Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 31, No. 4, 2001
Siblings Relationships of Children with Autism
Laura Kaminsky1 and Deborah Dewey2,3
This study investigated sibling relationships of children with autism compared to children with
Down syndrome and siblings of normally developing children. Ninety siblings (30 per group)
between the ages of 8 and 18 participated in this study. Results indicated that sibling relationships in families of children with autism were characterized by less intimacy, prosocial
behavior, and nurturance than those of the two comparison groups. Both siblings of children
with autism and siblings of children with Down syndrome reported greater admiration of their
sibling and less quarreling and competition in their relationships relative to normally developing comparison children.
KEY WORDS: Autism; sibling relationships; Down syndrome.
INTRODUCTION
Knott et al., 1995); however, the results of these studies suggest that children with developmental disabilities and their siblings show marked similarities in their
interactions compared to normally developing sibling
pairs. Children with disabilities such as Down syndrome have been found to spend similar amounts of
time interacting with their sibling and engage in similar types of interactions as normally developing sibling
pairs (Abramovitch et al., 1987; Knott et al., 1995;
Stoneman, Brody, Davis, & Crapps, 1987). Observational studies, however, have reported some differences
in interactions. Specifically, children with disabilities,
regardless of their birth order were more likely to show
interactions with their sibling that were more typical
of younger children, such as imitative behaviors
(Abramovitch et al., 1987). Children with disabilities
were also less likely to initiate interactions with their
sibling than nondisabled children (Abramovitch et al.,
1987). Further, siblings of children with disabilities
tended to be more prosocial and nurturing towards their
affected sibling than siblings of normally developing
children (Abramovich et al., 1987; Lobato, 1991).
Because impairment in social abilities is one of the
deficits associated with autism, it seems likely that interactions of children with autism and their siblings may
differ from other children with developmental disabilities. One study that specifically investigated the inter-
Research in child development has tended to emphasize parent–child relationships and relationships
with peers (Stocker, 1994; Stocker & Dunn, 1990). Siblings, however, share a unique and important relationship with one another within the family unit (Knott,
Lewis, & Williams, 1995). These relationships have
been found to be important in the development of social skills in early childhood (Dunn, 1988; Dunn &
Munn, 1986). Positive relationships among siblings can
also be an important source of social support for children and are associated with lower levels of conduct
disorder and loneliness in children and higher selfworth (Jenkins & Smith, 1990; Stocker, 1994).
Few studies have investigated how developmental
disability in children influences interactions with siblings (Abramovitch, Stanhope, Pepler, & Corter, 1987;
1
Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta,
Canada.
2
Departments of Psychology and Paediatrics, University of Calgary
and Behavioural Research Unit, Alberta Children’s Hospital, Alberta, Canada.
3
Address all correspondence to Deborah Dewey, Behavioural Research Unit, Alberta Children’s Hospital, 1820 Richmond Road
S.W., Calgary, Alberta, Canada, T2T 5C7; e-mail: dmdewey@ucalgary.ca
399
0162-3257/01/0800-0399$19.50/0 © 2001 Plenum Publishing Corporation
400
actions of children with autism and their siblings compared to children with Down syndrome found that children with autism spent less time with their sibling, used
a smaller number and less variety of both prosocial and
antagonistic initiations towards their sibling, and imitated their sibling less (Knott et al., 1995). Despite these
differences, however, Knott et al. noted that children
with autism did demonstrate some social skills in their
play and did spontaneously initiate some of the interactions with their sibling. Further, although children
with autism interacted less often than children with
Down syndrome, they showed similar patterns of interactions and similar levels of initiations and responding
compared to pairs of normally developing siblings.
Very little research has investigated nondisabled siblings’ perspectives on their relationship with their sibling
with autism. Because of the social deficits demonstrated
by children with autism, one might hypothesize that these
siblings would report differences in their relationships
compared to siblings of children with developmental delays and siblings of normally developing children. In a
study by McHale, Sloan, and Simeonsson (1986), siblings of children with autism, siblings of children with
mental retardation, and siblings of normally developing
children were interviewed about their relationship with
their sibling. In addition, the mothers of these children
were interviewed about the relationships of their children.
Results indicated that both children and mothers in all
three sibling groups reported positive relationships between siblings. No group differences were found on the
children’s reports of their sibling relationships. Mothers
of children with disabilities, however, rated their children’s sibling relationships more positively than mothers of normally developing sibling pairs. Despite the
similarity between groups, the authors noted that siblings
of children with autism and Down syndrome tended to
show a much wider range of responses than siblings of
normally developing children. Some of the siblings of
children with disabilities gave very positive reports of
their relationship with their sibling, whereas others gave
very negative reports. In contrast, siblings of normally
developing children tended to have less varied responses.
McHale et al. (1986) also reported that siblings
viewed their relationship with their sibling with autism
or Down syndrome more positively when they accepted
the child’s role as a member of the family, perceived
minimal parental favoritism, and were not worried
about the future of the child with the disability. Welldeveloped coping abilities, understanding of their sibling’s disability, and positive responses from parents
and peers towards the child with autism or mental disability also encouraged positive sibling relationships.
Kaminsky and Dewey
Several demographic variables were also found to have
an influence on sibling perceptions. Children who were
younger than the child with the disability tended to feel
more rejecting towards their sibling than older siblings
did. Siblings also reported more concerns about the future and a less positive role of their sibling in the family when the child was male. In addition, larger family
size was related to fewer feelings of embarrassment
about the child with the disability and fewer feelings
of burden.
Bagenholm and Gillberg (1991) conducted a study
with a sample of siblings of children with autism, mental disabilities, and normally developing children in
Sweden. In contrast to the results of McHale et al.
(1986), differences were found between groups in this
study. Although all groups reported quite good relationships with siblings, siblings of children with autism
reported significantly less positive attitudes towards
their sibling and significantly less positive descriptions
of their sibling’s role in the family relative to comparison siblings.
Although McHale et al. (1986) and Bagenholm and
Gillberg (1991) investigated siblings perceptions of their
relationship with their sibling with autism, these two
studies examined only some of the many aspects involved in sibling relationships. Important factors that
may play a significant role in the relationship of normally developing children with their sibling with autism
such as dominance, affection, companionship, intimacy,
admiration, and competitiveness were not investigated.
In the present study, we used the Sibling Relationships
Questionnaire (Buhrmester & Furman, 1990), a questionnaire that has not been used in previous research
with children with autism, to investigate these qualities
of sibling relationships in families of children with
autism, families of children with Down syndrome, and
families of normally developing children.
METHOD
Participants
Participants were 90 children and adolescents between the ages of 8 and 18 years of age and one of their
parents. Three groups of 30 children and adolescents
were included in the study: (a) individuals who had a
sibling who had been diagnosed with autism, (b) those
who had a sibling who had Down syndrome, and (c) individuals who had a sibling with no known disability.
For the sake of clarity the sibling who participated in the
study is referred to as the “participating sibling.” The
children with autism, Down syndrome, and the compar-
Sibling Relationships of Children with Autism
ison normally developing siblings are called “reference
siblings.” Participating siblings in the three sibling
groups were matched for gender and approximate age of
the participating sibling, and birth order in relation to
the reference sibling (younger or older). Each group of
participating siblings was composed of an equal number
of male and female children, with the average age of participating siblings being 11 years for each group. Most
participating siblings in each of the three groups (80%)
were older than the reference siblings.
Only families of children with autism or Down
syndrome who had been diagnosed with these disorders
by a physician were included in this study. Parents provided this information before the child participated in
this research. Families of children with other Pervasive
Developmental disorders such as Asperger Disorder
and Pervasive Developmental Disorders Not Otherwise
Specified were not included as participants in this
study.
Participating families were recruited from parent
support groups, special schools for children with disabilities, and the Calgary Catholic School District. In
families where there was more than one participating
sibling who was willing to volunteer for the study, the
child closest in age to the reference sibling was selected
as a participant. In cases where there were two possible participating siblings who were equally close in age
to the reference sibling, both children completed questionnaires, and the sibling who provided the best match
with the other sibling groups was included as a participant. All participating siblings were living in the same
household as the reference sibling. Twenty-one siblings
completed the questionnaires but were excluded from
the study due to inadequate matching.
Measures
Sibling Relationship Questionnaire–Revised
The Sibling Relationship Questionnaire (SRQ;
brief version, Buhrmester & Furman, 1990), consists
of 15 scales each containing 2–3 items, for a total of
39 items that assess children’s perceptions of their siblings. These scales are also grouped into four SRQ factors: Closeness/Intimacy, Power, Conflict, and Rivalry.
The Closeness/Intimacy factor is made up of seven individual scales: Prosocial Behavior, Companionship,
Similarity, Admiration of Sibling, Admiration by Sibling, and Affection. The Power factor is calculated by
adding the Nurturance of Sibling and Dominance over
Sibling scales minus the Nurturance by Sibling and
Dominance by Sibling scales. The Conflict Factor con-
401
sists of Quarreling, Antagonism, and Competition. Finally the Rivalry Factor consists of the individual
scales of Maternal Partiality and Paternal Partiality.
On the SRQ, the child was asked to rate how well
a particular characteristic described their relationship
with their sibling on a five-point Likert scale which
ranged from hardly ever true to extremely much. Higher
scores on each of the scales or factors indicated greater
levels of the specified quality in the sibling relationship. For example, higher scores on the Affection scale
indicate greater perceived affection between siblings.
Buhrmester and Furman (1990) reported internal consistency coefficients for the SRQ that range from
.71–.81 for children in the 3rd, 6th, 9th, and 12th
grades. Of the 60 alpha coefficients calculated for the
15 scales in each age group of children, 55 were greater
than .60 (Buhrmester & Furman, 1990).
The Gilliam Autism Rating Scale
The Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS; Gilliam,
1995), is a behavioral checklist that helps to identify the
presence and severity of autism in individuals between
the ages of 3 and 22 years. The GARS was included in
this study as a measure of severity of autism. It was
normed on a sample of 1,092 children and young adults
who had been diagnosed with autism. The checklist includes three subtests, each of which have 42 items. The
subtests measure Stereotyped Behaviors, Communication, and Social Interaction. A fourth subtest, Developmental Disturbances is optional for parents to complete.
The total of the subtest scores yields an Autism Quotient. Items in the first three subtests are presented on a
four-point Likert scale (0–3). The fourth subtest, Developmental Disturbance uses yes/no questions.
Alpha coefficients for the GARS subtests and
overall quotient are as follows: Stereotyped Behaviors
.90, Communication .89, Social Interaction .93, Developmental Disturbances .88, and Autism Quotient
.96. Test–retest reliability for a 2-week interval for each
subtest ranged from .81 to .88. Interrater reliabilities
have been calculated between teachers, parents, and between a teacher and a parent. Reliabilities ranged from
.55 for parent-to-parent ratings of Social Interaction to
.99 for teacher-to-parent ratings of Stereotyped Behaviors and the Autism Quotient.
Adaptive Behaviors Questionnaire
Parents of children who had autism and parents of
children who had Down syndrome were asked a series
of 15 items partially adapted from the Vineland Adaptive Behavioral Scales (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti,
402
1984). These items were administered to parents in
order to gain information about the adaptive level of
functioning of the child (Appendix A). The first items
ask the parent questions about the child’s degree of disability, performance in school, and special services provided to the child. The remaining nine questions ask
about a variety of adaptive behaviors. Scores on this
portion ranged from 0–10, with higher scores indicating greater adaptive functioning.
Demographics Questionnaire
Parents were asked a series of 13 questions about
family demographics (Appendix B). Specifically, this
questionnaire asks about family size, presence of children with disabilities in the home, family income, marital status, maternal and paternal education, and
maternal and paternal occupations.
Procedure
After parental and child consent was obtained, participants completed questionnaires administered by the
experimenter. The Sibling Relationship Questionnaire
was carefully explained to the participating sibling by
the researcher. The first two items of the questionnaire
were completed with the researcher’s assistance. Children below the age of 12 years were asked to read the
first few questions aloud, in order to evaluate the child’s
reading skills. Children who required additional help with
reading items on the questionnaires were provided with
assistance. Only two children needed help reading numerous items on the questionnaire. Parents were asked
to complete a family demographics questionnaire. Parents of children with autism were also administered the
GARS. In addition, parents of children with autism or
Down syndrome were administered a questionnaire on
their child’s adaptive behaviors. All participants were encouraged to ask the experimenter any questions that arose.
The majority of participants (92%) took part in the
study in their homes. Three participants in the group of
siblings with Down syndrome and one in the normally
developing group completed the questionnaires at the
researcher’s office. In addition, for two families who
resided outside of Calgary the parent’s demographic
questionnaire was completed and mailed to the researcher, and the sibling was interviewed by telephone.
The two siblings interviewed by telephone were 11 and
14 years of age and appeared to have no difficulty answering the questions over the telephone.
Children were not informed before participating
in the study whether or not their parents would have
access to their responses on the SRQ. However, when
Kaminsky and Dewey
parents expressed a desire to see their children’s answers, they were shared with the parent only when the
children gave consent.
RESULTS
Family Characteristics
Group differences were examined on a variety of
family demographic variables using a series of analyses
of variance (ANOVA) and chi-square tests. For all of
the analyses, the autism, Down syndrome, and normally
developing groups were compared with each other.
ANOVAs indicated no group differences on the following demographic variables: participating siblings’ age,
F(2, 87) 5 0.14, p . .05, reference siblings age, F(2,
87) 5 0.07, p . .05, and number of children per household, F(2, 87) 5 1.33, p . .05. Chi-square analyses indicated that the gender of the participating siblings was
not significantly different among groups, x2(2, N 5 90),
p . .05. However, group differences were found in the
gender of the reference siblings, x2(2, N 5 90) 5 8.90,
p , .05. To control for experimentwise error rate, a Bonferonni adjustment of .05/3 5 .017 was utilized. The
Mann-Whitney test was used for follow-up testing. A
significant difference was found between the gender of
the reference siblings with autism and the reference siblings in the normally developing group, U 5 285, p ,
.01. There was a substantially larger portion of boys with
autism, with a 6:1 ratio of male to female. Thus, participating siblings of children with autism were more likely
to be reporting about their relationships with brothers
rather than sisters, compared to participating siblings of
normally developing children. This finding was anticipated, given that the ratio of male to female with autism
in the general population is reported to be 4–5:1 (American Psychological Association, 1994). The Down syndrome group was not found to differ from the autism or
the normally developing group in terms of the gender of
the reference sibling.
No significant group differences were identified on
a variety of family constellation variables including: the
birth order of participating siblings in relation to the reference sibling, x2(4, N 5 90) 5 2.12, p . .05, type of
family (two parent, single, or blended two parent), x2(4,
N 5 90) 5 7.09, p . .05, and the presence of an adopted
child in the family, x2(2, N 5 88) 5 2.03, p . 05. Two
children with Down syndrome were adopted and were
not biologically related to their sibling who participated
in the study. Otherwise all sibling pairs were biological
siblings. No significant difference was found in the proportion of reference siblings in each group who had ad-
Sibling Relationships of Children with Autism
403
ditional illnesses or disabilities, x2(2, N 5 90) 5 4.28,
p . .05. Similarly, no significant group differences were
found for the presence of disability or illness in participating siblings, as well as other children in the family,
x2(2, N 5 90) 5 4.32, p . .05.
Sociodemographic factors were assessed using a
number of variables. No significant differences were
found between groups for fathers’ years of education,
x2(10, N 5 90) 5 11.50, p . .05, or annual family income, x2(16, N 5 89) 5 13.34, p . .05. Years of mothers’ education significantly differed by group, x2(10,
N 5 90) 5 22.50, p , .05, with mothers of participating siblings of normally developing children having
greater years of education than mothers of children with
autism or mothers of children with Down syndrome.
Follow-up analyses using the Mann-Whitney U test,
and a Bonferroni correction of .05/3 5 .017, however,
failed to reach significance. No significant correlations
were found between SRQ scores and mothers’ years of
education, thus this variable was not included as a covariate in any of the analyses.
Socioeconomic status (SES) of the groups was also
compared by rating mothers’ and fathers’ occupations
on the Blishen Index of Canadian Occupations
(Blishen, Carroll, & Moore, 1986). This index provides
a numerical rating of occupations based on generated
income, prestige and education requirements for specific occupations. Occupations can be divided into one
of six class values based on the numerical ratings, with
higher class values being associated with greater SES.
No significant group differences were found on either
mothers’ SES, x2(10, N 5 89) 5 8.22, p . .05, or fathers’ SES, x2(10, N 5 89) 5 8.00, p . .05 (Table I).
Severity of Disability
An ANOVA was used to examine whether there
were differences in overall severity of disability in the
autism and Down syndrome groups, as measured by
total scores on the adaptation questionnaire. No effect
for group on overall level of disability was found, F(1,
58) 5 2.60, p , .05. The average adaptive ability scores
were 6.4 (SD 5 2.3) for the autism group and 7.2
(SD 5 1.63) for the Down syndrome group. Group differences in ability on each of the nine basic adaptation
skills were also assessed. Only the ability to communicate through language (either spoken or sign) was found
to be significantly different, x2(1, N 5 60) 5 6.67, p ,
.05. Results indicated that children with autism were
less likely to communicate through language compared
to children with Down syndrome. Ten percent of children with autism did not communicate through either
spoke or sign language, whereas all of the children with
Down syndrome used language to communicate with
others. Interestingly, however, the ability of the child
with autism to communicate was not significantly correlated with any of the four SRQ factors.
The GARS also provided information as to the
severity of disability in the group of children with
autism. Children with autism in this study had a mean
overall Autism Quotient of 93.2. Thus, the mean score
on autism severity as measured by the GARS was in
the average range for the reference siblings with autism.
The reference siblings’ Autism Quotient scores, however, ranged from very low (i.e., 67) to very high (i.e.,
132), with the majority of children (72%) scoring in
the average range (i.e., 80–120).
Sibling Relationships
Sibling Relationship Factor Scores
To identify how autism influenced aspects of sibling relationships; SRQ factor scores were assessed in relation to participating sibling group and gender using a
multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Four SRQ
factor scores were included in the analysis: Closeness/
Intimacy, Power, Conflict, and Rivalry. Results indicated
Table I. Family Characteristics
Group
Autism
Variables
Sibling’s age
Child’s age
No. of children
Father’s SES value
Mother’s SES value
Down syndrome
Normally developing
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
11.67
9.79
2.47
3.77
2.80
2.95
3.86
0.68
1.57
1.56
11.88
9.44
2.80
4.00
2.97
2.14
3.62
0.85
1.41
1.76
11.54
9.43
2.60
4.00
2.77
2.33
4.13
0.86
1.39
1.77
404
a significant overall effect for participating sibling group,
Wilks’s lambda, F(8, 83) 5 4.62, p , .001, and a significant overall effect for participating sibling gender,
Wilks’s lambda, F(4, 87) 5 3.49, p , .05. A Bonferroni
adjustment of .05/4 5 .013, was utilized to control for
experimentwise error rate in subsequent analyses. All post
hoc comparisons were performed using Scheffé’s
method. In all analyses of group differences all three sibling groups were compared with one another.
Univariate Fs indicated a significant group difference in the Closeness/Intimacy factor, F(2, 87) 5 5.31,
p , .01. Post hoc comparisons showed that participating siblings of children with Down syndrome report
significantly higher levels of closeness and intimacy
than the other two groups of participating siblings. A
significant effect for group was also found on the Conflict factor, F(2, 87) 5 8.72, p , .001. Participating
siblings of children with autism and Down syndrome
reported significantly less conflict with their sibling
compared to participating siblings of normally developing children. No significant group differences were
found for the SRQ Power, F(2, 87) 5 3.04, p . .02,
and Rivalry factors, F(2, 87) 5 1.95, p . .02.
Univariate Fs indicated no significant effects of
sibling gender on the following SRQ factors: Closeness/Intimacy, F(1, 88) 5 0.26, p . .02, Power, F(1,
88) 5 2.04, p . .02, and Rivalry, F(1, 88) 5 6.12,
p . .02. A trend towards a significant effect for sibling gender was found on the Conflict factor, F(1, 88)
5 6.12, p 5 .02, with males reporting more conflict
with their siblings than females, irrespective of group.
Sibling Relationship Individual Scales
To investigate group and gender differences on the
15 individuals subscales of the SRQ, a series of
ANOVAs were conducted. Due to the large number of
scales analyzed, only alpha values of .01 and less were
considered significant results. As these analyses were
exploratory in nature, group differences with an alpha
level of p , .10 were noted as trends.
Significant effects of group were found on the following scales: Nurturance of sibling, F(2, 87) 5 6.28,
p , .01; Nurturance by sibling, F(2, 87) 5 5.37, p ,
.01; Affection, F(2, 87) 5 5.67, p , .01; Intimacy, F(2,
87) 5 7.79, p 5 .001; Competition, F(2, 87) 5 9.20,
p , .001; Admiration of sibling, F(2, 87) 5 7.60, p 5
.001; Admiration by sibling, F(2, 87) 5 4.80, p 5 .01;
Quarreling, F(2, 87) 5 8.82, p , .001. A trend for a
group effect was found for the scales measuring Prosocial behavior, F(2, 87) 5 3.96, p , .05; and Companionship, F(2, 87) 5 2.45, p 5 .09. No significant
Kaminsky and Dewey
group effects were identified for the Maternal Partiality, Dominance of Sibling, Dominance by Sibling, Paternal Partiality, and Antagonism scales.
Post hoc analyses using Scheffé’s method indicated a number of differences between groups on the
SRQ scales. The Prosocial, Intimacy and Nurturance
by Sibling scales differentiated participating siblings
of children with autism from the other groups of siblings. Participating siblings of children with autism report less prosocial behavior compared to participating
siblings of children with Down syndrome. No differences were found between the participating siblings of
children with autism and the participating siblings of
children normally developing children on prosocial behavior. Further, the participating siblings of children
with Down syndrome were not significantly different
from the participating siblings of normally developing
children on prosocial behaviour. Participating siblings
of children with autism also report less intimacy with
their reference sibling and less nurturance by their reference sibling than both participating siblings of children with Down syndrome and participating siblings of
normally developing children. No differences were
found between participating siblings of Down syndrome and participating siblings of normally developing children on this measure.
Post hoc comparisons on the Nurturance of Sibling, Admiration of Sibling, and Affection scales, identified differences in the relationships of siblings of
children with Down syndrome compared to siblings of
children with autism and siblings of normally developing children. Participating siblings of children with
Down syndrome reported that they were more nurturing of their reference sibling than the other groups of
siblings. They also reported that their sibling with
Down syndrome admired them more than participating
siblings of children with autism or normally developing children. Participating siblings of children with
Down syndrome also reported more affection in their
sibling relationships than participating siblings of normally developing children, but not participating siblings of children with autism.
On the Competition, Admiration by Sibling, and
Quarrel subscales post hoc comparisons identified differences between siblings of children with autism and
Down syndrome, and siblings of normally developing
children. Participating siblings of children with autism
and participating siblings of children with Down syndrome reported greater admiration of their reference
sibling than participating siblings of normally developing children. Participating siblings of children with
autism and Down syndrome also reported that they
Sibling Relationships of Children with Autism
405
were less competitive with their sibling and quarreled
less than participating siblings of normally developing
children.
In general, siblings in all three groups reported
quite positive relationships with their siblings. Table II
presents the means and standard deviations of SRQ factor and scale scores for each group.
Significant effects for gender were found on the
scales measuring Prosocial behavior, F(1, 88) 5 7.50,
p , .01; Nurturance by sibling, F(1, 88) 5 7.78 p ,
.01; Nurturance of sibling, F(1, 88) 5 6.30, p 5 .003;
Affection, F(1, 88) 5 6.23, p 5 .01; and Competition,
F(1, 88) 5 10.16, p 5 .002. Table III presents the
means and standard deviations of SRQ factor and scale
scores for male and female. Examination of means for
each gender indicated that participating males, irrespective of group, reported less prosocial behavior and
affection in their sibling relationships, less nurturance
by their siblings, and greater competition with their siblings as compared to participating females. A trend towards a gender effect was found for the Maternal
Partiality scale, with males regardless of group indi-
cating greater maternal partiality, F(1, 88) 5 2.86, p 5
.09. No significant differences due to gender were
found on the remaining scales.
DISCUSSION
Very few studies have investigated siblings’ perceptions of their relationship with their sibling with
autism. Using the SRQ, this study investigates aspects
of sibling relationships that have not been investigated
previously in siblings of children with disabilities such
as dominance, affection, companionship, intimacy, admiration, and competitiveness. Results indicate that
participating siblings of children with autism report less
intimacy with their reference sibling and less nurturance by their reference sibling than participating siblings of children with Down syndrome and participating
siblings of normally developing children. Participating
siblings of children with autism also reported less
prosocial behavior in their sibling relationship than participating siblings of children with Down syndrome.
Table II. Group Means on Sibling Relationship Questionnaire (SRQ) Factors and Scales
Group
Autistic
Variables
SRQ factors
Closeness
Power
Conflict
Rivalry
SRQ Closeness factor scales
Intimacy
Prosocial
Companion
Similarity
Admiration of sibling
Admiration by sibling
Affection
SRQ Power factor scales
Nurturance of sibling
Nurturance by sibling
Dominance of sibling
Dominance by sibling
SRQ Conflict factor scales
Quarreling
Antagonism
Competition
SRQ Rivalry factor scales
Maternal partiality
Paternal partiality
Down syndrome
Normal develop
M
SD
M
SD
M
SD
3.0
2.6
2.3
2.9
0.6
1.5
0.8
0.3
3.5
1.9
2.4
2.8
0.7
1.7
0.9
0.4
3.1
1.4
3.1
3.0
0.6
2.2
0.9
0.3
1.4
2.8
3.2
2.6
3.7
3.3
4.4
0.6
0.9
1.1
0.9
0.9
1.1
0.8
2.1
3.4
3.7
3.1
3.9
3.9
4.5
0.9
0.8
0.9
0.8
1.0
1.0
0.8
2.1
3.1
3.4
2.8
3.1
3.2
3.9
0.9
0.7
0.8
1.2
0.8
0.7
0.8
3.3
1.5
2.5
1.9
0.8
0.7
1.0
0.9
3.7
2.1
2.6
2.4
0.8
1.0
0.9
1.1
3.1
2.1
2.7
2.2
0.7
1.0
0.9
0.9
2.6
2.4
1.8
0.9
0.9
1.0
2.6
2.7
1.9
1.2
1.1
1.0
3.5
2.9
2.8
0.9
0.7
1.3
2.8
2.8
0.3
0.5
2.7
2.8
0.5
0.5
2.9
2.9
0.5
0.3
406
Kaminsky and Dewey
Table III. Gender Means on Sibling Relationship Questionnaire
(SRQ) Factors and Scales
Gender
Male
Variables
SRQ factors
Closeness
Power
Conflict
Rivalry
SRQ Closeness factor scales
Intimacy
Prosocial
Companion
Similarity
Admiration of sibling
Admiration by sibling
Affection
SRQ Power factor scales
Nurturance of sibling
Nurturance by sibling
Dominance of sibling
Dominance by sibling
SRQ Conflict factor scales
Quarreling
Antagonism
Competition
SRQ Rivalry factor scales
Maternal partiality
Paternal partiality
Female
M
SD
M
SD
3.1
2.2
2.8
2.9
0.7
1.6
0.9
0.4
3.3
1.7
2.4
2.8
0.6
2.0
0.9
0.4
1.9
2.9
3.5
2.7
3.5
3.5
4.0
0.8
0.8
1.0
1.1
1.0
1.1
0.9
1.8
3.3
3.4
2.9
3.6
3.5
4.4
0.9
0.8
0.9
0.8
0.9
0.9
0.7
3.3
1.7
2.7
2.2
0.8
0.8
1.0
1.0
3.4
2.2
2.5
2.1
0.9
1.0
0.8
1.0
3.0
2.8
2.5
1.0
0.9
1.2
2.7
2.9
1.8
1.1
0.8
1.0
2.9
2.9
0.4
0.4
2.7
2.9
0.5
0.5
Although no previous literature has addressed how
autism influences these aspects of sibling relationships,
discussion of some of the particular features of autism
provides insight into these findings.
Siblings of children with autism may be less likely
to report high levels of prosocial behavior, intimacy, and
nurturance by their sibling, because of the variety of social deficits related to autism. Children with autism may
be rated as less nurturant by their siblings, because they
often have deficits in understanding other’s perspectives,
and tend to be unresponsive to other’s emotions (Rutter
& Schopler, 1987). Limitations in cognitive functioning
may further impede children with autism from assisting
and attending to their normally developing siblings.
Similarly, intimacy and prosocial behavior in sibling relationships with children with autism may be inhibited by social and communicative deficits, which are
characteristic of autism. Children with autism have
deficits such as a lack of reciprocity in conversation, or
an overall lack of speech, which likely inhibit intimate
interchanges with siblings such as sharing thoughts and
feelings (Rutter & Schopler, 1987). Of the children with
autism in the present study, 10% could not communicate through either spoken or sign language.
Furthermore, many children with autism rarely
seek out others for comfort, affection, or to initiate play.
Sibling relationships with children with autism may be
characterized by less prosocial behavior because children with autism are less likely to initiate interactions
with their siblings. This finding is consistent with that
of Knott et al. (1995), who reported that children with
autism tend to initiate both less prosocial and antagonistic initiations with their siblings. Knott et al. also reported that both children with autism and their siblings
were less likely to respond to each other’s initiations.
Participating siblings of children with both autism
and Down syndrome reported differences in their relationships compared to siblings of normally developing
children. Specifically, participating siblings of children
with disabilities reported greater admiration of their
reference sibling than participating siblings of normally
developing children. They also reported that they were
less competitive and quarreled less with their sibling
than normally developing siblings. These results confirm previous studies, which have indicated that siblings of children with disabilities tend to feel quite
positive about their sibling (Bagenholm & Gillberg,
1991; McHale et al., 1986).
Previous research has not addressed how having a
sibling with a disability influences antagonistic interaction such as quarreling and competitiveness. However, it can be speculated that the cognitive limitations
of the child with the disability may have resulted in the
normally developing siblings feeling less inclined to
compete with the child with the disability. Furthermore,
a portion of the children with disabilities, particularly
children with autism, had considerable deficits in communication abilities that may have resulted in a reduction in the level of negative verbal interactions among
siblings.
Particular aspects of sibling relationships also differentiated siblings of children with Down syndrome
from siblings of children with autism and siblings of normally developing children. Participating siblings of children with Down syndrome reported that they were more
nurturant of their sibling and had higher levels of overall closeness and intimacy than the other groups of participating siblings. They also indicated that their reference
sibling admired them more than did participating siblings
of children with autism or participating siblings of normally developing children. Participating siblings of children with Down syndrome reported greater affection in
their sibling relationships than participating siblings of
normally developing children, but not more than participating siblings of children with autism.
Sibling Relationships of Children with Autism
These results are consistent with previous observational studies reporting that siblings of children with
Down syndrome tend to be more prosocial towards their
sibling than siblings of normally developing children
(Abramovitch et al., 1987). The present results also correspond with research on preschool siblings of children
with disabilities, which has found that siblings of children with disabilities tend to be more nurturing of their
sibling than normally developing controls (Lobato,
1981).
An additional interesting result was that participating siblings of children with autism and Down syndrome did not report greater parental partiality towards
their sibling than normally developing children. This
finding contradicts anecdotal reports that have stated
that siblings of children with intellectual disabilities tend
to resent the attention and time that the child with a disability requires from parents (Howlin, 1988; Lobato,
1983).
One limitation of the present study is the moderate sample size in relation to the number of variables
included in this study. Since autism is a relatively rare
disorder in the population, the number of possible participants was restricted. It should be noted, however,
that the sample size in this study is similar to or exceeds the sample sizes of published studies involving
siblings of children with disabilities (Bagenholm &
Gillberg, 1991; McHale et al., 1986).
A second limitation of this study is that the majority of participating siblings in this sample (80%)
were older rather than younger than their reference sibling in the autism, Down syndrome, or normally developing group. This limits the generalizability of the
results of this study to siblings who are younger than
the child with autism. Furthermore, because of the
small number of younger participating siblings, it was
not possible to carry out analyses that compared the
younger and older participating siblings. Additional research is needed on the sibling relationships of younger
siblings of children with autism.
A third limitation of this research relates to the gender distribution of children with autism in this study.
As was expected, given the higher rate of autism in boys
in the general population, the sample of reference siblings with autism had a larger number of boys than the
sample of reference siblings with Down syndrome and
the sample of normally developing reference siblings.
This resulted in many more participating siblings in the
autism group reporting about their relationship with a
brother rather than a sister. Due to the lack of reference
girls in the autism group, investigation of how samesex versus opposite-sex sibling dyads differ in terms of
sibling relationships was not possible.
407
A fourth limitation relates to the applicability of
this study’s findings to families of children with
autism in the general population. A large portion of
families of children with autism and Down syndrome
in this sample were recruited from parent support
groups. Several of these families were very active in
support groups. As only a small number of families
in the study did not attend support groups, no formal
analyses were conducted to determine whether families who were support group attendees differed from
the other families. In addition, this study largely included families who were middle class or upper middle class in terms of their SES. Although there were
no differences between groups in SES, the fact that
most of the participants were middle to upper class in
terms of SES may also limit the applicability of results to siblings of children with autism in the general
population.
In conclusion, the results of this research provide
new information on how autism influences relationships between siblings. Siblings of children with autism
reported lower levels of intimacy, prosocial behavior,
and nurturance by their siblings compared to children
with Down syndrome and normally developing children. This study also highlights some of the positive
influences of having a sibling with a disability on sibling relationships. Both siblings of children with autism
and Down syndrome reported greater admiration by
their siblings and less competitiveness and quarreling
in their relationship with their sibling than normally developing sibling pairs.
APPENDIX A
Adaptive Behavior Questionnaire Form for
Parents of Children Who Have Autism or Down
Syndrome
Please answer the following questions about your
child’s abilities and level of independence.
1. What is the name and age of your child with
autism or Down’s syndrome?
Name
Age
2. What is this child’s I.Q., mental age, and/or degree
of disability (if known), please specify.
408
Kaminsky and Dewey
3. What grade is your child in at school?
4. At what grade level or age level does your child
currently perform at in school?
16. Can your child feed him/herself with a fork or
spoon?
Yes
No
APPENDIX B
5. Have you received any special services from the
school system for this child such as placement in
special classes, teachers aid. Please specify.
General Questionnaire for Parents
First we would like to ask you some questions
about your family.
1. Please complete the following information about
all members of your household.
6. Have you received any other types of support services for this child such as respite services, assistance of a professional caregiver in the home etc.
Please specify.
7. Can your child communicate with others through
spoken language (or sign language)?
Yes
No
8. Does your child use sentences of four or more
words?
Yes
No
9. Can your child communicate his/her wants or
needs to others?
Yes
No
Sex
No
11. If your child can read, at what grade level are they
currently reading at?
Date of Birth
Father
Mother
Child
Child
Child
Child
Child
2. Have any children in your family been diagnosed
with autism, Down’s syndrome, a language disorder, learning disability, developmental problem,
attentional problem or a chronic illness?
Yes
10. Can your child read?
Yes
Name
No
3. If YES, what specific illness (es) or type of problem(s) and which child (ren) is/are affected?
Name
Type of Problem
12. Can your child print/write his or her first name?
Yes
No
13. Can your child follow simple instructions given to
them by a parent?
Yes
No
14. Can your child dress his/herself completely?
Yes
No
15. Can your child tie shoelaces into a bow without assistance?
Yes
No
4. If you have a child with Down’s syndrome or
autism please complete questions 1–16 on form E
(attached).
5. Are any of your children adopted?
Yes
No
6. If YES, specify which child (ren) is/are adopted.
Name
Sibling Relationships of Children with Autism
7. What is your annual family income?
below 10,000
51,000–60,000
10,000–20,000
61,000–70,000
21,000–30,000
71,000–80,000
31,000–40,000
81,000–90,000
41,000–50,000
91,000–100,000
FOR MOTHER:
8. What is your present marital status? (please check
all that apply)
Married
Separated
Living with
someone
Never married
and not living
with someone
Divorced
Widowed
9. From the list below, please indicate the highest level
of education that you completed.
a)
b)
c)
d)
e)
f)
No high school
Some high school
High school diploma
Some post-secondary, but no diploma or degree
Post-secondary diploma (e.g., technical)
University degree
10. What is your occupation?
FOR FATHER:
11. What is your present marital status? (please check
all that apply)
Married
Separated
Living with
someone
Never married
and not living
with someone
Divorced
Widowed
12. From the list below, please indicate the highest level
of education that you completed.
a)
b)
c)
d)
No high school
Some high school
High school diploma
Some post-secondary, but no diploma or degree
409
e) Post-secondary diploma (e.g., technical)
f) University degree
13. What is your occupation?
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Support for this research was provided by grants
from the Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research, the Surrey Place Centre Foundation and the Alberta Children’s Hospital Foundation. This study was
presented at a meeting of the Society for Research in
Child Development, Albuquerque, NM, April 1999. We
thank the parents and children who participated in this
study.
REFERENCES
Abramovitch, R., Stanhope, L., Pepler, D., & Corter, C. (1987). The
influence of Down syndrome on sibling interaction. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 865–879.
American Psychological Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC:
Author.
Bagenholm, A., & Gillberg, C. (1991). Psychosocial effects on siblings
of children with autism and mental retardation: A population based
study. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 35, 291–307.
Blishen, B. R., Carroll, W. K., & Moore, C. (1986). The 1981 socioeconomic index for occupations in Canada. Canadian Review
of Sociology and Anthropology, 24, 465–488.
Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1990). Perceptions of sibling relationships during middle childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 61, 1387–1398.
Dunn, J. (1988). Sibling influences on childhood development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 119–127.
Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1986). Sibling quarrels and maternal intervention: Individual differences in understanding and aggression.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 583–595.
Gilliam, J. E. (1995). Gilliam Autism Rating Scale Manual. Austin,
TX: Pro-Ed.
Howlin, P. (1988). Living with impairment: The effects on children
of having an autistic sibling. Child: Care, Health and Development, 14, 395–408.
Jenkins, J. M., & Smith, M. A. (1990). Factors protecting children
living in disharmonious homes: Maternal reports. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29,
60–69.
Knott, F., Lewis, C., & Williams, T. (1995). Sibling interaction of
children with learning disabilities: A comparison of autism and
Down’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 6, 965–976.
Lobato, D. (1985). Brief report: Preschool siblings of handicapped
children: Impact of peer support and training. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 15, 345–350.
McHale, S. M., Sloan, J., & Simeonsson, R. J. (1986). Sibling relationships of children with autistic, mentally retarded, and nonhandicapped brothers and sisters. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 16, 399–413.
Rutter, M., & Schopler, E. (1987). Autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Concepts and diagnostic issues. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 159–186.
410
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland
Adaptive Behavior Scales Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American
Guidance Services.
Stocker, C. M. (1994). Children’s perceptions of relationships with
siblings, friends, and mothers: Compensatory processes and
links with adjustment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1447–1459.
Kaminsky and Dewey
Stocker, C., & Dunn, J. (1990). Sibling relationships in childhood:
Links with friendships and peer relationships. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 8, 227–244.
Stoneman, Z., Brody, G. H., Davis, C. H., & Crapps, J. M. (1987).
Mentally retarded children and their older same-sex siblings:
Naturalistic in-home observations. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 92, 290–298.