 times per day
according to the increased activity of the animals (during the morning and late afternoon) and to
reduce the time squirrels were confined in traps. Each trapped squirrel was flushed into a cotton
handling bag (zipper-tube, Wauters & Dhondt 1989), weighed to the nearest 5 g with a Pesola
spring balance, identified to species, sex and reproductive status (Wauters et al. 2000). Squirrels
were individually marked with numbered metal ear tags (type 1003 S National Band and Tag Co,
Newport, Kentucky, USA) and, after the arena test, released near the trap location. Trapping and
handling squirrels complied with current laws on animal research and welfare in Italy and were
carried out under specific permissions (Stelvio National Park Protocol no. 414 of 28 February
2014; Province of Turin Authorisation no. 294-34626/2014 of 12 September 2014; Lombardy
Region Decree no. 9523 of 15 October 2014).
Measuring personality
Once caught and handled, we put a marked squirrel inside the arena by opening a sliding
door (28 × 15 cm, internal opening 12 × 12 cm) and allowing the animal to move from the handling
bag into the arena. The arena is a white extruded polycarbonate box of 50 × 51 × 51 cm; the floor of
the arena consists of a panel with four blind holes (7 cm diameter × 4 cm deep), that allow to
differentiate between exploration and activity behaviours (hole board test, Martin & Réale 2008).
The wall at the opposite site of the entrance has a sliding panel that can be removed to reveal a
mirror (24 × 51 cm). In the lid of the arena (inside a 5 cm diameter hole) we fit a web camera (Drift,
Professional HD Action Camera, model: FD9960, Ghost S) to record the animal’s behaviour. To
quantify individual personality, we performed two different experiments inside the arena: Open
Field Test (OFT) to estimate activity and exploration levels in a novel environment (Walsh &
Cummins 1976; Martin & Réale 2008) and Mirror Image Stimulation (MIS) to test aggressiveness,
sociability or avoidance towards conspecifics (Svendsen & Armitage 1973). The Open Field Test,
introduced during the first decades of 1900 (Hall & Ballechey 1932), is still one of the most widely
used instruments to measure personality traits. The Mirror Image Stimulation test is a technique
for studying aggressive and social patterns in a wide variety of animals which respond to their
reflection in a mirror (Gallup 1968; Svendsen & Armitage 1973). The two tests were performed in
the same testing session, with the OFT also serving as habituation time before the MIS. The arena
was placed on the ground near the trap where the squirrel was caught and recording (OFT
experiment) started before we released the animal inside the arena. After 6 min we opened the
mirror and began MIS test for another 4 min. At the end of MIS the squirrel was released by
opening the sliding door. After each experiment the arena was cleaned with 90% ethyl alcohol to
eliminate urine and faecal pellets when present and to eliminate effects of squirrel’s scent on
behaviour of the next animal.
We performed arena tests for each individual only once per capture-session to reduce stress
and habituation in animals (minimum time between tests for the same individual: 28 days); in
addition to check the assumptions of repeatability of personality traits we repeated both experiments (OFT and MIS) in different capture-sessions to have at least two arena tests for most
individuals.
We analysed digital videos of OFT and MIS with CowLog 3.0.2 software (Hänninen & Pastell
2009) and used ethograms similar to Boon et al. (2007) (Table 1); for each experiment, the software
calculates the time that an individual spent in each behaviour.
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
5
Data analysis
We first transformed the time calculated by CowLog 3.0.2 in percentage of time spent by
each squirrel in a given behavioural state. Since, in previous studies, PCA scores gave sometimes
poor results (low % variance explained by first two or three PC components, e.g. Martin & Réale
2008; Boyer et al. 2010; Montiglio et al. 2012) we decided to test also an “expert-based” (EB)
method to reduce number of variables and create behavioural groups of personality traits, relying
on ethological knowledge (Wauters & Dhondt 1987, 1989, 1992; Wauters et al. 2001) and video
observations (see Table 2 for classifying behaviours from OFT and MIS into personality traits).
With the EB approach the researcher defines groups of behaviours, with each group related to a
specific personality trait, summing the single behaviours percentages and obtaining a general
personality trait score. In contrast, with the PCA or FA the loadings for the different behaviours
are calculated and then the new scores (along the first few principal components or factors)
interpreted as measures of personality traits, leading to a changing in the loadings (and hence
behaviour scores) every time new animals are added to the dataset.
Aggressiveness was considered as the number of attacks towards the mirror during MIS.
All data analysis was performed using the software R 3.4.2 (R Development Core Team 2016).
To reduce the variables with a statistical method, we ran a Principal Component Analysis (PCA)
and a Factor Analysis (FA) separately for OFT and MIS. Next, we compared the EB personality
traits’ values (square root transformed) with the PC and FA scores for each individual using
Pearson correlation coefficient. Only the principal components and the factors explaining the
greater part of the total variance were retained with the Kaiser-Guttman criterion (Martin &
Réale 2008).
To assess individual consistency of behaviours in both experiments, we used a restricted
sample of individuals (20 red squirrels, 38 grey squirrels) that were caught in at least two different
trapping sessions. We estimated the repeatability, also called Intra-class Correlation Coefficient
(ICC) with Linear Mixed Models (LMM, lme4 package); (Bates et al. 2014a, 2014b). PC scores, FA
scores or EB personality traits were the dependent variable, squirrel ID the random intercept term,
to account for repeated tests on the same individuals, and sex, study site, and number of test (first,
second or third test to the same animal) were included as fixed effects. For each final model, we
estimated repeatability of personality traits, PC and FA scores as R = (Vi × 100)/Vi + Vr) where Vi is
the variance associated with the individual random effect, and Vr is the residual variance of the
model. Repeatability, in fact, represents the percentage of the total phenotypic variance explained
by within-individual variance (Lessells & Boag 1987). Repeatability was estimated using the R
package rptR v 0.9.2 (CI = 95%, number of parametric bootstraps for interval estimation = 5000,
number of permutations used when calculating asymptotic P-values = 1000) (Bohn et al. 2017;
Stoffel et al. 2017).
In a final step, we critically evaluated the effects of some methodological parameters
(duration of the trial; animal tested at first capture or during a recapture; results of first against
second trial) on personality-trait measurements.
To investigate the effect of the duration of both OFT and MIS tests on the estimates of
personality traits, we used the behavioural coding over just 4 min for OFT and 3 min for MIS and
calculated again, on this shorter experiment time, the proportion of time spent by each squirrel on
a single behaviour. This allowed us to determine whether a shorter arena test would allow defining
personality in our study species without losing information and thus without altering our results
(see also Montiglio et al. 2010). Using the square-root transformed proportion time of the different
behaviours, we ran again PCA, FA and EB groups (shyness, exploration and activity for OFT and
sociability, avoidance, alert and other for MIS) for the shorter OFT and MIS. We used linear mixed
models (LMM) with the short time personality-trait estimates as response variable, the long
personality-trait estimates as explanatory variable and squirrel ID as random intercept to explore
whether short- and long-time arena tests (OFT 4 min compared with OFT 6 min, and MIS 3 min
compared with MIS 4 min) gave comparable results .
In a second step, since we will show that a shorter duration did not affect the results, we
used the short time personality-trait estimates as response variable on a linear mixed model (LMM)
6
Table 1.
Species
Test
Dipodomys merriami
MIS
Duration (min)
15
Research question
Stat
Reference
Behavioural syndrome
Other
Dochtermann & Jenkins 2007
AT
7 days
Food-hoarding behaviour
Other
Jenkins 2011
Dipodomys ordii
AT
7 days
Food-hoarding behaviour
Other
Jenkins 2011
Marmota flaviventris
MIS
15
Behavioural profile
FA
Svendsen & Armitage 1973
MIS
15
Recruitment
Other
Armitage 1984
Reproductive success
FA
Armitage 1986
Coping style
PCA
Ferrari et al. 2013
PCA
Blumstein et al. 2006
MIS
15
Marmota marmota
OFT
3
Marmota vancouverensis
MIS
10
Response to a predator
Microtus arvalis
OFT
10
Effect of season on personality
Other
Gracceva et al. 2014
Microtus ochrogaster
AT
10
Dispersal
Other
Myers & Krebs 1971
Microtus oeconomus
OFT
Microtus pennsylvanicus
AT
Mus musculus
Linkage between behavior and metabolism
PCA
Lantová et al. 2011
10
5
Dispersal
PCA
Myers & Krebs 1971
AT
10
Dispersal
Other
Krackow 2003
OFT
15
Pattern of behaviour in OFT
Other
Simeonovska-Nikolova 2000
Mus spicilegus
OFT
15
Pattern of behaviour in OFT
Other
Simeonovska-Nikolova 2000
Myodes glareolus
AT
10
Personality and population density
PCA
Korpela et al. 2011
OFT
5
Relation personality – physiology
PCA
Šíchová et al. 2014
Necromys lasiurus
OFT
5
Invasiveness
PCA
Malange et al. 2016
Sciurus niger
OFT
10
Behavioral effects of an immunocontraceptive vaccine
PCA
Krause et al. 2015
MIS
10
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Papers published since 1970 about free-ranging rodents and personality using arena test (OFT = Open Field Test; MIS = Mirror Image Stimulation test;
AT = other types of Arena Test; STAT = statistical methods used for data analysis of personality traits).
Sciurus vulgaris
7.5
MIS
5
OFT
7
MIS
7
Tamias sibiricus
OFT
5
Parasite load, space use and personality
PCA
Boyer et al. 2010
Tamias striatus
OFT
1.5
Long-term stress and activity
PCA
Montiglio et al. 2012
OFT
5
Behavioural profile
PCA
Martin & Réale 2008
OFT
3
Temporal activity patterns
Other
Montiglio et al. 2010
OFT
7.5
Reproduction and offspring fitness and trappability
PCA
Boon et al. 2007, 2008
MIS
5
OFT
7.5
Personality and ontogeny
PCA
Kelley et al. 2015
MIS
5
OFT
7.5
Natal dispersal
PCA
Merrick & Koprowski 2017
MIS
5
MIS
5
Territorial defence
PCA
Shonfield et al. 2012
OFT
7.5
Behaviour heritability
PCA
Taylor et al. 2012
Tamias dorsalis
Tamiasciurus hudsonicus
Personality and population density
PCA
Haigh et al. 2017
Reproductive success and juvenile dispersal
PCA
Kilanowski 2015
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
OFT
7
8
Ethogram for open field and mirror-image stimulation tests. Description of the single behaviours and indication of the expert-based grouping into categories
that represent personality traits (see methods for definition expert-based method).
Open Field Test
Behaviour
Behaviour description
Mirror Image Stimulation Test
Personality
traits
Activity
Behaviour
Behaviour description
Locomotion
Jump, walk
Locomotion
Jump, walk
Rise
Rise up on hind legs
Rise
Rise up on hind legs
Scan
Head moving
Scan
Head moving
Scratch
Scratch or chew floors/walls
Scratch
Scratch or chew floors/walls
Exploration
Sniff
Sniff the corner of arena
Sniff
Sniff the corner of arena
Head dip
Put head in holes in the floor
Head dip
Put head in holes in the floor
Hang
Hang on walls
Immobile
No movement
Shyness
Hang
Hang on walls
Back
Immobile in back half of arena furthest from mirror
Slow
Slow approach towards mirror, with hind legs
stretched out behind
Personality traits
Other
Avoidance
Sociability
No-aggressive
Non aggressive contact with the mirror
Front
Immobile in front half of arena closest to mirror
Watch
Immobile, watching directly to mirror
Alert
Attack
Strike the mirror with front legs or head
Aggressiveness
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Table 2.
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
9
with squirrel identity as random intercept. Number of test repetition per animal, capture event
(new capture or recapture), sex and body mass were included as fixed effects. Sex and body mass
were added because of possible relationships of personality with social dominance (heavy squirrels
tend to be older, more dominant animals) and sex-specific spacing patterns (Wauters & Dhondt
1989, 1992; Koprowski 1993; Gurnell et al. 2001).
RESULTS
Personality of red squirrels
We performed 76 arena tests on 58 different red squirrels (43 males and 15 female);
20 animals were tested more than 1 time (41 arena tests). The proportion of time spent by
red squirrels in different behaviours is reported in Table S1 (Supplemental material).
Comparison of analytical methods
For OFT, we retained the first three Principal Components (PCs) and two Factors of
FA; for MIS the first three PCs and FA factors. The retained PCA components explained
78% and 66% of the total variance in OFT and MIS respectively, while the retained
factors in FA explained 61% in OFT and 58% in MIS (Table 3). The first principal
component and FA factor from the open field (OF1) had high loadings for behaviours
related to exploration and activity. OF2 was characterised primarily by behaviours of
locomotion and immobility (“hang” and “locomotion” in PC2 and “locomotion” and
“immobile” in FA2). OF3 in PCA was composed of behaviours related to both activity
(“locomotion” and “rise”) and immobility (“hang”) of the animal. In the mirror test, MIS1
in both PCA and FA had high loadings for “locomotion” and exploration behaviours,
MIS2 for behaviours related to the sociability of the animal (“back”, “front”, “no aggressive”), while MIS3 in PCA was related to the behaviours “hang” and “watch” (Table 3).
There was a good correspondence between the grouping of behaviours from our EB
method and that obtained with PCA or FA (Tables S2 and S3, Supplemental material). For
OFT, the personality trait shyness was negatively correlated with PC1 (r = − 0.90,
P < 0.0001), FA1 (r = − 0.70, P < 0.0001) and FA2 (r = − 0.73, P < 0.0001) scores. Both
exploration and activity were positively related to PC1 (exploration: r = 0.82, P < 0.0001;
activity: r = 0.86, P < 0.0001) and FA1 (exploration: r = 0.83, P < 0.0001; activity: r = 0.71,
P < 0.0001). Activity was also positively correlated to FA2 (r = 0.70, P < 0.0001). Hence, the
first PC and FA factor determined an activity-shyness continuum but they were also
strongly correlated with the EB personality trait exploration. For MIS, PC2 scores were
positively associated with the personality trait avoidance (r = 0.75, P < 0.0001) and
negatively with sociability (r = − 0.90; P < 0.0001), and FA2 was negatively associated
with avoidance (r = − 0.95, P < 0.0001) and positively with sociability (r = 0.92; P < 0.0001).
In this case, the second factor and component determined the sociability axis (Table S3,
Supplemental Material). We did not record any event of scratch during both experiments
or events of attack towards the mirror during MIS.
10
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Table 3.
Principal component analysis and factor analysis for OFT (6 min) and MIS (4 min) of red squirrels. Bold
type indicates behaviours that contributed importantly to a component.
PCA
OF2
FA
Behaviour
OF1
OF3
Dip
0.370
0.282
− 0.244
Hang
0.160
− 0.582
0.650
OF1
0.588
0.159
0.188
Locomotion
0.122
− 0.679
− 0.526
Rise
0.426
0.106
0.371
0.773
Scan
0.456
− 0.271
0.663
Sniff
0.430
0.264
0.145
0.806
− 0.497
0.198
Immobile
OF2
0.433
0.602
− 0.667
− 0.705
% Total variance
47
17
14
35
16
SD
1.82
1.09
0.99
–
–
MIS1
MIS2
MIS3
MIS1
MIS2
Behaviour
Dip
− 0.258
Hang
− 0.191
0.415
− 0.181
0.814
0.987
0.142
−0.149
0.626
0.154
0.858
Locomotion
− 0.356
Rise
− 0.458
Scan
− 0.451
Sniff
− 0.380
0.153
Back
0.254
0.521
Slow
− 0.346
Watch
− 0.189
0.255
− 0.167
Front
No aggressive
0.848
MIS3
0.108
0.150
0.667
− 0.247
− 0.939
0.234
0.540
0.109
− 0.219
0.141
− 0.305
0.234
− 0.130
−0.130
− 0.560
− 0.293
− 0.193
0.892
− 0.136
− 0.513
− 0.141
0.102
0.761
% Total variance
32
23
11
26
21
10
SD
1.89
1.58
1.08
–
–
–
Repeatability
Using the restricted data (animals with more than one arena test, n = 20) we
observed repeatability within individuals for most of the PCA components and FA
factors and also for some EB personality traits. In OFT, both PC1 (R = 47%,
LRT = 3.86, df = 1, P = 0.02) and PC2 scores (R = 50%, LRT = 4.59, df = 1, P = 0.01)
were significantly consistent within individuals. FA1 had also a highly significant
repeatability (R = 55%, LRT = 5.63, df = 1; P = 0.009). PC3 and the other two FA factors
had poor and non-significant repeatability (Table S4, Supplemental Material). In MIS,
only PC1 and FA1 were repeatable (PC1: R = 57%, LRT = 6.53, df = 1; P = 0.005; FA1:
R = 46%, LRT = 3.65, df = 1; P = 0.03).
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
11
For the personality traits, in OFT repeatability was 55% for activity
(LRT = 5.75, df = 1; P = 0.008) and 48% in MIS for the group “other” (LRT = 4.02,
df = 1; P = 0.02). All other behaviours had lower repeatability (Table S4,
Supplemental Material).
Personality of grey squirrels
For grey squirrels, we performed 128 arena tests on 83 individuals (36 males and
47 females); 38 grey squirrels were tested more than 1 time (79 arena tests). The
proportion of time spent by grey squirrels in different behaviours is reported in
Table S1 (Supplemental Material).
Comparison of analytical methods
In OFT, two Principal Components (PC1 and PC2) and factors (FA1 and FA2)
were retained. In MIS, two principal components and three factors (Table 4). The
retained components of the PCA explained 68% and 50% of total variance in OFT and
MIS respectively. For Factor Analysis, the retained factors explained 61% of total
variance in OFT and 54% in MIS (Table 4). The first two PCs and FA factors of OFT
(OFT1-2) had high loadings for behaviours related to exploration and activity. MIS1 of
both PCA and FA was characterised by behaviours related to activity and exploration,
while MIS2 separated the animals that spent time in front of the arena from those that
stayed in the back far from the mirror. MIS3 in FA had high loadings for the behaviours
“hang” and “back” (Table 4).
PC and FA scores correlated significantly with EB personality traits. For OFT,
shyness was negatively correlated with PC1 (r = − 0.88, P < 0.0001) and FA1 (r = − 0.89,
P < 0.0001) scores. Both the exploration and activity traits were positively related to PC1
(exploration: r = 0.81, P < 0.0001; activity: r = 0.80, P < 0.0001) and FA1 (exploration:
r = 0.82, P < 0.0001; activity: r = 0.78, P < 0.0001). Hence, as for red squirrels, the first
PC and factor determined an activity-shyness continuum but could not separate activity
from exploration (Tables S2, Supplemental Material).
For MIS, PC1 scores were positively associated with the personality trait avoidance (r = 0.60, P < 0.0001) and negatively with sociability (r = − 0.56; P < 0.0001), and
also PC2 was negatively associated with sociability (r = − 0.53, P < 0.0001). FA2 was
negatively associated with avoidance (r = − 0.79, P < 0.0001) and positively with
sociability (r = 0.97; P < 0.0001). Hence, the second factor and the first component
determined the sociability axis (Table S3, Supplemental Material). The trait alert was
slightly correlated to PC1, PC2, FA1 and FA3 (all P ≤ 0.03). We did not record any event
of attack towards the mirror during MIS.
Repeatability
Using the restricted data (animals with more than one arena test, n = 38) we
observed repeatability within individuals for the first PCA component and FA factor and
also for three EB personality traits. In OFT, PC1 (R = 42%, LRT = 6.47, df = 1, P = 0.005)
and FA1 (R = 38%, LRT = 4.97, df = 1, P = 0.02) were significantly consistent within
12
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Table 4.
Principal component analysis and factor analysis for OFT (6 min) and MIS (4 min) of grey squirrels. Bold
type indicates behaviours that contributed importantly to a component.
PCA
Behavioural state
OF1
Dip
0.446
Hang
FA
OF2
OF1
− 0.617
− 0.207
0.750
Locomotion
0.385
0.600
Rise
0.485
0.798
Scan
0.309
Sniff
0.472
Scrat
Immobile
OF2
0.323
0.523
0.934
− 0.280
0.833
0.284
− 0.355
0.453
0.440
− 0.122
0.621
− 0.133
− 0.989
% Total variance
40
28
35
27
SD
1.79
1.50
–
–
MIS1
MIS2
MIS1
MIS2
Behavioural state
Dip
Hang
− 0.285
0.534
0.145
−0.462
− 0.198
Locomotion
− 0.314
0.182
0.567
Rise
− 0.357
0.203
0.740
Scan
− 0.352
0.214
− 0.197
0.958
0.668
0.103
− 0.175
0.841
0.151
Sniff
− 0.420
Scrat
− 0.132
− 0.235
Back
0.129
0.577
Slow
− 0.392
− 0.136
0.691
0.160
0.192
Watch
MIS3
0.219
0.212
− 0.518
− 0.715
0.259
− 0.221
Front
− 0.228
− 0.380
0.105
0.990
No aggressive
− 0.351
− 0.278
0.493
0.518
% Total variance
33
17
26
14
13
SD
2.00
1.42
–
–
–
individuals. In MIS, only PC1 and FA1 were repeatable (PC1: R = 49%, LRT = 10.5,
df = 1; P = 0.0005; FA1: R = 40%, LRT = 6.78, df = 1; P = 0.005).
EB personality traits in OFT had a repeatability of 45% for activity (LRT = 8.6,
df = 1; P = 0.002). In MIS, it was 35% for sociability (LRT = 4.44, df = 1; P = 0.02) and
49% for the group “other” (LRT = 10.3, df = 1; P = 0.0007). All other behaviours had
non-significant repeatability (Table S4, Supplemental Material).
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
13
Methodological factors affecting personality measures using arena tests
Comparing the personality indices derived from shorter-time (OFT 4 min, MIS
3 min) and longer-time arena tests (OFT 6 min, MIS 4 min), the LMM models for both
tests (OFT and MIS) were statistically significant for all the personality traits, PC and
FA scores (Table S5, Supplemental Material). All slope estimates are highly significant
and close to 1.0 showing that reducing time of the arena test did not alter proportion of
time spent in different behaviours (or values of PCA/FA scores). In other words, the
shorter and longer arena tests resulted in nearly identical measures of an individual’s
personality traits.
There was a significant effect of the number of test repetition per animal.
Considering only the consistent (hence repeatable) expert-based traits for each species
(activity for red and grey squirrels in OFT and sociability in MIS for grey squirrels) we
found that squirrels of both species were less active in the second and third trial than in
the first (Table S6, Supplemental Material). Sociability scores of grey squirrels did not
differ significantly among subsequent tests. Moreover, in both species, there were no
differences in activity or sociability measured in the arena test when an individual was
trapped for the first time compared to arena tests carried out during recaptures. There
was no significant relationship of a squirrel’s sex or body mass with the measured
personality traits (Table S6, Supplemental Material).
DISCUSSION
In this study we investigated whether individual variation in behaviour of
Eurasian red squirrels and Eastern grey squirrels recorded during the Open Field Test
(OFT) and the Mirror Image Stimulation test (MIS) could be used to determine their
personality. We compared the performance of commonly used Principal Component
Analysis and Factor Analysis to measure personality traits and then compared both with
an EB classification of behaviours into groups reflecting personality traits. In red
squirrels, the behaviour in OFT yielded one multivariate variable representing activity
and exploration and one representing shyness, while the behaviour during MIS yielded
one component reflecting activity and/or exploration and a second reflecting sociability.
PCA and FA produced comparable results. In grey squirrels, OFT yielded the same
pattern as in red squirrels. However, MIS produced a better discrimination of behaviours when FA was used than with PCA: only FA yielded two clearly separated multivariate factors, the first representing activity and exploration the second the sociability–
avoidance axis. For red squirrels, using OFT, the personality traits activity and exploration showed moderate to high repeatability with all three methods (PCA, FA and EB),
while shyness only with PCA and EB approach. In this species sociability measured
during MIS had low repeatability. For grey squirrels, the analytical methods PCA and
FA had high repeatabilities for the multivariate component/factor representing activity
and exploration during both OFT and MIS. During OFT the EB approach performed
less well (high repeatability for activity but low for exploration) while during MIS
moderate to high repeatabilities were reported for activity-exploration and sociability
with EB and FA. Thus, overall our EB classification of personality traits produced
similar results as the two analytical methods. There were some differences in performance between PCA and FA, with FA slightly better in discriminating personality traits.
However, our analyses also revealed limitations of using arena tests for the two
squirrel species: repeatability of measures, an essential characteristic for a behaviour to
14
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
be considered a personality trait (Bell et al. 2009; Carter et al. 2013) was moderate-high
for activity/exploration in both species and for being more or less social in grey squirrels
only. Also, PCA and FA did not distinguish between activity related behaviours and
exploration behaviours, grouping them in the same component or factor.
Use and duration of arena tests
In recent years, personality has begun to receive theoretical and empirical attention
from ecologists and both OFT and MIS are generally considered reliable techniques to
measure personality traits consistent over time and under different environmental conditions (Dingemanse et al. 2002; Bierbach et al. 2015; Haage et al. 2017).
A review on previous studies of free-ranging rodent personality showed that arena
test has been used to test the relationship between personality and different ecological
parameters, to study personality itself or its heritability and ontogeny. Remarkably,
there is no agreement among studies on a standard duration of the tests. For OFT the
duration of the test was between 1.5 and 10 min, while for MIS between 5 and 15 min
(Table 1). None of the papers reported why they used a specific duration (but see
Montiglio et al. 2010) and sometimes, even considering the same species, different
authors used different durations. However, test duration can affect the results (hence
reliability): too short tests may cause loss of information (e.g. not register infrequent
but potentially important behaviours), while longer tests may stress the animal or result
in habituation to the arena. In fact, if individuals express different temporal patterns of
behaviours within a single OFT (or MIS), then the reliability of cumulative proportions
of a behaviour type might be affected by the duration of the tests (see fig. 1 in Montiglio
et al. 2010).
Here, we showed that reducing the duration of OFT and MIS (of 6 + 4 min to
4 + 3 min) did not change an individual squirrel’s personality-trait measures. This is
important because shorter arena tests are likely to reduce physiological stress in animals (Dosmann et al. 2015; Dantzer et al. 2016) and the risk of habituation to the
experimental conditions (Montiglio et al. 2010).
Arena test reliability: comparing statistical analyses and expert-based methods
Our EB method that classified behaviours relying on ethological knowledge of the
species and operator experience, yielded personality traits that correlated strongly with
the scores derived from the retained components and factors of traditional PCA and FA.
One advantage of using EB approach is that the values (sum of percentages of the
behaviours grouped together in a trait) remain fixed independent of data management
operations (adding new data, using restricted data sets). With PCA and FA in contrast,
loadings and hence the derived scores along one of the components/factors for an
individual animal will change every time a new data set is analysed (or new animals
are added/removed). Moreover, reviewing previous studies on rodents’ personality, we
found that the first two-three components of PCA not always explained a wide proportion of the total variance (e.g Martin & Réale 2008; Montiglio et al. 2012; Merrick &
Koprowski 2017). Based on changes in loadings estimated with PCA, different behaviours can be associated to the same personality trait in different studies. For example,
for the American red squirrel Tamiasciurus hudsonicus, Boon et al. (2007) identified
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
15
“activity” as a group of locomotion + sniff + dip + rise (variables names have been
changed accordingly to our classification names to make the reading easier but the
behaviour described is the same) while Taylor et al. (2012) as locomotion + dip and
Merrick and Koprowski (2017) as locomotion + scratch/chew. These different interpretations, mainly due to analytical reasons inherent to PCA, do not have a clear ecological
meaning and, in our opinion, such results are difficult to compare or even to allow
developing a general theory of effects of personality on a species’ life-history. The
expert-based method we proposed avoids these problems, but might introduce others
related to different interpretation of certain behaviours by different researchers. For
example, Haigh et al. (2017) also used such an approach of grouping behaviours in
general categories in their study on variation in red squirrel behaviour under different
densities. However, in contrast with ours and previous studies on sciurids, they considered slow approach and touching the mirror as aggressive behaviours together with
actual attacking it (“bang” see Table 1 in Haigh et al. 2017), while we considered the
former two behaviours as part of social behaviour (“sociability”). Observing red squirrels in the wild indicates that aggressive interactions occur when one animal moves
quickly in on the other and chases it in tree canopy, along the trunk or on the ground,
while a slow approach is very rarely followed by an aggressive interaction (Wauters &
Dhondt 1987, 1989; Wauters & Gurnell 1999).
MIS and problems with measuring reaction to a conspecific
Surprisingly, we did not record any attack on the mirror in either red or grey
squirrels. This might be related to the type of test (MIS) where the animal’s reactions
control the image. In other words, the reflected animal is as active as the interacting
animal in the arena and the image can never present a submissive or aggressive gesture
unless it is initiated by the latter (Svendsen & Armitage 1973). This could explain why in
our case we had no cases of aggressive interaction with the mirror from both species
(but see Haigh et al. 2017 for red squirrels). Aggressiveness to the mirror is common in
territorial North American red squirrels (Boon et al. 2008; Shonfield et al. 2012; Kelley
et al. 2015). In contrast, grey squirrels form female-kin groups, with a dominance
hierarchy among resident adults, related to environmental cues (important resources
inside the animal’s home range, Koprowski 1993). Without these cues, MIS succeeded
to measure an individual’s tendency to behave sociable or avoid a conspecific, but not to
record aggressiveness. Eurasian red squirrels have overlapping home ranges, but adult
females defend exclusive core-areas against other females (intra-sexual territoriality,
Wauters & Dhondt 1992). Hence we expected some degree of aggressiveness at least in
adult females and dominant males (Wauters & Dhondt 1989). Possibly, aggressive
behaviour in the wild may be a conditional behaviour related to the intensity of intruder
pressure and thus to density of squirrels of the same sex. Densities in populations where
aggressive interactions were documented (Belgium) were higher than in the Italian sites
where we did arena tests (Belgium: 0.8–1.5 ind/ha; Italy: 0.1–0.6 ind/ha; Wauters et al.
2004, 2008; Rodrigues et al. 2010). Lack of an aggressive response to MIS was also
documented in fox squirrels (Sciurus niger) and cliff chipmunks (Tamias dorsalis),
which have similar social organisation as Eurasian red squirrels or engage in social
nesting (Kilanowski 2015; Krause et al. 2015).
16
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Repeatability: being critical about your results
For behaviours to be considered part of an individual’s personality, they should be
consistent over time and under different contexts (Bell et al. 2009). However, it must be
underlined that recent studies have shown that repeatability of personality traits, in
particular exploration speed and/or tendency, can vary over time (with age) and that the
behaviour may increase with test sequence (Carere et al. 2005; Dingemanse et al. 2012;
Kanda et al. 2012). Such variation in behaviour may reflect a true flexible component of
personality, but it may also be the result of a changing response to the artificial
conditions of the arena test. In our study, both squirrel species showed a consistent
activity-exploration pattern in OFT, but only grey squirrels showed a repeatable sociability trait in MIS, probably related to the above mentioned social organisation of this
species. The other EB traits had low or at best moderate repeatability. A non-significant
repeatability was reported also for the PC scores in other studies. In Boon et al. (2007)
the PC2 in MIS was associated to avoidance and in Kilanowski (2015) PC1 and PC2
were associated to sociality and image engagement but they were not repeatable. The
lack of repeatability precludes the possibility to measure relationships between personality and ecological or fitness parameters. In fact, lack of repeatability (lack of consistency over time), suggests the measured traits were not part of the individual’s
personality but were rather flexible phenotypic traits (Boyer et al. 2010), or that their
validity to measure a personality trait was low (Carter et al. 2013).
Other parameters to consider
Previous studies have shown that the number of times arena tests are repeated might
affect personality measures (e.g. Dingemanse et al. 2012 for birds). We found that individual
red and grey squirrels tended to be less active in subsequent tests than in the first one,
suggesting some degree of habituation to the arena reducing the activity during OFT. Such
a decrease in the intensity of activity/exploration behaviours over trails was found in many
studies on birds and small mammals (Archer 1973; Dingemanse et al. 2002; Boon et al.
2007, 2008; Martin & Réale 2008; Boyer et al. 2010; Montiglio et al. 2010; Taylor et al. 2012).
Carter et al. (2013) suggested that the context of the open-field test (free vs forced
test context) may create bias in behaviours: free open-field tests are more likely to
measure voluntary exploration/curiosity and information gathering behaviour, while
forced open-field tests might also be measuring fear and/or anxiety (Misslin & Cigrang
1986). In this study, we did not record any difference in personality between squirrels
trapped for the first time or recaptured. Animals trapped for the first time live a stressing
situation never experienced before but this did not affect their activity or sociability,
indicating that the tests used measured their personality and not their fearfulness. To
further exclude that OFT could be measuring a mixture of different personality traits
simultaneously, we will compare direct arena test measures of personality traits with
indirect ones derived from standardised capture-mark-recapture data (convergent validity) in future studies (see also Boon et al. 2008; Boyer et al. 2010; Carter et al. 2013).
CONCLUSIONS
The arena test allows researchers to study animal personality in a controlled
environment, determining personality traits that are repeatable within, and comparable
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
17
among individuals. We suggest that the use of arena tests should be preceded by studying
the behaviour of the animals in wild, to facilitate defining ethograms and the classification of single behaviours in groups linked to personality traits. Using different approaches
to measure a single personality trait (EB and FA) will be useful to evaluate whether an EB
distinction is also supported by the statistical approach. For rodent personality research,
we recommend to start testing a small group of animals for a longer duration (not more
than 7 min/test) and next evaluate which shorter duration is valid to measure interindividual differences in personality without losing information. Afterwards, animals can be
tested only for the short time, reducing stress and habituation as well as operator time.
The number and frequency of test replicates will depend on different parameters: the
research questions, the kind of repeatability that is in question (long-term, short-term),
and/or the species’ longevity. For tree squirrels, considering within-year repeatability, we
suggest to test an individual no more than 2 times per season (e.g. breeding season) or
year and to allow for at least 2 months between repetitions. Where personality is expected
to have both a fixed and a variable component (Carere et al. 2005; Dingemanse et al.
2012), personality traits should be examined each year to study potential variation linked
with, for example, age, dominance status or breeding experience.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Authors thank Regione Lombardia, Provincia di Torino, Stelvio National Park and all the
students involved in the survey. We are also grateful to the natural reserve – sic/zps “Bosco WWF
Vanzago” for access to the woodlands and housing facilities. Constructive comments by three
anonymous referees greatly helped to improve the manuscript. This paper is number 29 of ASPER
(Alpine Squirrel Population Ecology Research) project.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Maria Vittoria Mazzamuto and Giacomo Cremonesi contributed equally to this work.
SUPPLEMENTAL DATA
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed at http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03949370.
2018.1488768
REFERENCES
Archer J. 1973. Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: a review. Anim Behav. 21:205–235.
Armitage KB. 1984. The biology of ground-dwelling squirrels. In: Murie JO, Michener GR, editors.
Recruitment in yellow-bellied marmot populations: kinship, philopatry, and individual
variability. Lincoln (NE): University of Nebraska Press; p. 377-403.
18
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Armitage KB. 1986. Individuality, social behavior, and reproductive success in yellow-bellied
marmots. Ecology. 67:1186–1193.
Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2014a. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4.
ArXiv Prepr ArXiv14065823. Available from: http://arxiv.org/abs/1406.5823 [Accessed 13
May 2016].
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, Christensen RHB, Singmann H, Dai B, Eigen C,
Rcpp L. 2014b. Package ‘lme4’. Available from: http://cran.r-mirror.de/web/packages/lme4/
lme4.pdf [Accessed 5 Feb 2015].
Bell AM, Hankison SJ, Laskowski KL. 2009. The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis.
Anim Behav. 77:771–783.
Bester-Meredith JK, Marler CA. 2007. Social experience during development and female offspring aggression in Peromyscus mice. Ethology. 113:889–900.
Bierbach D, Sommer-Trembo C, Hanisch J, Wolf M, Plath M. 2015. Personality affects mate
choice: bolder males show stronger audience effects under high competition. Behav Ecol.
26:1314–1325.
Blumstein DT, Holland B-D, Daniel JC. 2006. Predator discrimination and “personality” in
captive Vancouver Island marmots (Marmota vancouverensis). Anim Conserv. 9:274–282.
Bohn SJ, Webber QMR, Florko KRN, Paslawski KR, Peterson AM, Piche JE, Menzies AK,
Willis CKR. 2017. Personality predicts ectoparasite abundance in an asocial sciurid.
Ethology. 123:761–771.
Boon AK, Réale D, Boutin S. 2007. The interaction between personality, offspring fitness and
food abundance in North American red squirrels. Ecol Lett. 10:1094–1104.
Boon AK, Réale D, Boutin S. 2008. Personality, habitat use, and their consequences for survival
in North American red squirrels Tamiasciurus hudsonicus. Oikos. 117:1321–1328.
Both C, Dingemanse NJ, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM. 2005. Pairs of extreme avian personalities
have highest reproductive success. J Anim Ecol. 74:667–674.
Boyer N, Réale D, Marmet J, Pisanu B, Chapuis J-L. 2010. Personality, space use and tick load in
an introduced population of Siberian chipmunks Tamias sibiricus. J Anim Ecol. 79:538–547.
Brown C, Burgess F, Braithwaite VA. 2007. Heritable and experiential effects on boldness in a
tropical poeciliid. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 62:237–243.
Budaev SV. 2010. Using principal components and factor analysis in animal behaviour research:
caveats and guidelines. Ethology. 116:472–480.
Budaev SV, Zworykin DD, Mochek AD. 1999. Consistency of individual differences in behaviour
of the lion-headed cichlid, Steatocranus casuarius. Behav Processes. 48:49–55.
Carere C, Drent PJ, Privitera L, Koolhaas JM, Groothuis TGG. 2005. Personalities in great tits,
Parus major: stability and consistency. Anim Behav. 70:795–805.
Carere C, van Oers K. 2004. Shy and bold great tits (Parus major): body temperature and breath
rate in response to handling stress. Physiol Behav. 82:905–912.
Carter AJ, Feeney WE, Marshall HH, Cowlishaw G, Heinsohn R. 2013. Animal personality:
what are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biol Rev. 88:465–475.
Cote J, Clobert J. 2007. Social personalities influence natal dispersal in a lizard. Proc R Soc Lond
B. 274:383–390.
Cote J, Clobert J, Brodin T, Fogarty S, Sih A. 2010. Personality-dependent dispersal: characterization, ontogeny and consequences for spatially structured populations. Philos Trans R Soc
Lond B. 365:4065–4076.
Dammhahn M. 2012. Are personality differences in a small iteroparous mammal maintained by a
life-history trade-off? Proc R Soc Lond B. 279:2645–2651.
Dantzer B, Santicchia F, van Kesteren F, Palme R, Martinoli A, Wauters LA. 2016.
Measurement of fecal glucocorticoid metabolite levels in Eurasian red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris): effects of captivity, sex, reproductive condition, and season. J Mammal. 97:1385–1398.
Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, van Oers K, van Noordwijk AJ. 2002. Repeatability and
heritability of exploratory behaviour in great tits from the wild. Anim Behav. 64:929–938.
Dingemanse NJ, Both C, van Noordwijk AJ, Rutten AL, Drent PJ. 2003. Natal dispersal and
personalities in great tits (Parus major). Proc R Soc Lond B. 270:741–747.
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
19
Dingemanse NJ, Bouwman KM, van de Pol M, van Overveld T, Patrick SC, Matthysen E,
Quinn JL. 2012. Variation in personality and behavioural plasticity across four populations
of the great tit Parus major: population variation in personality and behavioural plasticity. J
Anim Ecol. 81:116–126.
Dochtermann NA, Jenkins SH. 2007. Behavioural syndromes in Merriam’s kangaroo rats
(Dipodomys merriami): a test of competing hypotheses. Proc R Soc Lond B. 274:2343–2349.
Dosmann AJ, Brooks KC, Mateo JM. 2015. Within-individual correlations reveal link between a
behavioral syndrome, condition, and cortisol in free-ranging Belding’s ground squirrels.
Ethology. 121:125–134.
Drent PJ, van Oers K, van Noordwijk AJ. 2003. Realized heritability of personalities in the great
tit (Parus major). Proc R Soc Lond B. 270:45–51.
Ferrari C, Pasquaretta C, Carere C, Cavallone E, von Hardenberg A, Réale D. 2013. Testing for
the presence of coping styles in a wild mammal. Anim Behav. 85:1385–1396.
Fraser DF, Gilliam JF, Daley MJ, Le AN, Skalski GT. 2001. Explaining leptokurtic movement
distributions: intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration. Am Nat. 158:124–135.
Gallup GGJ. 1968. Mirror-image stimulation. Psycol Bull. 70:782–793.
Gracceva G, Herde A, Groothuis TGG, Koolhaas JM, Palme R, Eccard JA. 2014. Turning shy
on a winter’s day: effects of season on personality and stress response in Microtus arvalis.
Ethology. 120:753–767.
Gurnell J, Wauters LA, Preatoni D, Tosi G. 2001. Spacing behaviour, kinship, and population
dynamics of grey squirrels in a newly colonized broadleaf woodland in Italy. Can J Zool.
79:1533–1543.
Haage M, Maran T, Bergvall UA, Elmhagen B, Angerbjörn A. 2017. The influence of spatiotemporal conditions and personality on survival in reintroductions–evolutionary implications. Oecologia. 183:45–56.
Haigh A, O’Riordan R, Butler F. 2017. Variations in aggression and activity levels amongst
squirrels inhabiting low and high density areas. Ecol Res. 32:931–941.
Hall CS, Ballechey E. 1932. A study of the rat’s behavior in a field: a contribution to method in
comparative psychology. Univ Calif Publ Psycol. 6:1–12.
Hänninen L, Pastell M. 2009. CowLog: open-source software for coding behaviors from digital
video. Behav Res Methods. 41:472–476.
Jenkins SH. 2011. Sex differences in repeatability of food-hoarding behaviour of kangaroo rats.
Anim Behav. 81:1155–1162.
Kanda LL, Louon L, Straley K. 2012. Stability in activity and boldness across time and context in
captive Siberian dwarf hamsters. Ethology. 118:518–533.
Kelley AD, Humphries MM, McAdam AG, Boutin S. 2015. Changes in wild red squirrel personality across ontogeny: activity and aggression regress towards the mean. Behaviour.
152:1291–1306.
Kilanowski AL. 2015. Individual behavioral phenotypes of the cliff chipmunk (Tamias dorsalis):
effects on female reproductive success and juvenile habitat selection. Tucson (AZ):
University of Arizona. Available from: http://arizona.openrepository.com/arizona/handle/
10150/582370 [Accessed 16 Feb 2017].
Koprowski J. 1993. Alternative reproductive tactics in male eastern gray squirrels: “making the
best of a bad job”. Behav Ecol. 4:165–171.
Korpela K, Sundell J, Ylönen H. 2011. Does personality in small rodents vary depending on
population density? Oecologia. 165:67–77.
Kortet R, Hedrick A. 2007. A behavioural syndrome in the field cricket Gryllus integer: intrasexual
aggression is correlated with activity in a novel environment. Biol J Linn Soc. 91:475–482.
Krackow S. 2003. Motivational and heritable determinants of dispersal latency in wild male house
mice (Mus musculus musculus). Ethology. 109:671–689.
Krause SK, Van Vuren DH, Laursen C, Kelt DA. 2015. Behavioral effects of an immunocontraceptive vaccine on eastern fox squirrels. J Wildl Manag. 79:1255–1263.
Lantová P, Zub K, Koskela E, Šíchová K, Borowski Z. 2011. Is there a linkage between
metabolism and personality in small mammals? The root vole (Microtus oeconomus) example. Physiol Behav. 104:378–383.
20
M.V. Mazzamuto et al.
Le Cœur C, Thibault M, Pisanu B, Thibault S, Chapuis J-L, Baudry E. 2015. Temporally
fluctuating selection on a personality trait in a wild rodent population. Behav Ecol.
26:1285–1291.
Lessells CM, Boag PT. 1987. Unrepeatable repeatabilities: a common mistake. Auk. 104:116–121.
López P, Hawlena D, Polo V, Amo L, Martín J. 2005. Sources of individual shy–bold variations
in antipredator behaviour of male Iberian rock lizards. Anim Behav. 69:1–9.
Malange J, Izar P, Japyassú H. 2016. Personality and behavioural syndrome in Necromys lasiurus
(Rodentia: Cricetidae): notes on dispersal and invasion processes. Acta Ethologica. 19:189–195.
Martin JGA, Réale D. 2008. Temperament, risk assessment and habituation to novelty in eastern
chipmunks, Tamias striatus. Anim Behav. 75:309–318.
Merrick MJ, Koprowski JL. 2017. Should we consider individual behavior differences in applied
wildlife conservation studies? Biol Conserv. 209:34–44.
Misslin R, Cigrang M. 1986. Does neophobia necessarily imply fear or anxiety? Behav Processes.
12:45–50.
Montiglio P-O, Garant D, Pelletier F, Réale D. 2012. Personality differences are related to longterm stress reactivity in a population of wild eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus. Anim
Behav. 84:1071–1079.
Montiglio P-O, Garant D, Thomas D, Réale D. 2010. Individual variation in temporal activity
patterns in open-field tests. Anim Behav. 80:905–912.
Myers JH, Krebs CJ. 1971. Genetic, behavioral, and reproductive attributes of dispersing field
voles Microtus pennsylvanicus and Microtus ochrogaster. Ecol Monogr. 41:53–78.
R Development Core Team. 2016. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/
[Accessed 16 Oct 2016].
Réale D, Gallant BY, Leblanc M, Festa-Bianchet M. 2000. Consistency of temperament in
bighorn ewes and correlates with behaviour and life history. Anim Behav. 60:589–597.
Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ. 2007. Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev. 82:291–318.
Rehage JS, Sih A. 2004. Dispersal behavior, boldness, and the link to invasiveness: a comparison
of four Gambusia species. Biol Invasions. 6:379–391.
Rodrigues D, Wauters LA, Romeo C, Mari V, Preatoni D, Mathias ML, Tosi G, Martinoli A.
2010. Living on the edge: can eurasian red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris) persist in extreme
high-elevation habitats? Arct Antarct Alp Res. 42:106–112.
Salmaso F, Molinari A, Di Pierro E, Ghisla A, Martinoli A, Preatoni D, Serino G, Tosi G,
Bertolino S, Wauters LA. 2009. Estimating and comparing food availability for tree‐seed
predators in typical pulsed‐resource systems: alpine conifer forests. Plant Biosyst Int J Deal
Asp Plant Biol. 143:258–267.
Shonfield J, Humphries MM, Boutin S, McAdam AG, Taylor RW. 2012. Territorial defence
behaviour in red squirrels is influenced by local density. Behaviour. 149:369–390.
Šíchová K, Koskela E, Mappes T, Lantová P, Boratyński Z. 2014. On personality, energy
metabolism and mtDNA introgression in bank voles. Anim Behav. 92:229–237.
Sih A, Bell A, Johnson JC. 2004. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview.
Trends Ecol Evol. 19:372–378.
Sih A, Cote J, Evans M, Fogarty S, Pruitt J. 2012. Ecological implications of behavioural
syndromes. Ecol Lett. 15:278–289.
Simeonovska-Nikolova DM. 2000. Strategies in open field behaviour of Mus spicilegus and Mus
musculus musculus. Belg J Zool. 130:115–120.
Sinn DL, Apiolaza LA, Moltschaniwskyj NA. 2006. Heritability and fitness-related consequences
of squid personality traits. J Evol Biol. 19:1437–1447.
Smith BR, Blumstein DT. 2008. Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behav
Ecol. 19:448–455.
Stoffel M, Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. 2017. rptR: repeatability estimation for Gaussian and nonGaussian data. Available from: https://rdrr.io/cran/rptR/ [Accessed 11 Oct 2017].
Svartberg K, Tapper I, Temrin H, Radesäter T, Thorman S. 2005. Consistency of personality
traits in dogs. Anim Behav. 69:283–291.
Methods for measuring personality using arena test
21
Svendsen GE, Armitage KB. 1973. Mirror-image stimulation applied to field behavioral studies.
Ecology. 54:623–627.
Taylor RW, Boon AK, Dantzer B, Réale D, Humphries MM, Boutin S, Gorrell JC, Coltman
DW, McAdam AG. 2012. Low heritabilities, but genetic and maternal correlations between
red squirrel behaviours. J Evol Biol. 25:614–624.
Walsh RN, Cummins RA. 1976. The open-field test: a critical review. Psychol Bull. 83:482–504.
Wauters LA, Dhondt AA. 1987. Activity budget and foraging behaviour of the red squirrel (Sciurus
vulgaris Linnaeus, 1758) in a coniferous habitat. Z Säugetierkd. 52:341–353.
Wauters LA, Dhondt AA. 1989. Body weight, longevity and reproductive success in red squirrels
(Sciurus vulgaris). J Anim Ecol. 58:637–651.
Wauters LA, Dhondt AA. 1992. Spacing behaviour of red squirrels, Sciurus vulgaris: variation
between habitats and the sexes. Anim Behav. 43:297–311.
Wauters AL, Githiru M, Bertolino S, Molinari A, Tosi G, Lens L. 2008. Demography of alpine
red squirrel populations in relation to fluctuations in seed crop size. Ecography. 31:104–114.
Wauters LA, Gurnell J. 1999. The mechanism of replacement of red squirrels by grey squirrels: a
test of the interference competition hypothesis. Ethology. 105:1053–1071.
Wauters LA, Gurnell J, Martinoli A, Tosi G. 2001. Does interspecific competition with introduced grey squirrels affect foraging and food choice of Eurasian red squirrels? Anim Behav.
61:1079–1091.
Wauters LA, Lurz PWW, Gurnell J. 2000. Interspecific effects of grey squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) on the space use and population demography of red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris) in
conifer plantations. Ecol Res. 15:271–284.
Wauters LA, Matthysen E, Adriaensen F, Tosi G. 2004. Within-sex density dependence and
population dynamics of red squirrels Sciurus vulgaris. J Anim Ecol. 73:11–25.
Wilson ADM, McLaughlin RL. 2007. Behavioural syndromes in brook charr, Salvelinus fontinalis: prey-search in the field corresponds with space use in novel laboratory situations. Anim
Behav. 74:689–698.