Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                
Potentiated Amygdala Response to Repeated Emotional Pictures in Borderline Personality Disorder Erin A. Hazlett, Jing Zhang, Antonia S. New, Yuliya Zelmanova, Kim E. Goldstein, M. Mehmet Haznedar, David Meyerson, Marianne Goodman, Larry J. Siever, and King-Wai Chu Background: Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is characterized by an inability to regulate emotional responses. The amygdala is important in learning about the valence (goodness and badness) of stimuli and functions abnormally in BPD. Methods: Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was employed in three groups: unmedicated BPD (n ⫽ 33) and schizotypal personality disorder (n ⫽ 28) participants and healthy control subjects (n ⫽ 32) during a task involving an intermixed series of unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures each presented twice within their respective trial block/run. The amygdala was hand-traced on each participant’s structural MRI scan and co-registered to their MRI scan. Amygdala responses were examined with a mixed-model multivariate analysis of variance. Results: Compared with both control groups, BPD patients showed greater amygdala activation, particularly to the repeated emotional but not neutral pictures, and a prolonged return to baseline for the overall blood oxygen level-dependent response averaged across all pictures. Despite amygdala overactivation, BPD patients showed blunted self-report ratings of emotional but not neutral pictures. Fewer dissociative symptoms in both patient groups were associated with greater amygdala activation to repeated unpleasant pictures. Conclusions: The increased amygdala response to the repeated emotional pictures observed in BPD was not observed in schizotypal patients, suggesting diagnostic specificity. This BPD-related abnormality is consistent with the well-documented clinical feature of high sensitivity to emotional stimuli with unusually strong and long-lasting reactions. The finding of a mismatch between physiological and self-report measures of emotion reactivity in BPD patients suggests they may benefit from treatments targeting emotion recognition. Key Words: Amygdala, arousal, borderline personality disorder, emotion, fMRI, schizotypal personality disorder, valence D eficits in emotion regulation are a core feature of borderline personality disorder (BPD) (1-4). Patients with BPD have suicide rates 50 times the general population (5) and utilize more mental health resources than individuals with other psychiatric disorders (6,7); because BPD is present in approximately 2% to 5.9% of the general population, it is at least as prevalent as schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder (8-10). Affective instability in BPD is characterized as an inability to regulate emotional responses (11,12), with a high sensitivity to emotional stimuli and unusually strong and long-lasting reactions (2,13). This phenomenological description suggests that BPD patients may have an abnormality in their decrement of response to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Habituation is defined as the decrease in physiological responsivity that occurs to a repeated presentation of the same stimulus (14,15). Understanding the neural substrates of emotion-processing deficits in BPD may ultimately help target biological or psychological treatments and predict which individuals with BPD respond best to a specific type of treatment. This strategy has shown promise in predicting response to cognitive behavioral therapy in depressed patients (e.g., [16]). From the Department of Psychiatry (EAH, JZ, ASN, YZ, KEG, MMH, DM, MG, LJS), Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York; Mental Illness Research, Education and Clinical Center, Veterans Integrated Service Network (VISN) 3 (EAH, ASN, MG, LJS, K-WC); Department of Psychiatry (MMH, LJS), James J. Peters Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Bronx, New York. Address correspondence to Erin A. Hazlett, Ph.D., Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center, James J. Peters Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 130 West Kingsbridge Road, Room 6A-45, Bronx, NY 10468; E-mail: erin.hazlett@mssm.edu. Received Jun 22, 2011; revised Mar 19, 2012; accepted Mar 25, 2012. 0006-3223/$36.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.027 The amygdala plays an important role in modulating attention/ vigilance, particularly in potentially threatening social situations, and perceiving the valence of events/objects and emotional expressions of others (17-19). Translational neuroscience animal models indicate the amygdala plays a central role in learning about unpleasant and pleasantly valenced stimuli (20). Given BPD patients exhibit emotion dysregulation, it is not surprising that the amygdala is the most investigated brain structure in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of this disorder, and the majority employed standardized photographic images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (21). Yet, regardless of whether pictures, faces, or scripts were used, studies primarily show amygdala overactivity in BPD during the processing of unpleasant stimuli (13,22-27). One possible mechanism accounting for amygdala overactivity in BPD is impaired habituation. Habituation is one of the most documented and fundamental forms of nervous system plasticity (28). The initial response to a novel stimulus involves a rapid shift of attentional processes (i.e., an orienting response), but with one or more repeated presentations without meaningful consequences, response amplitude is reduced. Prior studies in healthy adults document strong evidence of a decrement in the amygdala blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli (e.g.,[18,29]). Animal models (30,31) and human work (32,33) also pinpoint the amygdala as an important component of the system involved in the acquisition and memory storage of unpleasant stimuli. The present study is the first to examine whether BPD patients show abnormal amygdala habituation and/or differences in the shape, amplitude, and habituation of the BOLD response curve to repeated emotional stimuli. We (Hazlett et al. [34]) and others (35-37) have shown that BPD patients exhibit exaggerated affective startle to borderline-salient stimuli compared with healthy control subjects (HCs), which may be mediated by symptom severity (e.g., [37]). Animal models indicate whole-body startle is modulated by the amygdala in the conBIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 © 2012 Society of Biological Psychiatry BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 449 E.A. Hazlett et al. text of fear conditioning (20), and we examined startle-eyeblink amplitude, a component of whole-body startle during the processing of borderline-salient (e.g., suicidal) and neutral (e.g., coin) words. Compared with HCs, BPD patients showed exaggerated startle amplitude during unpleasant but not neutral words (34). In contrast, on self-report, the BPD patients showed a blunted response by rating the unpleasant words as less unpleasant but did not differ from the HCs for the neutral word condition. This mismatch between the physiological and subjective response to emotional stimuli is consistent with a psychophysiological ambulatory monitoring study that also reported an inability to label emotions (35) and fMRI studies reporting a mismatch between amygdala activation and self-report ratings in BPD (22). The present study further examines the concept that BPD is characterized by a mismatch between physiological and self-report responses to emotional stimuli. This fMRI study addresses several key issues unresolved by prior BPD work. First, given unpleasant pictures are highly arousing compared with neutral pictures or a resting state, we controlled for both valence and arousal levels in our study by including three picture conditions (unpleasant/high arousal vs. neutral/low arousal vs. pleasant/high arousal) using the standardized IAPS library (21). Second, as a reliability check, subjective emotion was measured using self-report ratings of the pictures both in the magnet and following the session. Third, the diagnostic specificity of amygdala dysfunction in BPD was addressed by including BPD patients with no schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) traits and a psychiatric control group of SPD patients without BPD traits. Fourth, the shape of the amygdala BOLD response curve and its change over time was examined during novel and repeated presentations of the emotional and neutral pictures. To examine the hypothesis that BPD patients exhibit high sensitivity to emotional stimuli and unusually strong and long-lasting reactions as measured by amygdala activation, we presented each of the pictures twice within their respective trial block/run. This allowed an examination of changes in the BOLD response from the novel to the repeated picture presentation. Additionally, we examined the time course of the amygdala BOLD response curves. Compared with both control groups, we hypothesized that BPD patients would show a 1) protracted amygdala BOLD response (i.e., slower return to baseline), particularly following emotional pictures; 2) pattern of greater amygdala activation to repeated compared with novel emotional but not neutral pictures; and 3) mismatch between their amygdala and self-report response to emotional pictures. Exploratory correlations between amygdala activation to the repeated unpleasant pictures and self-reported symptom severity scales were conducted separately for the patient groups. We used the strict criteria of Vul et al. (38) to conduct our correlational analysis, which involved the mean BOLD response (area under curve) for all voxels within our amygdala region of interest (i.e., aggregated data) that was traced on structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) for each participant blind to their diagnosis and functional imaging data. A standard whole-brain analysis using FSL (4.1; FMRIB Analysis Group, Oxford, United Kingdom) (39) was also conducted to confirm our amygdala region of interest results and explore other regions. Methods and Materials Participants Thirty-three patients with BPD, 28 patients with SPD, and 32 HCs were included (Table 1; Table S1 in Supplement 1 for additional demographic/clinical/exclusionary criteria details). The groups did not significantly differ in age, gender, or education and all patients met DSM-IV criteria. All patients were unmedicated at the time of their fMRI scan (⬎6 weeks) and most were never previously medicated. Patients with a history of schizophrenia, psychotic disorder, bipolar (type I) affective disorder, or current major depressive disorder (MDD) (episode occurring within 2 months of the scan) were excluded. Healthy control participants had no Axis I or II diagnosis and no Axis I disorder in any first-degree family member. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Mount Sinai School of Medicine Institutional Review Board guidelines. With few exceptions noted in Table S1 in Supplement 1, all participants completed psychometric self-report measures of aggression (Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire [40]), impulsivity (Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 [41]), affective lability (Affective Lability Scale [42]), dissociative symptoms (Dissociative Experiences Scale [43]), affective intensity (Affective Intensity Measure [44]), and childhood abuse and neglect (Childhood Trauma Questionnaire [45]). One-way between-group (HC vs. BPD vs. SPD) analyses of variance were conducted on the total scores and follow-up t tests were conducted to determine which groups differed (Table S1 in Supplement 1). Functional and Structural MRI Acquisition The MRI scan procedure was conducted on an Allegra headdedicated 3T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) and included a T2, echo planar image, and T1-weighted structural magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) scan. See Figure S1 in Supplement 1 for scan parameter details. Event-Related fMRI Affective Picture Processing Task During the fMRI scan, participants viewed unpleasant (U), neutral (N), and pleasant (P) photographic pictures (from the IAPS [21]; see Supplement 1). A total of 96 intermixed unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant photographic pictures were presented.1 E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania) (46) was used for the design and presentation of all stimuli in the scanner. The 96 pictures were presented twice within their respective run for a total of 192 picture trials. Each trial was 8 seconds long and included either 1) the presentation of a picture (for 6 seconds) followed by a three-choice button press response prompt (for 2 seconds; described in detail below) or 2) a fixation cross (8 seconds). The presentation of either a picture or fixation cross was semirandomized with the number of consecutive trials varying from one to six for pictures and one to three for fixation trials. Each run contained 24 unique pictures (8 unpleasant, 8 neutral, 8 pleasant) that were repeated once (48 picture events) and 16 nonpicture (fixation cross) events (total ⫽ 64 contiguous trials per run). The total scan time was 38 minutes and 12 seconds, which was divided into four runs with 30 seconds before and 31 seconds after each run (30 ⫹ [8 * 64] ⫹ 31 ⫽ 573 seconds; four runs ⫽ 2292 seconds). We chose predominantly social pictures including faces and social interactions. Across the four runs, the unpleasant and pleasant pictures were matched based on the picture ratings from the 1 On half of the novel (initial) and repeated picture presentations, participants heard a brief static noise burst (50-msec duration, 105 dB) through headphones, which they were instructed to ignore. A subset of the fixation trials also contained the noise burst to ensure unpredictability across trial types. The rationale for presenting the noise burst during the scan was so that the fMRI paradigm closely resembled our psychophysiological paradigm involving affective startle eyeblink, which is conducted outside the scanner. www.sobp.org/journal 450 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 E.A. Hazlett et al. Table 1. Demographics for Healthy Control, Borderline Personality, and Schizotypal Personality Disorder Groups Healthy Control Subjects (n ⫽ 32) BPD Patients (n ⫽ 33) SPD Patients (n ⫽ 28) Characteristic Mean SD Range Mean SD Range Mean SD Range t Value Age (Years) Educationa Sex Male Female Handedness Right Left Both Symptom Severityb Past MDDc 32.8 4.53 n 12 20 n 29 2 1 — n — n — — 9.7 2.78 % 38% 62% % 91% 6% 3% — % — % — — 22–57 1–9 — — — — — — — — — — — — — 31.6 3.85 n 13 20 n 29 4 0 7.77 n 21 n 16 17d 9.1 2.29 % 39% 61% % 88% 12% 0% 1.26 % 64% % 48% 52% 18–51 1–8 — — — — — — — 5.5–10 — — — — — 35.9 4.0 n 16 12 n 24 3 1 7.27 n 6 n 23 5e 11.0 2.05 % 57% 43% % 86% 11% 3% 1.15 % 21% % 82% 18% 20–55 1–8 — — — — — — — 5–10.5 — — — — — Age HC vs. BPD: t(63) ⫽ .52 HC vs. SPD: t(59) ⫽ ⫺1.16 BPD vs. SPD: t(58) ⫽ ⫺1.67 Education HC vs. BPD: t(63) ⫽ 1.08 HC vs. SPD: t(59) ⫽ ⫺.27 BPD vs. SPD: t(58) ⫽ .83 Sex HC vs. BPD: t(63) ⫽ .15 HC vs. SPD: t(59) ⫽ 1.53 BPD vs. SPD: t(58) ⫽ 1.38 Handedness HC vs. BPD: t(63) ⫽ .04 HC vs. SPD: t(59) ⫽ ⫺.46 BPD vs. SPD: t(58) ⫽ ⫺.55 Psychoactive Medications Never medicated Previously medicated p Value .61 .25 .10 .28 .79 .41 .88 .13 .17 .97 .65 .58 All diagnoses were made using a structured diagnostic interview conducted by a psychologist using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I (74) and the Structured Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (75), followed by a consensus meeting. Only BPD (kappa ⫽ .82) and SPD (kappa ⫽ .73) patients without diagnostic comorbidity were included (i.e., BPD patients without SPD traits and SPD patients without BPD traits). Exclusion criteria for all participants included severe medical or neurological illness, head injury, and any prior substance dependence or substance abuse during the past 6 months. Healthy control subjects (100%) and patients (90%) were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and the remaining patients (10%) from psychiatric outpatient clinic referrals at Mount Sinai Hospital. All participants had a negative urine toxicology screen for drugs of abuse, and women had a negative pregnancy test on scan day. BPD, borderline personality disorder; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; GED, General Educational Development Test; HC, healthy control participants; MDD, major depressive disorder; SD, standard deviation; SPD, schizotypal personality disorder. a Education ⫽ highest degree earned: 1 ⫽ no high school diploma; 2 ⫽ GED; 3 ⫽ high school diploma; 4 ⫽ technical training; 5 ⫽ some college, no degree; 6 ⫽ associate degree; 7 ⫽ bachelor’s degree; 8 ⫽ master’s degree; 9 ⫽ MD/PhD/JD/PharmD. b Symptom severity: For each patient, each of the DSM-IV criteria for each personality disorder was rated on a 4-point scale (0 ⫽ absent, .5 ⫽ somewhat present, 1.0 ⫽ definitely present/prototypic, 2.0 ⫽ severe, pervasive). As required for a DSM-IV diagnosis of BPD, these patients met at least five of the nine DSM-IV criteria with a rating of 1.0. BPD patients were allowed no more than three SPD criteria with two items rated as 1.0 and one item rated as .5. As required for a DSM-IV diagnosis of SPD, these patients met at least five of the nine SPD criteria. SPD patients were allowed no more than three BPD criteria with two items rated as 1.0 and one item rated as .5 to control for comorbidity and/or traits. To quantify the level of clinical symptom severity, we added up the individual symptom ratings for each diagnostic criterion. c MDD ⫽ past major depressive disorder was defined as prior episode occurring ⬎ 2 months from the time of fMRI scan. d Of the 17 BPD patients who previously received psychoactive medications, 14 received antidepressants, 2 received antipsychotics, 2 received benzodiazepines, 3 received stimulants, and 2 received mood stabilizers. e Of the five SPD patients who previously received psychoactive medications, one received an antipsychotic, five received antidepressants, and two received stimulants. standardized IAPS manual for arousal (all p ⬎ .28) and for valence. They were equally divergent from neutral. The neutral pictures were matched across each of the four runs on arousal and valence. All participants viewed the same stimulus sequence. Participants were instructed to attend to the pictures and think about their meaning for them personally. Immediately following the offset of each picture, a cartoon-like picture of a right hand with the pointer finger labeled as pleasant, middle finger labeled as neutral, and the ring finger labeled as unpleasant appeared for 2 seconds. As soon as participants saw the hand prompt, they made a three-choice response with their right hand using a BrainLogics fiber optic button system (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania). The responses following each picture were recorded on a desktop computer and helped to ensure that participants were continuously engaged in the task. Immediately following the scan, participants viewed the same 96 pictures again outside the magnet on a laptop and rated them using the SelfAssessment Manikin scale (9-point scale) (47). Image Processing We conducted two kinds of event-related analyses of the functional imaging data: 1) BOLD response time-series analysis based www.sobp.org/journal on hand-traced amygdala region of interest; and 2) a standard whole-brain general linear model analysis using FSL. For both approaches, the FSL fMRI Expert Analysis Tool (39) was used for image processing. The BOLD data were preprocessed with motion correction using MCFLIRT(48), nonbrain removal using Brain Extraction Tool (49), spatial smoothing (full-width at half maximum ⫽ 5 mm), and a high-pass temporal filter (cutoff ⫽ 70 seconds). The MP-RAGE and echo planar images were co-registered with a 7 degrees of freedom linear transformation followed by alignment to the Montreal Neurological Institute brain template using a 12 degrees of freedom linear fit. Amygdala Delineation and Region of Interest Analysis For each participant, we traced the amygdala volume on anterior commissure-posterior commissure positioned structural/MPRAGE images using our published methods (Figure S1 in Supplement 1). Following the co-registration, we obtained the mean BOLD response time-series values (1 to 11 with each 3-second epoch beginning at picture onset and continuing to 33 seconds; 11 ⫻ 3 ⫽ 33 seconds) for the fMRI hemodynamic response curves averaged across the voxels within each hemisphere of the amygdala region of interest for each of the key stimulus conditions (novel/unpleasant/ E.A. Hazlett et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 451 startle, novel/unpleasant/no startle, repeated/unpleasant/startle, repeated/unpleasant/no startle, novel/pleasant/startle, novel/ pleasant/no startle, repeated/pleasant/startle, repeated/pleasant/no startle, novel/neutral/startle, novel/neutral/no startle, repeated/neutral/startle, repeated/neutral/no startle) averaged across all runs. We have previously published (50) our methods for using a similar time series approach for the BOLD response. We conducted a group (HC vs. BPD vs. SPD) ⫻ picture type (U, N, P) ⫻ picture repetition (novel, repeated) ⫻ startle stimulus (startle presented 4000 msec following picture onset, no startle stimulus) ⫻ hemisphere (left, right) ⫻ time (1 to 11: 3 seconds, 6, 9ѧ33 seconds following picture onset) multivariate analysis of variance using Statistica (StatSoft, Tulsa, Oklahoma) (51). Diagnostic group was the between-group factor and the remaining factors were all repeated measures. We report multivariate F values (Wilks’ lambda) or univariate F with Huynh-Feldt adjusted p values. Significant effects with group were followed-up using Fisher’s least significant difference tests (shown in figures). To limit the number of exploratory clinical correlations, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between BOLD activation (area under the curve) in the amygdala (averaged across hemisphere, startle/no startle, and time) during the repeated unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures for each patient group. Standard Whole-Brain General Linear Model Analysis The whole-brain analysis involved a standard whole-brain general linear model analysis. General linear model analysis was first carried out on the preprocessed fMRI data for each single subject with FILM (FMRIB’s Improved Linear Model) with six contrasts set for unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant pictures at novel presentation (Time 1) and repeated presentation (Time 2). Next, single-subject statistics were fed into second-level multi-session, multi-subject analysis. A between-group t test analysis was performed on the three separate groups with FLAME (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects). The statistical images were thresholded using clusters determined by Z ⬎ 2.0 and a corrected cluster significance threshold of p ⬍ .05 (52) with a color bar showing p ⬍ .05 (corrected). We also report the Z score and cluster size for the significant regions (Table S2 in Supplement 1). For the amygdala clusters, we used the amygdala mask from the Harvard-Oxford Atlas included in FSL. Results Amygdala Activation During Picture Processing Following picture onset, the BPD patients exhibited an overall amygdala BOLD response curve (averaged across all repeated measures except time) with a much slower return to baseline compared with the HC and SPD groups (Figure 1, top). The HCs showed the smallest amygdala BOLD response peak, while the SPD group showed the greatest and the BPD patients were intermediate. The HCs also had the fastest peak latency; SPD patients showed the slowest; and BPD patients were intermediate, group ⫻ time interaction, F (20,162) ⫽ 1.96, p ⫽ .012, Wilks’. The BPD group showed the smallest overall peak response in the amygdala during the novel pictures; yet, they showed the greatest overall peak response during the repeated pictures, group ⫻ picture repetition (novel, repeated) ⫻ time interaction, F (20,162) ⫽ 1.73, p ⫽ .033, Wilks’ (Figure 1, bottom). Averaged across the time factor discussed above, the BPD patients exhibited a normal pattern of overall amygdala activation during novel pictures but greater amygdala activation during repeated pictures compared with both HCs and SPD patients (Figure 2, top). Interestingly, the SPD group showed an opposite pattern Figure 1. Top: The amygdala blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response curve is shown for each of the three groups averaged across all repeated measures (picture type, repetition, startle/no startle, and hemisphere) except for time (3, 6, 9ѧ33 seconds following picture onset). The amygdala was hand-traced on each individual study participant’s structural magnetic resonance (magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradientecho) image and co-registered to their BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging scan. The borderline personality disorder (BPD) patients showed an amygdala BOLD response curve that peaked later than the healthy control subjects (HC) and took longer to return to baseline compared with both healthy control subjects and schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) patients. This BPD-related pattern of a protracted amygdala response is consistent with the concept that BPD patients have long-lasting responses to emotional stimuli. The SPD patients showed the longest peak latency and highest peak response in the amygdala, while the control subjects showed the shortest peak response and lower overall BOLD activation in the amygdala. As shown, this group ⫻ time interaction was significant. For both graphs, the significant post hoc Fisher’s least significant difference tests, p ⬍ .05, are noted and the standard error bars are provided. Bottom: The BPD group showed a greater overall BOLD response in the amygdala during the repeated pictures compared with both the healthy control subjects and the SPD patients. In contrast, the SPD group showed a higher peak during the novel pictures. As shown, this group ⫻ picture repetition (novel, repeated) ⫻ time interaction was significant. B, borderline personality disorder; H, healthy control subjects; S, schizotypal personality disorder. from BPD with greater amygdala activation during the novel pictures and normal activation during the repeated pictures, group ⫻ picture repetition interaction, F (2,90) ⫽ 4.41, p ⫽ .015. Of greatest interest, Figure 2, bottom, shows that compared with HCs and SPD patients, the BPD patients exhibited a pattern of greater www.sobp.org/journal 452 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 E.A. Hazlett et al. None of the BPD patients studied had a current MDD diagnosis. But to address the issue of whether our findings are related to a vulnerability to depression rather than BPD per se, we compared patients with a past history of MDD to those without any history of MDD. This two-group multivariate analysis of variance failed to show a main effect of group or any interaction with group indicating these BPD subgroups did not significantly differ from each other in terms of their amygdala BOLD response pattern (p values ⬎ .41). Whole-Brain Activation During Picture Processing The amygdala results revealed in the whole-brain analysis were very consistent with the results of our primary region of interest amygdala analysis (Figure 3). Like the amygdala region of interest analysis, the comparison between patients with a past history of MDD and those without any history of MDD failed to reach significance for the whole-brain analysis. Figure 2. Top: Overall, the borderline personality disorder (BPD) group showed greater amygdala activation during the repeated pictures compared with the healthy control (HC) and schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) groups. In contrast, the SPD group showed greater amygdala activation during the novel picture presentation. As shown, this group ⫻ picture repetition (novel, repeat) interaction was significant. Significant post hoc Fisher’s least significant difference tests, p ⬍ .05, and standard error bars are shown for both graphs. Bottom: For each of the groups, the mean amygdala blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response is shown during the novel and repeated picture presentations for each of the three picture types. Compared with healthy control subjects and schizotypal personality disorder patients, the borderline personality disorder patients showed an increase in their mean amygdala response from the novel to the repeated emotional (both unpleasant and pleasant) pictures. As shown, this group ⫻ picture type (unpleasant, neutral, pleasant) ⫻ picture repetition (novel, repeat) interaction was significant. amygdala BOLD activation to the emotional (both unpleasant and pleasant) but not the neutral pictures when they were repeated. In striking contrast, the SPD patients showed greater amygdala activation to the neutral pictures when repeated, compared with the HCs and BPD patients. Healthy control subjects showed greater amygdala activation than BPD patients to the novel pleasant pictures, while the BPD group showed greater activation to the repeated pleasant pictures. This complex pattern was significant, group ⫻ picture type (U, N, P) ⫻ picture repetition (novel, repeat) interaction, F(4,178) ⫽ 3.49, p ⫽ .009, Wilks’. The startle/no startle and hemisphere factors did not interact with group and none of the other interactions with group reached significance. www.sobp.org/journal Self-Report Ratings of Picture Valence During fMRI Session. Compared with HCs, both BPD and SPD patients showed a blunted response on self-report ratings of emotional picture valence, i.e., rating unpleasant pictures as less unpleasant and pleasant pictures as less pleasant [group ⫻ picture type interaction, F (4,178) ⫽ 2.76, p ⫽ .049 Huynh-Feldt; Figure 4, top]. There were no between-group differences for the neutral picture ratings. Follow-up post hoc tests revealed that both patient groups showed a pattern of a general blunted response to the emotional but not the neutral pictures (all p values ⬍ .007). Post-fMRI Session. The mean self-report valence ratings are shown in Figure 4, bottom, and, as expected, looked very similar to the three-point ratings obtained during the fMRI (described above). Compared with the HC group, both patient groups showed a blunted response on self-report ratings of the unpleasant pictures. The BPD patients also showed a blunted response for the pleasant condition compared with HCs and SPD patients [group ⫻ picture type interaction, F (4,178) ⫽ 2.44, p ⫽ .049, Huynh-Feldt; follow-up tests were significant with p values ⬍ .007]. Symptom Correlations Correlational analyses showed that among the BPD group, greater amygdala BOLD activation to repeated unpleasant (r ⫽ .36, p ⫽ .04; r ⫽ .37, p ⫽ .03) and neutral (r ⫽ .39, p ⫽ .03; r ⫽ .40, p ⫽ .02) pictures was associated with higher aggression and affective lability (measured by the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire and Affective Lability Scale, respectively). Among both patient groups, greater BOLD activation during the repeated unpleasant pictures was associated with fewer dissociative symptoms (BPD: r ⫽ ⫺.36, p ⫽ .047; SPD: r ⫽ ⫺.40, p ⫽ .04; see Figure S2 in Supplement 1). Schizotypal personality disorder patients who showed greater BOLD activation during the repeated pleasant pictures also showed fewer dissociative symptoms (r ⫽ ⫺.43, p ⫽ .03). The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11, Affective Intensity Measure, and Childhood Trauma Questionnaire scores were not correlated with amygdala responses. Spearman’s correlations produced the same pattern of significance. Discussion Our study has two novel findings reflecting BPD-related abnormalities in amygdala function. First, BPD patients showed reduced overall habituation (time series) in terms of their BOLD response curve returning to baseline following picture onset. That is, averaged across the three picture conditions (unpleasant, neutral, and pleasant), BPD patients showed a prolonged amygdala response E.A. Hazlett et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 453 Figure 3. Statistical probability maps from FSL (4.1) (39) for the between-group effects in response to unpleasant pictures are shown. The maps are thresholded at Z ⬎ 2.0 (or p ⬍ .05, corrected) and the color bar shows p ⬍ .05 (corrected). See Table S2 in Supplement 1 for the size and location of the clusters. (A1,2) These standard parametric mapping results demonstrate increased amygdala activity in schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) patients compared with healthy control subjects (HCs) (A1) and in the SPD patients compared with borderline personality disorder (BPD) patients (A2) in response to the novel or first (i.e., time 1) presentation of the unpleasant pictures. (B1,2) With the repeated presentation of unpleasant pictures, the whole-brain analysis showed increased activity in the right amygdala (region inside the green circle) and left fusiform gyrus (Brodmann area [BA] 37) in the BPD patients compared with the HCs (B1) and in regions including the bilateral amygdala and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA10) in the SPD patients compared with the HCs (B2). (C1,2) Between-group effects for (time 2 [repeated] ⫺ time 1 [novel]) differences. Activity was increased in brain areas, including the right fusiform gyrus (BA 37), anterior cingulate (BA 24), and inferior frontal gyrus (BA 44) in BPD patients compared with HCs (C1). Activity was also increased in BPD compared with SPD patients in regions including left and right (green circle) amygdala and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 10) (C2). compared with the HC and SPD groups. The region of interest time series showed and the whole-brain analysis confirmed that in the BPD group, the amygdala was more strongly activated when the same emotional, but not neutral, pictures were presented the second time. This pattern is consistent with the concept that BPD patients exhibit unusually long-lasting reactions to emotional cues (2,13). Secondly, BPD patients showed a potentiated amygdala response to emotional pictures when repeated relative to the first (novel) presentation. In contrast, HCs and SPD patients showed either the same level of amygdala response or a decrement in their amygdala response to the repeated emotional pictures, consistent with a pattern of amygdala habituation or emotional learning. This BPD pattern of increased amygdala activation to repeated emotional pictures was not observed in our psychiatric control group of SPD patients, suggesting it has diagnostic specificity. The potentiated amygdala response to the repeated emotional pictures in BPD indicates an abnormality in a very basic function that has important evolutionary relevance because it allows for optimal allocation of information processing resources, away from emotional stimuli not associated with real threat or reward (29). Our HC finding of similar levels of amygdala activation to novel and repeated unpleasant pictures and a pattern of habituation for pleasant pictures is consistent with other normative studies using the IAPS (e.g., [53]). However, our HCs showed less difference between novel and repeated unpleasant pictures than reported in Phan et al. (53). This is likely due to paradigmatic differences, e.g., we compared novel with repeated pictures presented within their respective run while Phan et al. (53) examined differences across early and late runs, which would likely evince more significant habituation. To differentiate between an abnormality in general arousal versus valence, the unpleasant and pleasant pictures employed were matched on high arousal level, yet opposite in valence while the neutral pictures were low on arousal level. Thus, our finding of exaggerated and prolonged amygdala response to emotional but not neutral pictures in BPD is consistent with a general arousal deficit rather than an abnormal response to unpleasant/negative stimuli per se. This arousal-deficit interpretation is consistent with psychophysiological work showing that BPD patients exhibit greater than normal skin conductance responsivity—a measure of autonomic arousal—while imagining scripts with highly unpleasant BPD-salient themes (abandonment/rejection), as well as positive themes (36). Future fMRI studies comparing unpleasant stimuli varying in arousal level (e.g., highly vs. generally unpleasant picwww.sobp.org/journal 454 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 E.A. Hazlett et al. pain perception (e.g., [57]) and our results suggest abnormalities in amygdala function may play an important role. Among all patients, those reporting fewer dissociative symptoms showed greater amygdala activation with repeated unpleasant picture viewing, while those reporting more dissociative symptoms showed less amygdala activation. It seems counterintuitive that patients with greater symptom severity would show more normal amygdala habituation; yet, this finding suggests that dissociation may serve as a defensive response for coping with unpleasant stimuli. This is consistent with studies showing that BPD patients with low present-state dissociation exhibited larger amplitude startle responses compared with those with high present-state dissociation (58). Borderline personality disorder patients with high state-dissociative experiences also show poor emotional learning evidenced by no increase in self-report valence ratings or a skin conductance measure of arousal to a conditioned stimulus (59). To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine amygdala function during an emotion paradigm in SPD. Consistent with our finding that the SPD group showed greater overall amygdala activation averaged across all picture types, prior work shows that schizophrenia patients demonstrate greater skin conductance reactivity to emotional and neutral films (60). Unlike the BPD patients, the SPD patients in our study did exhibit habituation of the amygdala response to repeated emotional pictures. However, they differed from both HCs and BPD patients by showing a pattern of greater activation to the repeated compared with novel neutral pictures. One possibility is that because a key feature of SPD is paranoid ideation, the patients perceived the neutral pictures as more threatening. We found a blunted self-report response in SPD to the unpleasant but not pleasant pictures (nine-point Self-Assessment Manikin scale). This is consistent with work showing that individuals with high scores on the suspiciousness subscale of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (61) demonstrate reduced perception of affect in body posture (62). Figure 4. Group means for the self-report ratings of picture valence are shown for the pictures viewed in the magnet (top graph) and following the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session (bottom graph using the Self-Assessment Manikin scale [47]). There were no between-group differences for the neutral picture ratings. Both group ⫻ picture type interactions are significant as shown. Top: During the fMRI session, both the borderline personality disorder (BPD) and schizotypal personality disorder (SPD) groups showed blunted (i.e., rated unpleasant and pleasant as more neutral) self-report ratings of the emotional pictures compared with healthy control subjects. Asterisks denote significant differences from the healthy control subjects (follow-up Fisher’s least significant difference tests), all p values ⬍ .007. Bottom: Similarly, the post-fMRI session self-report ratings show that compared with the healthy control group, both the BPD and SPD groups exhibited a blunted response pattern to the unpleasant pictures. Borderline personality disorder patients also showed a blunted response for the pleasant pictures compared with healthy control subjects and SPD patients. Asterisks denote significant differences from the healthy control subjects (Fisher’s least significant difference tests, all p values ⬍ .007). The “a” represents BPD ⬎ SPD, p ⫽ .0002. H-F, Huynh-Feldt. tures) are needed to further understand the neural substrates of this BPD-related arousal deficit. The BPD patients exhibited a mismatch between their physiological (overactive amygdala) and subjective experience (blunted self-report response) of emotion. This finding is consistent with prior imaging (22,26) and affective startle (an amygdala-based psychophysiological measure) (34) studies showing a disconnect between the subjective experience of emotion and the physiological response in BPD. A blunted response to emotional pictures is consistent with prior work indicating impaired emotion recognition abilities (54-56) and reduced www.sobp.org/journal Study Limitations While this BPD study has clear strengths, including a large sample of unmedicated patients, personality disorder control group of SPD patients, and gold standard tracing of the amygdala, several limitations merit comment. There are many ways to examine habituation and we examined only two ways: the neural response to a repeated picture within the same run to minimize scanner drift issues that occur across runs and the habituation (time series) of the BOLD response. Future fMRI research should design experiments to examine other types of habituation to affective stimuli in BPD. The current study, our prior affective startle study (34), and other recent work (36) employed stimuli with primarily social and borderlinesalient contexts (e.g., picture of a man hitting a woman, the word “alone,” scripts describing scenes of abandonment) and found significant normal-BPD differences. However, it is unclear whether HC-BPD group differences would be observed using nonsocial stimuli. An experimental manipulation of social versus nonsocial stimulus content in BPD would be useful. No significant differences in brain activation were observed between BPD patients with and without a past history of MDD, suggesting that our results are not related to the vulnerability to depression. It will be important to replicate this finding in a larger sample. Recent work indicates that compared with HCs, MDD patients show greater amygdala responses to masked sad faces but smaller responses to happy faces (63), suggesting a MDD-related trait-like bias toward excessive processing of unpleasant stimuli. In contrast to MDD, our findings indicate that BPD patients show E.A. Hazlett et al. excessive amygdala activation to both unpleasant and pleasant pictures, consistent with a general arousal deficit. Summary and Possible Implications We provide evidence that BPD patients exhibit an exaggerated amygdala response when exposed to repeated emotional pictures and a blunted subjective experience of emotion. This finding may have important clinical implications for the type of treatment— specifically, therapies that focus on developing the skill to recognize one’s own emotional responses—that might be particularly helpful in BPD. The concept that there is a deficit in mentalization in BPD has long been recognized by clinicians developing psychotherapeutic treatment for BPD (64). In fact, the psychotherapies that have been empirically demonstrated to be most effective in BPD focus on learning the skill of recognizing one’s own emotions, including mentalization-based therapy (65,66) and dialectical behavior therapy (67-70). Our findings also have implications for pharmacotherapy treatment, given evidence from animal models demonstrating the possibility of altering emotional memories via disruption of reconsolidation at the level of the amygdala (71). There is some evidence that glucocorticoids directly facilitate extinction learning based on their actions in potentiating effects of the glutamatergic N-methyl-Daspartate receptors in the amygdala (72,73). Future work in this area has direct implications for the treatment of BPD, with excellent potential to further refine diagnostic specificity, provide new targets for therapeutic interventions, and glean a useful biological predictor of treatment response in BPD. This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01MH073911 to EAH and Grant UL1RR029887 from the National Center for Research Resources, National Institutes of Health. We thank Dr. Monte Buchsbaum for use of the Multi-Image-Processing-Software used to trace the amygdala on structural magnetic resonance imaging scans and Dr. Jin Fan for assisting with the E-prime program for the functional magnetic resonance imaging experiment. The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest. Supplementary material cited in this article is available online. 1. Gunderson JG, Phillips KA (1991): A current view of the interface between borderline personality disorder and depression. Am J Psychiatry 148:967–975. 2. Linehan MM (1993): Cognitive Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York: The Guilford Press. 3. Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Pfohl B, Widiger TA, Livesley WJ, Siever LJ (2002): The borderline diagnosis I: Psychopathology, comorbidity, and personality structure. Biol Psychiatry 51:936 –950. 4. Siever LJ, Torgersen S, Gunderson JG, Livesley WJ, Kendler KS (2002): The borderline diagnosis III: Identifying endophenotypes for genetic studies. Biol Psychiatry 51:964 –968. 5. Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, McGlashan TH, Dyck IR, Stout RL, Bender DS, et al. (2002): Functional impairment in patients with schizotypal, borderline, avoidant, or obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 159:276 –283. 6. Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Khera GS, Bleichmar J (2001): Treatment histories of borderline inpatients. Compr Psychiatry 42:144 –150. 7. Lieb K, Zanarini MC, Schmahl C, Linehan MM, Bohus M (2004): Borderline personality disorder. Lancet 364:453– 461. 8. Coid J, Yang M, Tyrer P, Roberts A, Ullrich S (2006): Prevalence and correlates of personality disorder in Great Britain. Br J Psychiatry 188: 423– 431. 9. Lenzenweger MF, Lane MC, Loranger AW, Kessler RC (2007): DSM-IV personality disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biol Psychiatry 62:553–564. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 455 10. Grant BF, Chou SP, Goldstein RB, Huang B, Stinson FS, Saha TD, et al. (2008): Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: Results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry 69:533–545. 11. Gunderson JG, Zanarini MC (1989): Pathogenesis in borderline personality. In: Tasman A, Hales RE, Frances AJ, editors. Review of Psychiatry. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 25– 48. 12. Linehan MM (1995): Understanding Borderline Personality Disorder. New York: The Guilford Press. 13. Donegan NH, Sanislow CA, Blumberg HP, Fulbright RK, Lacadie C, Skudlarski P, et al. (2003): Amygdala hyperreactivity in borderline personality disorder: Implications for emotional dysregulation. Biol Psychiatry 54: 1284 –1293. 14. Andreassi JL (1989): Psychophysiology: Human Behavior and Physiological Response, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 15. Stern RM, Ray WJ, Quigley KS (2001): Psychophysiological Recording, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press. 16. Siegle GJ, Carter CS, Thase ME (2006): Use of fMRI to predict recovery from unipolar depression with cognitive behavior therapy. Am J Psychiatry 163:735–738. 17. Amaral DG (2002): The primate amygdala and the neurobiology of social behavior: Implications for understanding social anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 51:11–17. 18. Whalen PJ, Rauch SL, Etcoff NL, McInerney SC, Lee MB, Jenike MA (1998): Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. J Neurosci 18:411– 418. 19. Phelps EA (2006): Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies of the human amygdala. Annu Rev Psychol 57:27–53. 20. Davis M, Whalen PJ (2001): The amygdala: Vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 6:13–34. 21. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (2005): International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. Technical Report A-6. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida. 22. Herpertz SC, Dietrich TM, Wenning B, Krings T, Erberich SG, Willmes K, et al. (2001): Evidence of abnormal amygdala functioning in borderline personality disorder: A functional MRI study. Biol Psychiatry 50:292–298. 23. Driessen M, Beblo T, Mertens M, Piefke M, Rullkoetter N, Silva-Saavedra A, et al. (2004): Posttraumatic stress disorder and fMRI activation patterns of traumatic memory in patients with borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry 55:603– 611. 24. Minzenberg MJ, Fan J, New AS, Tang CY, Siever LJ (2007): Fronto-limbic dysfunction in response to facial emotion in borderline personality disorder: An event-related fMRI study. Psychiatry Res 155:231–243. 25. Koenigsberg HW, Siever LJ, Lee H, Pizzarello S, New AS, Goodman M, et al. (2009): Neural correlates of emotion processing in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res 172:192–199. 26. Koenigsberg HW, Fan J, Ochsner KN, Liu X, Guise KG, Pizzarello S, et al. (2009): Neural correlates of the use of psychological distancing to regulate responses to negative social cues: A study of patients with borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry 66:854 – 863. 27. Schulze L, Domes G, Kruger A, Berger C, Fleischer M, Prehn K, et al. (2011): Neuronal correlates of cognitive reappraisal in borderline patients with affective instability. Biol Psychiatry 69:564 –573. 28. Siddle DA (1991): Orienting, habituation, and resource allocation: An associative analysis. Psychophysiology 28:245–259. 29. Wright CI, Fischer H, Whalen PJ, McInerney SC, Shin LM, Rauch SL (2001): Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli. Neuroreport 12:379 –383. 30. LeDoux J (1996): Emotional networks and motor control: A fearful view. Prog Brain Res 107:437– 446. 31. Davis M (1992): The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu Rev Neurosci 15:353–375. 32. Canli T, Zhao Z, Brewer J, Gabrieli JD, Cahill L (2000): Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. J Neurosci 20:RC99. 33. LaBar KS, Cabeza R (2006): Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:54 – 64. 34. Hazlett EA, Speiser LJ, Goodman M, Roy M, Carrizal M, Wynn JK, et al. (2007): Exaggerated affect-modulated startle during unpleasant stimuli in borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry 62:250 –255. 35. Ebner-Priemer UW, Kuo J, Schlotz W, Kleindienst N, Rosenthal MZ, Detterer L, et al. (2008): Distress and affective dysregulation in patients with www.sobp.org/journal 456 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2012;72:448 – 456 36. 37. 38. 39. 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. borderline personality disorder: A psychophysiological ambulatory monitoring study. J Nerv Ment Dis 196:314 –320. Limberg A, Barnow S, Freyberger HJ, Hamm AO (2011): Emotional vulnerability in borderline personality disorder is cue specific and modulated by traumatization. Biol Psychiatry 69:574 –582. Barnow S, Limberg A, Stopsack M, Spitzer C, Grabe HJ, Freyberger HJ, Hamm A (2011). Dissociation and emotion regulation in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 9:1–12. Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P, Pashler H (2009): Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect Psychol Sci 4:274 –290. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, et al. (2004): Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(suppl 1):S208 – S219. Buss AH, Perry M (1992): The aggression questionnaire. J Pers Soc Psychol 63:452– 459. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES (1995): Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol 51:768 –774. Harvey PD, Greenberg BR, Serper MR (1989): The affective lability scales: Development, reliability, and validity. J Clin Psychol 45:786 –793. Bernstein-Carlson E, Putnam FW (1993): An update on the Dissociative Experiences Scale. Dissociation 6:16 –27. Larsen RJ, Diener E (1985): A multitrait-multimethod examination of affect structure: Hedonic level and emotional intensity. Person Individ Diff 6:631– 636. Bernstein D, Fink L (1998): Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A Retrospective Self-Report. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. E-Prime (2002): E-Prime, 1.1 ed. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools. Bradley MM, Lang PJ (1994): Measuring emotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the Semantic Differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 25: 49 –59. Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S (2002): Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 17:825– 841. Smith SM (2002): Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 17:143–155. Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Zhang J, Newmark RE, Glanton CF, Zelmanova Y, et al. (2008): Frontal-striatal-thalamic mediodorsal nucleus dysfunction in schizophrenia-spectrum patients during sensorimotor gating. Neuroimage 42:1164 –1177. StatSoft, Inc (2010): STATISTICA (data analysis software system), version 9.1. Tulsa, OK: StatSoft, Inc. Worsley KJ (2001): Statistical analysis of activation images. In: Jezzard P, Matthews PM, Smith SM, editors. Functional MRI: An Introduction to Methods. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Phan KL, Liberzon I, Welsh RC, Britton JC, Taylor SF (2003): Habituation of rostral anterior cingulate cortex to repeated emotionally salient pictures. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:1344 –1350. Dyck M, Habel U, Slodczyk J, Schlummer J, Backes V, Schneider F, Reske M (2008): Negative bias in fast emotion discrimination in borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 39:855– 864. Dziobek I, Preibler S, Grozdanovic Z, Heuser I, Heekeren HR, Roepke S (2011): Neuronal correlates of altered empathy and social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage 57:539 –548. New AS, aan het Rot M, Ripoll LS, Perez-Rodriguez M, Lazarus S, Zipursky E, et al. (in press): Empathy and alexithymia in borderline personality disorder: Clinical and laboratory measures. J Pers Disord. www.sobp.org/journal E.A. Hazlett et al. 57. Schmahl C, Bohus M, Esposito F, Treede RD, Di Salle F, Greffrath W (2006): Neural correlates of antinociception in borderline personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:659 – 667. 58. Ebner-Priemer UW, Badeck S, Beckmann C, Wagner A, Feige B, Weiss I, et al. (2005): Affective dysregulation and dissociative experience in female patients with borderline personality disorder: A startle response study. J Psychiatr Res 39:85–92. 59. Ebner-Priemer UW, Mauchnik J, Kleindienst N, Schmahl C, Peper M, Rosenthal MZ, et al. (2009): Emotional learning during dissociative states in borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 34:214 – 222. 60. Kring AM, Neale JM (1996): Do schizophrenic patients show a disjunctive relationship among expressive, experiential, and psychophysiological components of emotion? J Abnorm Psychol 105:249 –257. 61. Raine A (1991): The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 17:555–564. 62. Shean G, Bell E, Cameron CD (2007): Recognition of nonverbal affect and schizotypy. J Psychol 141:281–291. 63. Victor TA, Furey ML, Fromm SJ, Ohman A, Drevits WC (2010): Relationship between amygdala response to masked faces and mood state and treatment in major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67:1128 –1138. 64. Bateman AW, Fonagy P (2004): Mentalization-based treatment of BPD. J Pers Disord18:36 –51. 65. Bateman A, Fonagy P (2008): 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality disorder: Mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. Am J Psychiatry 165:631– 638. 66. Bateman AW, Ryle A, Fonagy P, Kerr IB (2007): Psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder: Mentalization based therapy and cognitive analytic therapy compared. Int Rev Psychiatry 19:51– 62. 67. Linehan MM, Comtois KA, Murray AM, Brown MZ, Gallop RJ, Heard HL, et al. (2006): Two-year randomized controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behavior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal behaviors and borderline personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:757–766. 68. Lynch TR, Trost WT, Salsman N, Linehan MM (2007): Dialetical behavior therapy for borderline personality disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 3:181– 205. 69. Scheel KR (2000): The empirical basis of dialectical behavior therapy: Summary, critique, and implications. Clin Psychol Sci Prac 7:68 – 86. 70. Linehan MM (2000): The empirical basis of dialectical behavior therapy: Development of new treatments versus evaluation of existing treatments. Clin Psychol Sci Prac 7:113–119. 71. Debiec J, Ledoux JE (2004): Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala. Neuroscience 129:267–272. 72. Yang YL, Chao PK, Lu KT (2006): Systemic and intra-amygdala administration of glucocorticoid agonist and antagonist modulate extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology 31:912–924. 73. Yang YL, Chao PK, Ro LS, Wo YY, Lu KT (2007): Glutamate NMDA receptors within the amygdala participate in the modulatory effect of glucocorticoids on extinction of conditioned fear in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:1042–1051. 74. First MB, Spitzer R, Gibbon M, Williams J (1996): Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I Disorders-Patient Edition. New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute. 75. Pfohl B, Blum N, Zimmerman M (1997): Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality (SIDP-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.