Plasticity of face processing in infancy
O. Pascalis*†, L. S. Scott‡, D. J. Kelly*, R. W. Shannon§, E. Nicholson§, M. Coleman¶, and C. A. Nelson§储
*LGF Group, Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S10 2TP, United Kingdom; ‡Department of Psychology, University of Colorado,
Boulder, CO 80309; §Institute of Child Development and 储Center for Neurobehavioral Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0345;
and ¶Department of Human Communication Science, University College London, London WC1N 1PG, United Kingdom
Experience plays a crucial role for the normal development of many
perceptual and cognitive functions, such as speech perception. For
example, between 6 and 10 months of age, the infant’s ability to
discriminate among native speech sounds improves, whereas the
ability to discriminate among foreign speech sounds declines.
However, a recent investigation suggests that some experience
with nonnative languages from 9 months of age facilitates the
maintenance of this ability at 12 months. Nelson has suggested
that the systems underlying face processing may be similarly
sculpted by experience with different kinds of faces. In the current
investigation, we demonstrate that, in human infants between 6
and 9 months of age, exposure to nonnative faces, in this case,
faces of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus), facilitates the discrimination of monkey faces, an ability that is otherwise lost
around 9 months of age. These data support, and further elucidate,
the role of early experience in the development of face processing.
development 兩 early experience 兩 monkey 兩 recognition
A
mong the numerous visual inputs that we receive each
moment, the human face is perhaps one of the most salient.
The importance of the many signals it conveys (e.g., emotion,
identity, direction of eye gaze, etc.) and the speed and ease with
which adults typically process this information are compelling
reasons to suppose that brain circuits specialized for processing
faces may exist (1). However, there is still considerable debate
as to whether face processing is a truly special perceptual process
and is organized as such at birth, or, instead, has its origin in a
more general-purpose perceptual system that becomes specialized with experience (2).
Developmental studies can provide important information to
constrain the claims of the different sides of this debate. It is well
documented that experience is crucial for the normal development of many perceptual and cognitive functions, such as speech
perception. For example, before 6–8 months of age, infants are
able to discriminate among a wide range of phonemes (3). This
ability tends to narrow with repeated exposure to phonemes in
the infant’s native language, and a lack of exposure to phonemes
outside the native language (4–6). However, Kuhl et al. (7)
recently demonstrated that some experience with nonnative
languages from 9 months of age facilitates the maintenance of
this ability at 12 months. The same pattern of results is observed
in infants raised in a bilingual environment (8). Nelson (9, 10)
has suggested that the systems underlying face processing may be
similarly sculpted by experience with different kinds of faces.
Although the pattern of development across speech and face
processing may be similar, it is unlikely that the mechanisms and
developmental trajectory underlying these different perceptual
systems are the same.
Indeed, recent developmental studies have underscored the
importance of visual experience in the development of face
processing. For example, patients with congenital cataracts who
were deprived of patterned visual input for the first months of
life demonstrate intact object processing but subtle deficits in
face processing (11, 12). Moreover, when patients whose visual
input had been restricted mainly to one hemisphere during
infancy were examined, it was found that visual input to the right
hemisphere, but not the left hemisphere, was critical for expert
www.pnas.org兾cgi兾doi兾10.1073兾pnas.0406627102
levels of face processing to develop. This result is consistent with
a model put forth by de Schonen and Mathivet (13) concerning
the precocity of the development of the right hemisphere and its
involvement in face processing.
In addition, Quinn et al. (14) demonstrated that the social
environment also influences the tuning of face processing during
the first months of life. They have shown that 3-month-old
infants prefer to look at female faces when paired with male
faces. This preference may reflect a gender bias of the face
prototype toward the primary caregiver, which in most cases is
female. Importantly, they have identified a population of infants
for whom the father was the primary caregiver; such infants
demonstrate a bias for male faces when tested in the same
manner.
The face-processing system is also influenced by the type of
face experienced during the course of development. One example is the well known ‘‘other-race effect’’ (ORE), in which adults
find it easier to differentiate faces from their own ethnic group
(15). Children demonstrate the same effect (16–22), although
reports differ regarding the onset of the effect, ranging from 3
months (23) to 8 years (18, 19). Recently, Sangrigoli et al. (21)
reported that native French adults and Korean adults who had
moved to France during adulthood both demonstrated the ORE.
Conversely, Korean adults adopted by French families during
childhood (3–9 years old at time of adoption) performed identically to the native French adult population (21). This finding
indicates that the face-processing system remains relatively
plastic throughout childhood, allowing the ORE observed at 3
months of age to be reversed. Furthermore, intensive training
with other-race faces can extinguish the ORE in adults who
initially demonstrate the effect (24–26).
A final example of the importance of early experience is the
‘‘other-species effect,’’ in which both monkey and human adults
are better at recognizing faces from their own species as assessed
with the visual paired comparison (VPC) task (27) or with a
forced-choice task (28). Many researchers attribute the ORE
and other-species effects to the relatively common experience of
having greater exposure to faces of one’s own race compared
with other races (15) and greater experience with faces within
one’s own species compared with other species. Thus, it appears
that these effects can be accounted for by the notion that we are
best at recognizing faces similar to those we see most often (i.e.,
faces of individuals with whom we have most contact, be they of
the same race or the same species). However, it is important to
differentiate between other-race faces, which belong to the same
face category as own-race faces (i.e., human faces), and otherspecies faces, which belong to a separate face category (i.e.,
nonhuman primate). Whereas the face-processing system remains flexible for the category of faces to which we are most
exposed, this plasticity may not extend to other face categories.
Collectively, these studies suggest that visual input during
early infancy and childhood influences the development of many
This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.
Abbreviations: ORE, other-race effect; VPC, visual paired comparison.
†To
whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: o.pascalis@sheffield.ac.uk.
© 2005 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA
PNAS 兩 April 5, 2005 兩 vol. 102 兩 no. 14 兩 5297–5300
PSYCHOLOGY
Edited by Susan E. Carey, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, and approved February 10, 2005 (received for review September 9, 2004)
aspects of face processing. However, the exact nature and origins
of this experience and its effect on the development of face
processing has been the subject of considerable discussion.
According to Valentine’s (29) model, faces are encoded as
individual points within a multidimensional face space defined
by a set of dimensions (gender, eye color, etc.). Valentine
proposes a norm-based coding model, whereby faces are encoded as vectors according to their deviation from the central
tendency, or prototypical average of the face space. Nelson (9,
10) has proposed that this face prototype is broadly tuned at birth
and that the dimensions this prototype encodes may differ both
qualitatively and quantitatively in infants compared with adults.
One way to think about the development or formation of a face
prototype is based on the experience or kinds of faces one
encounters. For example, if this prototype is thought of as a
continuum of all incoming faces, then the more a face deviates
from the prototype (other-race and other-species faces), the less
this face is easily discriminated, compared with faces that are
more similar to the prototype. The development of the face
prototype is most likely influenced by a number of factors,
including, but not limited to, exposure time (number of faces
seen), dynamic and emotionally salient information provided
within the face, the timing and preferences of the development
of the visual system, and changes in the categorization of
individuation of people (i.e., the mother’s face may have more
‘‘weight’’ in the formation of the prototype). Combined, these
experiences gradually lead to the face prototype becoming more
precise.
Early in life, infants possess a remarkable ability to discriminate among and between a large corpus of different faces, such
as faces from an unfamiliar species or an unfamiliar race. With
experience, the infant’s face-representation system becomes
more precise and increasingly restricted to faces with which
infants are most familiar. This, in turn, results in the development of expertise, in which the ability to discriminate between
faces that one has not had exposure to (or has had less exposure
to) is not as good as discrimination between faces with which one
has had experience.
An example of this specialization of the face processing system
was demonstrated in a previous study (30), in which we reported
that although 6-month-olds, 9-month-olds, and adults are all
equally good at discriminating two human faces, only 6-montholds can also discriminate two monkey faces. Thus, it seems that
some time after 6 months of age the face prototype becomes less
generalized and more specific to faces commonly experienced in
one’s environment. Furthermore, uncommon faces, or faces that
differ on the defined prototypical dimensions, are no longer
easily discriminated. This observation led us to ask how flexible
this representation is, and whether we can maintain its early,
more general nature by exposing infants to other-species faces
between the ages of 6 and 9 months.
In our investigation, 6-month-olds were exposed regularly to
Barbary Macaque monkey faces during a 3-month period, and
their ability to discriminate monkey faces was then assessed at 9
months. Their discrimination performance was compared with a
control group of 9-month-olds who received no training. We
hypothesize that if the ontogeny of the face-processing system
progresses from being very broadly tuned to more specific and
narrowly tuned, then exposure to the monkey faces should
extend the ability to discriminate faces from another species.
Experimental Procedures
Infants were tested at 6 and 9 months of age. Each infant was
tested three times. On his兾her first visit, the 6-month-old’s
pretraining ability to discriminate between monkey faces was
assessed with a VPC task. Parents were then provided with a
folder for training purposes, which contained six labeled monkey
images (each monkey had a name) that were different from those
5298 兩 www.pnas.org兾cgi兾doi兾10.1073兾pnas.0406627102
used in the baseline test. Parents of the infants were asked to
present the pictures in the folder to their child for 1–2 min every
day for 2 weeks and then less frequently following a fixed
schedule of exposures during the 3-month period. The infants
returned after the 3-month training period, and their ability to
recognize the monkey faces from the folders was measured by
using a Visual Preference task. During the third test, the infant’s
ability to discriminate between novel monkey faces (i.e., those
not previously seen) was assessed during a posttraining recognition test by using the same recognition task used during their
first visit. A separate group of 9-month-olds, with no previous
exposure to monkey faces, was used to assess an infant’s ability
to discriminate between monkey faces without experience.
Participants. Eighteen 6-month-olds (7 boys and 11 girls; mean
age ⫽ 186 days, ranging from 83 to 194 days) were included in
the training group. Three infants at 6 months of age were not
included because of crying or fussing. Two infants were excluded
because they did not return for testing at 9 months of age. All
infants included for analysis returned at 9 months of age (mean
age ⫽ 274 days, ranging from 268 to 292 days). Thirteen
9-month-olds (8 boys and 5 girls; mean age ⫽ 274 days, ranging
from 269 to 283 days) were included in the control group. One
infant was excluded from the control group because of excessive
crying.
Stimuli. The face stimuli used were 24 color pictures (12 in the
training group and 12 in the control group) of Barbary macaques
(M. sylvanus) presented against a white background (Fig. 1).
Four different series of faces, containing six faces in each, were
used for the experiment. Folders were made for each of the four
series of faces, each containing the images and names of six test
stimuli. Faces were presented in a frontal orientation, with the
most neutral expression possible. All of the pictures were
cropped in a standard oval, removing salient cues (e.g., ears).
Stimulus size and brightness were kept uniform by using PHOTOSHOP (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA). The pairing of the
pictures was completed by the experimenters on the basis of
pictures being similar but distinguishable. When projected onto
the screen, each picture was 15 cm high and 10 cm wide (14° of
visual angle). Only one stimulus was projected in the center of
the screen for the familiarization period, and two stimuli were
projected side by side separated by a 12-cm gap during the
retention tests.
General Method. VPC was used to assess facial discrimination in
infants both before and after an exposure兾training period with
monkey faces. The VPC task developed by Fantz (31, 32) is
commonly used to measure visual recognition memory in preverbal and nonverbal individuals. The VPC task exploits an
infant’s attraction to novelty to assess his兾her recognition memory for previously seen stimuli. In this task, infants are first
presented with a stimulus for a familiarization period. Thereafter, the participant is presented with the same stimulus paired
simultaneously with a novel stimulus. The key dependent measure is the length of time spent fixating each of the two stimuli.
Longer duration of looking to one stimulus, generally the novel
one, indicates discrimination and recognition memory.
Detailed Procedure. Half of the infants were tested in Minneapolis
and the other half in Sheffield, U.K. All testing took place in a
sound-attenuated chamber. Infants were seated on their mother’s lap in front of a screen onto which the images were projected.
A black-and-white CCD camera (Maplin Electronics, U.K.)
(specialized for low-light conditions) was used to film the infant’s
eye movements. This eye movement was displayed to the experimenters, during recording, on an ITC control monitor. Time
was recorded and displayed on the control monitor by using a
Pascalis et al.
Fig. 1. Examples of stimuli used. Two different pairs of stimuli were used in each condition.
Pretraining Testing. During the experiment, parents were asked to
look above the screen to avoid influencing their infants looking
to either stimulus. Infants were familiarized to the monkey face
for 20 s of cumulative looking. Stimulus fixation was assessed by
corneal reflection of the stimuli. An observer hidden from the
infant’s view examined the eyes on a TV monitor and controlled
the time for the presentation of different stimuli during the
familiarization, the intertrial intervals, and the preference tests.
A computer algorithm determined when 20 s of cumulative
looking time was reached. The discrimination tests started when
the infant looked at one of the two stimuli, and they ended after
5 s had elapsed. After the first 5-s test, the side on which the
images were presented was reversed, and a second 5-s test was
completed. After the pretraining session, parents were sent
home with a folder containing six images of Barbary macaques,
each with individual names. These six images were different from
the one used during the pretraining test.
Parents were advised to note the date when testing began in
the ‘‘Date Started’’ box and then check the relevant box after
each presentation, working through the weeks as shown. The
experimenters ensured that all parents understood exactly what
was expected of them and how to complete the training schedule.
Parents were also supplied with detailed written information
along with contact information.
Posttraining Testing. Three months later, the infant’s ability to
recognize the folder’s pictures was assessed. There were six trials,
and for each trial, a familiar image from the learned series was
presented with a novel image for 5 s, followed by a blank screen
for 5 s. After a short break, a posttraining VPC test was
conducted in the same way as the pretraining task by using a new
set of pictures with completely novel monkey images. Only one
trial was conducted for each infant.
After testing, the videotapes were played back, and for each
Pascalis et al.
trial, the time each participant spent looking at both stimuli
during the retention tests was recorded by using a frame-byframe video recorder. Samples of videotapes were analyzed by
two observers. The observers were blind to the lateral location
of the novel and familiar test items. Interobserver reliability was
calculated for 50% of the infants chosen from the sample at
random. The amount of time required to reach the familiarization time and the two-test trial were double-scored for each of
these infants. Direction of looking (left, right, or blinking兾no
fixation) was compared for each 40-ms frame. The average level
of agreement was 95%.
Results
The looking time toward the novel and familiar stimuli is
expressed as the percentage of looking time compared with the
total looking time to stimuli (time spent looking at the novel
stimulus divided by the time spent looking at the novel stimulus
plus the time spent looking at the familiar). The data were
analyzed by using a one-tailed t test that compared the fixation
toward the novel stimulus with chance (50%).
Pretraining Test. VPC results for 6-month-olds reveal significantly
longer fixations to the novel (56.2%) compared with the familiar
monkey face (43.7%) (t ⫽ 2.712, df ⫽ 17, P ⬍ 0.05), thus
demonstrating discrimination. These data replicate our previous
result (30), suggesting that the effect found at 6 months of age
is robust to changes in stimulus type (in this study, we used a new
set of monkey faces from a different species).
Recognition of the Folder’s Pictures. The average looking time
toward the familiar and the novel picture was calculated across
the six discrimination trials. The infants demonstrated longer
looking time to the novel stimulus (53.8%) compared with the
familiar (pictures from the training folder) stimulus (46.2%) (t ⫽
2.994, df ⫽ 17, P ⬍ 0.05). These results suggest that infants
successfully discriminated the six familiar monkey faces from
unfamiliar monkey faces.
PNAS 兩 April 5, 2005 兩 vol. 102 兩 no. 14 兩 5299
PSYCHOLOGY
HORITA (Mission Viejo, CA) TG-50 time code generator at 25
frames per second. The film was then digitized to be analyzed
frame by frame on a computer by using specialized software.
Discrimination Test Posttraining. Nine-month-old participants with
monkey face training looked significantly longer toward the
novel monkey face (55.8%) than toward the familiar monkey
face (44.2%) (t ⫽ 2.963, df ⫽ 17, P ⬍ 0.05), thus demonstrating
recognition.
Thirteen 9-month-old control infants with no previous exposure to monkey faces were tested in the same way. Results
indicate that they looked equally long at the novel stimulus
(49.8%) compared with the familiar stimulus (51.2%) (t ⫽ 0.43,
df ⫽ 12, P ⬎ 0.05), replicating our previous report (30). These
findings demonstrate that exposure to monkey faces from 6 to 9
months of age was sufficient to extend the infants’ ability to
discriminate monkey faces.
Discussion
The experiment reported here examined the effect of exposure
to monkey faces on the specialization of the face-processing
system to human faces during the first year of life. Our results are
consistent with Nelson’s hypothesis stating that a broadly defined face prototype exists at birth, and its development is
influenced by the visual environment, leading to a more precise
face prototype. Specifically, here and in our previous work, we
observed a specialization of the face-processing system, as shown
by the loss of ability to discriminate between faces from other
species. However, with exposure to other species’ faces, this loss
is prevented in infants. Our results indicate, as hypothesized by
Nelson (9, 10), that the development of face processing follows
a trend similar to the one observed for speech processing. The
duration of this effect has yet to be determined; similarly we also
do not know how much exposure 6 month olds need to be able
to discriminate monkey faces.
Kuhl et al. (7) have shown that social interaction is an
important part of the learning mechanism. In their study, they
1. Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A. & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). Trends Neurosci. 4,
223–233.
2. Farah, M. J., Rabinowitz, C., Quinn, G. E. & Liu, G. T. (2002) Cognit.
Neuropsychol. 17, 117–123.
3. Werker, J. F. & Vouloumanos, A. (2001) in Handbook of Developmental
Cognitive Neuroscience, eds. Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), pp. 269–307.
4. Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984) Infant Behav. Dev. 7, 49–63.
5. Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F. & Stevens, K. N. (1992) Science 255,
606–608.
6. Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho, K. &
Naatanen, R. (1998) Nat. Neurosci. 1, 351–353.
7. Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M. & Liu, H. M. (2003) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100,
9096–9101.
8. Burns, T. C., Werker, J. F. & McVie, K. (2003) in Proceedings of the 27th Annual
Boston University Conference on Language Development, eds. Beachley, B.,
Brown, A. & Conlin, F. (Cascadilla, Somerville, MA), pp. 173–184.
9. Nelson, C. A. (2001) Infant Child Dev. 10, 3–18.
10. Nelson, C. A. (2003) The Development of Face Processing in Infancy and Early
Childhood: Current Perspectives, eds. Pascalis, O. & Slater, A. (Nova Science
Publishers, Hauppauge NY).
11. Legrand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D. & Brent, H. P. (2003) Nat. Neurosci.
6, 1108–1112.
12. LeGrand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D. & Brent, H. (2001) Nature 410, 786.
13. de Schonen, S. & Mathivet, E. (1989) Eur. Bull. Cognit. Psychol. 9, 3–44.
5300 兩 www.pnas.org兾cgi兾doi兾10.1073兾pnas.0406627102
compared a group of American infants passively exposed to a
video of a native Mandarin speaker with a group of infants who
were read stories by a native Mandarin speaker. Passive exposure
did not prevent the loss of discrimination at 10 months of age.
In our study, the procedure involved social interaction because
the parents were asked to present the pictures of the monkey
faces and their labels (or names) in a friendly way. It will
therefore be important to replicate this study with passive
exposure to monkey faces with and without labels to determine
the importance of social interaction and individuation and how
they facilitate learning.
These findings have implications for elucidating the role of
experience in brain specialization. From our studies, we would
speculate that the regions of the inferior temporal cortex (e.g.,
fusiform gyrus) that are known to be involved in face processing
in adults are beginning to come on line during the first year of
life. These data combined with our previous report (30) suggest
that there may be a sensitive period, during the first year of life,
for the development of face processing. However, at this time the
nature and specificity of this sensitive period is relatively unknown. These data and the speculation of a sensitive period are
consistent with work in early cataract patients who demonstrate
a sensitive period for the development of a normal vision and a
normal configural face processing (11, 12). Furthermore, the
current findings support the notion that experience with faces
early in life may influence and shape the development of a face
prototype. The development of this prototype leads to biases in
discriminating own-race and own-species faces compared with
other-race and other-species faces.
This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01
HD46526-01 (to O.P.) and NS32976 (to C.A.N.), and a Royal Society
research grant (to O.P.).
14. Quinn, P. C., Yarr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M. & Pascalis, O. (2002) Perception
31, 1109–1121.
15. Meissner, C. A. & Brigham, J. C. (2001) Psychol. Public Policy Law 7, 3–35.
16. Cross, J. F., Cross, J. & Daly, J. (1971) Percept. Psychophys. 10, 393–396.
17. Feinman, S. & Entwhistle, D. R. (1976). Child Dev. 47, 506–510.
18. Goldstein, A. G. & Chance, J. E. (1980) J. Psychol. 105, 47–59.
19. Chance, J. E., Turner, A. L. & Goldstein, A. G. (1982) J. Psychol. 112, 29–37.
20. Pedzek, K., Blandon-Gitlin, I. & Moore, C. (2003) J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 760–763.
21. Sangrigoli, S., Pallier, C., Argenti, A. M., Ventureyra, V. A. G. & de Schonen,
S. (2005) Psychol. Sci., in press.
22. Sangrigoli, S. & de Schonen, S. (2004) Dev. Sci. 7, 74–87.
23. Sangrigoli, S. & de Schonen, S. (2004) J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45,
1219–1227.
24. Elliott, E. S., Wills, E. J. & Goldstein, A. G. (1973) Bull. Psychon. Soc. 2, 71–73.
25. Goldstein, A. G. & Chance, J. E. (1985) Bull. Psychon. Soc. 23, 211–214.
26. Malpass, R. S., Lavigueur, H. & Weldon, D. E. (1973) Percept. Psychophys. 14,
285–292.
27. Pascalis, O. & Bachevalier, J. (1998) Behav. Processes 43, 87–96.
28. Dufour, V., Coleman, M., Campbell, R., Petit, O. & Pascalis, O. (2004) Curr.
Psychol. Cognit. 22, 315–333.
29. Valentine, T. (1991) Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 43A, 161–204.
30. Pascalis, O., de Haan, M. & Nelson, C. A. (2002) Science 296, 1321–1323.
31. Fantz, R. L. (1964) Science 146, 668–670.
32. Pascalis, O. & de Haan, M. (2003) in Progress in Infancy Research, eds. Hayne, H.
& Fagen, J. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ), Vol. 3, pp. 95–120.
Pascalis et al.