AIso published in
Oxford Handbools for Language Teachers
Teaching American English Pronunciation
PeterAuery and Susan Ehrlich
Success in English Teaching
Paul Dauies and Eric Pearse
Doing Second Language Rgsearch
James Dean Brown andTheodore S. Rodgers
Teaching Business English
Marh Ellis and Christine Johnson
Intercultural Business Communication
Robert Gibson
Teaching and Learning in the Language Classroom
Tricia Hedge
Teaching English Overseas: An Introduction
Sandra Lee Mcl{ay
Teaching English
as
an International Language
Sandra Lee Mcl{ay
Communication in the Language Classroom
Tbn! Lynch
Teaching Young Language Learners
Annamaria Pinter
Explaining English Grammar
GeorgeYule
HowLanguages
are
l*arned
Third edition
Patsy
M. Ligbtbou/n andNina Spada
OXFORD
IJNIVERSITY PRESS
OXTORD
IJNIVERSITY
PRESS
Great Clarendon Street, Oxford ox2 6Dp
Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford.
It firrthers tfie University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship,
and education by publishing worldwide in
Oxford NewYork
Auckland CapeTown Dares Salaam HongKong Karachi
Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi
NewDelhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto
Withoffices in
Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece
Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore
South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam
oxFoRD and oxtonn ENGLISH are registered trade marks of
Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries
@
Oxford University Press zoo6
The moral rights of the author have been asserted
Database right Oxford University Press (maker)
First published zoo6
2011 2O7O 2OOg
109876s
All rights reserved. No part of this publication maybe reproduced,
stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press (with
the sole exception ofphotocopying carried out under the conditions stated
in the paragraph headed 'Photocopying'), or as expressly permitted by law, or
under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics rights organization.
Enquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope ofthe above should
be sent to the EIT Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the
address above
You must not circulate this book in any other binding or cover
and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer
Photocopying
The Publisher grants permission for the photocopying of those pages marked
'photocopiable' according to the following conditions. Individual purchasers
may make copies for their own use or for use by classes that they teach.
School purchasers may make copies for use by staffand students, but this
permission does not extend to additional schools or branches
Under no circumstances may any part of this book be photocopied for resale
Anywebsites referred to in this publication are in the public domain and
their addresses are provided by Oxford University Press for information only.
Oxford University Press disclaims any responsibility for the content
IsnN: 978 ot94zzz46
Printed in China
CONTENTS
Acknowledgements
Preface to the third edition
Introduction
Language learning in early childhood
The first three years: Milestones and developmental sequences
Grammatical morphemes
Negation
Questions
The pre-school years
The school years
Explaining first language acquisition
The behaviourist perspective: Saywhat I say
The innatist perspective: Itt all in your mind
Interactionist/developmental perspectives: Learning from
inside and out
Language disorders and delays
x1
xiii
XV
I
1
3
4
5
7
8
10
l0
T5
I9
24
Childhood bilingualism
Summary
25
Explaining second language learning
Contexts for language learning
2g
29
30
32
34
34
35
36
Learner characteristics
Learning conditions
Behaviourism
Second language applications: Mimicry and memorization
The innatist perspective: Universal Grammar
Seco nd language applications : I(rashen's'monito r mo del'
Current psychological theories: The cognitivist/developmental
perspective
Information processing
Connectionism
The competition model
Second language applications: Interacting, noticing,
and processing
The sociocultural perspective
S
econd language applications: I-earning by talkin g
Theory into practice
27
38
39
4r
42
43
47
47
49
vlll
Contents
3
Individual differences in second language learning
tVho is a'good language learner'?
Research on learner characteristics
Intelligence
Aptitude
Learning sryles
Personality
Motivation and attitudes
Identiry and ethnic group affiliation
Learner beliefs
Age of acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis
4
67
Summary
74
Learner language
Studying the language ofsecond language learners
Contrastive analysis, error analysis, and interlanguage
Developmental sequences
Grammatical morphemes
77
77
Negation
78
82
83
85
86
88
90
Questions
Possessive determiners
Relative clauses
Reference to pasr
9t
Movement through developmentd sequences
More about first language infuence
Vocabulary
92
93
96
Pragmatics
100
Phonology
104
5 Observing learning and teaching
in the second language
classroom
Natural and instructional settings
Observation schemes
Classroom comparisons : Teacher-student interactions
Classroom comparisons : Student-student inreractions
Corrective feedback in the classroom
Questions in the classroom
Ethnography
Summary
6
53
54
54
57
57
59
60
53
65
56
Second language learning in the classroom
Six proposals for classroom teaching
I
2
Get it right from the beginning
Just listen ... and read
109
109
1r4
rr5
t2I
125
r30
r33
135
r37
r37
138
r43
Contents
3 Lett talk
4 Two for one
150
5
160
155
Teach what is teachable
6 Get it right in the end
The implications of classroom research for teaching
Summary
165
176
179
I
7
Popular ideas about language learning revisited
Glossary
Bibliography
Index
183
195
207
229
INTRODUCTION
\(hen new foreign language
teaching methods and textbooks are introduced, they are often said to be based on the latest research in psychology,
linguistics, or pedagogy. Teachers are told that they will be more effective
than those that have gone before. In many cases, the new approaches are
prescribed for immediate implementation in a school or region. Sometimes,
the new materials come with opportunities for extensive training in their
implementation. Sometimes, they are simply ordered and distributed to
teachers who have to do their best to use them effectively.
Teachers have seen many different approaches over the past fifty years. One
approach requires students to learn rules of grammar and lists ofvocabulary
to use in translating literary texts. Another emphasizes the value of having
students imitate and practise a set of correct sentences and memorize entire
dialogues. Yet another stresses the importance of encouraging'natural' com-
munication berween students as they engage co-operatively in tasks or
projects while using the new language. In some classrooms, the second
language is used as the medium to teach subject matter, with the assumption
that the language itself will be learned incidentally as students focus on the
academic content.
How are teachers to evaluate the potential effectiveness of new methods? To
be sure, the most important infuence on teachers' decisions is their own
experience with previous successes or disappointments, as well as their
understanding of the needs and abilities of their students. \We believe thar
ideas drawn from research and theory in second language acquisition are also
valuable in helping teachers evaluate claims made by proponents of various
language teaching methods. The goal of this book is to introduce 1s2gfrs15both novice and experienced-to some of the language acquisition research
that may help them not only to evaluate existing textbooks and materials but
also to adapt them in ways that are more consistent with our understanding
of how languages are learned.
The book begins with a chapter on language learning in early childhood.
This background is important because both second language research and
second language teaching have been influenced by changes in our understanding of how children acquire their first language. In fact, one significant
research finding concerns the similarities berween first and second language
acquisition.
XVl
Introduction
In Chapter 2,
several theories that have been advanced to explain second
language learning are presented and discussed. In Chapter 3, we rurn our
attention to how individual learner characteristics may affecr success. In
Chapter 4, we look at second language learners' developing knowledge and
their abiliry to use that knowledge. Chapter 5 begins with a compariion of
natural and instructional environments for second language learning. \7e
then examine some different ways in which classroom researcherc h",r.
observed and described teaching and learning practices in second language
classrooms.
In Chapter 6, we examine some of the proposals that have been made for
second language teaching. Examples of research related to each of the
proposals are presented, leading to a discussion of the evidence available for
assessing their effectiveness. The chapter ends with a discussion of what
research findings suggest about the most effective ways to teach and learn a
second language in the classroom.
A Glossary provides a quick reference for a number of terms that may be new
or have specific technical meanings in the conrexr of language acquisition
research. Glossary words are shown in small capital letrers where they first
appear in the text. For readers who would like to find our more, a list of
suggestions for further reading is included at the end of each chapter. The
Bib_liography provides full reference information for the suggestedreadings
and all the works that are referred to in the text.
\7e have tried to present the information in away that does nor assume rhar
readers are already familiar with research methods or rheoretical issues in
second language learning. Examples and case studies are included
throughout the book to illustrate the research ideas. Many of the examples
are taken from second language classrooms. \7e have included a numbei of
opportunities for readers to practise some of the techniques of observation
and analysis used in the research that we review in this book.
Before we begin ...
It is probably true, as some have claimed, that most of us teach as we were
taught or in a way that matches our ideas and preferences about how we
learn. Thke a moment to reflect on your views about how languages are
learned and what you think this means about how they should 6. t"ttght.
The statements on the following pages summarize some popularviews abtut
language learning and teaching. Think about whether you agree or disagree
with each opinion. Keep these statements and your reactions to them in
mind as you read about current research and theory in second language
learning. \7e will rerurn to rhese opinions in Chapter 7.
Irutoduction
Popakr opinions about
la.nguage lcarning and' teaching
lndicate the extent to which you agree wkh each statement by marking an X at
the appropriate point on the line between 'strongly agree' and 'strongly
disagree'.
I
Languages are learned mainlythrough imitation.
strongly agree
2
strongly disagree
Parents usually correct young children when they make grammatical
errors.
strongly agree
3
trlll
tl
ll
strongly disagree
Highly intelligent people are good language learners.
strongly agree
trtll
strongly disagree
The most important predictor of success in second language acquisition is
motivation.
stronglyagree I
I
I
I
I
stronglydisagree
The earlier a secondJanguage is introduced in school programmes, the
greater the likelihood of success in learning.
strongly agree
trtll
strongly disagree
Most of the mistakes that second language learners make are due
to
interference from their first language.
strongly agree
7
trtll
The best way to learn new vocabulary
strongly agree
rrtll
strongly disagree
is
through reading.
strongly disagree
It is essentialfor learners to be able to pronounce all the individual sounds
in the second language.
strongly agree
trtll
strongly disagree
Once learners know roughly 1000 words and the basic structure of a
language, they can easily participate in conversations with native speakers.
strongly agree
l0
tltll
strongly disagree
Teachers should presentgrammatical rules one at a time, and learners
should practise examples of each one before going on to another.
strongly agree
trtll
strongly disagree
XVlI
xviii
Intodttction
I
I
Teachers should teach simple language structures before complex ones.
stronglyagree I
12
I
I
I
Learners' errors should be corrected as soon
prevent the formation of bad habits.
I
as
strongly agree
I
3
they are made in order to
strongly disagree
Teachers should use materials that expose students to only those
language structures they have already been taught.
stronglyagree I
l4
strongly disagree
I
I
I
I
strongly disagree
When learners are allowed to interact freely (for example, in group or
pair activities), they copy each other's mistakes.
stronglyagree I
l5
I
strongly disagree
I
I
I
strongly disagree
Teachers should respond to students' errors by correctly rephrasing what
they have said rather than by expricitry pointing out the error.
stronglyagree I
,7
I
Students learn what they are taught.
stronglyagree I
l6
I
I
I
I
I
strongly disagree
students can learn both language and academic content (for example,
science and history) simultaneously in classes where the subject matter is
taught in their second language.
stronglyagree I
Photocopiable
@
I
Oxford University press
I
I
strongly disagree
LANGUAGE LEARNING IN
EARLY CHILDHOOD
AceursrrroN is one of the most impressive and fascinating
human
development. \tre listen with pleasure to the sounds made
aspecrs of
by a three-monrh-old baby. We laugh and'answer' the conversational'ba-baba babbling of older babies, and we share in the pride and joy of parents
whose one-year-old has uttered the first 'bye-bye'. Indeed, learning a
ianguage is an amazingfeat-one that has attracted the attention of linguists
and psychologists for generations. How do children accomplish this? What
LANGUAGE
enables a
child not only to learn words, but to put them together in
meaningful sentences? \fhat pushes children to go on developing complex
grammarical language even though their early simple communication is
successful for most purposes? Does child language develop similarly around
the world? How do bilingual children acquire more than one language?
characteristics of the
will then consider several theories that have
been offered as explanations for how language is learned. There is an
immense body of research on child language. Although much research has
been done in middle-class North American and European families, there is a
In this
chapter, we
will look briefly at some of the
language ofyoung children. \We
rich body of cross-linguistic and cross-cultural research as well. Researchers
have travelled all over the world to observe, record, and study children's early
in this chapter is to touch on a few
main points in this research, primarily as a preparation for the discussion of
language development. Our purpose
sECoND LANGUAGn acquisition, which is the focus of this book.
The first three years: Milestones and
developmental sequences
One remarkable thing about FrRST LANGUAGT acquisition is the high
degree of siry_ilariqy- in 1fu- early t
tttany aspects
F6."tah.rr f,;e described o rvs r o p M E N rAL s e qffiE6
of first language acquisition. The earliest vocalizations are simply the
involuntary crying that babies do rvhen thev are hungry or uncomfortable.
Language learning in early childhood
Soon, however, we hear the cooing and gurgling sounds of contented babies,
beds looking at fascinating shapes and moyement around
lyt"g in their
them. Even though they have little control over the sounds they make in
these early weeks of life, infants are able to hear very subtle differences
between the sounds of human languages. In cleverly designed experiments,
Peter Eimas and his colleagues (1971) demonstrated that tiny babies can
hear the difference bern'ee" 'pd and 'bd, for example. And yer, ir may be
many months before their own vocalizations (babbling) begin to reflect the
characteristics of the language or languages they hear.
By the end of their first yeaL most babies understand quite a few frequently
repeated words. They wave when someone says 'bye-by.'; they clap when
someone says pat-a-cake'; they eagerly hurry to the kitchen when'juice and
cookies' are mentioned. At twelve months, most babies will have begun to
produce a word or two that everyone recognizes. By the age of two, most
children reliably produce at least fifty different words and some produce
many more. About this time, they begin to combine words into simple
sentences such as 'Mommy juice' and 'baby fall down . These senrences are
sometimes called 'telegraphic' because they leave out suchiffiiiiTes. ,
preFaFqtf"d
\7e recognizs- qhem as sentences because,
-tra=Bp-g*"nd c ne.r
missing, the word order refects the word orde
hbaring a
use tne com
words have a meaning relationshi
more tnan lust a lrst ot wo
us, for an
i3h-_qeaking
;"gqtrffi
f-Ir N
"*iliqy
qrl-o N
".tbs.
t
does notffihing
as 'baby kiss'. Remarkably,
we also see evidence, even in these early sentences, that children are doing
more than imperfectly imitatingwhat they have heard. Their rwo- and threeword sentences show signs that they can creatively combine words. For
example, 'more outside' may mean 'I want to go outside again.' Depending
on the situation, 'Daddy uh-oh' might mean 'Daddy fell down or 'Daddy
dropped something' or eyen 'Daddy, please do that funny thing where you
pretend to drop me offyour lap.'
fu children progress through the discovery of language in their first three
years, there are predictable patterns in the emergence and development of
many features of the language they are learning. For some language features,
these patterns have been described in terms of developmental sequences or
'stages'. To some extent, these stages in language acquisition are related to
childrent cognitive development. For example, children do not use temporal
adverbs such as 'tomorrow' or 'last week' until they develop some understanding of time. In other cases, the developmental sequences seem to refect
the gradual mastery of the linguistic elements for expressing ideas that have
been present in children's cognitive understanding for a long time. For
example, children can distinguish between singular and plural long before
they reliably add plural endings to nouns. Mastering irregular plurals takes
Language learning in early childhood
even more time and may not be completely under control
until the school
years.
Grammatical morphemes
In the
1960s, several researchers focused on how children acquire grammatical morphemes in English. One of the best-known studies was carried
out by Roger Brown and his colleagues and students. In a roNcrruDINAL
study of the language development of three children (called Adam, Eve, and
Sarah) they found that fourteen grammatical morphemes were acquired in a
remarkably similar sequence. That research is reported in Brown's 1973
book. The list below (adapted from that book) shows some
of
the
morphemes they studied.
present progressi ve - ing (Mommy runn ing)
plural -s (Two books)
irregular past forms (Baby went)
possessive 's (Daddy's hat)
copula (Annie ishappy)
articles the and a
regular past -ed(She walkel)
third person singular simple present
auxiliary be (He
zi
-s (She
runs)
coming)
Brown and his colleagues found that a child who had mastered the
grammatical morphemes at the bottom of the list was sure to have mastered
those at the top, but the reverse was not true. Thus, there was evidence for a
'developmental sequence'or order of acquisition. However, the children did
not acquire the morphemes at the same age or rate. Eve had mastered nearly
all the morphemes before she was two-and-a-halfyears old, while Sarah and
Adam were still working on them when theywere three-and-a-half or four.
Brown's longitudinal work was confirmed in a cnoss-sEcTloNer study of
rwenry-one children. Jill and Peter de Villiers (1973) found that children
who correcdy used the morphemes that Adam, Eve, and Sarah had acquired
late were also able to use the ones that Adam, Eve, and Sarah had acquired
earlier. The children mastered the morphemes at different ages, just as
Adam, Eve, and Sarah had done, but the order of their acquisition was very
similar. Theywere similar to each other and similar to Adam, Eve, and Sarah.
Many hypotheses have been advanced to explain why these grammatical
morphemes are acquired in the observed order. Researchers have studied the
frequency with which the morphemes occur in parents' speech, the cognitive
complexiry of the meanings represented by each morpheme, and the
difficulry of perceiving or pronouncing them. In the end, there has been no
simple satisfactory explanation for the sequence, and most researchers agree
Language learning in early childhood
that the order is determined by an interaction among a number of different
factors.
To supplement the evidence we have from simply observing children, some
carefully designed procedures have been developed to further explore
childrent knowledge of grammatical morphemes. One of the first and best
known is the so-called *tg test' developed by Jean Berko Gleason in the
1950s. In this 'test', children are shown drawings of imaginary creatures with
n9v-elnames or people performing mysterious actions. For.""-ple, they are
told, 'Here is awug. Nowthere are two ofthem. There are two '.or'I{ere
is a man who knows how to bod. Yesterday he did the same thing. Yesterday,
he
By completing these sentences with 'wugs' and'bodda', children
demonstrate that theyknow rules for the formation ofplural and simple past
in -'.
English. By generalizing these patterns to words th.y have never heard
before, they show that their language is not just a list of memorized word
pairs such as'book/books' and'nod/nodded'.
The acquisition of other language features also shows how childrent
language develops systemati caIly, and how they go beyond what they have
heard to create new forms and strucrures.
Negation
Children learn the functions of negation very early. That is, they learn to
comment on the disappearance ofobjects, to refuse a suggestion, or reject an
assertion, eYen at the single word stage. However, as Lois Bloomt (f ggf
)
longitudinal studies show, erren though children understand these functions
a1d express them with single words and gestures, ir takes some time before
ah.y ca]l exp_ress them in sentences, using the appropriate words and word
order. The following srages in the derrilopm."t oi negation have been
observed in the acquisition of English. Simil"r rt"g., h"rre-b.en observed in
other languages as well (\fode 1981).
Stage
I
Negation is usually expressed by the word'no', either all alone or
word in the utterance.
as
the first
No. No cookie. No comb hair.
Stage
2
(Jtterances grow longer and the sentence subject may be included.
The
negative word appears just before the verb. Sentences expressing rejection or
prohibition often use'dont'.
Daddy no comb hair.
Dont touch that!
Language learning in early childhood
Stage 3
The negative element is inserted into a more complex sentence. Children
may add forms of the negative other than 'no', including words like 'can't'
and 'dont'. These sentences appear to follow the correct English pattern of
attaching the negative to the auxiliary or modal verb. However, children do
not yet vary these forms for different persons or tenses:
I can't do it. He don't want it.
Stage 4
Children begin to attach the negative element to the correct form of
auxiliary verbs such as 'do' and 'be':
You
didnt have supper.
She doesnt want
it.
Even though their language system is by now quite complex, they may sdll
have difficulry with some other features related to negatives.
I don't have no more candies.
Questions
The challenge of learning complex language systems is also illustrated in the
developmental stages through which children learn to ask questions.
There is a remarkable consistency
in the way children learn to
form
questions in English. For one thing, there is a predictable order in which the
'wh-words' emerge (Bloom 1991). '\7hat'is generally the first wh- question
word to be used. It is often learned as part of a cnuNr ('\X/hassat?') and it is
some time before the child learns that there are variations of the form, such
as '\fhat is that?' and '\7hat are these?'
'\(/here' and 'who' emerge very soon. Identifying and locating people and
objects are within the childs understanding of the world. Furthermore,
adults tend to ask children just these types of questions in the early days of
language learning, for example, '\fhere's Mommy?', or'\(ho's that?'
'\trhy' emerges around the end of the second year and becomes a favourite
for the nexr year or rwo. Children seem to ask an endless number of
quesrions beginning with '*hy', having discovered how effectively this little
word gets adults to engage in conversation, for example, '\(hy that lady has
blue hair?'
Finally, when the child has a better understanding of manner and time,
'how' and'when' emerge. In contrast to'what','where', and'who' questions,
children sometimes ask the more cognitively difficult 'why', 'when', and
'how' quesrions without always understanding the answers they get, as the
following conversation with a four-t'ear-old clearly shows:
Language learruing in early childhood
Child \fhen can we go outside?
Parent In about five minutes.
Child l-2-3-4-5!! Can we go now?
The abiliry to use these question words is at least pardy tied to children's
cognitive development. It is also predicted in part by the quesrions children
are asked and the linguistic complexiry of quesiionr with different w/t-words.
Thus it does not seem surprising that there is consistency in the sequence of
their acquisition._Perhaps more remarkable is the consiitency in the acquisition ofword order in questions. This development is not baied orr l."rning
new meanings, but rather on learning different linguistic forms ro express
meanings that are already understood.
Stage
I
Children's earliest questions are single words or simple rwo- or three-word
sentences
with rising intonation:
Cookie? Mummy book?
At the same time, they may
produce some correcr questions-correct
because they have been learned as chunks:
\Theret Daddy? \7hatt that?
Stage
2
As they begin to ask more new questions, children use the word order of the
declarative sentence, with rising intonation.
I
I
I
(
You like this? I have some?
T
They continue to produce the correct chunk-learned forms such
that?' alongside their own created questions.
Stage
as
'\Whatt
3
Gradually, children notice that the structure of questions is different and
begin to produce quesrions such
as:
Can I go? Are you happy?
Although_some questions at this stage match the adult parrern, they may be
right for the wrong reason. To describe this, we need ro ,.. the pattern from
j!..hjJd-k perspective rather than from the perspective ofthe
grammar.
\7e call this stage'fronting'because the childt rule seems to be"d.rlt
th"tlu.rtions
1e form_ed by putting somethin g-. verb form or question word^-at the
'front' of a sentence, leaving the rest of the sentence in its statement form.
Is the teddy is tired? Do I can have a cookie?
\7hy you dont have one? \Vhy you catched it?
E
fi
o
a
Langaage learning in early cltildhood
Stage
4
At
stage 4, some questions are formed by subject-auxiliary inversion. The
questions resemble those of stage 3, but there is more variety in the
auxiliaries that appear before the subject.
Are you going to play with me?
At this stage, children can even add 'do' in questions in which there would be
no auxiliary in the declarative version of the sentence.
Do dogs like ice cream?
Even at this stage, however, children seem able to use either inversion or a
uth- word, but not both. Therefore, we may find inversion in 'yes/no'
questions but not in wlt- questions, unless they are FoRMULerc units such as
'-Whatt that?'
Stage
5
At stage 5, both wlt- and'yes/no' questions are formed correctly.
Are these your boots? \7hy did you do that? Does Daddy have a box?
Negative questions may still be
a
bit too difficult.
\Vhy the teddy bear cant go outside?
And even tlough performance on most questions is correct, there is still one
more hurdle. \7hen w/t- words appear in subordinate clauses or embedded
questions, children overgeneralize the inverted form that would be correcr
for simple questions and produce senrences such as:
Ask him why cant he go out.
Stage
6
At this stage, children are able to correctly form all question rypes, including
negative and complex embedded questions
through developmental-sequences does not always follow a steady
uninterrupted path. Children appear to learn new things and then fall back
on old patterns when there is added stress in a newsituation orwhen theyare
using other new elements in their language. But the overall path takes them
toward mastery of the language that is spoken around them.
Passage
The pre-school years
By the age of four, most children can ask questions, give commands, report
real events, and create stories about irnaginary ones-using correct word
order and grammadcal markers most of-the time. In fact,-it is generally
accepted that by age four, chil&en have ma.s1s1sd the basic srrucrures of the
Language learning in early childhood
language or languages spoken to them in these early years. Three- and fouryear-olds continue to learn vocabulary at the rate ofseveral words a day. They
begin to acquire less frequent and more complex linguistic structures such as
passives and relative clauses.
Much of childrent language acquisition effort in the late pre-school years is
spent in developirg their ability to use language in a widening social
environment. They use language in a greater variety of situations. They
interact more often with unfamiliar adults. They begin to talk sensibly on
the telephone to invisible grandparents (younger children do not understand that their telephone paftner cannot see what they see). They acquire
the aggressive or cajoling language that is needed to defend their toys in the
playground. They show that they have learned the difference between how
adults talk to babies and how they talk to each other, and they use this
knowledge in elaborate pretend play in which they practise using these
difFerent'voices'. In this way, they explore and begin to understand how and
why language varies.
In the pre-school years, they also develop METALrNGUrsrrc A\tr/'ARENESS,
the abiliry to ffeat language as an object separate from the meaning it
conveys. Three-year-old children can tell you that itt 'silly' to say'drink the
chair', because it doesnt make sense. However, although they would neyer
say 'cake the eat', they are less sure that there's anything wrong with it. They
may show that they know itt a bit odd, but they will focus mainly on the
fact that they can understand what it means. Five year-olds, on the other
hand, know that'drink the chair' is wrong in a different way from'cake the
eat'. They can tell you that one is 'silly' but the other is 'the wrong way
around'.
The school years
Although pre-school children acquire complex knowledge and skills for
language and language use, the school settingwill require newways of using
language and bring new opportunities for language developmenr.
Children develop the ability to understand language and to use it to express
themselves in the pre-school years. In the school years, these abilities expand
and grow. Children also develop more sophisticated metalinguistic
awareness. Learning to read gives a major boost to this aspect of language
development. Seeing words represented by letters and other symbols on a
page leads children to a new understanding that language has form as well as
meaning. Reading reinforces the understanding that a 'word' is separate
from the thing it represents. Unlike three-year-olds, children who can read
understand that 'the' is a word, just as 'house' is. They understand that
Language learning in early childhood
'caterpillar' is a longer word than 'train', even though the object it represents
is substantially shorter! Metalinguistic awareness also includes the discovery
of such things as ambiguiry. Knowing that words and sentences can have
multiple meaning gives children access to word jokes, trick questions, and
riddles, which they love to share with their friends and family.
One of the most impressive language developments in the early school years
is the astonishing growth of vocabulary. Many words are acquired in early
childhood, when the repetition of ordinary events and experiences provides
frequent exposure to a limited number ofwords. Children enter school with
the abiliry to understand and produce hundreds or even a few thousand
words. Many more are learned at school. In both the spoken and written
language at school, some words (for example, 'homework', 'ruler', and
'workbook ) appear frequently in situations where their meaning is either
immediately or gradually revealed. \7ords like 'population' or 'latitude'
occur less frequently, but they are made important by their significance in
academic subject mafter. Vocabulary grows atatatebetween several hundred
and more than a thousand words a year, depending mainly on how much
and how widely children read (Nagy, Herman, and Anderson 1985). The
kind ofvocabulary growth required for school success is likely to come from
both reading for assignments and reading for pleasure, whether narrative or
non-fiction. Dee Gardner (2004) suggests that reading a variery of text types
is an essential part of vocabulary growth. His research has shown how the
range of vocabulary in narrative texts is different from that in non-fiction.
There are words in non-fiction texts that are unlikely to occur in stories or
novels. In addition, non-fiction tends to include more oppoftunities to see a
word in its different forms (for example, 'mummy', 'mummies', 'mummified'). The importance of reading for vocabulary growth is seen when
observant parents report a child using a new word but mispronouncing it in
away that reveals it has been encountered only in written form.
Another important development in the school years is the acquisition of
different language REGISTERs. Children learn how written language differs
from spoken language, how the language used to speak to the principal is
different from the language of the playground, how the language of a science
report is different from the language of a narrative. AsTerry Piper (1998)
and others have documented, some children will have even more to learn.
They come to school speaking an ethnic or regional venrnrv of the school
language that is quite different from the one used by the teacher. They will
have to learn that another variery often referred to as the sTINDARD
vARIETv is required for successful academic work. Other children arrive at
school speaking a different language altogether. For these children, the work
of language learning in the earlv school years presents additional
opportunities and challenges. We rviil return to this topic when we discuss
BTLTNGUALTsM in early childhood.
10
Language learning in early childhood
Explaining first language acquisition
These descriptions of language development from infancy through the early
school years show that we have considerable knowledge of what children
learn in their early language development. More controversial, however, are
questions about how this remarkable development takes place. Over the past
fifty years, three main theoretical positions have been advanced to explain it:
behaviourist, innatist, and interactional/developmental perspectives.
The behauiourist perspectiue: Say what I say
BEHAVIOURISM was a theory of learning that was very infuential in the
1940s and 1950s, especially in the United States. \fith regard to language
learning, the best-known proponent of this psychological theory was
B. F. Skinner. Thaditional behaviourists hypothesized that when children
imitated the language produced by those around them, their arremprs ro
reproduce what they heard received positive reinforcemenr'. This could take
the form of praise or just successful communication. Thus encouraged by
their environment, children would continue to imitate and practisi these
sounds and patterns until they formed 'habits' of correct language use.
According to this view, the qualiry and quantity of the language the child
hears, as well as the consistency of the reinforcemenr offered by others in the
environment, would shape the childt language behaviour. This theory gives
great importance to the environment as the source of everything the child
needs to learn.
Analysing children's speech: Definitions and examples
The behaviourists viewed imitation and practiceas the primary processes in
language development. To clarifywhat is meant by these rwo rerms, consider
the following definitions and examples.
Imitation: word-for-word repetition
of all or part of
someone
else's
utterance.
Mother
Lo.y
Shall we play with the dolls?
Play with dolls.
Practice: repetitive manipulation of form.
Cindy He eat carrots. The other one eat carrots. They both eat
carrots.
Now examine the transcripts from Peter, Cindy, and Kathryn. Theywere all
about twenty-four months old when theywere recorded as they played with
a visiting adult. Using the definitions above, notice how Peter imitates the
adult in the following dialogue.
Language learning in early childhood
Peter (24 months) is playrng with a dump truck while rwo adults, patsy
and
Lois, look on.
Peter
Lois
Get more.
You're gonna put more wheels in the dump truck?
Peter Dump
truck. Wheels. Dump truck.
(later)
Patsy \7hat happened to it (the truck)?
(lo_oking under chair for it) Lose it. Dump truck! Dump truck!
Peter
Lois
Fall! Fall!
Yes, the
Peter Dump
dump truck fell down.
truck fell down. Dump truck.
(Unpublished data from P M. Lightbown)
Ifwe analysed alarger sample of Peter'sspeech, we would see rhar 30-40 per
cent of his sentences were imitations of what someone else had just
said. \fe
rvould also see that his imitations were not random. That ir, h. did not
simply imitate 3!-40 per cent of everything he heard. Detailed analyses
of
large samples of Petert speech orr.,
showed that he imitated
rvords and sentence srrucrures that "bo.rtl,yw
were .iust beginning ro appear in his
spontaneous speech. Once these new elements beiarn. rolidly
gio""ded in
his language sysrem, he stopped imitating them and wenr'oi ,o imitate
others. Unlike a parrot who imitates the f"-ili"r and continues to
repeat the
t1-.t thinry again and again, children appear to imitate selectiv.ty. n.
choice ofwhat to imitate seems to be basedon something
new th"t th.y have
just begun to understand and use, not simply
on what is 'available' in the
environment. For- example, consider how Cindy imitates and
practises
ianguage in the following conversations.
Cindy (24 months, 15 days) is looking at a picture of a carrot in a
book and
trying ro ger Patsyt attention.
Cindy Kawo? kawo? kawo? kawo? kawo?
Patsy What are rhe rabbits eating?
Cindy They eating ... kando?
Patsy No, thatt a carror.
cindy carrot. (pointing to each carror on the page) The other ...
carror. The other carror. The other carrot.
(A few minutes later, cindy brings patsy
a
stuffed toy rabbit.)
Patsy \flhat does this rabbit like to eat?
Cindy (incomprehensible) eat the carrors.
(Cindy gers anorher stuffed rabbit.)
Cindy He (incomprehensible)
They both ear carrors.
eat carrots. The other one eat carrors.
11
T2
Language learning in early childhood
(One week later, Cindy opens the book to the same page.)
Cindy Here's the carrots. (pointing) Is that a carror?
Patsy Yes.
(Unpublished data from
P.
(
M. Lightbown)
Cindy appears to be working hard on her language acquisition. She practises
newwords and structures in a way that sounds like a student in some foreign
language classes! Perhaps most interesting is that she remembers the 'hlnguage lesson' a week later and turns straight to the pagein the book she had
not seen since Patsyt last visit. \7hat is most suiking is that, like Peter, her
imitation and practice appear to be focused on what
on.
she is
Not all children imitate and practise' as much as Peter and Cindy did. The
amount of imitation in the speech of other children, whose development
proceeded at a rare comparable to that of Cindy and Peter, has been
calculated at less than 10 per cenr.
Consider the examples of imitation and practice in the following conversation between Kathryn and Lois.
Kathryn (24 months)
Lois
Did you
see
Lois
IGthry
rhe toys I brought?
the choo choo train?
brought the choo choo train.
(reaching for bag) I want play with choo choo train. I want
play with choo choo train. (taking out slide) 'Want play.
Yes, Lois
\flhat's this?
Oh you know what that is.
I(athty- Put down on floor. This. I do this.
Lois
(Kathryn puts the slide on rhe foor.)
I(athty"
Lois
(taking out rwo cars of train) Do this. I wanr do this. (trying
ro put train together) I do this. I do this.
.OK. You can do it. You can do it. Look I'll show you how.
(Lois puts it together.)
IGthryo
(searching in box) I get more. Get a more. No more choo
choo train. Get truck. (taking out truck) Kathryn truck.
\Where?'$7"here
a
more choo choo train?
a
s
currently working
The samples of speech from Peter and Cindy seem to lend some supporr to
the behaviourist explanation of language acquisition. Even so, as we saw, rhe
choice of what to imitate and practise seemed determined by somerhing
inside the child rather than by the environment.
I(athty" I bring toys? Choo choo? Lois brought
I
I
1
Language learning in early childhood
Lois
Inside. Itt in the box.
Kathryn A choo choo? (taking out part of train) This is a choo choo
train.
(from Bloom and Lahey 1978:135)
Like Cindy, Kathryn sometimes repeats herself or produces a series of related
'practice' sentences, but she rarely imitates the other speaker. Instead, she
asks and answers questions and elaborates on the other speaker's questions or
statements.
Thus, children vary in the amount of imitation they do. In addition, many
of the things they say show that they are using language creatively, not just
repeating what they have heard. This is evident in the following examples.
Patterns in language
The first example shows a child in the process of learning patterns in
language, in this case the rules of word formation, and overgeneralizing
them to new contexts. Randall (35 months) had a sore on his hand.
Mother Maybe
we need to take you to the doctor.
Randall \Mhy? So he can doc my little bump?
Randall forms the verb'doc' from the noun'doctor', by analogywith farmers
who farm, swimmers who swim, and actors who act.
I putted. the ptata olL
q+ie ta.Ile !
/Yon nrce.rt, t p,*E
the pLates onthe
/
No, I putted
tltcn on aIL by
rryself
!
Unfamiliarformulas
Even older children have to work out some puzzles, for example, when
familiar language is used in unfamiliar ways, as in the example below. \fhen
T3
t4
Language learning in early childhood
David (5 years, 1 month) was at his older sister's birthday parry, toasrs were
proposed with grape juice in stemmed glasses:
Father I d like to propose a roasr.
Several minutes later, David raised his glass:
David
I d like to propose a piece of bread.
Onlywhen laughter sent David slinking from the table did the group realize
that he wasn't intentionally making a play on words! He was concenrrating
so hard on performing the fascinating new gesture and the formulaic
expression'I'd like to propose ...'that he failed to realize that the word he
thought he knew-'1s251'-1ry25 not the same toast and could not be
replaced with its apparent near-synonym-'a piece of bread'.
Questionformation
Randall (2 yearc,9 months) asked the following questions in various situations over the course of a day.
Are dogs can wiggle their tails?
Are those are my boots?
Are this is hot?
Randall had concluded that the trick of asking questions was to put 'are' ar
the beginning of the sentence. His questions are good examples of Stage 3 in
question development.
Ordcr ofeuents
Randall (3 years, 5 months) was looking for
a towel.
You took all the towels away because I cant dry my hands.
He meant 'I cant dry my hands because you took all the towels awat' , but he
made a mistake about which clause comes first. Children at this stage of
language development tend to mention events in the order of their
occurrence. In this case, the towels disappeared before Randall attempted to
dry his hands, so thatt what he said first. He did not yet understand how a
word like'before'or'because' changes the order of cause and effect.
These examples of children's speech provide us with a window on rhe process
of language learning. Imitation and practice alone cannot explain some of the
forms created by the children. They are not merely repetitions of sentences
that they have heard from adults. Rather, children appear to pick out parrerns
and gene nltze them to new contexts. They create new forms or new uses of
words. Their new sentences are usually comprehensible and often correcr.
Behaviourism seems to offer a reasonable way of understanding how
children learn some of the regular and routine aspects of language, esp"ecially
at the earliest stages. However, children who do little overr imitation acquire
(
Language learning in early childhood
language as fully and rapidly as those who imitate a lot. And although behaviourism goes some way to explaining the sorrs of ovsncENERALrzATroN
that children make, classical behaviourism is nor a satisfactory explanarion
for the acquisition of the more complex grammar that children acquire.
These limitations led researchers to look for different explanarion; for
language acquisition.
The innatist perspectiae: It's
all in your mind
Noam Chomsky is one of the most influential figures in linguistics, and his
ideas about how language is acquired and how it is stor.d i" the mind
sparked a revolution in many aspecrs of linguistics and psychology,
including the study of language acquisition. A central part of his thinking is
that all hr*"r l"rgurg::
universal@lofthem."li Fld"-.rl
I"hir
r"t.
"nd
th"t-t
.
t
eed the behaviourist explanation for
language acquisition. He argued that children are b_iolggrglly programmed
t"J l.".g"ig.
,
other btologtcal functions develop. For example, every child willJe'arn to
rit and reasonable freedom ofmovement
are provided. The child does not have to be taught. Most children learn to
ri-alk at about the same age, and walking is essentially the same in all normal
human beings. For Chomsky, language acquisition is very similar. The
f* *",
.!"iron.-.qi-"k
tt
rather, the child's biological
endowment, will do the rest.
Chomsky argued that the behaviourist theory failed ro account for 'the
logical problem of language acquisition'-the fact that children come to
know more about the structure of their language than they could reasonably
be expected to learn on the basis of the samplis of language they hear. Th!
language children are €xposed to includes fals. rt"rts, incoirpl.t. r.rrt.nces,
and slips of the tongue, and yet they learn to distinguish t.*..., gramrnatical and ungrammatical sentences. He concluded ihat childrent irindt
are not blank slates to be filled by imitating language t[.y h."t in tf*
envlr9gmerr!,. lnstead, he hypothes
ftlren-ere boin with a speci
innate abiliw to di
henselves the:ilfr?ElfriE-iul.r of 1""
sijte1 on the basis of the :ampleg of a.natural language they ari "
15 lnnate e
ent was seen as a sort of template, containine the
principles that a
TVERSAL
1_l.-L
:11'pOtneses aDou
iloren are preequipped with UG, then what they have to learn is the ways in whiclrthe
nanguage they are acquiring makes use of these principles.
I5
t5
Language learning in early childhood
Consider the following sentences, taken from a book by Lydia \7hite
(1989). These English sentences contain the refexive pronoun 'himself'.
Both the pronoun and the noun it refers to (the antecedent) are printed in
italics. An asterisk at the beginning of a sentence indicates that the sentence
is ungrammatical.
a John
b
*
saw
himself
Himselfsaw John.
In (a) and (b), it looks as if the reflexive pronoun must follow the noun it
refers to. But (c) disproves this:
c Looking
after himselfbores John.
Ifwe consider sentences such
as:
d John said that Fred liked himself
e
*Johnsaid that Fred liked himself,
g
John told Billto wash himself
*John told Bill to wash himself
f
we might conclude that the noun closest to the reflexive pronoun is the
antecedent. However, (h) shows that this rule won't work either:
h Johnpromised Bill to wash himself
And it's even more complicated than that. Usually the refexive must be in
the same clause as the antecedent as in (a) and (d), but not always, as in (h).
Furthermore, the refexive can be in the subject position in (i) but not i" (j).
i
j
Johnbelieves himselfto be intelligent (non-finite clause).
*Johnbelieves that himself isintelligent (finite
clause).
In some cases, more than one antecedent is possible, as in (k) where the
refexive could refer to eitherJohn or Bill:
k
John showed Bill
a
picture of himself.
\7hen we look at this kind of complexity, it seems it would be very hard to
learn. And yet, most school age children would be able to correcdy interpret
l
(
(
F
ir
e
c
(r
d
Il
n
2.
TI
l)
d
the grammatical sentences and recognize the ungrammaticality of the
others. Researchers who study language acquisition from the innatist
et
perspective argue that such complex grammar could never be learned purely
on the basis of imitating and practising sentences available in the input.
cl
They hypothesize that since all children acquire the language of their
environment, they must have some innate mechanism or knowledge that
allows them to discover such complex syntax in spite of limitations of the
input. They hypothesize furthermore that the innate mechanism is used
rl
exclusively for language acquisition.
N
b
st
fr
et
s
w
Language learning in early childhood
The innatist perspective emphasizes the fact that all children successfully
acquire their native language (or languages if they live in a multilingual
community). Children who are profoundly deaf will learn sign language if
they are exposed to it in infancy, and their progress in the acquisition of that
language system is similar to hearing childrent acquisition of spoken
Ianguage. Even children with very limited cognitive abiliry develop quite
complex language systems if they are brought up in environments in which
people interact with them. Children master the basic syntax and morphoiogy of the language spoken to them in a variery of conditions-some
x'hich would be expected to enhance language development (for example,
caring, attentive parents who focus on the child s language), and some which
might be expected to inhibit it (for example, abusive or re.jecting parents).
Children achieve different levels ofvocabulary, creativiry social gtace, and so
on, but virtually all achieve mastery of the structure of the language or
languages spoken to them. This is seen as support for the hypothesis that
language is somehow separate from other aspects of cognitive developmenr
and may depend on a specific module of the brain.
The Critical Period Hypothesis
at specific times
er%rarnmed to acquire certain kinds ofknowledge an
qirh.r=dffi*
s',
tt
it
is
ir
either
difficult
or
imoossible to
ffidp*ie^d.rj",
acqurre
abilities. With ;%"rd-r"
EG?bil+jqs.
language, the
CPH sugg.rt, th"t
;ffini,,.'"......,.,l,no.,,oJinInf,,,..,,,.'.l",,lJ.hilrlhoorl
given access to language in infancy and early childhood
because of deafness or extreme isolation) will never acquire language if these
deprivations go on for too long.
difficult to find evidence for or against the CPH, since nearly all childien are exposed to language at an early age. However, history has documented
nt is
a few 'natural experiments' where children have been deprived of contact
'".,-ith language. Two of the most famous cases are those ofVictor and Genie.
In 1799, a boywho became known as Victor was found wandering naked in
woods in France. \7hen he was captured, he was about twelve years old
and completely wild, apparently having had no contact with humans. Jean\larc-Gaspard Itard, a young doctor accustomed to working with deaf
;hildren, devoted five years to socializing Victor and trying to teach him
.anguage. Although he succeeded to some extent in developing Victort
sociabiliry memory, and judgement, there was little progress in his language
abiliry. Victor responded only to sounds that had had meaning for him in the
irest, such as the cracking of a nut, animal sounds, or the sound of rain. He
er-entually spoke only rwo words, his favourite food 'lait' (milk) and his
so\rernesst frequent exclamation 'O Dieu!' (Oh, God!). He said 'lait' only
o,n-hen he saw a glass of milk. He never used the word to ask for it.
rFre
17
18
Laruguage learning in early childhood
Nearly rwo hundred years later Genie, a thirteen-year-old girl who had been
isolated, neglected, and abused, was discovered in California (Rymer 1993).
Because of the irrational demands of a disturbed father and the submission
and fear of an abused mother, Genie had spent more than eleven years tied to
a chair or a crib in a small, darkened room. Her father had forbidden his wife
and son to speak to Genie and had himself only growled and barked at her.
She was beaten when she made any kind of noise, and she had long since
resorted to complete silence. Genie was undeveloped physically, emotionally, and intellectually. She had no language.
After she was discovered, Genie was cared for and educated with the
participation of many teachers and therapists, including Susan Curtiss
(1977). After a brief period in a rehabilitation centre, she lived in a foster
home and attended special schools. Genie made remarkable progress in
becoming socialized and cognitively aware. She developed deep personal
relationships and strong individual tastes and traits. Nevertheless, after five
years of exposure to language, Genie's language was not like that of a rypical
five-year old. There was a lTger than:ft:rylg€ between comprehg_ffJo1
anj!_producdqn. She used [r"--"tica1-6rrru irrconsisfentry and overused
Foimulaic and routine speech.
Although Victor and Genie appear to provide evidence in support of the
CPH, it is difficult to argue that the hypothesis is confirmed on the basis of
evidence from such unusual cases. \7e cannot know with certainty what other
factors besides biological maturity might have contributed to their inabiliry to
learn language. It is not possible to determine whether either of them suffered
from brain damage, developmental delays, or a specific language impairment,
even before they were separated from normal human interaction. However,
there are some children who come from ordinary homes, yet do not have
access to language at the usual time . This is the case for some profoundly deaf
children who have hearing parents. Hearing parents may not reahze that their
child cannor hear because the child uses other senses to interact in fi
apparently normal way. Thus, the early childhood period may be normal and
loving but devoid of language that the children can access. These children's
later experience in learning sign language has been the subject of some
important research related to the critical period.
Elissa Newport (1990) and her colleagues studied deaf users of eMEnrceN
srcN LANGUAGT (asr). Only 5-10 per cent of the profoundly deaf are born
to deaf parents, and only these children are likely to be exposed to ASL from
birth. The remainder of the profoundly deaf population begin learningASL
at differenr ages, often when they start attending a residential school where
sign language is used for day-to-d^y communication.
Like oral and written languages, ASL makes use of grammatical markers to
indicate such things as time (for example, past tense) and number. These
I
:
a
t
Language learning in early childhood
markers are expressed through specific hand or body movements. The
researchers studied the abiliry to produce and comprehend grammatical
markers in Native signers (who were exposed to ASL from birth), Early
learners (who began using ASL between four and six years of age), and Late
iearners (who began learningASL after age twelve).
They found no difference berween the groups in some aspects of their use of
ASL. However, on tests focusing on grammatical markers, the Native group
used the forms more consistently than the Early group who, in turn, used
them more consistently than the Late group. The researchers concluded that
their study supports the hypothesis that there is a critical period for first
language acquisition, whether that language is oral or gestural.
We
will return to a discussion of the CPH in Chapter
age issue
3 when we look at the
in second language acquisition.
The innatist perspective is thus partly based on evidence for
a
critical period.
It is also seen as an explanation for 'the logical problem of language acquisition', that is, the question of how adult speakers come to know the complex
structure of their first language on the basis of language that they actually
hear.
Interacti o n is t/ deu e lop m enta I p ersp e ctia es :
Learningfrom inside and out
Cognitive and developmental psychologists argue that the innatists place too
much emphasis on the 'final state' (the coupETENCE of adult NATTvE
snnernns) and not enough on the developmental aspects of language
acquisition. In their view, language acquisition is but one example of the
human child s remarkable abiliry to learn from experience, and they see no
need to assume that there are specific brain structures devoted to language
acquisition. They hypothesize that what children need to know is essentially
available in the language they are exposed to as they hear it used in thousands
of hours of interactions with the people and objects around them.
Developmental psychologists and psycholinguists have focused on the
interplay between the innate learning abiliry of children and the environment in which they develop. These researchers attribute considerably more
importance to the environment than the innatists do even though they also
recognize a powerful learning mechanism in the human brain. They see
language acquisition as similar to and influenced by the acquisition of other
kinds of skill and knowledge, rather than as something that is different from
and largely independent of the child's experience and cognitive development. Indeed, researchers such as Dan Slobin (I973) have long emphasized
the close relationship between chiidren's cognitive development and their
acquisition of language.
r9
20
Language learning in early childhood
Piaget and Vygotslqy
One of the earliest proponents of the view that childrent language is built on
their cognitive development was the Swiss psychologist/epistemologist, Jean
Piaget (19511L946). In the early decades of the twentieth century, Piaget
observed infants and children in their play and in their interaction with
objects and people. He was able to t
g[gpment of their cognitive
understanding of such
ti!rc
hidden from si
uantril
chanses in their
that
long line are not more numerous than ten pennies in a tightly sqqgqzqd b.e),
Fring out which properties of a set of rod.-"i-e,
weight, material, etc.-cause some rods to sink and others to
W.,,,
#
a
determine how they use language. For example, the use of certain terms suqb
..hildr.t'r'r .rtt
derstanding is built on the
@
interaction berween the child and the things that can be observed or
manipulated.
For Piaget, language was one of a number of symbol systems
7
that are developed in childhood. Lan
t
that children h
ith the
\
envrronment.
c,
Another influential student of child development was the psychologist
Lev Vygotsky (1978). He observed interactions among children and also
berween children and adults in schools in the Soviet Union in the 1920s and
1 930s.
ial interactiorl-He
that in a suppoftivElnteraaive environmen
abletoadvance@o
and performance.
U€g6kf
uld do more
_rglerleg !-q lu!_lqggprrgll
than they would be capqblg_of independently as the zoNE oF pRoxrMer
DEVELopMENT (zro). He observed tfre i
s that
g-41
;hffidults
and
*it
conversations the oriei:rs of boi
iFGFFrom Piaget's.
w lan
used to ex
ledse a
worl
I .----:--:
emerged rn soc
tho
lnteractl0n.
and tllgy*t. Vygotskyt view
symbol system that
rouqh interaction with the phvsical
as a
, and
Cross-cultural research
Since the I970s, researchers have studied childrent language learning
environments in a great many different cultural communities. The research
has focused not only on the development of language itself, but also on the
ways in which the environment provides what children need for language
acquisition. Starting in the mid-1980s, Dan Slobin has edited a series of
\
ld
-.ll
-tl
*.
::"T,
Language learning in early childhood
volumes devoted to international research on language acquisition, providing examples and analyses of child language and the language learning
environment from communities around the world. One of the most
remarkable resources for child language researchers is the Child Language
Data Exchange System (cHrrons), where researchers have contributed
rnillions of words of child language data in dozens of languages in recorded
and transcribed forms (Mac\Thinney 19 9 5 ; http //childes. psy. cmu. edu/) .
:
One result of the crosscultural research is the description of the differences in
childrearing patterns. Catherine Snow (1995) and others have studied the
apparent effects on language acquisition of the ways in which adults talk to
.ind interact with young children. In middle-class North American homes,
:esearchers observed that adults often modify the way they speak when
ralking to little chiidren. This cHILD-DIRECTED sIEECH may be character-zed by a slower rate of delivery, higher pitch, more varied intonation,
shorter, simpler sentence patterns, stress on key words, frequent r€petition,
ind paraphrase. Furthermore, topics of conversation emphasize the child s
:rnmediate environment, the 'here and now', or experiences that the adult
inows the child has had. Adults often repeat the content of a child's utterjrlce, but they expand or RECAST it into a grammatically correct senrence.
For example, when Peter says, 'Dump truck! Dump truck! Fall! Fall!', Lois
:esponds, 'Yes, the dump truck fell d,own.'
Hu 9e
not \
the
Cotts;dere/
effects of
\
Sr"zn \
at,w,nc<.fi'cal)
2*_Y,
W?, 71--+--.'---
i/
in a 'ianguage socialization' framework have studied
acquisition
in
children from a varieqy of cultural groups. They have
-anguage
:.rund that the kind of child-directed speech observed in middle-class
-{merican homes is by no means universal. In some societies, adults do not
.:rgage in conversation or verbal play with very young children.
iesearchers working
21
'r',
Language learning in early childhood
For example, Bambi Schieffelin (1990) found that Kaluli mothers in Papua
New Guinea did not consider their children to be appropriate conversational partners. Martha Crago (1992) observed that in traditional Inuit
society, children are expected to watch and listen to adults. They are not
expected or encouraged to participate in conversations with adults until they
are older and have more developed language skills. Other researchers have
observed that in some societies, young children interact primarily with older
siblings who serve as their caregivers. Even within the United States, Shirley
Brice Heath (1983) and others have documented substantial differences in
the ways in which parents in different socioeconomic and ethnic groups
interact with their children. Thus, the patterns of parent-child interaction
and child-directed speech that were first observed in middle-class North
American families are far from universal. Nevertheless, in every society,
children are in situations in which they hear language that is meaningful to
them in their environment. And they achieve full competence in the
community language. Thus, it is difficult to judge the long-term effect of the
modifications that some adults make in speech addressed to children.
/l/
tr\
,l
The importance of interaction
The role of interaction between a language-learning child and an
INTERLocuroR who responds in some way to the child is illuminated by
interaction is missing. Jacqueline Sachs and her colleagues
(198 1) studied the language development of a child they called Jim. He was
a hearing child of deaf parents, and his only contact with oral language was
cases where such
through television, which he watched frequently. The familywas unusual in
that the parents did not use sign language with Jim. Thus, although in other
respects he was well cared fot, Ji- did not begin his linguistic development
in a normal environment in which a parent communicated with him in
either oral or sign language. A language assessment at three years and nine
months indicated that he was well below age level in all aspects of language.
Although he attempted to express ideas appropriate to his age, he used
unusual, ungrammatical word order.
\fhen Jim
began conversational sessions
with an adult, his expressive
abilities began to improve. By the age of four years and two months most of
the unusual speech patterns had disappeared, replaced by structures more
rypical of his age. Jim's younger brother Glenn did not display the same type
of language delay. Glenns linguistic environment was different in that he
had his older brother as a conversational partner.
Jim showed very rapid acquisition of the structures of English once he began
to interact with an adult on a one-to-one basis. The fact that he had failed to
acquire language normally prior to this experience suggests that impersonal
sources of language such as television or radio alone are not sufficient. One-
Language learning in early
childhood
ro-one interaction gives the child access to language that is adjusted to his or
her level of comprehension. \fhen a child does not understand, the adult
may repeat or paraphrase. The response of the adult may also allow children
ro find out when their own utterances are understood. Television, fot
obvious reasons, does not provide such interaction. Even in childrent
programmes, where simpler language is used and topics are relevant to
vounger viewers, no immediate adjustment is made for the needs of an
individual child. Once children have acquired some language, however,
television can be a source of language and cultural information.
,,
Connectionism
-{nother recent view of language acquisition comes from coNNECTToNISM.
Connectionists differ sharply from the Chomskyan innatists beiause-tE{y
hvpothesize thatlanguage acquisition does not require a separate- lmodule of
tne mlnc. Dut can
arned ln terms ot learnlng ln general.qurtnermor€,
connectionists argue that what children
t0Toma6f the research has
em ln tne lan
tney are ex
e samples are provided as
input to a fairly simple program. The goal is to show that the computer
program can 'learn' certain things if it is exposed to them enough. The
program can even generuIize beyond what it has actually been exposed to and
make the same kinds of creative 'mistakes' that children make, such as
putting a regular -edending on an irregular verb, for example, eated.
Researchers such as Jeffrey Elman
and his colleagues (1996) explain
language acquisition in terms of how children acquire links or'connections'
ber*een *oidr and phrases and the situations in which they occur. They
claim that when children hear a word or phrase in the context of a specific
otpcu
tnewordorPnraSeand'whatitrepresentS.ln*'J*
;
ngs to mind the word or
phraE,\
e-
retrieval6fTHe assoii"t.J*ria ot pttt":g no$ *.-o@a
may nrst recognlze tne woro cat only rn relerence to tne tamlly pet ano only
u'hen the cat is miaowing beside the kitchen door. As the word is heard in
more contexts-picture books, furry toys, someone else's g21-*rs child
for all these cats. However, at a
later point, the word may be generalized to other furry creatures as well,
indicating that connections have been made to characteristics of the cat
and not to an entiry that adults know as 'cat'. Then there is another
learning process involved in 'pruning' the connections so that 'cat' applies
only to felines-at least until more metaphorical meanings are learned later
recognizes and uses the word as the label
in life.
23
24
Language learning in early childhood
In a connectionist model, language acquisition is not just a process of associating words with elements of external realiry. It is also a process of associating
words and phrases with the other words and phrases that occur with them, or
words with grammatical morphemes that occur with them. For example,
children learning languages in which nouns have grammatical gender learn to
A
with nouns. Similarly, they
1a
associate the appropriate article and adjective forms
gI
d(
AI
in
learn to associate pronouns with the verb forms that mark person and number.
la
They learn which temporal adverbs go with which verb tenses. According to
connectionist theory, all this is possible because of the child's general abiliry to
na
develop associations between things that occur together.
ln
Of particular importance to the connectionist hypothesis is the fact that
children are exposed to many thousands of opportunities to learn words and
phrases. Learning takes place gradually, as the number of links between
language and meaning are built up. They argue that acquisition of language,
while remarkable, is not the only remarkable feat accomplished by the child.
They compare
it to other cognitive and perceptual
learning, including
learning to 'see'.
U
CL
pr
ia
rh
ri
In
-al
R.t
:h
S
Language disorders and delays
Although most children progress through the stages of language development without significant difficulry or delay, there are some children for
whom this is not the case. A discussion of the various types of disabilitiesincluding deafness, articulatory problems, dyslexia, erc.-that sometimes
affect language development is outside the scope of this book. It is essential
that parents and teachers be encouraged to seek professional advice if they
feel that a child is not developing language normally, keeping in mind that
the range for'normal' is wide indeed.
While most children produce recognizable first words by twelve monrhs,
some may not speak before the age of three years. In very young children,
one way to determine whether delayed language reflects a problem or simply
an individual difference within the normal range is to determine whether the
child responds to language and appears to understand even if he or she is not
speaking. For older children, delays in learning to read that seem out of
keeping with a child's overall intellectual functioning may suggest that there
is a specific problem in that domain. Some children seem to begin reading
almost by magic, discovering the mysteries of print with little direct
instruction. For most children, instruction that includes some systematic
attention to sound-letter correspondences allos's them to unlock the
treasure chest of reading. Both groups fall with a normal range. For some
children, however, reading presents such great challenges that they need
expert help beyond what is available in a typical classroom.
Langaage learning in early childhood
fu Jim Cummins (1984, 2000) and others have pointed out, one particular
group of children who have often been misdiagnosed as having language
delays or disorders are children who arrive at their first day of school without
an age-appropriate knowledge of the language of the school. This includes
immigrant children who speak another language at home, minoriry
language children whose home language is different from the school
language, and children who speak a different variety of the school language.
Unfortunately, it often happens that these childrent knowledge ofa different
language or language variety is interpreted as a lack ofknowledge oflanguage
in general. fu a result, they are sometimes placed in remedial or special
education classes. It is often the case that the school is not equipped to
provide an adequate assessment of childrent ability to use their home
language. Schools may not have programmes for second language learners
that allow them to continue to use their home language. The development of
bilingual or second language learning children is of enormous importance.
Indeed, the majoriry of the world's children are exposed to more than one
language, either
in
early childhood or from the time they enter school.
Researchers have recently made
important progress in providing guidelines
that can help educators distinguish between disabiliry and diversity
(Seymour and Pearson 2004).
Childhood bilingualism
E"tly childhood bilingualism is a realiry for millions of children throughout
the world. Some children learn multiple languages from earliest childhood;
others acquire additional languages when they go to school. The acquisition
and maintenance of more than one language can open doors to many
personal, social, and economic opportunities.
Children who learn more than one language from earliest childhood are
referred to as 'simultaneous bilinguals', whereas those who learn another
Ianguage later may be called 'seqtrential bilinguals'. There is a considerable
b"dy of research on children's abiliry to learn more than one language in
their earliest years. 'We sometimes hear people express the opinion that it
is too difficult for children to cope with two languages. They fear that
the children will be confused or will not learn either language well.
However, there is little suppoft for the myth that learning more than one
language in early childhood is a problem for children (Genesee, Crago, and
Paradis 2004). Although some studies show minor early delays for simululneous bilinguals, there is no evidence that learning two languages sub-
stantially slows down their linguistic development
or
interferes with
cognitive and academic development. Indeed many simultaneous bilinguals
achieve high levels of proficiency in both languages. Ellen Bialystok (1991,
25
26
Language learning in early childhood
2001) and other developmental psychologists have found convincing
evidence that bilingualism can have positive effects on abilities that are
related to academic success, such as metalinguistic awareness. Limitations
that may be observed in the language of bilingual individuals are more likely
to be related to the circumstances in which each language is learned than to
any limitation in the human capacity to learn more than one language. For
example, ifone language is heard much more often than the other or is more
highlyvalued in the communiry that language may eventually be used better
than, or in preference ro, the other.
There may be reason to be concerned, however, about situations where
children are cut off from their family language when they are very young.
Lily\7ong-Fillmore (1991) observed that when children are 'submerged' in
a different language for long periods in pre-school or day care, their
development of the family language may be slowed down or stalled before
they have developed an age-appropriate masrery of the new language.
Eventually they may srop speaking the family language altogether.
\Tallace Lambert (1987) called this loss of one language on the way ro learning another sunTRACTIVE BILINGUALTsM. It can have negative consequences for childrent self-esteem, and their relationships with family
members are also likely to be affected by such early loss of the family
language. In these cases, children seem to continue to be caught berween rwo
languages: they have nor yer mastered the one language, and they haye not
continued to develop the other. During the transition period, they may fall
behind in their academic learning. UnfortunarcIy, the 'solution educarors
somedmes propose to parents is that they should stop speaking the family
language at home and concentrate instead on speakirtg th. school language
with their children. The evidence suggests that a better solution is to strive
for enorrrvE BTLTNGU the maintenance of the home language
while the second language is being learned. This is especially true if t[.
parents are also learners of the second language. If parenrs continue ro use
the_language that they know best, they are able ro express their knowledge
and ideas in ways that are richer and more elaborate than they can
-"r"g. i.t
a language they do not know as well. Using their own language in family
settings is also away for parents to maintain their own self-esreem, especially
as they may be struggling with the new language outside the home, ar
work, or in the community. Maintaining the family language also creares
opportunities for the children to continue both cognitive and affective
development in a language they understand easily while they are still
learning the second language. As Virginia Collier (1939) and others have
shown, the process ofdeveloping a second language takes years. But teachers,
parents, and students need to know that the benefits ofadditive bilingualism
will reward patience and effort.
I^anguzge learning in early childhood
Summary
In this chapter we have focused on some of the research on
children's
language that has infuenced second language acquisition research. \We have
described three broad theoretical perspectives for explaining first language
acquisition. In Chapter 2, we will look at the theoretical perspectives that
have been proposed to explain second language acquisition.
Sources and suggestions for further readirg
Baker, C. 2001. Foundations of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 3rd
edn. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Berko Gleason, J. (.d.). 2005. The Deuelopment of Language
[-ondon: Allyn and Bacon and Longman Publishers.
6th
edn.
Cummins, J. 2000 . Language, Power and Pedagogy: Bilingual Children in the
Crossf re. Clevrdon : Multilingual Matters.
Elman, J. L., E. A. Bares, M. H. Johnson, A. Karmiloflsmirh, D. parisi,
and K. Plunkett. 1996. Rethinking fnnateness: A Connectionist Perspectiue on
Dnelopment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Genesee, F. (ed.). 1995. Educating Second Language Children: The Whole
Ch i A, t h e Wh o le Curri cu lum,
Universiry Press.
th e Wh o le C o mmuiity.Cambridge :
Cambridge
Ginsburg, H. and S. Opper. 1969. Piaget'|s Theory of Intellectual Deueloprnent: An Introductioz. Englewood cliffs, NJ: prentice-Hall.
oller, D. K. and R. E. Eilers (eds.). 2002. Language and Literacy
Danlopment in Bilingual children. clevedon: Multiling,rJ M",r.rr.
Pinker, S.1994. The Language Instincr. Newyork \Tilliam Morrow.
Piper, T. 1993. Language and Learning: The Home and SchoolYears 2nd edn.
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice-Hall.
Schieffelin, B. and E. Ochs (eds.). 1986. Language Socialization Across
&hares. Cam bridge : Camb ridge University Press.
lhlls, G. 1986.
Children Learning Language and Using Language to Learn.
hnsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
ktsch, J. V I 985. Vygotsky and the Social Formation
fu{A Harvard University
Press.
ofMind. Cambridge,
27
EXPLAINING SECOND
LANGUAGE LEARNING
Like the explanations for first language acquisition, some second language
acquisition theories give primary importance to learners' innate capacity for
language acquisition. Others emphasize the role of the environment,
especially opportunities to interact with speakers who adapt their language
and interaction patterns to meet learners' needs. Still others focus on
learners' engagement with the broader social conrexr.
Contexts for language learning
A second language learner is different from a.very young child acquiring a
first language. This is true in terms of both the learnert characteristics and
the environments in which first and second language acquisition typically
occur. Think about how the characteristics and learning conditions of the
following learners may differ: (1) a young child learning a first language; (2)
a child learning a second language in day care or on the playgrou"d; (3)
adolescents taking a foreign language class in their own count y; (4) an adult
immigrant with limited or disrupted education working in a second
language environment and having no opportunity to go to language classes.
Now askyourself the following questions about these different learners, and
complete the chart in Thble 2.1.
I
Do they already know at least one language?
2 Are they cognitively mature? Are they able to engage in problem solving,
deduction, and complex memory
tasks?
3 How well developed is their
metalinguistic awareness? Can they define a
word, say what sounds make up that word, or state a rule such as 'add an
-sto form the plural'?
4 How
extensive is their general knowledge of the world? Does this knowledge enable them to make good guesses about what a second language
interlocutor is probably saying?
30
Exp laining second language
learning
5 Are they likely to be anxious about making
mistakes and concerned
about sounding'silly when speaking the language?
6
Does the learning environment allow them to be silent in the early stages
of learning, or are they expected to speak from the beginning?
Do they have plenry of time available for language learning, plenry of
contact with proficient speakers of the language?
Do they frequently receive coRRECTTvE FEEDBACK when they make
errors in grammar or pronunciation, or do listeners usually overlook
these errors andpay attention to the meaning?
9 Do they receive corrective feedback when their meaning is not
clear,
when they use the wrong word, or when they say somerhing inappropriate or impolite?
10 Is uoorrrBo INpur
available?
That is, do interlocutors adapt their
speech so that learners can understand (..g., in terms ofspeed ofdelivery,
complexity of grammatical structure, or vocabulary?)
Using the chart inTable 2.1, giveyour opinion about the presence or absence
of learner characteristics and learning conditions for four types of learners.
Use the following notation:
+ = usualll
- = usuzlly absent
? = sofil€times present, sometimes absent, or you're not sure
Then, compare your views with the discussion of learner characteristics and
learning conditions below.
L e arn er c lt aracteris ti cs
By definition, all second language learners, regardless of age, have already
acquired at least one language. This prior knowledge may be an advantage in
the sense that they have an idea of how languages work. On the other hand,
knowledge of other languages can lead learners to make incorrect guesses
about how the second language works, and this may result in errors that first
language learners would not make.
Very young language learners begin the task of first language acquisition
without the cognitive maturiry or metalinguistic awareness that older
second language learners have. Although young second language learners
have begun to develop these characteristics, they will still have far to go in
these areas, as well as in the area of world knowledge, before they reach the
levels already attained by adults and adolescenrs.
On the one hand, cognitive maturity and metalinguistic awareness allow
older learners to solve problems and engage in discussions about language.
Exp
First
language
laining
se cond
language learnirug
Second language
Youngchild Youngchild
Adolescent
Adult
(othome) (ployground) (clossroom) (on the job)
Learner characteristics
Another language
Cognitive maturity
Metalinguistic awareness
World knowledge
Anxiety about speaking
Learning conditions
Freedom to be silent
Ample time
Corrective feedback
(grammar and pronunciation)
Corrective feedback
(meaning, word choice,
politeness)
Modified input
Photocopiable
Thble
2.1
@
Oxford University
Press
Contextsfor language learning
On the other hand, some researchers have suggested that the use of these
cognitive skills-so valuable for many kinds of tasks-can actually interfere
with language acquisition. Their hypothesis is that successful language
acquisition draws on different mental abilities, abilities that are specific to
language learning. This view is related to the idea that there is a critical
period for language acquisition. It has been suggested that older learners
draw on their problem solving and metalinguistic abilities precisely because
they can no longer access the innate language acquisition abiliry they had as
young children.
differences, there are also attitudinal and
cultural differences between children and adults. Most child learners are
willing to rry ro use the language-even when their proficiency is quite limited.
M*y adults and adolescents find it stressful when they are unable to express
themselves clearly and correctly. Nevertheless, even very young (pre-school)
children differ in their willingness to speak a language they do not know well.
Some children happily chatter away in their new language; others prefer to
listen and participate silently in social interaction with their peers.
In addition ro possible cognitive
3T
32
Exp
laining
second language
learning
Learning conditions
Younger learners, in an informal second language-learning environmenr, are
usually allowed to be silent until they are ready to speak. fh.y may also have
opportunities to practise their second language 'voice' in songs and games
that allow them to blend their voices with thor. of other childrerr. bld.t
learners are often forced to speak-to meet the requirements of a classroom
or to carty out everyday tasks such as shopping, medical visits, or job
interviews.
Young children in informal settings are usually exposed to the second
language for many hours every day. Older learners, ispecially students in
language classrooms, are more likely to receive only limited exposure to the
second language. Classroom learners not only spend less time in contact
the language, they also tend to be .*por.d to a far smaller range of
Yjrh
discourse rypes. For example, classroom learners are often taught l"ng".r"g.
that is somewhat formal in comparison to the language as it is irs.d ir,"*o"rt
social settings. In many foreign language .larrei telcherc switch to their
students' first langyag. for discipline or classroom managemenr, thus
depriving learners of opportunities to experience uses of the l"ig.r"g. in real
communication.
ln
ur
ha
thr
So
\MC
ab
gu
CI
inj
un
wc
wr
(
As we saw in Chapter 1, parents tend to respond to their children's language
in terms of its meaning rather than in terms of its grammarical a..ir"Jy.
Similarly, in second language learning outside of clasirooms, errors that do
not interfere with meaning are usually overlooked. Most people would feel
theywere being impolite if they interrupted and corrected ro-.on. who was
trying to have a conversation with them. Nevertheless, interlocutors may
react to an error if they cannot understand what the speaker is trying ro say.
Thus, errors of grammar and pronunciarion
-"y roi b. ,.m"rkedln, but
the wrong word choice may receive comment from apuzzled,interlocutor. In
a situation where a second language speaker
ro use inappropriate
"pp."irknowing
language, interlocutors may feel uncomfortable,^not
*irlth.r the
speaker intends to be rude or simply does not know the poli"te way ro say
what is intended. In this case too, especially bemeen adults, it is unlikely that
the second language speaker would be told that something had gone wrong.
The only place where feedback on error is rypically pt.s.nl with higi
frequency is the language classroom. Even there, it is-not always prorridld
A1
l*i
consistently.
of,
One condition that appears to be common ro learners of all ages-though
perhaps nor in equal qualiry or quanrity-is exposure to modifi.d "o,
Pro
adapted input. This adjusted speech rtyl., called .ftita-aitected speech in
first language acquisition, has sometimes been called FoRETcNER TALK or
TEACHER TALK in certain contexts of second language acquisition. Some
people who interact regularly with language learners seem to have an
all
r
and
gua
olol
r€ai
Exp
hining
second language
learning
intuitive sense of what adjustments they need to make to help learners
understand. Of course, some people are much better at this than others. \7e
have allwitnessed those painful conversations inwhich people seem to think
that they can make learners understand better if they simply talk louder!
Some Canadian friends told us of an experience they had in China. They
were visiting some historic temples and wanted to get more information
about them than they could glean from their guidebook. They asked their
guide some questions about the monuments. Unfortunately, their limited
Chinese and his non-existent English made it difficult for them to exchange
information. The guide kept speaking louder and louder, but our friends
understood very little. Finally, in frustration, the guide concluded that it
would help if they could see the information-so he took a stick and began
writing in the sand-in Chinese characters!
Exat Sc ntc ,
wh€ra tcilcLt
Furanr anwa;S
,N FoA HArr or.r
A
second language acquisition needs to account for
with a variety of characteristics in a variety
of contexts. The emphasis in this chapter is on theories that have been
proposed to explain the aspects of language acquisition that are common to
all second language learners and contexts.'We will look at how behaviourist
and innatist explanations have been extended to account for second language acquisition. \We will also look at some theories from cognitive psychology that have increasingly informed second language research in recent
years. These cocNrrrvrsr theories emphasize the way the mind perceives,
general theory
of
language acquisition by learners
33
34
Explainingsecond language learning
retains, organizes, and retrieves information. Finally, we will look at
SocIocULTURAL THEORY, a perspective that places second language
acquisition in a larger social conrexr.
Behaviourism
fu we saw in Chapter 1, behaviourist theory explained learning in terms of
imitation, pracrice, reinforcement (or feedback on success), and habit
formation. Much of the early research within behaviourist theory was done
with laboratory animals, but the learning process was hypothesized to be the
same for humans.
Second language applications:
Mimicry and
memorization
Behaviourism had a powerful infuence on second and foreign language
teaching, especially in North America, berween the 1940s
the t-gl1t.
"nJ
Nelson Brooks (1960) and Robert Lado (1964) were rwo proponenrs
of this
perspective whose influence was felt directly in the development of
AUDIoLINGUAL teaching materials and in teacher training. Classroom
activities emphasized mimicry and memorizarion, and students learned
dialogues and sentence patterns by heart. Because language development
was viewed as the formation of habits, it was assumed that a person learning
a second language would start offwith the habits formed in the first languagl
and that these habits would interfere with the new ones needed fo-r tf,.
second language. Thus, behaviourism was often linked to the coNTRASTTvE
ANALYSIS HYPorHrsIs (ceu), which was developed by structural linguists
in Europe and North America. According to the CAH, where th.- firrt
language and the target language are similat, l."rr,.rs should acquire TARGET
LANGUAGE sffuctures with ease; where there are differences, learners should
have difficulty. However, researchers have found that learners do not make
all the errors predicted by the CAH. Instead, many of their actual errors are
not predictable on the basis of their first language. Adult second language
learners produce sentences that sound more like aihild's. Also, many oirh."i,
sentences would be ungrammatical if translated into their first language.
\(hat is more, some characteristics of the simple strucrures they use are very
similar across learners from avariety of backgrounds, erren if their respecrive
first languages are different from each other and differenr from the t"rget
language.
In Chapter 4,wewill see ample evidence that second language learners draw
on what they already know. However, we will also see thaithey are sometimes reluctant to transfer certain first language parrerns, even when the
'
.,i
i
(
I
.
F*?kiningsecond language learning
translation equivalent would be correct. AIso, first language infuence may
become more apparent as more is learned about the second language,
leading learners to see similarities that ,h.y had not perceived at an earlier
stage. All this suggests that the influence of the learnert first language may
not simply be a matter of the TRANSFEn. of habits, but a more subtle and
complex process of identifying points of similarity, weighing the evidence in
support
of
some particular feature, and even reflecting (though not
necessarily consciousl, about whether a certain feature seems to 'belong' in
the target languagg. By the I970s, many researchers were convinced that
behaviourism and the contrastive analysis hypothesis were inadequate
explanations for second language acquisition. Some of these criticisms arose
as a result of the growing infuence of innatist views of language acquisition.
The innatist perspective: Universal Grammar
fu
we saw in Chapter 1, the rejection of behaviourism as an explanation for
first language acquisition was pardy triggered by Chomslcy's critique of it.
Cho
of the principles of Universal
Gramma
its all children to acquire
t.
idl-period oithErr
did not make specific claims about the implications o TS
ry for second
language learning, fZ4i" WhS e (2003a) a
Universal Gra
which to unde
second language acqrtisiti.'n. Others, for example Robert Bley-Vroman
(1983) and Jacquelyn Schachter (1990) argue that, although4l-;is'rg'ood
framework for understandin
tlsnota
n
anition for the acquisition of
ially by learners
CS
iod In their Ylew, ts
uage acquisition has to be
lained
; pefnaps one
mo
ries described below.
1
Vivian Cook (2003) and others point out that, even though many learners
fail to achievscomolete mastery of the
of
uisition. That is, we need to find an
rs eventuallv know more a
had to
nablv have
entirely on ths-input they are expo
This suggests that knowledge of
.o'd6g'r-learners *.tt as to first l"tg".r"g.
secon
r the evidence
learners. Some of the theorists who
availabiliwT
rs argue that
UG
"r
rst and second
may be present and avai
re and
ulsltlon.
-to seco
uage
learners, but that its exact nature has been altered by the acquisition of other
languages.
35
Exp
laining
second knguage learning
Researchers working within the UG framework also differ in their
hypotheses about how formal instruction or the availabiliry of feedback on
their learning will affect learners' knowledge of the second language. Bonnie
Schwartz (1993), for example, concludes that such instruction and feedback
change only the superficialappearance of language performance and do not
really affect the underlying systematic knowledge of the new language.
Rather, language acquisition is based on the availabiliry of natural language
in the learner's environment. Lydia -White (1991) and others who think that
the nature of UG is altered by the acquisition of the first language suggesr
that second language learners may sometimes need explicit information
about what is not grammatical in the second language. Otherwise, they may
assume that some structures of the first language have equivalents in the
second language when, in fact, they do not. \7e will see some examples of
language structures that are influenced by the learner's first language in
Chapter 4 andsome studies related to the effect of instruction and feedback
in Chapter 6.
---\\
/':'
Researchers who study second language acquisition fro6 IJG p.rspect_!
"
are usually interested in the language comperence of advancedlbar
their com
than in the simple lan
-12tl161
of beginni{g*ls3gSts. They are interested
in whether the c6mpffi6that
underlies the pBnToRMANCE or use of the second language resembles the
competence underlying the lan
Thus,
ce of native s
their investieations often involv
ATICALITY JUDGEMENT
r
methods to probe what learners know about
rather th
oL,s€rvauons or sfe^Krng Dy usmg
ing such
sucn methods,
metnocls, they hope to gain
msrght
galn insight
into what learners actually know about the language rath;r than ho- tf,ey
happen to use it in a given situation.
Ne
SPC
lear
pol
pl"
pra
Th
acq
Th
nec
thir
adv
con
Th,
lanl
levr
(*c
b.y
Thr
con
ACCI
me1
.E
atti
ma)
Second hnguage app lications : Kras h en's
'monitor model'
One model of second language
acquisition that was influenced by
Chomsky's theory of first language acquisition was Stephen lftashen's
(1982) tUoaitor.Uodsl. He first described this model in the early I970s, at a
time when there was growing dissatisfaction with language teaching
methods based on behaviourism. Krashen described his model in terms of
five hypotheses.
First, in the acquisition-learning hypothesis, I(rashen contrasrs these two
terms. \7e 'acquire' as we are exposed to samples of the second language we
understand in much the same way that children pick up their first
language-with no conscious attendon to language form. \7e'learri on the
other hand through conscious attention to form and rule learning.
.=
ia
:,
I-=
R
IN
il]
Exp
laining s econd knguage learning
Next, according to the monitor hypothesis, the acquired system initiates a
speaker's utterances and is responsible for spontaneous language use. The
learned system acts as an editor or'monitor', making minor changes and
polishing what the acquired system has produced. Such monitoring takes
place only when the speaker/writer has plenry of time, is concerned about
producing correct language, and has learned the relevant rules.
The natural order hypothesis was based on the finding that, as in first language
acquisition, second language acquisition unfolds in predictable sequences.
The language features that are easiest to state (and thus to learn) are not
necessarily the first to be acquired. For example, the rule for adding an -.t to
third person singular verbs in the present tense is easy to state, but even some
advanced second language speakers fail to apply it in spontaneous
conversation (see Chapter 4).
The input hypothesis is that acquisition occurs when one is exposed to
language that is comprehensible and that contains i + 1. The'i' represents the
level of language already acquired, and the'+f is a metaphor for language
(words, grammatical forms, aspects of pronunciation) that is just a step
beyond that level.
The fact that some people who are
exposed
to
large quantities of
comprehensible input do not necessarily acquire a language successfully is
accounted for by Krashens ffictiue flter hypothesis. Th. '"ffb.ti". fitt.'' ;s
metaphorical barrier that Drevents learners from acquiri
rs to feelin
,
and
A
learner_who
is
emotiond
stajgs.
tense-,
anxious,
bored
or
-amitudes.
may'filter out'infl't. making it
tl'uhL her
isry
unav@
37
Exp
laining s econd language learning
Both psychologists and linguists challenged lirashen's model. Linguist Lydia
'White (1987) questioned one of his hypotheses in a paper called Against
Comprehensible Input'. Psychologist Barry Mclaughlint 1978 articl,e was
one of the first to raise the question ofwhether the five hypotheses could be
tested by empirical research. For example, distinguishing between 'acquired'
and 'learned' knowledge can lead to circular definitions (if itt acquired, it's
fluent; if itt fuent, it's acquired) and to a reliance on intuition rather than
observable differences in behaviour.
In spite of lively criticism and debate, Krashent ideas were very influential
during a period when second language teaching was in transition from
approaches that emphasized learning rules or memo rizing dialogues to
approaches that emphasized using language with a focus on meaning. Since
then, coMMUNrcATrvE LANGUAGE TEAcHrNc, including rvruERsroN
and courENT-BAsED TNSTRUCTToN, has been widely implemented, and
Krashen's ideas have been a source of ideas for research in second language
acquisition. Classroom research has confirmed that students can make a
great deal of progress through exposure to comprehensible input without
direct instruction. Studies have also shown, however, that students may
reach a point from which they fail to make further progress on some features
of the second language unless they also have access to guided instruction (see
Chapter 6). Some insights from learning theories developed in psychology
help to explain why this may be so.
Current psycholo gical theories :
The co gnitivist/developmenral perspective
Since the 1990s, psychological theories have become increasingly central to
research in second language development. Some of these theories use rhe
computer as a metaphor for the mind, comparing language acquisition to
the capacities of computers for storing, integrating, and retrieving informa-
tion. Some draw on neurobiology, seeking to relate observed behaviour
directly
as
as
possible to brain activiry.
As in first language acquisition, cognitive and developmental psychologfsts
argue that there is no need to hypothesize that humans have a languagespecific module in the brain or that 'acquisition and 'learning' are distinct
mental processes. In their view, general theories of learning can accounr for
the gradual development of complex syntax and for learners' inability to
spontaneously use everything they know about a language at agiven time. As
noted above, some linguists have also concluded that, while UG provides a
plausible explanation for first language acquisition, something else is required
for second language acquisition since it so often falls short of full success.
I\
SI
E
a
d
ir
S
h,
(l
SE
le
L]
Pr
Exp
laining
s eco
nd language learnirug
Info rm ati o n p ro c e s s in g
Cognitive psychologists working in an information-processing model of
human learning and performance see second language acquisition as the
building up of knowledge that can eventually be called on automatically for
speaking and understanding. Norman Segalowitz (2003) and others have
suggested that learners have to pay attention at first to any aspect of the
language that they are trying to understand or produce. 'Pay attention' in
this context is accepted to mean using cognitive resources to process
information. However, there is a limit to how much information a learner
canpay attention to. Thus, learners at the earliest stages will use most oftheir
resources to understand the main words in a message. In that situation, they
may not notice the grammatical morphemes attached to some of the words,
especially those that do not substantially affect meaning. Gradually, through
experience and practice, information that was new becomes easier to
process, and learners become able to access it quickly and even automatically.
This frees them to pay attention to other aspects of the language that, in
turn, gradually become automatic.
For proficient speakers, choosing words, pronouncing them, and stringing
them together with the appropriate grammatical markers is essentially
automatic. \[hen proficient listeners hear a familiar word, even for a split
second, they cannot help but understand it. Such automatic responses do
not use up the kind of resources needed for processing new information.
Thus, proficient language users can give their full attention to the overall
meaning of a text or conversation, whereas learners use more of their
attention on processing the meaning of individual words. This helps to
explain why second language readers need more time to understand a text,
even if they eventually do fully comprehend it (Favreau and Segalowitz
19S3).The information processing model suggests that there is a limit to the
amount of focused mental activitywe can engage in at one time.
Note that the 'practice' needed for the development of automaticity is not
something mechanical, and it is not limited to the production of language.
Exposure to, and comprehension of, a language feature may also be counted
as practice. In information processing, practice involves cognitive effort on
the part of the learner, but it need not necessarily be available for the learnert
introspection. It can occur below the level of awareness.
Similar'information processing' approaches to second language acquisition
have been explored by other researchers. Drawing on J. R. Andersont
(1995) work, Robert DeKeyser (1998, 2001) and others have investigated
second language acquisition as 'skill learning'. They suggest that most
learning, including language learning, starts with DEcLARATIVE KNow.LEDGE, also referred to as knowledge that.The hypothesis is that, through
practice, declarative knowledge may become pRocEDURAL KNo'$?'LEDGE,
39
40
Exp
laining s econd language learning
or knowledge /tow, in the same way that someone learns other skills like
driving a car or skating. Indeed, once skills become proceduralized and
automatized, thinking about the declarative knowledge while trying to
perform the skill actually disrupts the smooth performance of it. In second
language acquisition, the path from declarative ro procedural knowledge is
sometimes associated with the kind of learning that takes place in a
classroom, where rule learning is followed by practice. \fith enough
practice, procedural knowledge eclipses the declarative knowledge, which,
in time, may be forgotten. For this reason, fluent speakers may not even
realize that they once possessed the declarative knowledge that set the
process in motion.
Co
tu
con.
neu.
mos
the
learr
sPec
i-p
thec
the
the i
Sometimes changes in language behaviour do nor seem to be explainable in
terms of a gradual build-up of fluency through practice. These .h"ng., have
been described in terms of 'restructuring' (Lightbown 1985; Mclaughlin
1990). They seem to be based on some qualitative change in the learnert
knowledge. Restructuring may account for what appear to be sudden bursts
ofprogress, when learners suddenly seem to 'put it all togerher', even though
they have not had any new instruction or apparently relevant exposure to th.
language. It may also explain apparent backsliding, when
aspect
".yrt.-"tic
of a learner's language incorporates too much or incorporates
the wrong
things. For example, when a learner finally masters the use of the ,rgul^t -ed
verb
ending
to show past tense,
Con
lang
featr
situr
strol
Ever
the,
irregular verbs that had previously been
hear
practised' correcdy may be affected. Thus, after months of saying 'I saw a
film', the learner may say'I seed' or even 'I sawed'. Such errors
.rot based
"r.
on practice of those specific items but rather on their integration
into a
pror
general pattern.
expe
Another concept from psychology offers insight into how learners store and
retrieve_ language. According to 'transfer appropriare processing', information is best retrieved in situations that are similar to those in which it was
acquired (Blaxton 1989). This is because when we learn something our
memories also record something about the context in which it was learned
and even about the way we learned it, for example, by reading or hearing it.
To date, most of the research on transfer appropriate processing has bien
done in laboratory experimenrs, for example, comparing the liarning of
word lists under different conditions. However, the hypothesis seems to offer
a plausible way of explaining a widely observed phenomenon in second
language learning: knowledge that is acquired mainly in rule learning or drill
activities may be easier to access on tests that resemble the learning activities
than in communicative situations (Gatbonton and Segalowitz 1988, 2005).
On the other hand, if, during learning, the learnert cognitive resources are
completely occupied with a focus on meaning in communicative activities,
retrieval of specific language features such as grammatical markers or word
order on a test of those features may be more difficult.
the c
pred
stror
may
Nicl
chur
toge
Asn
mecl
hearr
stud
and
inna
simp
knol
cont
Exp
laining
s eco
nd language learning
Connectionism
As seen in the discussion of first language acquisition in Chapter 1,
no need to hypothesize the existence of a
to language acquisition. Like
most cognitive psychologists, connectionists attribute greater importance to
the role of the environment than to any specific innate knowledge in the
learner, arguing that what is innate is simply the ability to learn, not any
connectionists, unlike innatists,
see
neurological module dedicated exclusively
specifically linguistic principles. Connectionists also attribute less
importance to the kind of declarative knowledge that characterizes some
theories of skill learning. As Nick Ellis (2002) explains, the emphasis is on
the frequency with which learners encounter specific linguistic features in
the input and the frequency with which features occur together.
Connectionists argue that learners gradually build up their knowledge of
language through exposure to the thousands of instances of the linguistic
features they eventually hear. After hearing language features in specific
situational or linguistic contexts over and over again, learners develop a
stronger and stronger network of 'connectionst between these elements.
Eventually, the presence of one situational or linguistic element will activate
the other(s) in the learner's mind. For example, learners might get subjectverb agreement correct, not because they know a rule but because they have
heard examples such as 'I say' and 'he says' so often that each subject
pronoun activates the correct verb form. Connections like these may be very
$rong because the elements have occurred together very frequendy or they
may be relatively weaker because there have been fewer opportunities to
enperience them together. Evidence for the connecdonist view comes from
the observation that much of the language we use in ordinary conversation is
predictable, in some cases to the point of being formulaic. As suggested by
Nick Ellis (2003,2005) and others, language is at least partly learned in
chunks larger than single words and not all sentences or phrases are put
together one word at a time.
fu noted in Chapter 1, connectionist
that a learning
mechanism, simulated by a computer program, cannot only'learn' what it
hears but can also generalize, even making overgeneralization errors. These
research has shown
studies have so far dealt almost exclusivelywith the acquisition ofvocabulary
and grammatical morphemes, that is, aspects of the language that even
innatists will grant may be acquired largely through memorization and
simple generalization. How this model of cumulative learning can lead to
knowledge of complex syntactic structures
continued research.
is an important area for
4T
42
Exp
laining
second language
learning
The competition model
The competition model is closely related to the connectionist perspective. It
is also based on the hypothesis that language acquisition occurs without the
necessity of a learnert focused attention or the need for any innate brain
module that is specifically for language. Elizabeth Bates and Brian
Mac\Mhinney (1981) described the competition model as an explanation for
language acquisition that takes into account not only language form but also
language meaning and language use. The competition model is proposed as
an explanation for both first and second language acquisition. Through
exposure to thousands of examples of language associated with particular
meanings, learners come to understand how to use the 'cues' with which a
language signals specific functions. For example, the relationship between
words in a sentence may be signalled by word order, grammarical markers,
and the animacy of the nouns in the sentence. Most languages make use of
multiple cues, but they differ in the primacy of each. This becomes clear in a
situation where the meaning of a sentence is not immediately obvious. \fhat
helps you figure out the meaning? English uses word order as the mosr
common indicator of the relationships between sentence componenrs. Most
English sentences have the order Subject-Verb-object (SVo). That is, the
rypical English sentence mentions the subject first, then the verb, then the
object. Two- and three-year old English speaking children use cues of
animacy and their knowledge of the way things work in the world to
interpret odd sentences. Thus, if they hear a string of words such as 'Box
push bol , they will act it out by making a boy doll push a dny box, focusing
on the fact that the 'boy is the natural agent of action in this situation.
However, the SVO paftern is so strong in English that, before they are four
years old, children will give an SVO interpretation to such strings of words.
They will ignore the fact that boxes dont normally move on their own, and
carefully demonstrate how the box pushes the boy. \ford order parterns are
stronger than animacy cues ar this point. Furthermore, at this age, they may
attribute the SVO relationship to sentences in the passive voice. That is,
'The box was pushed by the boy' may be interpreted as 'The box pushed the
boy.' Only later do they learn to pay attention to the grammarical markers
that distinguish the active voice sentence from the passive word order.
Other languages, for example, Spanish and Italian, have more flexible word
order. As Brian Mac\Whinney (1997) explains, speakers of these languages,
even as adults, rely more on grammatical markers (fot example, the
agreement of subject and verb, the case marking of pronouns) or on the
animacy of nouns to understand how sentence elements are related. \fhen
English speakers are learning these languages, they may have difficulry
suppressing their tendency ro rely on word order as the basis for
interpretation. For example, an English speaking learner of Italian may find
It
C(
_ir
wol
moi
to
t.
lear
lanl
Se,
AN
Ar
lanl
pen
.---"fi
)+"*
Sus,
gss
rese
thei
con
4
Kra
Ho.
con
-t-
ry
fl€er
-l
oDn
mu.
*h;
con
lear
E;
Int
infe
SUIT
1Ir
2C
The
3It
Exp
kining
s
econd language learning
it confusing to hear sentences such as'Ilgiocattoh guarda il bambina' (the toy
looking at-the boy). An Itdian speaker, accustomed to more fexible
-is order, focuses on the animacy ofthe two nouns and concludes that the
word
most reasonable interpretation is that the boy is looking at the toy. According
to the competition model, second language acquisition requires that learners
learn the relative importance of the different cues appropriate in the
language they are learning (Mac\Thinney 1997).
Second language applications:
fnteracting, noticing,
and processing
A number of hypotheses, theories, and
models for explaining second
language acquisition have been inspired by the cognitivist/developmental
velyn Hatch (1978), Michael Long (1983, 1996),Teresa Pica (1994) and
Susan Gass (1997), among others, argue that
conver"^ti.'.^l interacrion
is an
gssential. if not sufficient, conditiqn for-second langtt^g" ^"qrtisitiorl.Tlessresearchers have studied the ways in which s
modifu their
and
tmi
rners partlcrDate
nYersailon or
) agreed with
n that comprehensible input is necessary for language acquisition.
However, h. fo.nt.d -ot. otr th. qr.r.rtion of ho* itput .or'tld b.
ryhelf&lg.
-"d.
He argued that MopIFIEp INTERACTToN is the necessary
ins lan
ecessarily simoli
istic forms but rather an
rtunity to interact with other
wo
@Throughtheseinteractions,interlocutorsfigureout
what they need to do to keep the conversation going and mak. th. input
omprehensible. 4gcogd!gg-&-Long, there are no cases of besinner-level
lcarners acquiring a second
m natlve-s
tdk that has n6t
boen modified itjggJgy.
In the original (1983) formulation of the Interaction Hypothesis, Long
ilferred that modified interaction is necessary for language acquisition,
mmmarizing the relationship
as
follows:
I
Interactional modification makes input comprehensible.
2
C-omprehensible input promores acquisition.
Therefore,
t
lnteractional modification promotes acquisition.
,'4
SD
3=)a
,(*2
43
44
Exp laining
s
eco
nd language learning
provision of
s. Some examples of these conyersational rnodifica-
B
la
tc
ei
1 @-efforts
by the native speaker to ensure that rhe
(for
learner has underrqo4
example, 'The bus leaves at 6:30. Do you
tl
may also include
e
tlons are:
und€rsTeind.}
us515-sffoft5
clarify something that
rEpeat please?'). These requesrs from
modifications by the native speaker.
3
(for example, 'Could you
the learner lead to further
ffit.n.e.ith.t p"rti"ll)'.'r i'.' it. "@.,
*"y home from school. She was walking
o1
br
in
le
d;
la
(
is or
her
'Sh. goti;hfrtdrhome from school. She got
lost.').
n
A,
m
Pr
th
SO
that conversational adjustments can aid comprehension. Modification that takes place during interaction leads to beit.r
Research has shown
understanding than linguistic simplification or modification that is planned
in advance. \7hile some recent research has shown that specific kinds of
interaction behaviours aid learning in terms of immediate producrion, more
research is needed on how access to modified interaction affects second
language acquisition in the long rerm.
In Longt (1995) revised version of the Interaction Hypothesis, more
emphasis is placed on the importance of corrective feedback during
interaction. tVhen communication is difficult, interlocutors must'negotiatl
for meaning', and this negotiation is seen as the opporrunity for l"ngu"ge
development. Merrill Swain (1985) extended this thinking *h.n rh.
proposed 'the comprehensible output hypothesis'. She observed that it is
when learners must produce language that their interlocutor can understand
that they are most likely to see the limits of their second language ability and
the need to find better
ro express their meaning. The demands of
producing comprehensible output, she hypothesized, push learners ahead
in their development.
SO
TJ
th
tfi
ac
de
in
pa
Pr
SI:
lar
lez
pc
to
TI
se(
reI
In
The noticing hypothesis
Richard Schmidt (1990, 2001) proposed the 'noticing hypothesis',
suggesting that nothing is learned unless it has been noticed. Noticing does
not itself result in acquisition, but it is the essential starring point.
Schmidt's original proposal of the noticing hypothesis came from his own
experience as a learner of Portuguese. After months of taking classes, living in
In
lar
PI.
\\+
int
int
ser
Exp
hining
second language
learning
Brazll, and keeping a diary, he began to realize that certain features of
Ianguage that had been present in the environment for the whole time began
to enter his own second language system only when he had noticed them,
either because they were brought to his attention in class or because some
oF.t experience made them salient. Drawing on psychological learning
theories, Schmidt hypothesized that second language learnirs could not
begin to acquire a language feature until they had become aware of it in the
input. Susan Gass (1988) also described a learning process that begins when
learners notice something they hear or see in the second language that is
different from what they expected or that fills a gap in their knowleJge of the
Ianguage. The question of whether learners musr be aware that ihey are
hoticing' something in the input is the object of considerable debate.
According to information processing theories, anyrhing that uses up our
mental 'processing space', even ifwe are not aware of it or attending to it 'on
qurgose', can contribute to learning. From the connectionist peispective,
&e likelihood of acquisition is best predicted by the frequenry with which
something is available for processing, not by the learner's awareness of
something in the input.
These questions about the importance of awareness and atrention have been
the object of debate and research. Several researchers have found ways to
track learners' attention as they engage in second language interaction or
lctivity Alison Mackey, Susan Gass, and Kim McDonough (2000) have
described techniques, for example, having learners see and hear themselves
in videotaped interactions, to explore what they were thinking as they
participated in conversations. Ron Leow (1997) developed crossword
puzzles that learners had to solve while speaking aloud. Meirill Swain and
Sharon Lapkin (1998) recorded learners in pair work and kept track of the
language features they mendoned. These reiearch designs ."rrno, tell us if
learners noticed things they did nor mention. However, they do make it
possible to identify some things that learners showed theywere aware of and
to_compare these_toperformance on measures of their language knowledge.
The extent to which learners' awareness of language f."t,rr.i affects th-eir
second language development will come up again in our discussion of
research on second language acquisition in the classroom in Chapter 6.
lnput processing
In his research with American
university students learning foreign
languages, Bill VanPatten (2004) observed many cases of students misinterpreting sentences. For example, as predicted by the competition model,
when these English speakers heard sentences such as'La sigue el sefioi , they
interpreted it as 'She (subject pronoun) follows the man'. The correcr
interpretation is 'Her (object pronoun) follows the man' (subject of the
sentence). In other words, the correct English translation would be 'The
45
Fxp
ki n i ng s econd lnnguage le arning
man follows her'. In order to understand that, students need to learn that in
Spanish, a pronoun object precedes the verb and that it is essential to pay
attention to whether the pronoun is a subject or an object rather than to the
word order alone. (See the discussion ofthe competirion model earlier in this
chapter.)
VanPatten argued that the problem arose in part from the fact that learners
have limited processing capacity and cannot pay artenrion to form and
meaning at the same time. Not surprisingly, they tend to give prioriry to
meaning. \7hen the context in which they hear a senrence helps them make
sense of it, they do not notice details of the language form. In Chapter 6 we
will see how VanPatten developed instructional procedures rhar require
learners to focus on the language itself in order to interprer rhe meaning.
Jtrgen Meisel, Harald Clahsen, and Manfred Pienemann ( 1 98 1) studied the
acquisition of German by a group of adult migrant workers who had little or
no second language instruction. They analysed large samples of their speech
and described the details of developmental sequences in their ptodu.tion of
simpIeandcomplexSentences.Theyconcludedthatth.-w-g{*
@Ier
Asw
men
actio
Unli
but
*rinl
whic
cons
othe
inter
is,
in
beca
Processability theory
developm.nt
Th
of sygq4x and morphology usaffer.reit-U*-U"**
ea y these were to process. Ease of processing was found to depend to a
tent on th-e positio
urred at t
those that were tn t
ically
d of a sentence
e. All learners acquired the features in the same
sequence, even though they progressed at different rares. They also found
that some languase features did not seem to he affecterl h. rh"".--oo.traint.
and were used bv learnens who were at different
were refbrred to as variation
Pieneman n (1999, 2003) developed his processabiliry theory on the basis
his continued research with learners of different languag.i in a variery
of
of
settings, both instructional and informal. One imforln, aspec of his
theory is the integration of developmental sequences with firit language
influence..H.
rh"l
,h...g
r,o'
fi,r
"rgr.r
in second
isition: learners do noiJlmol
features from
q{
In
sc
enco
ZPL
addr
coml
how
whic
In I(
on tl
just t
ond
inter
vyg.
becar
unde
place
empl
trnter
the i
great
learn
that
they can
use
language. \(/e
owledeq qf the features that already
will see many examples oftIEIi
exiffi-iffiTist
medi
Sec'
Exter
(200
SCCOI
Exp
kining
s
econd language learning
The sociocultural perspective
in Chapter 1, Vygotskyt theory assumes that cognitive development, including language development, arises as a result of social interactions. Primary among these interactions are those between individuals.
Unlike the psychological theories that view thinking and speaking as related
but independent processes, sociocultural theory views speaking and
thinking as tightly interwoven. Speaking (and writing) mediate thinking,
which means that people can gain control over their mental processes as a
consequence of internalizing what others say to them and what they say to
others. Learning is thought to occr.lr when an individual interacts with an
interlocutor within his or her zone of proximal development (ZPD)-that
is, in a situation in which the learner is capable ofperforming at a higher level
because there is support from an interlocutor.
As we saw
In some ways, this approach may appear to restate some of the hypotheses
encountered elsewhere in this chapter. People sometimes wonder whether the
ZPD is the same as Krashent i+i. Vrilli"- Dunn andJames Lantolf (1998)
addressed this question in a review article, arguing that it is not possible to
compare the two concepts because they depend on very different ideas about
how development occurs. The ZPD is a metaphorical location or 'site' in
which learners co-construct knowledge in collaboration with an interlocutor.
In Iftashenk i+l the input comes from outside the learner and the emphasis is
on the comprehensibiliry of input that includes language structures that are
just beyond the learnert current developmental level. The emphasis in ZPD is
on development and how learners co-consffuct knowledge based on their
interaction with their interlocutor or in pnrverE spEECH.
Vygotskyan theory has also been compared to the interaction hypothesis
because of the interlocutor's role in helping learners understand and be
understood. These two perspectives differ primarily in the emphasis they
place on the internal cognitive processes. In the interaction hypothesis, the
emphasis is on the individual cognitive processes in the mind of the learner.
Interaction facilitates those cognitive processes by giving learners access to
the input they need to activate internal processes. In Vygotskyan theory,
greater importance is attached to the conversations themselves, with
learning occurring through the social interaction. Sociocultural theory holds
that people gain control of and reorganize their cognitive processes during
mediation as knowledge is internalized during social activity.
Second. Ianguage applications:
Learning by talhing
Extending Vygotstr<yan theory to second language acquisition, Jim Lantolf
(2000), Richard Donato (1994) and others are interested in showing how
second language learners acquire language when they collaborate and
47
48
Exp
laining
second language learning
interact with other speakers. Tiaditionally, the ZPD has been und.erstood to
involve an expert and a novice, however, recent work has broadened the term
to include novice/novice or learner/learner interlocutors. An example of this
is in Communication task B in Chapter 5. In that excerpt the learners are
struggling with French refexive verbs as they try to consrrucr a storyline
from pictures. That example is taken from the work of Merrill Swain and
Sharon Lapkin (2002),who have investigated sociocultural explanations for
second language learning in Canadian French immersion programmes.
Their work has its origins in Swain's'comprehensible outpur hypothesis' and
the notion that the production of language pushes learners ro process
language more deeply. In preparing to speak or write, they must pay more
attention to how meaning is expressed through language than they do for the
comprehension of language. Swain ( 1 9 8 5) fi rst proposed the'covr p RE H EN SIBLE ourPUT HYPOTHESIS' in response to Krashent comprehensible
input hypothesis, based on the observation that French immersion students
were considerably weaker in their spoken and written producrion than in
their reading and listening comprehension (see Chapter 6). She advocated
more opportunities for learners to engage in verbal production (i.e. 'output')
in French immersion classrooms. Since then, she and her colleagues have
carried out extensive research to investigate the effects of output on second
language learning.
Swain's (2000) early work on the outpur hypothesis was influenced by
cognitive theory, but more recent work has been motivated by sociocultural
theory. Using the term'collaborative dialogue', Swain and Lapkin and their
colleagues have carried out a series of studies to determine how second
language learners co-consffuct linguistic knowledge while engaging in
production tasks (i.e. speaking and writing) that simultaneously draw their
attention to form and meaning. In Communication task B in Chapter 5,
learners were testing hypotheses about the correct forms to use, discussing
together and deciding what forms were best to express their meaning.
them
Swain (2000) considers collaborative dialogues such as these as the conrexr
where 'language use and language learning can co-occur. It is language use
mediating language learning. It is cognitive activity and it is social
rity
(p. e7).
".ii
T
Int
for
soc
cha
Bor
neu
acti
Adr
acti
prel
beh
Mar
anir
abili
chal
inad
l*g
Nern
cont
carel
[irg,
coml
Ling
tnom
*dl
Criti
nrnov
ffePS
lnterr
acdor
rnsigl
Therefore, the difference between the sociocultural perspective and that of
other researchers who also view interaction as important in second language
acquisition is that sociocultural theorists assume that the cognitive processes
begin as an external socially mediated activity and eventually become
internalized. Other interactionist models assume that modified input and
interaction provide learners with the raw material for internal cognitive
processes.
abour
;ridcr
knorv
innatr
Resea
tiou g
&e
la
je.Irgui
Exp
laining
se
cond language learning
Theory into practice
In the end, what all theories of language acquisition are intended to account
for is the abiliry of human learners to acquire language within a variety of
social and instructional environments. All of the theories discussed in this
chapter and in Chapter I use metaphors to represent this invisible reality.
Both linguists and psychologists draw some of their evidence from
neurological research. At present, most of the research on specific brain
activity during language processing must be based on indirect evidence.
Advances in technology arcrapidly increasing oppoftunities to observe brain
activity more directly. Such research will eventually contribute to reinterpretations of research that, until now can examine only observable
behaviour.
M*y
claims from behaviourist theory were based on experiments with
animals learning a variety of responses to laboratory stimuli. Their applicabiliry to the natural learning of languages by humans was strongly
challenged by psychologists and linguists alike, primarily because of the
inadequacyofbehaviourist models to account for the complexityinvolved in
language learning.
Newer psychological theories have often involved computer simulations or
controlled laboratory experiments where people learn specific sets of
carefully chosen linguistic features, often in an invented language. Many
linguists argue that this does not entitle psychologists to generalize to the
complexities of the linguistic knowledge that learners eventually have.
Linguists working from an innatist perspective draw much of their evidence
from studies of the complexities of proficient speakers'language knowledge
and performance and from analysis of their own intuitions about language.
Critics of this view argue that it is not enough to knowwhat the final state of
knowledge is and that more attention should be paid to the developmental
steps leading up to this level of mastery.
lnteractionists emphasize the role of modification in conversarional interecdons. This perspective, as well as the sociocultural perspective, provides
insights into the ways in which learners can gain access to new knowledge
about the language when they have support from an interlocutor. Some
critics of the interactionist position argue that much ofwhat learners need to
know is not available in the input, and so they put greater emphasis on
innate principles of language that learners can draw on.
Researchers and educators who are hoping for language acquisition theories
Sat give them insight into language teaching practice are often frustrated by
the lack of agreement among the 'experts'. The complexities of second
hnguage acquisition, like those of first language acquisition, represent
49
50
Exp laining second language learning
puzzlesthat scientists will continue to work on for a long time. Research that
has theory development as its goal has important long-term significance for
language teaching and learning, but agreement on a 'complete' theory of
language acquisition is probably, at best, a long way off. Even if such
agreemenr were reached, there would still be quesdons about how the theory
should be interpreted for language teaching practice. Many teachers watch
Psyc'
pp.
McI
to r(
T.K
theory development with interest, but must continue to teach and plan
lessons and assess students' performance in the absence of a comprehensive
theory of second language learning.
Aca<
research draws on a wide range of theoretical
stated, sometimes merely implied. This
explicitly
sometimes
orientations,
research may provide information that is more helpful in guiding teachers'
reflections about pedagogy. In Chapters 5 and 6, we will examine language
acquisition research that has focused on learning in the classroom. First,
however, we will review research on individual diflerences that influence
learners' success in language acquisition (Chapter 3) and some detailed
descriptions of learners' developing language knowledge and use (Chapter 4).
Pier
A growing body of applied
Mit,
edn.
Ritc
I^an
Rob
Can
Skel
Sources and suggestions for further
Block, D.2003. The Social Turn in
Edinburgh Universiry Press.
Second Language
readitg
Acquisition.Edtnburgh:
Oxf
Tou
-4tqi
\:m
Brools, N. 1960. Language and Language Learnirug.NewYork Harcourt.
Can
Cook, V 2003. 'The poverty-of-the-stimulus argument and structuredependency in L2 users of English. International Reuiew of Applied
Linguistics 4l I 3: 201-21 .
wh
Doughty, C. J. and M. H. Long (eds.) . 2003. The Handbook of Second
Language Ac quis itio n. Oxford: Blackwell.
Ellis, R. 1994. The Study of Second Language Acquisitioru. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Gass, S. M. 1997. Input, Interaction, and the Second Language Learner.
Mahwah, NJr Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
Krashen, S.
D.
1982. Principles and Practice in Second LanguageAcquisition.
Oxford: Pergamon.
Lantolf, J. P. (.d.). 2000. Sociocuhural Theory arud Second Language
Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mac\fhinney, B. 1997.'second language acquisition and the competition
modef in A. M. B. de Groot and J. F. Kroll (eds.): Tutorials in Bilingualism:
Dot
-4tq',
Exphiningsecond language
learning
Rycholinguistic Perspectiues. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
pp.
11344.
Mclaughlin, B. and R. Heredia. 1995. 'Information-processing approaches
b research on second language acquisition and use' in \f. C. Ritchie and
I K. Bhatia (eds.): Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. New York:
Academic Press. pp. 213-28.
Mitchell, R. and F. Myles. 2004. Second Language Learning Theories 2nd
cdn.
: Arnold.
3. 'Language processing capacity in C. J. Doughry and
):
The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition.
Oxford:
Blackwell. pp. 67 9-7 14.
R.itchie,
\f. C. and T. K. Bhatia. L996.
Langaage Acquisitiaz. New
York Academic
(eds.). Handbook
of Second
Press.
Robinson, P. (.d.). 2001. Cognition and Second Language Instruction.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
$<ehan,
P. 1998. A Cognitiue Approach to Language Learning. Oxford:
Otrord University
Towell,
Press.
R. and R. Hawkins. 1994. Approaches to Second Language
Acq ui s i ti o n. Clev edo n : Multilin gual M atters.
VanPatten,
B. (.d.). 2004. Processing Insnuction:
Theory, Researclt, and
C-ommentary. Mahwah, NJr Lawrence Erlbaum and fusociates.
White, L. 2003b. 'On the nature of interlanguage representation in C. J.
Doughty and M. H. Long (eds.): The Handbooh of Second Language
Acquisition. Oxford: Blachvell. pp. 20-42.
5l
INDIVIDUAL
DIFFERENCES IN SECOND
LANGUAGE LEARNING
Before you read this chapter, reflect on your own experience as a language
learner. Then interview several friends, colleagues, or family members about
their experiences learning a second or foreign language. If there is a language
they speak with a high level of proficiency, ask about the environment in
which the language was heard and used, the kind of instruction (if any) they
received, how long they used the language, and the age at which they began
learning. fuk about the kinds of relationships they had with speakers of the
language and whether they felt a part of a community in which it is spoken.
fuk whether there is a language they failed to learn, even though they had
some exposure to, or instruction in, that language. Keep notes about your
own experiences and those of the people you interview and refer to them as
you read this chapter about individual differences in second
language
learning.
fu
we saw in Chapter 1, children are almost always successful in acquiring
the language or languages that are spoken (or signed) to them in early
childhood, provided that they have adequate opportunities to use the
language over a period of several years. This contrasts with our experience of
second language learners, whose success varies greatly.
Many of us believe that individual differences that are inherent in the learner
can predict success or failure in language learning. Such beliefs may be based
on.our own experience or that ofpeople we have known. For example, many
teachers are convinced that extroverted learners who interact without
inhibition in the second language and seek opportunities to practise
language skills will be the most successful learners. In addition to an
outgoing personaliry, other characteristics often believed to predict success
in language learningare intelligence, aptitude, motivation, and the age at
which learning begins.
ln this chapter, we will see whether these intuitions are supported by research
Gndings. To what extent can we predict differences in the success of second
language acquisition if we have information about learners' personalities,
their general and specific intellectual abilities, their motivation, or their age?
54
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
V/ho is a'good language learner'?
Rate
\7e know that some people learn languages more quickly than others. Even
in first language acquisition, the rate of development varies widely. Some
children can string together five-, six-, and seyen-word sentences ar an age
when other children are just beginning to label items in their immediaie
environment. Nevertheless, children eventually masrer their first language.
It has been observed countless times that, in the same foreign language class,
some students progress rapidlywhile others struggle along making very slow
progress. Even in what seem to be ideal conditions, some learners seem to
make little progress
in learning. Researchers-for
example, Neil Naiman
and his colleagues (1995)-have tried to identify the personal characteristics
that make one learner more successful than another.
Thble 3.1 shows a list ofsome ofthe characteristics that have been thought to
contribute to successful language learning. In your experience-as a second
language learner or teacher-which characteristics seem ro you mosr likely
to be associated with success in
second language acquisition
classroom? \X4rich ones do you think are less important?
The characteristics listed inThble
in
the
3.I
chan
you (
Ago
ais
btr
lar
cis
dcc
ePr
fan
of
g
at1
mr
hen
ibe
canbe classified into several categories:
motivation, intellectual abilities, personality, and learning preferences.
However, many of the characteristics cannot be assigned exclusively to one
ih"
category. For example, 'is willing to make mistakes' can be considered a
personaliry characteristic. It might also be seen as an aspecr of motivation if
the learner is willing to make mistakes in order ro ger a message across.
lha
kha
Photo
Research on learner characteristics
Tabk
Perhaps the best way to begin our discussion is to describe how research on
the influence of individual differences on second language learning is usually
done. \7hen researchers are interested in finding our whether to*ro"r"
" select
such as modvation affects second language learning, they usually
a
AIth<
group of learners and give them a questionnaire to measure rhe type and
degree of their motivation. Then some kind of test is used to
their
"rr.r,
second language proficiency. The test and the questionnaire are both
scored,
and the researcher uses a statistical procedure called a coRRErerroN. The
correlation shows how likely it is that learners with high scores on the
motivation questionnaire will also have high scores on the language test. If
the two variables (motivation and language proficienry) are found to be
positively correlated, the researcher will try ro discover just what the
relationship between them is.
entirr
carefi
38e'
moti
such
some
For e:
they I
The
:
frequ
reasot
frequ,
becau
Indiuidual differences in
second language learning
Rate each of the following characteristics
on a scale of l-5. Use I to indicate a
characteristic that you.think is 'very important'and
5 to indicate a characteristic that
you consider'not at all important' in predicting
success in second language learning.
A good language learner:
a is a willing and accurate guesser
b tries to get a message across even if specific
languageknowledgeislacking
c is willing to make mistakes
d constantly looks for patterns in the language
c pracdses as often as possible
I analyses his or her own speech and the speech
ofothers12345
3 attends to whether his or her performance
meets the standards he or she has learned
I enjoysgrammarexercises
i bqgins learning in childhood
i
k
I
has an above,-ave,rage
has good academic
has a
skills
good self-image and lots of
Photocopiable
Table
\e
3.1
@
confidence
|
2
I
|
I
I
I
2
Z
2
2
3 4
j 4
3 4
3 4
I
I
I
Z j 4
2 3 4
2 j 4
5
I
2
s
I
I
Z j 4
Z 3 4
4
3
4
5
5
5
5
5
5
5
5
5
Oxford University Press
Characteristics ofthe good language learner'
Although the correlation procedure seems straightforward,
careful interpretation. One problem is that, unlike variables such
it
requires
height or
age, it is not possible to directly observe and measure variables such as
motivation, extroversion, or even intelligence. These are just labels for an
entire range of behaviours and characteristics. Furthermore, characteristics
such as these are not independent of each other, and researchers have
sometimes used the same label to describe different sets ofbehavioural traits.
For example, in motivation questionnaires, learners may be asked how often
they have opporunities to use their second language with nativej speakers.
The assumption behind the question is that those who reporr that they
frequentlyhave such opporrunities are highlymodvated to learn. This seems
reasonable, but it is not so simple. If a learner responds that he or she
frequently interacts with speakers of the second language, it may not be
because he or she is more modvated to learn. Rather, it might be that this
as
55
55
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
individual lives where there are more opportunities for language practice in
informal contexts than those who report a low frequency of interaction.
Because it is usually impossible to separate these two variables (i...
willingness to interact and opportunities to interact), we cannor conclude
whether it is motivation or opportunity that is mosr closely associated with
success.
Perhaps the most serious error in interpreting correlations is the conclusion
that one of the variables causes the other. The fact that rwo things tend to
occur together or increase and decrease in a similar pattern does not
necessarily mean that one caused the other. \7hile it may be that one variable
influences the other, it may also be that both are influenced by something
else entirely. Research on motivation is perhaps the best contexr in which to
illustrate this. Learners who are successful may indeed be highly motivated.
But can we conclude that they became successful because of their motivation? It is also plausible that early success heightened their motivation, or
that both success and motivation are due to their special aptitude for
language learning or the favourable context in which they are learning.
Another difficulry in assessing the relationship berween individual learner
characteristics and second language learning is how language proficiency is
defined and measured. In the second language learning literature, some studies
report that learners with a higher I Q (intelligence quotient) are more
successful language learners than those with a lower I Q, while orher studies
report no such correlation. One explanation for these conficting findings is
that the language proficiency tests used in different studies do not measure the
same kind of knowledge. That is, I Q may be less closely correlated to measures
of conversational fuency than to tests that measure metalinguistic knowledge.
Research on individual differences must also take
into account the social and
educational settings in which learners find themselves. Bonny Norton and
Kelleen Toohey (2001) argue that, even when individuals possess some of
the characteristics that have been associated with the 'good language learner',
their language acquisition may not be successful if they are not able to gain
access to social relationships in situations where they are perceived as valued
partners in communication. Members of some immigrant and minoriry
groups are too often marginalizedby social and educational practices that
limit their opportunities ro engage in communication with peers,
colleagues, and even teachers. In these social conditions, individuals who
approach a new language with the cognitive and motivational characteristics
rypical of the 'good language learner' may not achieve the proficiency that
these characteristics would predict.
lJnderstanding the relationship between individual differences, social
situations, and success in second language learning is a great challenge.
Nevertheless, research in this area is of great importance to both researchers
and
pefSt
expe
Edu,
achir
conc
shap
socie
Int,
The
OII
Ct
schor
som€
score
How
meta
study
Gene
ment
unrel
measl
Iearni
ligenc
fbcus,
q'hosr
succel
In rec
trgg3
traditi
multi
interp
most
(
Ap tt
Specif
smdie,
rhis ar
abiliry
apdtur
also be
Indiuidual diferences in
second language
learning
and educators. Researchers seek to know how different cognitive and
personality variables are related and how they interact with learners'
experiences so that they can gain a better understanding of human learning.
Educators hope to find ways of helping learners with different characteristics
achieve success in second language learning. The larger community is also
concerned because of the enormous impact second language learning has in
shaping opportunities for education, employment, mobiliry and other
societal benefits.
Intelligence
The term 'intelligence' has traditionally been used to refer to performance
on certain kinds of tests. These tests are often associated with success in
school, and a link between intelligence and second language learning has
sometimes been reported. Over the years, some research has shown that IQ
scores were a good means of predicting success in second language learning.
However, as suggested above, I Q tests may be more strongly related to
metalinguistic knowledge than to communicative abiliry. For example, in a
studywith students in French TMMERSIoN pRocRAMMES in Canada, Fred
Genesee (1976) found that, while intelligence was related to the development of French second language reading, grammar, and vocabulary, it was
unrelated to oral production skills. This suggests that the kind of abiliry
measured by traditional I Q tests may be a strong predictor when it comes to
learning that involves language analysis and rule learning. This kind of intelligence' may play a less important role in classrooms where the instruction
focuses more on communication and interacdon. Indeed, many students
whose general academic performance is weak experience considerable
success in second language learning if they are given the right opportunities.
In recent years, many educators have been influenced by Howard Gardnert
(1993) proposal that individuals have 'multiple intelligences' and that
tradition"l I Q tests have assessed only a limited range of abilities. Among the
multiple intelligences' Gardner includes abilities in the areas of music,
interpersonal relations, and athletics, as well as the verbal intelligence that is
most often associated with success in school.
Aptitude
Specific abilities thought to predict success in language learning have been
snrdied under the title oflanguage learning'aptitude'. One ofthe pioneers in
this area, John Carroll (1991), has characterized aptitude in terms of the
abiliry to learn quickly. Thus, we may hypothesize that a learner with high
aptitude may learn with greater ease and speed but that other learners may
also be successful if they persevere.
57
58
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
Over several decades, the most widely used aptiude tests have been the
Modern T.anguage Aptitude Test (MLAT) (carroll and sapon 1959) and"
the Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery (PLAB) (pimsleur 1916).
Recently, Paul Meara (2005a) and his colleagues have developed tests that
are taken on a computer. All the tests are based on the view thai aptitude has
several components. AII measure the abiliry to (1) identify and mem orize
new sounds, (2) understand the function of particular words in senrences,
(3) figure out grammatical rules from language samples, and (4) remember
new words. \While early research revealed a substantial relationship between
performance on the MLAT or PLAB and performance in foreign language
learning, these studies were conducted at a time when ,..orid hr[,r"[.
teaching was based on grammar translation or audiolingual
-.thodi (r!.
Chapter 6). \7ith the adoption of a more communicative approach to
teaching, many teachers and researchers came to believe that ttre abilities
by these tests were irrelevant to the process of language acquisition.
largeted
However, others suggest that some of the abilities
-."r,rr.d b/aptitude tests
are predictive of success even in settings where the emphasis is on
communicative interaction. For example, Leila Ranta (2002) found that
children who we_re good at analysing language (one component of aptitude
that is targeted by aptitude tests) were the most ,.r...rrfrl learners in an
English second language program in which activities almost never involved
direct attention to grammar. Nick Ellis (2001) and others have hypothesized
that wonKING MEMoRY may be the most important variable in predicting
success for learners in many language learning situations. Peter Skehai
(1989) argues that successful language learners tn"ynot be strong in all ofthe
components of aptitude. For example, some individuals may h"rr. sffong
memories but o{y
in language analysis. Learnerr' ,tr.rrgthf
?veraqe-?bilities
and weaknesses in these different componenrs may accounr for their
"Ultity
to succeed in differenr rypes of instructional programs.
In a Canadian language programme for adult learners of French, Marjorie
\wesche (1981) studied
the progress of students who were pl"..d in
instructional programmes that were either compatible or inco-priibl. with
their aptitude profile and information about their learning e"jerien.es. In
the compatible groupings, students who were high o.r
abiliry, but
"rrilytic
average on memory, were assigned to teaching that focused
on grammatical
structures, and learners with good memory but average analytic skills were
placed in a class where the teaching was organized
the functional use
of the second language in specific situations. In the"round
incompatible groupings,
stu4ents_were_placed
in
classes
that did nor correspondto the[
"piir,ri.
profiles. \Tesche reported a high level of student
teacher sati#action
"nd
when students were matched with compatible teaching environments. In
addition, some evidence indicated that matched studenri *.r. able to attain
significantly higher levels of achievement than those who were mismatched.
\rh
able
mo(
Le,
The
habi
info
canl
the
I
learr
learr
learr
Con
cogr
PEN,
sepa
holis
stror
langr
and
ident
Ther
langt
reflec
chan,
resea.
thing
assun
conf
encot
reseat
teach
Befor
is per
Some
rhemr
Lenor
snrdie
trcee(
differ
Ineast
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
'While few
schools could offer such choices to their students, teachers may be
able to ensure that their teaching activities are sufficiently varied to accommodate learners with different aptitude profiles.
Learning styles
The term 'learning style' has been used to describe an individualt natural,
habitual, and preferred way of absorbing, processing, and retaining new
information and skills (Reid 1995). \7e have all heard people say that they
qrnnot learn something until they have seen it. Such learners would fall into
the group called 'visual' learners. Other people, who may be called 'aural'
learners, seem to learn best 'by ear'. For others, referred to as 'kinaesthetic'
learners, physical action such as miming or role-play seems to help the
learning process. These are referred to as perceptually-based learning sryles.
Considerable research has also focused on distinctions between different
cognitive learning sryles. Individuals have been described as FrELD INDEIENDENT or FIELD DEIENDENT, according to whether they tend to
separare details from the general background or tend to see things more
holistically. For a number of years, it was widely reported that there was a
strong relationship between field independence and success in second
language learning. However, a review of the research leads Zoltin Dornyei
and Peter Skehan (2003) to conclude that more research will be needed to
identify the nature of the relationship.
There are many questions about how learning styles interact with success in
language learning. For one thing, it is difficult to determine whether they
refect immutable differences or whether they develop (and thus can be
changed) through experience. There is a need for considerably more
research. Nevertheless, when learners express a preference for seeing somethingwritten or spending more time in a language laboratory,we should not
assume that their ways of working are wrong, even if they seem to be in
confict with the pedagogical approach we have adopted. Instead, we should
encourage learners to use all means available to them. At a minimum,
research on learning sryles should make us sceptical of claims that a single
teaching method or textbook will suit the needs of all learners.
Before we leave the topic of language learning aptitude and learning styles, it
is perhaps appropriate to look at two extremes of the aptitude continuum.
Some people, whose academic performance is usually very good, find
themselves terribly frustrated in their attempts to learn a foreign language.
knore Ganschow and Richard Sparks (2001) and their colleagues have
srudied many cases of young adults who find foreign language learning
exceedingly difficult. They identified several ways in which these students
differ from successful learners. Most perform poorly on at least some of the
measures that make up aptitude tests. Some have problems with certain
59
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
I 665
WAJ'I
wC
of
a4 haue O./,r owL
(eatn;,rg
\
lhr^gr!//
-Er qf)r-
that
r
extro
situa
Anot
sugg(
Progr
prob
Ina
--\]I
S--.r--
supp
langr
effecr
Pron
bette
kinds of verbal skills, even in their own language. \X/hat is perhaps mosr
important about this research is that, with great effort and instructional
support, some of these students are able to succeed in spite of their
difficulties. The challenge is to find instructional approaches that meet the
needs of learners with a variery of aptitude and learning sryl. profiles.
At the other end of the aptitude continuum we find individuals whose
achievements seem to defy every prediction about what is possible in second
language learning. Lorraine Obler (1939) reported on rhe case of one
American man who seemed able to acquire oral fluenry in a new language in
'a matter of weeks'. Neil Smith and Ianthi-Maria Tsimpli (1995) have
followed a polyglot sauantwho learned many languages with apparenr ease.
This achievement was particularly astonishing in light of the fact that his
overall cognitive functioning and social skills were quite limited. Such
exceptional learners suggest that an aptitude for language learning is at least
partly independent of cognitive, social, and personaliry characteristics that
are often associated with successful learning.
Personality
A number ofpersonaliry characteristics have been proposed as likely to affect
second language learning, but it has not been .^/ to demonsffare their
effects in empirical studies. fu with other research investigating the effects of
individual characteristics on second language learning, different studies
measuring a similar personaliry trait produce different results. For example,
it is often argued that an extroverted person is well suited to language learning. However, research does not always support this conclusion. Although
some studies have found that success in language learning is correlated with
learners' scores on questionnaires measuring characteristics associated with
extroversion such as assertiveness and adventurousness, others have found
resulr
ance
of alc
Learr
stude
inves
featu
scaler
Horl
stude
such
classt
fr.t
recen
acknr
partir
made
Prese
in
gr<
secof
Macl
rhe
te
relaxr
Of
cc
arnot
Expe
right
is oftt
other
Indiuidual differences in
second langaage
learning
that many successful language learners do not get high scores on measures of
extroyersion. Lily \fong-Fillmore (L979) found that, in certain learning
situations, the quiet observant learner may have greater success.
Another aspect of personaliry that has been studied is inhibition. It has been
that inhibition discourages risk-taking, which is necessary for
in language learning. This is often considered to be a particular
ptobl.- for adoles..ntr, *ho ar. more self-conscious than yotrng.i learners.
In a series of studies, Alexander Guiora and his colleagues (1972) found
support for the claim that inhibition is a negative force, at least for second
suggested
progress
language pronunciation performance. One study involved an analysis of the
effects ofsmall doses ofalcohol, known for its abiliryto reduce inhibition, on
pronunciation. Study participants who drank small amounts of alcohol did
better on pronunciation tests than those who did not drink any. \7hile
results such as these are interesting, they may have more to do with performance than with learning. \7e may also note, in passing, that when larger doses
of alcohol were administered, pronunciation rapidly deteriorated!
Learner anxiety-feelings of worry, nervousness, and stress that many
students experience when learning a second language-has been extensively
investigated. For a long time, researchers thought of anxiery as a permanent
feature of a learner's personaliry. In fact, the majoriry of language anxiety
scales, like the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (Horwitz,
Horwitz, and Cope 1986) measure anxiety in this way. So, for example,
students are assumed to be 'anxious' if they'strongly agree' with statements
such as 'I become anxious when I have to speak in the second language
classroom'. However, such questionnaire responses do not take account of
the possibiliry that anxiety can be temporaiy attd context-specific. More
recent research investigating learner anxiety in second language classrooms
acknowledges that anxiery is more likely to be dynamic and dependent on
particular situations and circumstances. This permits distinctions to be
made between for example, a student who feels anxious when giving an oral
presentation in front of the whole class but not when interacting with peers
in group-work. \il/hatever the context, anxiety can play an important role in
second language learning if it interferes with the learning process. Peter
Maclntyre (1995) argues that'because anxious studen$ are focused on both
the task at hand and their reactions to it ... [they] will not learn as quickly as
relaxed students' (p. 96).
Of course, it has also been argued that not all anxiety is bad and that a certain
amount of tension can have a positive effect and even facilitate learning.
Experiencing anxiery before a test or an oral presentation can provide the
right combination of motivation and focus to succeed on it. Because anxiety
is often considered to be a negative term, some researchers have chosen to use
other terms they consider to be more neutral. In an ethnographic study
of
6L
62
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
young adults learning French in an intensive summer programme, Guy
Spielmann and Mary Radnofsky (2001) use the term 'tension'. They found
that tension, as experienced by the learners in their study, was perceived as
both beneficial and detrimental and that it was also related to the learners'
social interactions inside and outside the classroom.
A learners willingness to communicate' has also been related to anxiery. \7e
have all experienced occasions when we have gone to great lengths to avoid
communicating in a second/foreign language. This often has to do with the
number ofpeople present, the topic of conversation, and the formaliry of the
circumstances. A colleague in Canada, who works in the area of second
language learning and speaks several languages, recenrly confessed that he
avoided the corner store in his neighbourhood because the proprietor always
spoke French to him. He recognized the proprietort efforts to help him
improve his skills in this new language, and was grateful for it, bur, as he told
us with embarrassment, itwas just easier to go to the storewhere he could use
English. According to some researchers, learners who willingly communicate in a wide range of conversational interactions are able to do so because
'their prior language learning has led to development of self-confidence,
which is based on a lack of anxiety combined with a sufficient level of
communicative competence, arising from a series of reasonably pleasant
fsecond language] experiences' (Maclntyre, Cl6menr, Ddrnyei, and Noels
1998: 548).
Several other personality characteristics such as self-esteem, empathy,
dominance, talkativeness, and responsiveness have also been rt,tdi.d.
Hoyev_er, in general, the available research does not show a single clearlydefined relationship between personality traits and r..orJ hnguage
acquisition. And, as indicated earlier, the major difficulry in investigating
personality characteristics is that of identification and measuremenr.
Another explanation that has been offered for the mixed findings of
personality studies is that personaliryvariables may be a major factor only irt
the acquisition of conversational skills, not in the acquisition of literacy or
academic skills. The confused picture of the research on personaliry factors
may be due in part to the fact that comparisons are made berween studies
that measure communicative abiliry and studies that measure grammatical
accuracy or metalinguistic knowledge. Personaliry variables seem to be
consistently related to the former, but not to the latter. Finally, mosr of the
research on personality variables has been carried our within a
QUANTITATIVE research paradig-, that is, an approach that relies heavily on
measuring learners' scores on personaliry questionnaires and relating these to
language test performance. Some researchers have argued that a more
QUALITATIVE approach to understanding and investigating personaliry
variables is needed to adequately capture their depth and complexity,
especially as they emerge and evolve over time.
Desp
resea
that
la"gi
prob
facto
Mo
Robe
the
rr
langu
(Mas
wheti
enger
AIthc
(::luse
associ
I{oti''
been
comn
langu
range
percei
bem
favou.
rnore
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
Despite the contradictory results and the problems involved in carrying our
in the area of personality characteristics, many researcherr b.li.rr.
that personaliry will be shown to have an important infuence on success in
language learning. This relationship is an intricate one, however, in that it is
probably not personaliry alone, but the way in which it combines with other
factors, that contributes to second language learning.
research
Motiuation and attitudes
Robert Gardner and his colleagues have carried our a program of research on
the relationship between a learner's attitudes toward thJ second or foreign
lan8ua8e and its communiry, and success in second language learniig
(Masgoret and Gardner 2003). As suggested above, it is diFficJt to krroi
whether positive attitudes produce successful learning or successful learning
engenders positive attitudes, or whether both
by other factorsl
"r. "G.ted
Although the research cannot prove that positive
attitudes and motivation
cause success in learning, there is ample evidence that positive motivation is
associated with a willingness ro keep learning.
Motivation in second language learning is a complex phenomenon. It has
been defined in terms of rwo factors: on th. or. hand, learners'
communicative needs, and, on the other, their attitudes towards the second
language community. If learners need to speak the second language in a wide
range of social situations or to fulfil professional ambitions, they will
perceive the communicative value of the second language and will therefore
be motivated to acquire proficiency in it. Likewise, if learners have
favourable attitudes towards the speakers of the language, rhey will desire
more contact with them. Robert Gardner and \(allace Lambert (1972)
63
64
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
coined the terms TNSTRUMENTAL MoTIVATIoN (language learning for
more immediate or practical goals) and INTEGRATIVE MoTIVATIoN
(language learning for personal growth and cultural enrichment). Research
has shown that these types of motivation are related to success in second
language learning, but the distinction is not always as clear as it was in the
research context in which the contrast was first described. In some learning
environmenrs, it is difficult to distinguish between these two types of
orientation to the target language and its communiry. Furthermore, early
research on motivation tended to conceptualizeit as a stable characteristic of
the learner. More recent work emphasizes the dynamic nature of motivation
and tries to account for the changes that take place over time.
ZoltinDornyei (2001a) developed a process-oriented model of motivation
that consists of three phases. The first phase, 'choice motivatiori refers to
getting started and to setting goals, the second phase, 'executive motivation',
is about carrying out the necessary tasks to maintain motivation, and the
third phase, 'motivation retrospection', refers to students' appraisal of and
reaction to their performance. An example of how one might cycle through
these phases would be: a secondary school learner in Poland is excited about
an upcoming trip to Spain and decides to take a Spanish course (choice
motivation). After a few months of grammar lessons he becomes frustrated
with the course, stops going to classes (executive motivation) and finally
decides to drop the course. A week later a friend tells him about a great
Spanish conversation course she is taking, and his 'choice motivation is
activated again. He decides to register in the conversation course and in just
a few weeks he develops some basic Spanish conversational skills and a
feeling of accomplishment. His satisfaction level is so positive (motivation
reffospection) that he decides to enrol in a more advanced Spanish course
when he returns from his trip to Spain.
In a book
Altl
witl
don
woI
whe
StUC
Moi
witl
fort
stuc
Varl
exisr
alw:
to
l<
acti''
inte
Uit
acri\
a tas
self-
ina
tean
Cuh
teacl
com
succ
devoted to helping second language teachers generate and
maintain learners' motivation, Dcirnyei (2001b) proposes and describes
concrete and innovative methods and techniques that can help teachers
motivate learners throughout these three phases.
lang,
Motivation in the classroom
In a teacher's mind, motivated students
relat
grou
and
are usually those who participate
matter, and study a great deal.
interest
in
the
subject
in
class,
express
actively
Teachers also have more infuence on these behaviours and the motivation
they represent than on students' reasons for studying the second language or
their attitudes toward the language and its speakers. Teachers can make a
positive contribution to students' motivation to learn if classrooms are places
that students enjoy coming to because the content is interesting and relevant
to their age and level of ability, the learning goals are challenging yet
manageable and clear, and the atmosphere is supportive.
Ide
Soci,
langr
socie
socia
varia
sensi
Ag,
inter
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
Although little research has directly investigated how ped agogy interacts
with motivation in second language classrooms, considerable work has been
done within the field of educational psychology. In a review of some of this
work, Graham Crookes and Richard Schmidt (1991) point to several areas
where educational research has reported increased levels of motivation for
students in relation to pedagogical practices. Included among these are:
Motiuating students into the lesson At the opening stages of lessons (and
within transitions), it has been observed that remarts teachers make about
forthcoming activities can lead to higher levels of interest on the part of the
students.
Varying the actiuities, tasks, and materials Students are reassured by the
existence of classroom routines they can depend on. However, lessons that
always consist of the same routines, patterns, and formats have been shown
to lead to a decrease in attention and an increase in boredom. Varying the
activities, tasks, and materials can help to avoid this and increase students'
interest levels.
Uing co-operatiue rather than czmpetitiae goak Co-operative learning
activities are those in which students must work together in order to complete
a task or solve a problem. These techniques have been found to increase the
self-confidence of students, including weaker ones, because every participant
in a co-operative task has an important role to play. Knowing that their
team-mates are counting on them can increase students' motivation.
Cultural and age differences will determine the most appropriate way for
teachers to modvate students. In some classrooms, students may thrive on
competitive interaction, while in others, co-operative activities will be more
successful.
Identity and ethnic groap
ffiliation
Social factors at a more general level can affect motivation, attitudes, and
language learning ,.r...rJ. One such factor is the social dynamic or power
relationship benveen the languages. For example, members of a minority
group learning the language of a majority group may have different attitudes
and motivation from those of majority group members learning a minority
language. Even though it is impossible to predict the exact effect of such
societal factors on second language learning, the fact that languages exist in
social contexts cannot be overlooked when we seek to understand the
variables that affect success in learning. Children as well as adults are
sensitive to social dynamics and power relationships.
A good example of how relations of power in the social world affect
interaction between second language learners and target language speakers
65
66
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
comes from the work of Bonny Norton. Drawing from data collected in a
longitudinal case study of the language learning experiences of immigrant
women in Canada, she argues that concepts such as instrumental and
integrative motivation do not adequately caprure the complex relations of
power, identity and language learning. Instead, she uses the term 'invesrment' to 'capture the relationship of the language learner fand his/her
identiryl to the changing social world.' (Norton Peirce 1995:10). All the
participants in her study were highly motivated to learn English. However,
there were social situations in which they were reluctant to speak and these
were rypically ones in which there was a power imbalance. Their experiences
in those situations limited the opportunities they had ro pracrise and to
continue to develop the second language outside the classroom.
Kelleen Toohey (2000) observed that immigrant children in Englishmedium kindergarten classes were quickly assigned identities such as
successful/unsuccessful, big/small, talkative/quiet, erc., in their first year of
school. Of course, rhey also had the identiry of 'being ESL. Because
learners' identities impact on what they can do and how they can participate
in classrooms, this naturally affects how much they can learn. For example,
one of the learners was consistently excluded from imaginative interactive
activities with her peers; another learner was perceived as someone who
never listened or did the 'right thing'. Toohey argues that these identities
could eventually lead to their isolation and to restricted or less powerful
participation in their classroom communiry. \(zhile Toohey is careful to
point out that identities are not static and can change over time, it is equally
important to keep in mind that 'classrooms are organized to provide
occasions upon which some children look more and some less able, and
judgements are made which become social facts about individual children'
(p.77).
Elizabeth Gatbonton, Pavel Thofimovich, and Michael Magid (2005) found a
complex relationship between feelings of ethnic affiliation and second
language learners' mastery ofpronunciation. Among other things, they found
that learners who had achieved a high degree of accuracy in prorrorm.ing the
second language were sometimes perceived as being less loyal to their ethnic
group than those whose second language speech retained a srrong'foreign
accent'. Such perceptions can affect learners' desire to master the r..o.rd
language, especially in contexts where there are conficts berween groups or
where power relationships imply a threat ro one groupt identiry.
Learner beliefi
be
an
rh
do
StI
in
prr
stL
ex(
int
EXI
the
cer
pr(
sev
an
bes
Mc
cor
AIT
Rer
hav
add
CSSC
tea(
enc
sho
pafl
abo
and
Lear
rhei
lea-rr
XTIAT
reP€
$-ayi
Ag
W-e
r
Second language learners are not always aware of their individual cognitive
l,ga.If
or perceptual learning sryles, but virtually all learners, particularly older
ftrs(
learners, have strong beliefs and opinions about horv their instruction should
$llCCr
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
be delivered. These beliefs are usually based on previous learning experiences
and the assumption (right or wrong) that a particular rype of instruction is
the best way for them to learn. This is anothe r areawhere little work has been
done. However, the available research indicates that learner beliefs can be
strong mediating factors in their experience in the classroom. For example,
in a survey of adult international
students
in a communicative
ESL
program, Carlos Yorio (1986) found high levels of dissatisfaction among the
students. The rype of communicative instruction they received focused
exclusively on meaning and spontaneous communication in group-work
interaction. In their responses to a questionnaire, the majoriry of students
expressed concerns about several aspects of their instruction, most notably,
the absence of attention to language form, corrective feedback, or teachercentred instruction. Although this study did not directly examine learners'
progress
in relation to their opinions about the instruction they
received,
several of them were convinced that their progress was negatively affected by
an instructional approach that was not consistent with their beliefs about the
best ways for them to learn.
More recent research on learner beliefs about the role of grammar and
corrective feedback in second language learning confirms that there is often
a mismatch between students' and teachers'views. In rlvo large-scale studies
Renate Schulz (2001) found that virtually all students expressed a desire to
have their errors corrected while very few teachers felt this was desirable. In
addition, while most students believed that'formal study of the language is
essential to the eventual mastery of the language', just over half of the
teachers shared this view. In our own research on learner beliefs and preferences for learning, we are exploring not whether grammatical instrucdon
should be provided but how Learners prefer grammar to be taught. \7e are
particularly interested in exploring whether learners prefer to be taught
about language forms in separate lessons or in lessons where form-focused
and meaning-focused instruction are integrated.
lrarners' instructional preferences, whether due to inherent differences in
their approach to learning or to their beliefs about how languages are
learned, will infuence the kinds of strategies they use in trying to learn new
material. Teachers can use this information to help learners expand their
repertoire of learning strategies and thus develop greater fexibiliry in their
ways of approaching language learning.
Agt of acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis
\(e now turn to a learner characteristic of a different type: the age at which
learning begins. This characteristic is easier to define and measure than
personaliry aptitude, or motivation, but the relationship between age and
success in second language acquisition is hardly less complex or controversial.
67
68
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
It
is frequently observed that most children from immigrant families
eventually speak the language of their new community with NATIVE-LrKE
fuency, while their parents often fall short of such high levels of mastery of
the spoken language. To be sure, there are cases where adult second language
learners have distinguished themselves by their excellent language skills.
One often sees reference to Joseph Conrad, a native speaker of Polish who
became a major writer in the English language. Many adult second language
learners communicate very successfully in the language even though subtle
differences of accent, word choice, or grammatical features distinguish them
from monolingual native speakers and from second language speakers who
began learning the language while they were very young.
It has been hypothesized that there is a critical period for second language
acquisition just as there is for first language acquisition. fu we saw in
Chapter 1, the Critical Period Hypothesis is that there is a time in human
development when the brain is predisposed for success in language learning.
Developmental changes in the brain, it is argued, affect the nature of
language acquisition, and language learning that occurs after the end of the
critical period may not be based on the innate biological structures believed
to contribute to first language acquisition or second language acquisition in
early childhood. Rather, older learners may depend on more general learning
abilities-the same ones they might use to learn other kinds of skills or
information. It is argued that these general learning abilities are not as
effective for language learning as the more specific, innate capacities that are
available to the young child. It is most often claimed that the critical period
ends somewhere around puberry, but some researchers suggest it could be
even earlier.
Of course, as we saw in Chapter 2, it is difficult to compare children and
adults as second language learners. In addition to possible biological
differences suggested by the Critical Period Hypothesis, the conditions for
language learning are often very different. Younger learners in informal
language learning environments usually have more time to devote to
learning language. They often have more opportunities to hear and use the
language in environments where they do not experience strong pressure to
speak fluently and accurately from the very beginning. Furthermore, their
early imperfect efforts are often praised or, at least, accepted. Older learners
are more likely to find themselves in situations that demand more complex
language and the expression of more complicated ideas. Adults are often
embarrassed by their lack of mastery of the language and they may develop a
sense of inadequacy after experiences of frustration in trying to say exactly
what they mean. Such negative feelings may affect their motivation and
willingness to place themselves in situations where they will need to use the
new language.
On
olde
that
learr
lingr
mak
serri
level
rvho
olde
lang
acqr
The
Mos
1*g
thest
notir
si'nt:
sitior
morl
\{arl
acqu
sized
rheir
like
sLrry
Smrt(
tbr
n
nativ
ofEn
The
berw,
begar
Ciscu
;hara
g:ood
secoIl
ipeaft
trstrt
-{ len
rn'ant(
Indiuidual diferences in
second language
learning
On the other hand, some studies of the second language development of
older and younger learners, learning in similar circumstances, have shown
that, at least in the early srages of second language development, older
learners are more efficient than younger learners. By using their metalinguistic knowledge, memory sffaregies, and problem-solving skills, they
make the most of second or foreign language instruction. In educational
settings, learners who begin learning a second language ar primary school
level do not always achieve greater proficiensy in the long run rhan those
who begin in adolescence. Furthermore, there are countless anecdotes about
older learners (adolescents and adults) who achieve excellence in the second
language. Does this mean that there is no critical period for second language
acquisition?
The critical period: More than just accent?
Most studies of the relationship between age of acquisition and second
language development have focused on learners' pronunciation. In general,
these studies have concluded that older learners almost inevitably-have a
noticeable 'foreign accent'. But what about other linguistic features? Is
syntax (word order, overall sentence structure) as dependent on age of acquisition as phonological developmenr? \fhat about morphology (grammatical
morphemes that markverb tense or the number and gender of nouns)?
Mark Patkowski (1980) studied the relationship berween age and the
acquisition of features of a second language other than accenr. He hypothesized that, even if accent were ignored, only those who had begun learning
their second language before the age of fifteen could ever achie,r. f*ll, native-
like mastery of that language. Patkowski recorded the spoken English of
sixty-seven highly educated immigrants to the United States. Tlrey had
started to learn English at various ages, but all had lived in the United Srates
for more than five years. He also recorded the spoken English of fifteen
native-born Americans from a similarly high level of education. Their variery
ofEnglish could be considered the second language speakers'rargff language.
The main question in Patkowski's research was: '\fill there be a difference
between learners who began to learn English before puberry and those who
began learning English later?' However, in the light of some of the issues
discussed above, he also compared learners on the basis of other
characteristics and experiences that some people have suggested might be as
good as age in predicting or explaining a person's success in mast ering a
second language. For example, he looked at the total amounr of timi a
speaker had been in the United States as well as the amounr of formal ESL
instruction each speaker had had.
A lengthy interviewwith each person was tape-recorded. Because Patkowski
wanted to remove the possibiliry that the results would be affected by accent,
69
70
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
he did not ask the raters to judge the tape-recorded interviews themselves.
Instead, he transcribed five-minute samples from the interviews and
removed from them any identifying or revealing commenrs about
immigration history or language background. These transcribed samples
were rated by trained native-speaker judges. They were asked to place each
speaker on a scale from 0, representing no knowledge of the language, to 5,
representing a level of English expected from an educated native speaker.
b,
a
\Sl
Nr
The findings were quite dramatic. All native speakers and thirry-mo out of
thirry-three second language speakers who had begun learning English
before the age of fifteen were rated 4+ or 5. The homogeneity of the prepuberry learners seemed to suggest that, for this group, success in learning a
second language was almost inevitable (see Figure 3.1).In contrasr, the
majority of the post-puberty group were rated around the 3+ level, but there
was a great deal ofvariation. The performance of this group looked more like
the sort of range one would expect if one were measuring success in learning
almost any kind ofskill or knowledge: some people did extremelywell; some
did poorly; most were in the middle.
\
\When Patkowski examined the other factors that might
be thought to affect
success in second language acquisition, the picture was much less clear.
There was, naturally, some relationship between those factors and learning
S1
\\r
.u
success. However, it often turned out that age was so closely related to the
other factors that it was not really possible to separate them completely. For
example, length of residence in the United States somerimes seemed to be a
fairly good predictor. However, while it was true thar a person who had lived
in the country for fifteen years might speak better than one who had been
there for only ten years, it was often the case that the one with longer
residence had also arrived at an earlier age. Similarly, amounr of instruction,
when separated from age, did not predict success to the exrent that age of
immigration did. Thus, Patkowski found that age of acquisition is a very
important factor in setting limits on the development of native-like mastery
of a second language and that this limitation does not apply only ro accenr.
These results gave added support to the Critical Period Hypothesis for
second language acquisition.
>.
\
Figur
heard
COff€r
Johns
signi{
the
s:
€xpos
arrive
Intuitions of grammatic.lity
Jacqueline Johnson and Elissa Newport (1989) conducted a study of forrysix Chinese and I{orean speakers who had begun to learn English at different
ages. AII were students or faculry members at an American universiry and all
had been in the United States for at least three y€ars. The study also included
a comparison group of twenry-three narive
SJ
speakers
of English.
The
participants were asked to judge the grammaticaliry of a large number of
sentences that tested twelve rules of English morphology and syntax. They
-Johns
highe
native
rvidel'
Rober
Newp
He ak
langur
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learnirug
-,
\
s10
\
b,
\
N
$10
d.,
2+33+44+5
Post-puberty learners
Figure
3.1 Number of speakers at each profciency rating (Patkowski 1950)
heard sentences on a tape and had to indicate whether each sentence was
correct. Half of the sentences were grammatical, halfwere not.
Johnson and Newport found that age of arrival in the United States was a
significant predictor of success on the test. They grouped the participants in
the same way as Patkowski, comparing those who began their intensive
exposure to English between the ages of three and fifteen with those who
arrived in the United States between the ages of seventeen and thirry-nine.
Johnson and Newport found that learners who began earliest achieved the
highest scores on the judgement task. Those who began later did not have
native-like language abilities and their performance on the test varied more
widely.
Robert DeKeyser (2000) carried out a replication of the Johnson and
Newport study, working with Hungarian immigrants to the United States.
He also found a strong relationship benveen age of immigration and second
language proficiency. An aspect of his study that makes it particularly
7r
72
Indiuidual dffirences
iru second language
learning
valuable is that, in addition to examining their judgements of grammaticality, he asked participants to take language aptitude tests. He found
that, for participants who began learning English as adults, aptitude scores
were correlated with success. However, there was no such correlation for
those who learned English in childhood. These findings appear to confirm
the hypothesis that adult learners may learn language in a way that is
different from the way children learn.
ler
th
su
co
pa
m:
in
aca
Rate of learning
Ar
Some research suggests that older learners may have one important
advantage: they appear to learn faster in the early stages of second language
learning. In 1978, Catherine Snow and Marian Hoefnagel-Hohle published
an article based on a research project they carried out in Holland. They
studied the progress of a group of English speakers who were learning Dutch
as a second language. The learners they were following included children as
young as three years old as well as older children, adolescents, and adults.
Furthermore, they used a large number of tasks to measure different types of
language use and language knowledge. They assessed pronunciation,
AUDTTORY DTSCRTMTNATTON, grammatical morphemes, grammatical
complexiry sentence translation, grammaticaliry judgement, vocabulary,
story comprehension and storytelling.
Mr
tha
ist
atti
eSSt
rhe
ne(
Th
acq
Ptro
con
Participants were first tested within six months of their arrival in Holland
and within six weeks of their starting school or work in a Dutch-language
environment. They were tested two more times at four- or five-month
intervals. The Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle study found that adolescents
were by far the most successful learners. They were ahead of everyone on all
but one of the tests (pronunciation) on the first test session. Surprisingly, it
was the adults, not the children, whose scores were second best on the other
tests at the first test session. In other words, adolescents and adults learned
faster than children in the first few monrhs of exposure to Dutch.
Spei
rhe,
airv
recc
inst
5rur
5€CC
goir
:-me.
By the end of th e year, the children were catching up, or had surpassed, the
adults on several measures. Nevertheless, it was the adolescents who retained
the highest levels of performance overall.
Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle concluded that their results provide evidence
against the critical period for language acquisition. However, other researchers have interpreted the results differently. Some of the poor performance ofyounger learners could be accounted for by the fact that some of the
tasks, (for example, sentence judgement or translation) were too hard for
young learners. In fact, young Dutch native speakers with whom the second
language learners were compared also had trouble with these tasks. Snow and
Hoefnagel-Hohle's study shows that adults and adolescents learned faster in
the first year of second language development. This may be because theywere
--kel
:nr-i
:1as
S0al
5rs
ieco
;hilc
:enir
:elat
]r$d_[L
3it
en
Indiuidual dffirences in
learning
a
second language
learning
language that is very similar to the one they already knew. Even so,
the young children were catching up and evidence from other studies
suggests that they would probably surpass the older learners if they
continued to have adequate opportunity to use the language. The study is
particularly valuable in showing, however, that adults and adolescents can
make considerable and rapid progress towards mastery of a second language
in contexts where they use the language in social, personal, professional, or
academic interaction.
At what age should second language instruction begin?
Many people who have never heard of the critical period hypothesis believe
that, in school programmes for second or foreign language teaching, younger
is better'. However, both experience and research show that older learners can
attain high levels of proficiency in their second language. Furthermore, it is
essential to think carefully about the goals of an instructional programme and
the context in which it occurs before we jump to conclusions about the
necessity-or even the desirabiliry-of the earliest possible start.
There is strong evidence that there are maturational constraints on language
It is also the case that reaching high levels of second language
acquisition.
proficiency involves aptitude, motivation, and the appropriate social
conditions for learning. Some researchers argue that older learners may well
speak with an accent because they want to continue being identified with
their first language cultural group. \(e have also seen that adults do not
always get the same quantiry and qualiry of language input that children
receive in school and play settings. Thus, decisions about the age at which
instruction should begin cannot be based solely on evidence for the CPH.
Studies such as those by Patkowski or Newport and Johnson dealt with
second language speakers who had spent many years living, working, and
going to school in the second language environment. They found that, even
after twenty years, only those who had had an early start had a high
likelihood of being indistinguishable from people who had been born in that
environment. It is important to acknowledge that achieving native-like
mastery of the second language is neither a realistic nor necessarily a desired
goal for second language learners in many educational contexts. The study
by Snow and Hoefnagel-Hohle dealt with the achievement of a variery of
second language skills after a few months. They found that it was the older
children and adolescents who had made the most progress in that time
period. The kinds of skills the older learners were able to acquire in a
relatively short period of time will satisfy the needs of learners in many
learning contexts where the goal is the abiliry to use the language for
everyday communication rather than native-like mastery.
73
74
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language learning
\When the objective of second language learning is native-like mastery of the
target language, it may indeed be desirable for the learner to be completely
surrounded by the language as early as possible. However, as we saw in
or thr
Chapter 1, early intensive exposure to the second language may entail the
childt first language.
alway
loss or incomplete development of the
to ch
on in
and
r
fact
t
\7hen the goal is basic communicative abiliry for all students in an
educational system, and when it is assumed that the child's native language
varial
will remain the primary language, it may be more efficient to begin second or
foreign language teaching later. \7hen learners receive only a few hours of
ways,
instruction per week, learners who start later (for example, at age ten, eleven,
or twelve) often catch up with those who began earlier. Some second or
foreign language programmes that begin with very young learners but offer
only minimal contact with the language do not lead to much progress. In
Clare Burstallt (1975) landmark study, students who had made progress in
early-start programmes, sometimes found themselves placed in secondary
school classes with students who had had no previous instruction. Teachers
tended to teach to a lower common denominator. This situation is not at all
uncommon. Thus, after years of classes, learners who have had an early start
may feel frustrated by the lack of progress, and their motivation to continue
may be diminished. Clearly the age at which instruction begins is not the
only variable that determines success in the second language classroom.
Decisions about when to start second language programmes in schools
should be based on realistic estimates of how long it takes to learn a second
language. One or two hours a week will not produce advanced second
language speakers, no matter how young they were when they began. Older
learners may be able to make better use of the limited time they have for
second language instruction.
rhat i
interr
chara
the lr
indiv
rvhicl
Sot
Breer
Diret
Diirr
Diffe
andl
Diirr
langr
afSet
Gans
Age is one of the characteristics that determine the way in which an
individual approaches second language learning. But the opportunities for
learning (both inside and outside the classroom), the motivation to learn,
and individual differences in aptitude for language learning are also
important determining factors that affect both rate of learning and eventual
success in learning. It is useful to look back at the graphic representation of
Patkowskit research and to remind ourselves that some older learners do
langr-
achieve the highest level of success.
Basic
3412:
Garc
rhe
I
\{att
Gard
Hon
Resea
Summary
Look back at the notes you took about your language learning experience
and that of your colleagues and friends. You will probably find some cases
that confirm hypotheses about what variables are associated with success-
Hy'tr
SLA'
Lang
Indiuidual dffirences in
second language
learning
it-in
second language learning. You may find others that seem
m challenge those hypotheses. In this chapter, we have learned that research
on individual differences is complex and that the results of research are not
always easy to interpret. This is partly because of the lack of clear definitions
or the lack of
and methods for measuring individual characteristics. It is also due to the
frct that the characteristics are not independent of one another: learner
uariables interact in complex ways. The complexity grows when we realize
fiat individual learners will react to different learning conditions in different
ways. Researchers are beginning to explore the nature of these complex
interactions, but it remains difficult to predict how a particular individualt
characteristics will influence his or her success as a language learner. None
the less, in a classroom, the goal of the sensitive teacher is to take learners'
individual differences into account and to create a learning environment in
which more learners can be successful in learning a second language.
Sources and suggestions for further readitg
Breen, M. P. (.d.). 200I. Learner Corutributions to Language Learning: Netu
Directions in Research.Harlow, UK: Pearson Education.
Donyei,Z. 2005. The Psychologt of the
Dffirences in
Language Learner: Indiuidual
Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
andfusociates.
Dornyei,
Z. and P. Skehan .
2003. 'Individual differences in second
learning' in C. J. Doughry and M. H. Long (eds.): The Handbook
language
ofSecond Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blaclaryell. pp. 589-530.
Ganschow, L. and R. L. Sparls. 2001. 'Learning difficulties and foreign
language learning: A review of research and instructioi. Language Teaching
3412:79-98.
Garcia Mayo, M. del P. and M. L. Garcia Lecumberri (eds.). 2003. Age and
tlte Acquisition of English as a Foreign Language. Clevedon: Multilingual
Matters.
Gardner,
H. 1993. Muhiple
Intelligences: The Theory in Practice. NewYork:
Basic Books.
Horwitz, E. K. and D. J. Young. L991. Language Anxiety: From
Theory and
fusearch to Classroom Implications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hyltenstam, K. and N. Abrahamsson. 2003.'Maturational constraints in
SLA' in C. J. Doughry and M. H. Long (eds.): The Handbook of Second
Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 540-88.
75
76
Indiuidual dffirences in
Masgoret,
second language
learning
A.-M. and R. C. Gardner. 2003. Attitudes, modvation,
and
second language learning: A meta-analysis of studies conducted by Gardner
and associates'. Language Learning53l 1: 123-63.
Norton, B. 2000. Identity and Language Learning: Gender Ethnicity and
Educati o nal C h ange. London : Longman/ Pearson Education.
Oxford, R. 1990. Language Learning Strategies: What Euery
Know. NewYork Newbury House.
Teacher Should
Robinson, P. (ed.). 2002. Indiuidual Dffirences and lrustructed Language
Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Singleton, D. and L. Ryan. 2004. LanguageAcquisition: TheAge Factor2nd
edn. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Toohq', K. 2000. Learning English at School: Identity, Social Relations and
C lassroom Practice. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
In
tl
and
mak
langr
sequ
rhe
s
ofvc
Stu
lan
Knor
essesl
accot
l*rg,
rhe st
Io pt
inclu
addit
langt,
ro de
dosel
alwal
nefec
nD€fllr
abilir
nith
indic
I"rgu
comn
That
LEARNER LANGUAGE
ln this chapter we focus on second language learners' developing knowledge
and use of their new language. \Wb examine some of the errors that learners
make and discuss what errors can tell us about their knowledge of the
language and their ability to use that knowledge. \7e look at stages and
sequences in the acquisition of some syntactic and morphological features in
the second language. \7e also review some aspects of learners' development
of vocabulary, pragmatics, and phonology.
Studying the language of second
language learners
Knowing more about the development of learner language helps teachers to
teaching procedures in the light ofwhat they can reasonably expect to
accomplish in the classroom. As we will see, some characteristics of learner
language can be quite perplexing if one does not have an overall picture of
the steps learners go through in acquiring features of the second language.
assess
In presenting some of the findings of second language research, we have
included a number of examples of learner language as wefl as some
additional samples to give you an opportuniry to pracdse analysing learner
language. Of course, teachers analyse learner language all the time. They try
to determine whether students have learned what has been taught and how
closely their language matches the target language. But progress cannot
always be measured in these terms. Sometimes language acquisition is
refected in a decrease in the use of a correct form that was based on rote
memorization or chunk learning. New errors may be based on an emerging
ability to extend a particular grammatical form beyond the specific items
with which it was first learned. In this sense, an increase in error may be an
indication of progress. For example, like first language learners, second
language learners usually learn the irregular past tense forms of certain
common verbs before they learn to apply the regular simple past -edmarker.
That means that a learner who says 'I buyed a bus ticket' may know more
78
Learner language
about English grammar than one who says 'I bought a bus ticket'. The one
who says 'buyed' knows a rule for forming the past tense and has applied it to
an irregular verb. \Tithout further information, we cannot conclude that the
one who says 'bought' would use the regular past -ed marker where it is
appropriate, but the learner who says 'buyed' has provided evidence of
developing knowledge of a systematic aspect of English. Teachers and
researchers cannot read learners' minds, so they must infer what learners
know by observing what they do. We observe their spontaneous language
use, but we also design procedures that help to reveal more about the
knowledge underlying their observable use of language. \Tithout these
procedures, it is often difficult to determine whether a particular behaviour
is representative of something systematic in a learnert current language
knowledge or simply an isolated item, learned as a chunk.
*re rt
,=,
@
rerms
errofl
dr;;
[erns
ffit
a
r€gt
\.-g_
-{ sim
errors
Englir
[ingui
case.
Like first language learners, second language learners do not learn language
simply through imitation and practice. They produce sentences that are not
exactly like those they have heard. These new sentences appear to be based
on internal cognitive processes and prior knowledge that interact with the
language they hear around them. Both first and second language acquisition
are best described as developing systems with their own evolving rules and
patterns, not as imperfect versions of the target language.
In Chapter I we saw that children's knowledge of the grammatical system is
built up in predictable sequences. For instance, grammatical morphemes
such as the -ingof the present progressive or the -edof the simple pasr are nor
acquired at the same time, but in sequence. Furthermore, the acquisition of
certain grammatical features is similar for children in different environments. As children continue to hear and use their language, they are able to
revise these systems so that they increasingly resemble the language spoken
in their environment. Are there developmental sequences for second
language acquisition? How does the prior knowledge of the first language
affect the acquisition of the second (or third) language? How does instruc-
tion affect second language acquisition? Are there differences
berween
learners whose only contact with the new language is in a language course
and those who use the language in daily life? These are some of the questions
researchers have sought to answer, and we will address them in this chapter as
well as in Chapters 5 and 5.
come
direct
literal
rhe ve
predi<
rhien
rnight
Iearnir
speake
Iearnir
u-ord
t
Eenera
er-iden
r
erb.
I
seeing
rend tc
Eric K
about't
Cre tarl
most
l,
sirnply
-{s a res
C ontrastiae analys is, error analys is,
and interlanguage
Until the late 1950s, people tended to see second language learners' speech
simply as an incorrect version of the target language. According to the
Contrastive Analysis Hypothesis (CAH), errors were often assumed to be
splain
ipProa(
during
,iescrip
nake. I
aircut tl
*hen
lt
Learner
,
Se result of t
all errors made
terms of first
can
structure
fu
language
in Chapter 2,
can be exolained in
we saw
ners
second
one. A number of studies show that
r ln terms ot leafners
of
ryny
than an'atternpt to t
f
t-
ir first
first
learners
onanl
A simplified version of the CAH would predict that, where differences exist,
eirors would be bi-directional, that is, for example, French speakers learning
English and English speakers learning French would make errors on pardlel
linguistic features. Helmut Zobl (1980) observed that this is not always the
case. For example, in English, direct objects, whether nouns or pronouns,
come after the verb ('The dog eats the cookie. The dog eats it.'). In French,
direct objects that are nouns follow the verb (Le chien mange le biscuitliterally, 'The dog eats the cookie'). However, direct object pronouns precede
the verb (Le chien le mangAiterally, 'The dog it eats'). The CAH would
predict that a native speaker of English might make the error of sayrng: Le
chien mange le when learning French, and that a native speaker of French
might say'The dog it ate' when learning English. In fact, English speakers
learning French are more likely to make the predicted error than French
speakers learning English. This may be due to the fact that English speakers
learning French hear many examples of sentences with subject-verb-object
word order (for example, Le chien mange le biscuit) and make the incorrect
generdization-based on both the word order of their first language and
evidence from the second language-that all direct objects come after the
verb. French-speaking learners of English, on the other hand, hearing and
seeing no evidence that English direct object pronouns precede verbs, do
tend to use this pattern from their first language.
not
Eric Kellerman (1986) and others also observed that learners have intuitions
about which language features they can transfer from their first language to
the target language and which are less likely to be transferable. For example,
most learners believe that idiomatic or metaphorical expressions cannot
simply be translated word for word.
As a result ofthe finding that manyaspects oflearners'language could not be
explained by the CAH, a number of researchers began to take a different
to analysing learners' errors. This approach, which developed
during the 1970s, became known as 'error analysis' and involved detailed
description and analysis of the kinds of errors second language learners
make. The goal of this research \{ras to discover what learners really know
about the language. As Pit Corder said in a famous article published in lg67 ,
when learners produce 'correct' sentences, th.y may simply be repeating
approach
79
Learner language
something they have already heard; when they produce sentences that differ
from the target language, we may assume that these sentences refect the
learners' current understanding of the rules and patterns of that language.
'Error analysis' differed from contrastive analysis in that it did not set out to
predict errors. Rather, it sought to discover and describe different kinds of
errors in an effort to understand how learners process second language data.
Error analysis was based on the hypothesis that, like child language, second
language learner language is a system in its own right-one that is rulegoverned and predictable.
Larry Selinker (1972) gave the name INTERLANGUAGE to learners'
developing second language knowledge. Analysis of a learner's interlanguage
shows that it has some characteristics influenced by previously learned
languages, some characteristics of the second language, and some characteristics, such as the omission of function words and grammatical morphemes,
that seem to be general and to occur in all or most interlanguage systems.
Interlanguages have been found to be systematic, but they are also dynamic,
continually evolving as learners receive more input and revise their hypotheses about the second language. The path through language acquisition is
not necessarily smooth and even. Learners have bursts ofprogress, then seem
to reach a plateau for a while before something stimulates further progress.
Selinker also coined the term FossILIzATIoN to refer to the fact that, some
features in a learnert language may stop changing. This may be especially
true for learners whose exposure to the second language does not include
instruction or the kind of feedback that would help them to recognize
differences between their interlanguage and the target language.
Pe
bc
fir
Lts
CI
m(
sul
hv
erI
rat
a-re
Analysing learner language
Io
The following texts were written by mo learners of English, one a Frenchspeaking secondary school student, the other a Chinese-speaking adult
learner. Both learners were describing a cartoon film entitled The Great To!
Robbery (National Film Board of Canada). After viewing the film, they were
asked to retell the story in writing, as if they were telling it to someone who
had not seen the film.
dn
Read the texts and examine the errors made by each learner. Do they make
the same kinds of errors? In what ways do the two interlanguages differ?
Learner 1: French
frst
language, secondary school student
sunny day, acowboy go in the desert with his horse. he has a
His
horse eat a flour. In the same time, Santa Clause go in aciq,
big hat.
to give some surprises. He has a red costume and a red packet of
surprises. You have three robbers in the mountain who sees Santa
Clause with a king of glaces that it permitted us to see at a long
distance. Every robbers have a horse. They go in the way of Santa
During
a
i-N(
on
5r\
har
Cr
ert
0P(
hac
io\\
For
Fre
irn
5irr
jrJn
Learner language
Clause, not Santa Clause but his pocket of surprises. After
in a city and they go in a saloon. [...]
(unpublished data from
P.
theywill go
M. Lightbown and B. Barkman)
Learner 2: Chinesef.rst language, aduh
This year Christmas comes soon! Santa Claus ride a one horse open
sleigh to sent present for children. on the back of his body has big
packet. it have a lot of toys. in the way he meet three robbers. They
want to take his big packet. Santa Claus no way and no body help, so
only a way give them, then three robbers ride their horse dashing
through the town. There have saloon, they go to drink some beer and
open the big packent. They plays toys in the Bar. They meet a cow boy
in the saloon.
(unpublished data provided by M. J. Martens)
the most striking thing here is that many error rypes are common to
learners. Both make errors of spelling and puncruarion that we might
in the writing of ayoung native speaker of English. Even though French
grammatical morphemes to indicate person and number on verbs and
not, both these learners make errors of subject-verb agreeboth leaving off the third person +marker and overusing it when the
ject is plural ('a cowboy go' and three robbers in the mountain who sees'
lrarner 1 and 'Santa Claus ride' and 'they plays' by Learner 2). Such
reflect learners' understanding of the second language system itself
than an attempt to transfer characteristics of their first language. They
sometimes referred to as 'developmental' errors because they are similar
those made by children acquiring English as their first language. Somethese are errors of overgeneralization, that is, errors caused by trying to
a rule in a context where it does not belong, for example, the -s ending
the verb in 'they plays'. Sometimes the errors are better described as
MpLIFICATIoN, where elements of a sentence are left out orwhere all verbs
inese does
the same form regardless of person, number, or tense.
can also see, especially in Learner 2t text, the infuence of classroom
rience. An example is the use of formulaic expressions such as 'one horse
sleigh which is taken verbatim from awell-known Christmas song that
been taught and sung in his ESL class. The vivid 'dashing through the
'probably comes from the same source.
For those who are familiar with the English spoken by native speakers of
rench, some of the errors (for example, preposition choice 'in the same
time') made by the first learner will be seen as probably based on French.
Similarly, those familiar with the English of Chinese speakers may recognize
some word order patterns (for example, 'on the back of his body has big
8l
82
Learner language
packet') as based on Chinese pafferns. These are called transfer or 'interference' errors. \(hat is most cleat however, is that it is often difficult to
determine the source of errors. Thus, while error analysis has the advantage
of describing what learners actually do rather than what they might do, it
does not always give us clear insights into why they do it. Furthermore, as
Jacquelyn Schachter pointed out in a 1974 article, learners sometimes avoid
using certain features of language that they perceiye to be difficult for them.
This avoidance may lead to the absence of certain errors, leaving the analyst
without information about the learners' developing interlanguage. That is,
the absence of particular errors is difficult to interpret. The phenomenon of
'avoidance' may itself be a part of the learnert systematic second language
performance.
Gra
Some
by lea
differe
each l,
each r
necess
senten
creates
the rec
{wasp
'hours'
pheme
plural,
Developmental sequences
Second language learners, like first language learners, pass through
sequences of development: what is learned early by one is learned early by
the nu
supplie
supplie
others.
rhe qu
Among first language learners, the existence of developmental sequences
may not seem surprising because their language learning is partly tied to
their cognitive development and to their experiences in learning about
relationships among people, events, and objects around them. But the
cognitive development of adult or adolescent second language learners is
much more stable, and their experiences with the language are likely to be
quite different, not only from the experiences of a little child, but also
different from each other. Furthermore, second language learners already
know another language that has different patterns for creating sentences and
word forms. In light of this, it is more remarkable that we find developmental sequences that are similar in the developing interlanguage of learners
from different backgrounds and also similar to those observed in first
language acquisition of the same language. Moreovet the features of the
language that are heard most frequently are not always easiest to learn. For
example, virtually every English sentence has one or more articles ('d or
'the'), but even advanced learners have difficulty using these forms correcdy
in all contexts. Finally, although the learners' first language does have an
infuence, many aspects of these developmental stages are similar among
learners from many different first language backgrounds.
accura(
In Chapter 1 we saw some developmental
trom hl
The or
identic:
nas sin
backgrr
for plu
Stepher
should
in high
ni*rin I
The sin
describe
end son
a
rloror
l,sarners'
crample
ffie Eng
Fossessi
for English child
tfurming
language acquisition of grammatical morphemes, negation, and questionsResearchers in second language acquisition have also examined these
features, as well as others.
flFPears
Lilncludir
sequences
rupect ol
rum
suppl
Learner language
Grammatical moryhemes
some studies have examined the.development of
grammatical morphemes
by learners of English as a second.l*g,r"g. in a
varieq, of environmenrs, ar
different ages, and from different.
f ^; Fr.""g. u*kjro urrJr. l' anarysing
eacf learner's speech, researchers identify
Ih.i"rrcAToRy coNTEXTs for
each morphem:, ,h.", is, the places in a sentence
where the morpheme is
necessary to make the sentence grammatically
correct. For exampie, in the
sentence 'Yesterday I play baseball for nvo lro.rrr,,
the adverb yest.rday'
creates an obligatory context for a past tense,
and for *. h""tritells us that
se required form is a.simpl.- p"ri (played'j rather than a pasr
prog..rrirr.
Similldl, '*,1'
.r.",., an obrigarory
[::tt"lt_"g').
aours'. For the analysir, obligatory
conr.", io, pr,rr"l -ron
"
gr"--Jtical
morseparat.tylth"t is, one .our,, f", ,fi;l.;;;, one for
*-.:.al, one
T: for
:"lted
third person singular present rense, and so or. aia.,
counting
.ont."tr,
,99."1i1,9r counrs the correctly
:blttiiry
.th.
Ieq morehemes., The next step is to divide the number of .orrJJ#
lied morphemes by the total rumb., of oblig";ory
conrexrs ro answer
question 'what ir ,,h^. perc€nrage accuracy 6r .".h
An
vaq score is created for each morpheme, and these."r,-"rph;;;i,
th* be ranked
.o.rr.*L for
each
ilTY :l
highest to lowest, giving
similariry
"n
o".r*cy
oRDEn for the
morpil;.
learners suggesrs that the accuraq./ order
cannot be
of transfer from the learners, firsr f""g""g.,
:T:::-.:p*::1_T-1.'-s
some
researchers saw this as sffong evidence
,i. -AFi:
"g"irrr,
orough review of all the_'morpheme acquisition'
studies shows that the
i first language does have an infuence
on acquisition sequences. For
:, learners whose_first language has a possessive
"f:"q
;{.il;?;
inio",rirh)
,..-
form that resembles
rTL'lLD
to acquire the English
3*.:i:,!._*:r-,Germ1n
ssive earlier than those whose first ranguage
has ;".{r;]ff;;,
*i;;;
y*-., r::sessive (s uch as Fren ch orspitriih ) A"d.r..r, rio uil'.rrri.r.,
early
il ,!. sequence, learners fri*
.
many ranguage u"&gro.rrra,
'
languages and
.o'ti'L.
to'rrr",rggle #th this
1.g_S^?"jc
{g""..r.2
ofEnglish,
.rr.n
l.rr.rr. For examfi., t."rrrers may do weil
"drr"'..i
plyrng articles in"t certain
obligatory conrexrs but not others. If the
83
84
Irarnr
knguage
Salier
exam
-ing(progressive)
plural
copula ('to be')
Paren
frequ,
Neg
The a
path
auxiliary (progressive
as in'He is going')
article
r
langu
langu
This
speak
irregular past
speak,
Stage
The n
or tht
regular past -ed
third person singular
Sentel
-s
possessive's
l
"No'ir
Figure
4.1
Krashenls (1977) summdryt ofsecond language grammatical
mo rp h eme acquis
negati
ExPOS(
itio n s e quence
corres
language sample
that is analysed contains only
the 'easier' obligatory
contexts, the learner may have a misleadingly high accuracy score. Another
reason why something as difficult as English articles appears to be acquired
early is that the order in the diagram is based on the analysis of correct use in
obligatory contexts only. It does not take into account uses of grammatical
morphemes in places where they do notbelong, for example, when a learner
says, 'The France is in Europe'. These issues have led researchers to question
the adequacy of obligatory context analyses as the sole basis for understanding developmental sequences.
The morpheme acquisition literature raises other issues, not least of them
the question of why there should be an order of acquisition for these
language features. Some of the similarities observed in different studies
seemed to be due to the use of particular tasks for collecting the data, and
researchers found that different tasks tended to yield different results.
Nevertheless, a number of studies have revealed similarities that cannot be
explained by the data collection procedures alone. As with first language
acquisition, researchers have not found a single simple explanation for the
order. Jennifer Goldschneider and Robert DeKeyser (2001) reviewed this
research and identified a number of variables that contribute to the order.
libros)
becaur
d.y
p
Sog.
simila
particr
Stage.
-\r thir
marke
like'ce
I
kage:
Learnr
-is',
an
]
At
thir
places
Framerl
Learner language
Salience (how easy it is to noti ce the moqpheme),linguistic complexiry ffor
aample, how many elements you have to keep track of), semantic transtrnrency (how clear the meaning is), similarity to a first language form, and
frequency in the input all seem to play a role.
Negation
The acquisition of negative sentences by second language learners follows a
path that looks nearly identical to the stages we saw in Chapter 1 for first
bnguage acquisition. However, second language learners from different first
hnguage backgrounds behave somewhat differently within those stages.
This was illustrated in John Schumannt (1979) research with Spanish
qreakers learning English and Henning \fodet (1978) work on German
qreakers learning English.
fuge
I
The negative element (usually'no' or 'not') is typically placed before the verb
or the element being negated. Often, it occurs as the first word in the
rntence because the subject is not there.
No birycle. I no like it. Not my friend.
No' is preferred by most learners in this early stage, perhaps because it is the
negative form that is easiest to hear and reco gnrze in the speech they are
aposed to. Italian- and Spanish-speaking learners may prefer 'no' because it
q)rresponds to the negative form in Italian and Spanish (/Va tienen mucltos
Iibrol.They may continue to use Stage I negation longer than other learners
because of the similarity to a pattern from their first language. Even when
fiey produce negative sentences at more advanced stages, they may also use
Smge 1 negatives in longer sentences or when they are under pressure. Thus,
similarity to the first language may slow down a learnert progress through a
particular developmental stage.
fuge2
At this stage, 'no' and 'not' may alternate with 'dont'. However, 'dont' is not
marked for person, number, or tense and it may even be used before modals
like'cart' and'should'.
He dont like it. I dont can sing.
tuge3
to place the negative element after auxiliary verbs like 'are',
'is', and'can'. But at this stage, the'dont' form is still not fully analysed:
Lea.rners begin
You can not go there. He was not happy. She dont like rice.
At this
stage, German speakers, whose first language has a structure that
places the negative aftei the verb may generalize the auxiliary-negative
Frffern to verb-negative and produce sentences such as:
85
86
Learner language
They come not [to] home. (Sie kommen nicht nach Hause)
4
In this stage, 'do' is marked for tense, person, and number, and most
Stage
interlanguage sentences appear to be just like those of the target language:
It doesnt work. \7e didnt have supper.
However, some learners continue to mark tense, person, and number on
both the auxiliary andthe verb:
Fren<
decla
becor
Fren<
conti
Stagt
Inver
I didnt went there.
At
Qaestions
In the 1980s, Manfred Pienemann and his colleagues undertook studies that
related the second language acquisition of German and English. Pienemann,
Johnston, and Brindley (198S) described a sequence in the acquisition of
questions by learners of English from avariety of first language backgrounds.
An adapted version of the sequence is shown in Stages 1-6 below. The
examples come from French speakers who were playing a game in which they
had to ask questions in order to find out which picture the other player was
holding. fu we saw for negation, the overall sequence is similar to the one
observed in first language acquisition. And again, there are some differences
that are attributable to first language influence.
Suge
I
Str
inverr
leadir
baseb
Suge
Inver
Frenc
whicl
as gra
chocc
Single words, formulae, or sentence fragments.
matic
cltoco,
Dog?
Four children?
Stage
2
Snge
Coml
Declarative word order, no inversion, no fronting.
Itt
a monster
in the right corner?
The boys throw the shoes?
Declarative order with rising intonation is common in yes/no questions in
informal spoken French. French speakers may hypothesize that in English,
as in French, inversion is optional.
Stage
3
Fronting: do-ftonting; wh-fronting, no inversion; other fronting.
Do you have a shoes on your picture?
tVhere the children are playing?
Does in this picture there is four astronauts?
Is the picture has two planets on top?
Piene
numt
Mack
provir
Japan
a nati
simila
n'ork
interl,
rhat l<
Learner language
French has an invariant form 'est-ce que' that can be placed before a
tive sentence to make a question, for example, Jean aime le cindma
Est-ce que Jean aime le cinema?-'[is it that] John likes movies?'
speakers may think that 'do' or'does' is such an invariant form and
tinue to produce Stage 3 questions for some time.
fuge4
version in wlt- + copula; 'yes/no' questions with other auxiliaries.
\7here is the sun?
Is there a fish
in the water?
Stage 4, German speakers may infer that
if English uses subject-auxiliary
ion, it may also permit inversion with full verbs, as German does,
them to produce questions such as 'Like you baseball?'-Magst du
5
rion
in w/r questions with both an atxiliary and a main verb.
How do you say prochd
\What's the boy doing?
nch-speakirg learners may have difficulry using Stage 5 questions in
rich the subject is a noun rather than a pronoun. They may say (and accepr
grammatical) '!(hy do you like chocolate?' but not ''Why do children like
;olate?' In this, they are drawing on French, where it is often ungramicd to use inversion with a noun subject (*Pourquoi aiment les enfants le
).
mplex questions.
question
tag:
It's better, isnt it?
question: \flhy cant you go?
embedded question: Can you tell me what the date is today?
negative
emannt developmental sequence for questions has been the basis for a
number of studies, some of which will be discussed in Chapter 6. Alison
Mackey and her colleagues have done a number of these studies, and she
ided the data in Table 4.1. These examples come from three adult
learners of English as a second language who were interacting with
e native speaker in a 'spot the differences' task. In this task, learners have
similar but not identicd pictures and they have to ask questions until they
out how the picture they can see is different from the one their
interlocutor has. Note that progress to a higher stage does not always mean
that learners produce fewer errors.
87
88
Learner knguage
natut
Using the information about the developmental sequence for questions in the
stages above, circle the stage of second language question development that best
langu
corresponds to each question. Hint: Read all of each learner's questions before
you begin.
gram
illustr
Learner
Sa m,
I
I Where is he going and what is he saying?
2 ls the room his room?
3 ls he taking out his skate board?
4 What is he thinkingl
5 The girl, what do you, what does she do,
what is she doing?
Learner 2
6 Are they buying some things?
7 ls they bought present?
8 ls they're retirement peoplel
9 ls this perfume or ... I don't know.
l0 And it is necktie?
Learner
123456
123456
t23456
t23456
t23456
t23456
Son c
Ses err
Note
defin
Il
iest.
Joanr
Frenc
Prop(
s€que
show
Engli
3
I
Are there any shuttle?
12
lnside, is there any girl?
l3
l4
l5
You don't
I
Stoge
123456
1234s6
123456
123456
Space shuttle?
see?
What are, what the people wearing?
And they are carrying pink boxl
t23456
t23456
123456
t23456
t23456
Stage
Nou
gende
Answer key
Leorner l: Questions 1,4, and 5 are Stage 5 questions. Question 5 is interesting
because it shows the speaker self-correcting, suggesting that Stage 5 is still a level
that requires some greater effort. Questions 2 and 3 are Stage 4 questions.
Leorner 2: Questions 6 and 9 could be Stage 4 questions. However, the fact that
questions 7 and 8 are Stage 3 questions suggests that this speaker has not actually
Suge
Emerr
rhe fo
Progressed from 'fronting'to 'inversion', particularly since question l0 is a Stage 2
question.
Leorner 3:
l
Questions I I and 12are Stage 4 questions. Questions l3 and l5 are
Stage 2 questions.
Question l4 shows the speaker apparently on the verge of a
Stage 5 question, then retreating to a Stage 3 question.
Tdbh 4.1 Questions
Possessia e
by
Japdnese-speaking learners
of English
determiners
A developmental sequence for the English possessive forms 'his' and'her' has
been observed in the interlanguage of French- and Spanish-speaking
learners. In English, the choice of 'his' or'her' (or'its') is determined by the
I
fugt
Differ
Eaff2
fugt
Errorhodv
1
Learner language
natural gender of the possessor. In French and Spanish (and many other
languages), the correct form of the possessive determiner matches the
grammatical gender of the object or person that is possessed. This can be
illustratedwith the following translation equivalents for French and English:
Sa mire= his mother or her mother
Son chien= his dog or her dog
Ses
enfants = his children or her children
Note that when the object possessed is a body part, French often uses a
definite article rather than a possessive determiner.
Il iest cassi le brar-He broke the [his] arm.
Joanna \(hite (1998) studied the acquisition of possessive determiners by
French-speaking students, adapting a developmental sequence that was first
proposed by HelmutZobl (1984). \7hite found a total of eight steps in the
but they can be grouped into four main stages. The examples
shown in Stages 1-4 below come from French-speaking students learning
itrt.tactions.
English, desciibing cartoon drawings of family.,r.rrt,
sequence,
"nd
Suge 1: Pre-ernergence
No use of 'his' and 'her'. Definite article
genders, and numbers.
or 'your' used for all persons,
The little boy play with the bicycle.
He have band-aid on the arm, the leg, the stomach.
This boy cry in the arm ofyour mother.
There is one girl talkwith your dad.
Stage 2: Emergence
Emergence of 'his' and/or'her', with a strong preference to use only one
the forms.
of
The mother is dressing her little boy, and she put her clothes, her pant,
her coat, and then she finish.
The girl making hisselfbeautiful. She put the make-up on his hand, on
his head, and his father is surprise.
Stage 3 : Post-ernergence
Differentiated use of 'his' and 'her' but not when the object possessed has
natural gender.
The girl fell on her bicycle. She look his father and cry.
The dad put her little girl on his shoulder, and after, on his back.
Stage
4
Error-free use of 'his' and 'her' in all contexts including natural gender and
body parts.
89\
\
\
\.
Learner language
The little girl with her dad play together. And the dad take his girl on
his shoulder and he hurt his back.
Despl
rypes
English speakers learning French, or other languages that use grammatical
relatir
gender as the basis for choosing possessive determiners, also have to learn a
new way of determining the gender of the possessive determiner. Learning
-compi
the grammatical gender of each and every noun further adds ro the
langur
rvhere
--.formir
relatlv
clauser
M::::;,eckuses
\Y
Second language learners first acquire relative clauses that refer to nouns in
the subject and direct object positions, and only later (and in some cases,
never) learn to use them to modify nouns in other senrence roles (for
example, indirect object and obje.i of preposition). A summary of the
observed paftern ofacquisition for relative clauses is shown inThble 4.2.Itis
referred to as the 'accessibiliry hierarchy, and it reflects the apparenr ease
with which learners have 'access' to certain structures in the target language.
@
speake
(for
ex
-{rabic
Rtftt
-t num
,develol
shown
a
. r.ariet
Part of speech
Relative clause
Subject
The girlwho was sickwent home.
Direct object
The story that I read was long.
Indirect object
The man who [m] Susan gave the presenr ro was h"ppy.
Object ofpreposition
I found the book thatJohn was talking about.
Possessive
I know the woman whose father is visiting.
Object of comparison
The person that Susan is taller than is Mary.
st'l
-ike
yc
nents i
show th
M
Vi
Larer, le
Table
4.2 Accessibility hierarchyfor relatiue
Doughty
clauses in English (adaptedfrom
I99I)
Unlike the study of
:ast, alt
::,eaning
mmatical morphemes, n
tion, and questions
on child language.
Rather, it came
patterns found in studies of a
of
languages by Edward Keenan and Bernard Comrie (1977). They found that
li@!ffier
those lan
which included the strucrures at the bot
able 4.2 would also hii
at tne top, but t
necessarily true. Subsequently, Susan Gassll982j andothers found that if
-seqqnd.langffileatner could use one of the structures at the botto
ist. he or she qquldpebably be fficede
it. On the other
hand, a learner who could produce sen
subiect or direct o
at the top of the list) would not necessari
itions further down
M(
&sr tens
rciiably.
\7e
A:rer the
$rneraliz
erample,
My
She
Learner language
espite the similarity
of the general paffern that has been
several
o
in the
ion of
t has been observed that for learners whose first
does i
ticular clause
r example, objesq o
mparison), it ir mot. diffi.,rlt to learn to
that
ming
cla9s43ffi .*"mple, J"p"ner. ar,d Chinese, where the
16-,
rf,,noun
noun it
ir modifiir)Jthry
@
-odiiirtJrhey m,
-, avoid usins relative
nses even when their interlansuase is-Fairiild
7lEiffi. first
,relative
For.""fifri.rffi
influence is sdG
often produce both the relative marker and the prono,rn it replaces
r example, 'The man who I saw him was very angry), as they *o,tld irt
rbic.
ference to past
umber of researchers, includingJtirgen Meisel (1987),have observed the
doping abiliry to use language to locate events in time. The research has
rn that learners from different first lan
uiery of
uire the
rts in a similar
Battern- t--
young children, learners with limited language may simply refer to
in the order in which they occurred or mention a time or place to
rthat the event occurred in the past.
B
rts
My son come. He work in restaurant.
Viet Nam. \7e work too hard.
4learners staff to attach a grammatical morpheme marking the verb for
; although it may not be the one that the targer language rr.r for that
ning.
Me working long time. Now srop.
:tense forms of irregular verbs may be used before the regular pasr is used
blv.
Ve went to school every day. \7e spoke Spanish.
r
they begin marking pasr rense on regular verbs, learners may overrahze the regular -edending or the use of the wrong pasr rense form, for
nple, the present perfect rather than the simple pasr.
My sister catched
big fish.
She has lived here since fifteen years.
a
9t
92
Learner language
Kathleen Bardovi-Harlig (2000) and others have found th4l l.g-gl_ur.
maf\ pasi tense on some verbs ,h"r, on orh.ffipl.,
more likelv to
l
-
s
,I
Of
bacl
broke the
mea
'My sister fixed it with glue' than in sentences such as 'She seemed
'My father swam in that lake'. These differggqe sppgu to
l"ppy
't.ri.r
U. d". t" ,h.
ct', that is,
meanln
L.,":tgl5slug find it easier
vase' and
coff,
last weeli or
ro@
1tftt.l, ".tut.'so'nethi't
'accomplishments'
t'efbrred to as '"..o-plirh
:.t lt.a fl
brother took
(I ranthree miles.
and 'achievements' ('r
miles- My
M.,
an aspirin and uent to be
fuse
fg5lglqqpglld ('I swam all afrernoon') or 'srares' rh"r@,
constants ('He seemedhappy to sit by the lake'), l."r".rr rr. ri-pl. p,
First- language) can have an infuence here too. Laura Collins (2002)
investigated the different English verb forms used by French speakers. The
past tense that is most commonly used in spoken French and that is usually a
translation of a simple past form in English is a form that resembles ihe
present perfect in English. Thus, the equivalent of 'Yesterday he ate an apple'
yHi:, il a mangi une pomme-literally, 'Yesterday he has eaten
"n "ppl.'.
Teachers o
the
p.-$:nt qelftft. In Collins' study, learners completed passages by filling in
blanks with the appropriate form of a verb. In places where English ,p."f.r,
would have used the simple past, French speakers did someti-., use the
perfect (either present perfect or past perfect) forms. Furthermore, they used
them more frequently than a comparison group of Japan.r. ,p."k rr.
However, the French speakers were more likely to use perfect forms for
achievement and accomplishment verbs than for the ,t"i., and activities.
Collins observes,
'Th. [first language] lgg
--___
t
tne elfect ot lexrcal aspeeu rather rt
ont
Stag
that
"" ".tus
thEt refer
Ci
are f,
_
sam(
placi
the L
Anor
,h.y
that
SCCOI
obser
g9
{
patte
genel
of otl
Mo
Resea
'trans
rhe ta
$-as c
85).
rejecti
aSas(
r}e Er
Moa ement through deue lopmental sequences
-We
have seen in this section that, as in first language
systematic and predictable dev
uences
ttoem
acqu$1rc
stages are
ike closed
ers do
uisition, there are
rn secon
ize that developmen
no
enter another. In examining a language sample from an individual learner,
one
sh-ld not expect to find behaviours from
only one srage.
On-&-
several different _slgges. It is perhaps better to think of a stag.
U.i"g
"i forrnl
characteriz€[Ty the emergence and increasing frequenry of-n.*
rather than by the complete disappearance of earlier ones. Even when a more
advanced stage comes to dominate in a learnert speech, conditions of stress
r,aluaI
langu:
instru
For ea
backgr
rowat(
Perdut
3anefl
-angua
seconc
Despit
end te
Learner language
complexity in a communicative interaction can cause the learner to 'slip
' to an earlier stage. Note that progress to a higher stage does not always
that learners produce fewer errors. For example, a learner may produce
questions at Stage 1 or Stage 3, but those correct forms are not based
underlying knowledge of subject-verb inversion. Correct questions at
L arcchunl<s, not sentences that have been constructed from the words
make them up.At Stage 2, learners have advanced, in the sense that they
forming original questions, but the word order of those questions is the
as that of declarative sentences. At Stage 3, questions are formed by
irg question form (most often a wlt-word or a form of the verb 'do') at
"
beginning of a sentence with declarative word order.
important observation about developmental sequences is'the way
assusle
interact with first language infuence. Lg@4tl;o
imolv transfer the structures of their
as Henning
;when
reach a develo
n their first
)
and Helmut Zobl (1980)
.t.
encounter
a
language and their interlanguage
Beneralize their first language pattern and end up makinq errqrs that speakerq
ofother languages are less likely to make.
More about first l*goage infuence
the interpretation of contrastive analysis that made
'transfer' or'interference' the explanation for all of a learner's difficulties with
Se target language. This was due in part to the fact that contrastive analysis
was closely associated with behaviourist views of language acquisition. In
rejecting behaviourism, some researchers also discarded contrastive analysis
as a source of valuable information about learners' language. Researchers at
the European Science Foundadon carried out a study that created some
valuable opportunities to examine the influence of the first language. Adult
language learners, most of whom had little or no formal second language
instruction, were followed as they learned particular European languages.
For each target language, groups of learners from two different first language
backgrounds were compared. Also, for each group of learners, their progress
towards two target sffuctures was studied. As Wolfgang Klein and Clive
Perdue (1993) report, there were substantial similarities in the interlanguage
parterns of the learners, in spite of dre great variety in the first and second
language combinations. The similarities were greatest in the earliest stages of
Researchers rejected
second language acquisition.
Despite the similarities, there is no doubt in the minds of most researchers
and teachers that learners draw on their knowledge of other languages as
93
94
Learner language
they try to discover the complexities of the new language they are learning.
\7e have
seen some ways
in which the first language
interacts with
developmental sequences. \When learners reach a certain stage and perceive a
similarity to their first language, they may linger longer at that stage (for
example, the extended use of preverbaT'no' by Spanish speakett) ot add a
substage (for example, the German speakert inversion of subject and lexical
verbs in questions) to the sequence which, overall, is very similar across
learners, regardless of their first language. They may learn a second language
rule but restrict its application (for example, the French speaker's rejection of
subj ect-auxiliary inversion with noun subj ects) .
The first language may infuence learners' interlanguage in other ways as
well. The phenomenon of 'avoidance' that Jacquelyn Schachter (1974)
described appeared to be caused at least in part by learners' perception that a
feature in the target language was so distant and different from their first
language that they preferred not to try it.
Other researchers have also found evidence oflearners' sensitivity to degrees of
distance or difference and a reluctance to attempt a transfer over too great a
distance. In one very revealing study, Hikan Ringbom (1986) found that the
'interference' errors made in English by both Finnish-Swedish and SwedishFinnish bilinguals were most often traceable to Swedish, not Finnish. The fact
that Swedish and English are closely related languages that actually do share
many characteristics seems to have led learners to take a chance that a word or
a sentence structure that worked in Swedish would have an English
equivalent. Finnish, on the other hand, belongs to a completely different
language family, and learners used Finnish as a source of possible transfer far
less often, whether their own first language was Swedish or Finnish.
The risk-taking associated with this percepdon of similariry has its limits,
however. fu we noted earliet learners seem to know that idiomatic or
metaphorical uses of words are often unique to a particular language. Eric
Kellerman (1986) found that Dutch learners of English were often reluctant
to accept certain idiomatic expressions or unusual uses ofwords such as'The
wavebroke on the shord but accepted'tlebroke the cup' even thoughboth
are straightforward translations of sentences with the Dutch verb breken.
Another way in which learners' first language can affect second language
acquisition is in making it difficult for them to notice that something they
are saying is not a feature of the language as it is used by mote profi.cient
speakers. Lydia \fhite (1991) gave the example of adverb placement in
Fr.nch and English. Both languages allow adverbs in several positions in
simple sentences. F{.owever, as the examples inTable 4.3 show, there are some
diffirences. English, but not French, allows SAVO order; French, but not
English, allows SVAO.
s=
AI
o
Sc
S\
M
M
sl
M
xA
S\
xl\
M
N(,
Tal
It sr
thei
but
first
lear
ers
(
sorr
equ
Th,
sin
the
pri
Jiit
de
R(
ha
m
sp
.\
u
ir
(
b
n
Learner language
S=
Subject
V
=
Verb
O=Object
A=Adverb
ASVO
Often, M^ry drinks tea.
Souvent, Marie boit du thd.
SVOA
M"ry drinks
tea often.
Marie boit du thd souvent.
SAVO
M^ry often drinks tea.
*Marie souvent boit du th6.
SVAO
*M^ry drinks often tea.
Marie boit souvent du thd.
Note:The asterisk (*) means that the sentence is not grammatical.
Table
It
4.3 Aduerb placement in French and English
seems fairly easy for French-speaking learners of English to add
SAVO to
fieir
repertoire and for English-speaking learners of French to add SVAO,
but both groups have difficulry geffing rid of a form similar to a form in their
first language that does not occur in the target language. English-speaking
learners ofFrench accept SAVO as grammatical, and French-speakinglearncrs ofEnglish accept SVAO. As \Mhite points out, it is difficult to notice that
mmething is not present
in the input,
especially when its translation
oquivalent sounds perfectly all right and communication is not disrupted.
There are patterns in the development of syntax and morphology that are
similar among learners from different language backgrounds. Evidence for
&ese developmental patterns first came from studies of learners whose
primary learning environment was outside the classroom. For example,
fiirgen Meisel, Harald Clahsen, and Manfred Pienemann (1931) identified
developmental sequences in the acquisition of German byspeakers ofseveral
Romance languages who had little or no instruction. Subsequent research
hrs shown that learners who receive instruction exhibit similar develop-
mental sequences and error pafferns. In the interlanguage of Englishqpeakers whose only exposure to German was in university classes in
Australia, Pienemann (1988) found patterns thatwere similar to those of the
uninstructed learners. In Chapter 6, we will discuss other srudies that have
investigated the influence of instruction on developmental sequences.
Our understanding of the infuence of the first language on the second has
been refined in recent decades. Current views of second language develop-
ment emphasize the interaction between the first language (ot other
95
96
Learner language
previously learned languages), cognitive processes, and the samples of the
target language that learners encounter in the input. As Terence Odlin's
(1989, 2003) extensive reviews show, the complexity of this relationship has
inspired scores of investigations. Many questions remain to be answered.
So far this chapter has focused on the acquisition of morphology and syntax
second language. tWe now turn to the learning of other important
components of coMMUNrcATrvE coMIETENCE: vocabulary pragmatics,
in the
and pronunciation.
Vocabulary
In 1980, Paul Meara characterized vocabulary learning
as
For most people, the importance of vocabulary seems very clear. As it has
often been remarked, we can communicate by using words that are not
placed in the proper order, pronounced perfectly, or marked with the proper
grammatical morphemes, but communication often breaks down if we-do
not use the correct word. Although circumlocurion and gestures can sometimes compensare, the importance of vocabulary can hardly be overestimated.
The challenge of acquiring a large enough vocabulary for successfi.rl
communication in a variery of settings has been the focus of much recenr
research. Every language has an astonishingly large number of words.
Fnglish, which has built its vocabulary from a great variery of source
languages, is variously estimated to have anywhere from 100,000 ro one
million words, depending in part on how words are counted. For example,
some would treat 'teach, teacher, teaching, and taught' as separate words
while others would count all of them
as
paft ofa single root word from which
s
{
charac
ry
,{s we
l*rgut
rs
quitt
robet
c()ntex
nor
be
frousar
rhe wo
meanin
o,nder t<
hnorvn
karners
m,on frr
ilM)[r-tec
ffiings r
about a
*meffiirno
all the others are derived.
An educated adult
words. Fortuna
d
a'neglected aspecr
of language learning'. Researchers in the 1970s and early 1980s were drawn
to syntax and morphology because of the way error pafterns and
developmental sequences of these features might reveal something about
universals in languages and language acquisition. How different things are
now!Just as Meara was commenting on the state of neglect, an explosion of
research on vocabulary learning was beginning, and the acquisition of
vocabulary has become one of the most active areas in second language
acquisition research.
r of English is believed to know at least 20,000
number, somethinq more li
Xpenesthave rens of thousan
T'be fusr
Ihurtr Me
Similarly, although Chinese and
,m rhis
characters, most are rare, and nontechnical material can usually be read with a knowledge of about 2,00A
knrners
.
of
mr
I
rhey
Learner knguage
characters.
so, ?qquiring ? basic vocabulary is a significant accom-
plish-.nt
^Even
,t{s we saw
in Chapter 1, children learn
thousands of words
in their first
language with little observable effort. The task of learning a large vocabulary
is quite different for second language learners. For one thing, they are likely
m be exposed to far smdler samples of the language to be learned. The
contexts in which second language learners encounter new vocabulary may
not be as helpful as those in which children learn the first one or rwo
thousand words of their first language. If they are older children or adults,
the words they are exposed to may also be more difficult, referring to
meanings that are not easily guessed from context. It is estimated that, in
order to guess the meaning of aword even in a helpful context, one needs to
know nearly all the other words in the text-a rare event for second language
learners at most stages of acquisition. Although the two or three thousand
most frequent words in English make up as much as 80-90 per cenr of most
non-technical texts, less frequent words are crucial to the meaning of many
things we hear and read. For example, the meaning of tt.*rp"fer article
about a court case may be lost without the knowledge" of
'testimony', alleged', or'accomplice'.
*orir
such
as
first step in knowing a word may simply be ro recogn ize thatit is a word.
Paul Meara and his colleagues (2005b) developed tests that took advantage
The_
of this fact. Some of these tests take the form of simple word lists, arid
learners are instructed to simply check'yes' or 'no' acconding to whether or
not they know the word. Each list also includes some itemi that look like
97
Learner language
English words but are not. The number of real words that the learner
identifies is adjusted for guessing by factor that takes account of the
^
number of non-words that are also chosen. Such a simple procedure is more
effective than it might sound. A carefully constructed list can be used to
estimate the vocabulary size of erren advanced learners. For example, if
shown the following list: 'frolip, I^gW, scrule, and albeit', a proficient speaker
of English would know that only one of these words is a real English word,
albeit a rate and somewhat odd one. On the other hand, even proficient
speakers might recognize none of the following items: 'gonion, micelle,
lairage, throstle'. Even our computert spellchecker rejected three out offout
but all are real English words.
Among the factors that make new vocabulary more easily learnable by
second language learners is the frequenrywith which the word is seen, heard,
and understood. Paul Nation (2001) reviews a number of studies suggesting
that a learner needs to have many meaningful encounters with a new word
before it becomes firmly established in meirory. The esdmates range as high
as sixteen times in some studies. Even more encounters may be needed
before a learner can retrieve the word in fluent speech or automatically
understand the meaning of the word when it occurs in a new context. The
ability to understand the meaning of most words without focused aftention
is essential for fuent reading as well as for fuent speaking.
Frequenry is not the only factor that determines how easily words are
learned, however. Look at the words in List 1 and List 2. \Mhich one would
vou expect beginning second language learners to recognize andunderstand?
List I
List 2
List 3
Friend
Hamburger
Government
More
Coke
Responsibiliry
Town
T:shirt
Dictionary
Book
\Talkman
Elementary
Hunt
Taxi
Remarkable
Sing
Pizza
Description
Box
Hotel
Expression
Smile
Dollar
International
Ey.
Internet
Preparation
Night
Disco
Activiry
All
r
wor(
com
wor(
prev
scho
pron
to
lea
man)
Ont
alrear
vocat
langu
Studr
they i
The
v
and r
woulr
These
langu
that
l,
COGN
Thus,
thing
borro't
On th
look s
come
Sarne
c
from
i
even d
Teachr
words
identic
how s;
and W
Iikely
r
spoken
illustra
hockey
you
'do
Learner language
All of
in List
I
look easy because they are simple one-syllable
words that refer to easily illustrated actions or objects. They are also quite
common words in English, appearing among the 1,000 mosr frequent
the words
words. And yet, they are not likely to be known to students who have not had
previous instrucdon in English or exposure to the language outside of
school. Furthermore, there
is nothing in the written form or
pronunciation of the words that gives
the
clue to their meaning. If studenrs are
to learn them, they must see or hear the words and connect them to meaning
many times before they are well established.
a
On the other hand, some students who have never studied English might
already know words in List 2, because they are part of an international
vocabulary. \7ith increasing internationalization of communications, many
languages have 'borrowed' and adapted words from other languages.
Students throughout the world may be surprised to learn how many words
.h.y already know in the language they are trying to learn.
The words in List 3 look difficult. They are rather long, not easily illustrated,
and most are fairly infrequent in the language. And yer, many students
would either 'knovi them on sight or learn them after a single exposure.
These words have a clear resemblance to their translation equivalenr in other
languages-not just romance languages with shared Latin origins. \fords
that look similar and have the same meaning in two languages are called
COGNATES.
Thus, when students are learning a new language, frequenry is not the only
thing that makes words more
accessible. The presence of cognates and
borrowed words can also be exploited for vocabulary developmenr.
On the other hand, students may have particular difficulty with words that
look similar in the two languages but have different meanings. They may
come from different origins or they may have evolved differently from the
same origin. For example, the English verb 'demand' has a different meaning
from its French cousin fumander which means 'request' or 'ask a question',
even though they developed from the same Latin verb.
not assume that students will always recognize borrowed
words or cognate words in their second language. Some cognates are
Teachers should
identical in form and meaning, while others may require some knowledge of
how spelling patterns are related in the two languages (for example, 'water'
and Wasser in English and German). Even with different spellings, words are
likely to be easier to recognize in their written form than they are in the
spoken language. Learners may need guidance in recognizing them, as
illustrated in the following question, asked by * eight-year-old in a Quebec
hockey arena: Hd coach, czmment on dit coaclt en anglaisi ('Hey, coach. How
do you say coach in English?'). And after a momentt refection, English
99
100
Learner language
speakers may realize that they know both specialiry items
restaurant that calls itself 'Sushi and Bisusteki.'
in
a Japanese
Some second language theorists have argued that second language learners,
like children learning their first language, can learn a great deal ofvocabulary
with little intentional effort. Stephen Krashen (1985, 1989) has asserted that
the best source of vocabulary growth is reading for pleasure. There is no
doubt that reading is an important potential source of
vocabulary
development for second language learners as it is for first language learners.
However, there are some problems with the notion that vocabulary growth
through reading requires little effort. Bhatia Laufer (1992) and others have
shown that
it
is difficult to infer the meaning and learn new words from
reading unless one already knows 95 per cent or more of the words in a text.
In addition, as we have seen, learners usually need to encounter aword many
times in order to learn it well enough to recognize tt in new contexts or
produce it in their own speaking and writing. As we saw in Chapter 1, Dee
Gardner (2004) has shown how rare certain rypes ofwords are in narratives.
Thus, students who read mainly fiction may have little chance of learning
words that are essential for their academic pursuits. Research on vocabulary
learning through reading without focused instruction confirms that some
language, including vocabulary, can be learned without explicit instruction
(see Chapter 5). On the other hand, Jan Hulstijn and Bhatia Laufer (2001)
provide evidence that vocabulary development is more successful when
learners are fully engaged in activities that require them to attend carefully to
the newwords and even to use them in productive tasks. Izabella Kojic-Sabo
and Patsy Lightbown (1999) found that effort and the use of good learning
strategies, such as keeping a notebook, looking words up in a dictionary, and
reviewing what has been learned were associated with better vocabulary
development.
Even with instruction and good strategies, the task is daunting. \What does it
mean to know a word? Grasp the general meaning in a familiar context?
Provide a definition or a translation equivalent? Identify its componenr parrs
or etymology? Use the word to complete a sentence or to create a new
sentence? Use it metaphorically? Understand a joke that uses homonyms
(words that sound alike but mean different things, such as 'cents', 'sense',
'scents')? Second language learners whose goal is to use the language for
academic purposes must learn to do all these things.
*g
usmg
resPoi
xnar]a
-st
:fany
rour (
Ir mig
Sirnile
wa)'s.
speakr
wondt
wirh
ir
The st
:nelr I
reques
-rrdcs
fo
ione t
:nr-itin
erea
Pragmatics
PraE
tics is the
thi6
as directn
a
:leir f
of how lan
is used in context to
"bili.y
guage
3:rppfr
Learner language
glnl +lAs /UotE
#;ili?t^-i
sa{st
/*-
'l t]
of the target
can still encounter difficulry in
usmg -l?
r lnterpretlng requests,
to acquire ski
responding politely to compliments or apologies, recognizinghumour, and
managing conyersations. They need to learn to recognize the many meanings that the same sentence can have in different situations. Think of the
manyways one might interpret an apparently simple question such as 'Is that
your dog?' It might precede an expression ofadmiration for an attractive pet.
It might be an urgent request to get the dog out of the speakert flower bed.
Similarly, the same basic meaning is altered when it is expressed in different
ways. For example, we would probably assume that the relationship between
speaker and listener is very different if we hear 'Give me the book or 'I
momho
wonder
with it'.
if youd mind letting me have that book when youve finished
The study of how second language learners develop the ability to express
their intentions and meanings through different speech acts (for example,
requesting, refusing, apologizing, etc.) is referred to as interlanguage pragmatics (Bardovi-Harlig 1999). For a long time, most of the research in this
area focused on learners' use ofpragmatic features. For example, studies were
done to describe the ways in which learners expressed speech acts such as
inviting and apolo gizingin relation to differences in their proficienry level or
their first language background. Other studies have examined learners'
abiliry to perceive and comprehend pragmatic features in the second language and to judge whether a panicular request is appropriate or
inappropriate in a specific context.
101
I02
Learner language
Since the early 1990s more research has directly investigated the acquisition
of second language pragmatic abiliry. This includes longitudinal and crosssectional studies describing the acquisition of several different speech acts.
One that has been the focus of considerable attention is 'requesting'.
Requests are an interesting pragmatic feature to examine because there are
identifiable ways in which requests are made in different languages as well as
differences in how they are expressed across languages and cultures.
In a review of longitudinal and cross-sectional studies on the acquisition of
requests in English, Gabriele Kasper and Kenneth Rose (2002) outline a
series of five stages of development. Stage 1 consists of minimal language
that is often incomplete and highly context-dependent. Stage 2 includes
primarily memorized routines and frequent use of imperatives. Stage 3 is
marked by less use of formulas, more productive speech and some
MTTIGATIoN of requests. Stage 4 involves more complex language and
increased use of mitigation, especially supportive statements. Stage 5 is
marked by more refinement of the force of requests. The five stages, their
characteristics and examples are given below.
Stage
Fine
Learr
inves
gated
Engli
obser
speal.
select
negor
tendr
with
on tl
advis
exam
Stage 1: Pre-basic
by sa
Highly context-dependent, no synrax, no relational goals.
schec
bette
Me no blue.
respo
Sir.
speak
Stage 2:
Formuhic
or ge
Reliance on unanalysed formulas and imperarives.
say'I
said'
Lett play the game.
Stage
Let's eat breakfast.
resear
Dont look.
Pragr
3: Unpaching
advis
inter:
Formulas incorporated into productive language use, shift ro conventional
indirectness.
Can you pass the pencil please?
Can you do another one for me?
For a
was p
Addition of new forms to repertoire, increased use of mitigation, more
complex syntax.
I
have another chocolate because my
children.
Can I see it so I can copy it?
reject
provi
Stage 4: Pragrnatic expansion
Could
conti
children-I
have five
partit
patte
muni
instrr
input
orgar
learn,
funct
Learner
language
Suge 5: Fine tuntng
Fine tuning of requestive force to pamicipanrs, goals, and contexts.
You could put some blue tack down there.
Is there any more white?
karning how to make and reject suggestions has also been extensively
investigated. Kathleen Bardovi-Harlig and Beverly Hartford (1993) investig..9 rejections and suggestions with native and non-native speakers of
Elglish in academic advising
sessions
observed differences between the way
at an American univ.tiiry. They
in which native and non-native
speakers communicated
with their professors as they discussed their course
selections. These differences contributed to their greater or lesser success in
negotiating their academic plans. For example, the non-native speakers
tended to take on a passive role and did not initiate suggestions compared
with the native speakers who initiated a grear deal. There-was also a r.ni.r.y
on the part of the non-native speakers to reject suggestions made by thl
in_ ways that the advisors might find rude-or inappropriate. For
aample, theywould reject an advisort suggestion to take a parti.ular course
b{ t"flg 'I think I am not interested in that course', instead of saying 'My
schedule conflicts with that course', or 'I think this othe, .o,rri. would
bemer mee-t my needs', which was more typical of native-speaker rejection
responses. The non-native speakers were also much less adept than the native
sPeakels at using mitigation-language that can be used to soften a rejection
or gently make a suggestion. For example, native speakers were observed tq
say'I think I would like to take this course', *hereai the non-native speakers
said'I will take that course'. Over a period of four and a half monihr, th.
researchers observed progress in some aspects of the non-native speakers'
pragmatic ability. For example, they took a more active role in th.
"drririrrg
interactions. They provided reasons for rejecting suggestions that
th!
advisors were likely to perceive as more credibie or.I..p,Jble. Even so, they
continued to experience difficulty in mitigating ttreir suggestions and
advisor
rejections.
For a
lotg time, it was assumed that second language classrooms could not
provide appropriate input for learning how to t.Jir. many speech acts. This
was particularly the case with structure-based approach., toi.".hing and in
particular,
in teacher-fronted
classrooms wheie the dominant
intiraction
Paftern was 'teacher initiation-learner response-teacher feedbacli. In communicative, content-based, and task-based approaches ro second language
in-struction, there are more opportunities not only for a greater ,r"ri.ry of
input but also for learners to engage in different roles-and participant
organization structures (for example, pair and group work). This .n"bl.t
to produce and respond to a wider r"rg. of communicative
functions. Furthermore, research on the teaching of pragmatics has
learners
103
r04
Learner language
demonstrated that pragmatic features can be successfully learned in
classroom settings and that explicit rather than implicit instruction is most
effective (Kasper and Rose 2002).This is particularly good news for foreign
language learners who do not have extensive exposure to conversational
interaction outside the classroom. Thus, the question is no longer whether
second language pragmatics should be taught but rather how it can be best
integrated into classroom instruction.
rvord
th
sot
soun(
howe
Thus,
are si
morp
50un(
prodr
Phonology
Grammar has been the focus for second language teachers and researchers
for a long time. As we saw, vocabulary and pragmatics have also received
more attention in recent years. However, we know less about pronunciation
and how it is learned and taught. Pronunciation was a central component in
language teaching during the audiolingual eta. Several techniques for
teaching pronunciation were developed at that time and most of them
focused on getting learners to perceive and to produce distinctions between
single sounds (i.e. sncvrnNrers) in minimal pair drills (for example, 'ship'
and 'sheep'). \fhen audiolingualism and behaviourism fell into disfavour
and were replaced by other views of learning, the teaching of pronunciation
was minimized
if not totally discarded. Evidence for the critical
period
hypothesis suggested that native-like pronunciation was an unrealistic goal
for second language learners, pafticularly older learners (see Chapter 3). It
was argued, therefore, that instructional time would be better spenr on
teaching features that learners might learn more easily, mosr specifically
grammar. \fhen communicative language teaching was first introduced in
the late l970s,little attention was given to the teaching of pronunciation.
tVhen it was included, the emphasis was on rhythm, srress,
and intonation
(i.e. sunnASEGMENrers), areas considered more likely to affect communication (Celce-Murcia, Brinton, and Goodwi n 1996).
Although research on the teaching and learning of pronunciation is not as
extensive as that in other language domains, there is theoretical and
empirical work to help us understand the processes involved in phonological
development in a second language and the factors that contribute to it.
Contrastive analysis has helped to explain some aspects of first language
infuence on second language learners' phonological development. \(/e can
all think of examples of these from our own experiences or those of our
students. Japanese and Korean learners of English often have problems
hearing and producing land rbecause these sounds are not distinct in their
language. Spanish speakers will often say 'I e-speak e-Spanish' because
Spanish words do not have consonant clusters beginning with s at the
beginning ofaword. French speakers may place stress on the last syllable of a
The r
Evely
speak
begin
words
recogl
pseud
rvords
could
as spa
still in
Itisw
langu:
supPo
Iearne
tbreig
they a
in
lear
distan
Bonga
speake
s.ide
l
\\'ere
a
langue
uere j
iangua
iudgec
There
indivi<
rnore,
featurr
plays
r
exposr
Learner
language
word because French usuallysffesses thelastsyllable. Fewlanguages have the
th sounds that are frequent in English. Learners -"y r.tbstiiute similar
sounds from their first language (for example, t or d, s or z). Somedmes,
however, learners overcompensate for sounds that they know are difficult.
Thus, learners may pronounce a th (asin tbn) where a rbelongs. Such errors
are similar to the overgeneralization errors that we saw for grammatical
morphemes. That is,
if they
replace earlier 'correct' pronunci"tion
sounds, they may represent progress
produce the thsound.
in
learners' abiliry
of t or d
ro notice and
The relationship bem'een perception and production of sounds is complex.
Altenberg Q005) developed a series of tasla to explore Spanish
qreakers' perceptions and production of English consonant clusters at the
beginning of a word. In one task, they had to say whether certain invented
words were possible 'new English words'. The learners were quite good at
recognizing what English words are supposed to sound like. They accepted
lxeudowords like 'spus' and rejected those like 'zban, even though both
words would be unacceptable as 'new Spanish words'. She found that they
E*lyt
ould
usually write (from dictation) pseudowords with initial clusters r,r.L
8 sp and sm. However, in their own production, these same learners
might
still insert avowel at the beginning ofwords such as'spoon'and'smile'.
ft
is widely believed that the degree of difference between the learner's native
hnguage and the target language can lead to grearer difficulry. The evidence
nrpporting the hypothesis comes partly from the observation that it takes
learners longer to reach a high level of fuency in a particular second or
language if that language is substantidly different from the languages
!*iglalready
Sq
know Fot example, a Chinese-speaker faces a greater .hJl.tig.
in learning English than does a speaker of German or Dutch. Larrgua[.
distance affects pronunciation as well as other language sysrems. Ttt.o
Bongaerts (1999) collected speech samples from many-highly proficient
from a
ryeakers who had learned Dutch in their adulthood and *ho
""-e
ride variety of first language backgrounds. \fhen native speakers
of Dutch
asked to judge these speech samples, only those learners who spoke a
Iere
hogo"g. thatwas closelyrelated to Dutch (for example, English or German)
were judged to have native-like accents. None of the rp.ik tr whose first
hnguages were more distant from Dutch (for exampl., Vi.tnamese) were
i"dg.d to have nativelike pronunciation.
There has been little research to document the developmental sequences of
individual sounds in second language phonological acquisition- Furthermore, while there is evidence for similariry in the acquisition of some
features of stress and rhythm, it is also clear that the learner's first language
plays an imporranr role. other factors such as the amounr and rype of
oryosure to the target language and the degree of use of the first language
105
106
Learner language
infuential contributors to pronunciation. Thorsten
Piske, Ian MacKay, and James Flege (2001) have reported that longer
periods of exposure to the second language can lead to improved
pronunciation. They also found that adults who continue to make grearer
use of their first language may have stronger accents in the second language.
As noted in Chapter 3, learners' ethnic affiliation and sense of their identity
are also related to some ofthe choices they make about how theyproduce the
have been identified as
sounds and rhythms of a second language.
Reseat
rnore
Ftronu
instrur
a5
we
ahiliry
]garner
In Chr
Few studies have investigated the effectiveness ofpronunciation instruction,
Cassro
but the results of recent studies suggest that it can make a difference,
particularly if the instruction focuses on suprasegmental rather than
Chapt
:o und
segmental aspects of pronunciation (Hahn 2004).Tracey Derwing and her
colleagues (1998, 2003) carried out a series of studies on how intelligible
learners were judged to be. They found that learners who received pronunciation lessons emphasizing stress and rhythm were judged to be easier to
understand than learners who received lessons focused on individual sounds.
Even though the learners who received instruction on individual sounds
were more accurate in their use of those sounds, this did not seem to increase
listeners'perception of the intelligibiliry of their speech to others. Findings
like these support the current emphasis on suprasegmentals in pronunciation
classes.
One of the controversial issues in pronunciation research is whether
intelligibiliry rather than native-like abiliry is the standard that learners
should strive toward. Studies of relationships between English narive
speakers' perceptions of foreign accent, their perceptions of comprehensibiliry and their actual abiliry to understand non-native utterances show clear
relationships among all three. However, it is also evident, as Murray Munro
and tacey Derwing (1995) suggest, that the presence of a strong foreign
accent does not necessarily result in reduced intelligibiliry or comprehensibiliry. Of course, evidence like this does not change the fact that foreign
accents sometimes cause listeners to respond negatively to second language
speakers. Jennifer Jenkins (2000, 2004) and Barbara Seidlhofer (200 4) are
among the many who argue for the acceptance of language varieties orher
than those spoken in the language's 'country of origin . People increasingly
interact with speakers who have learned a different variety of the same
language. Even so, in some situations, accent still serves as a marker of group
membership and is used as the basis for discrimination. Many second
language learners, particularly those who have achieved a high level of
knowledge and performance in other aspects of the target language, may be
motivated to approximate a particular target language accent in their
pronunciation. Others view this as irrelevant to their goals and objectives as
users of the second language.
Sou
Ar-ery
tJriforr
Bardo.
Form,
Crlce-
trranu|
'*tugu,
Daltor
L-niver
Cdss,
I
ia,tory
Hinke
Fs;tur,
Ienkin
-telxua
Ilasper
-,engul
]uatior
C;rnbr
Odlin,
-"fng
S,racls
Learner language
to the teaching and learning of pronunciation is gaining
is clear, however, is that decontextualized
pronunciation instruction is not enough and that a combination of
instruction, exposure, experience, and motivation is required. Furthermore,
as we learned in Chapter 3, achieving native or near-native pronunciation
abiliry is an accomplishment not experienced by most second language
Research related
more attention. \7hat
learners.
In Chapter 6 we will focus on the second language acquisition oflearners in
classroom settings. First, however, we will look at the classroom itself In
Chapter 5,wewill explore the manyways in which researchers have sought
to understand the classroom environment for second language acquisition.
Sources and suggestions for further
readitg
Avery P. and S. Ehrlich. L992. Tbaching American English Pronunciation.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Bardovi-Harlig, IC 2000. Tense and '4spect in Second Language Acquisition:
Form, Meaning and Ue. Oxford: Blackwell.
Celce-Murcia,
pronunciation:
M., D. M. Brinton, and J. M. Goodwin. 1996. Teaching
A Reference for Teachers of English to Speakers of Other
Langu age s. Camb ridge : Cambridge University Press.
Dalton, C. and B. Seidlhofer. 1994. Pronunciation. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Gass,
S. and L. Selinker. 2001. Second Language Acquisition: An Intro-
ductory Course2nd edn. Mahwah, NJr Lawrence Erlbaum and fusociates.
Hinkel, E. 2002.
Second Language Writers' Text: Linguistic and Rhetorical
Features. Mahwah, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
]enkins,
I.
2004. 'Research in teaching pronunciation and intonatiort'.
ofApp li ed Linguisti cs 24: | 09 :2.5 .
Annua I Reu iew
Kasper,
G. and K. R. Rose. 2002. 'Pragmatic Development in a Second
Language .' Language Learning52lSupplement 1.
Nation, I. S. P. 2001. Learning Vocabulary
in
Another
Language.
Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
Odlin, T. 2003. 'Cross-linguistic infuence' in C. J. Doughty and M. H.
Long (eds.): The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford:
Blackrvell. pp. 436-86.
r07
108
Learner knguage
Robinett, B.\f/.andJ. Schachter (eds.). 1983. Second Language Learning:
Contrastiue Analysis, Error Analysis, arud Rekted Aspectrs. Ann Arbor: The
University of Michigan Press.
Schmitt, N. 2000. Vocabulary in Language Tbaching. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
M. and T
5
S. Paribakht (eds.).
1999.'Incidental L2 vocabulary
acquisition: Theory, current research, and instructional implications'.
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 2l I 2, Special Issue.
W'esche,
In this
and de
this, le
setting
langua
Most
I
instruc
believe
on the
outside
Can
w
rhere
er
qm prc
Natt
Narura
learner
learner
nadve s
roward
classroc
I)eers as
n"n
struc
group
c
fuself,, ra
is to see
ueryet lz
r'nities
oders,
r
t
OBSERVING LEARNING
AND TEACHING IN THE
SECOND LANGUAGE
CLASSROOM
)
this chapter we explore different ways in which researchers have observed
i described what goes on in second language classrooms. Before we do
s. let us take a moment to reflect on the differences berween classroom
tings for language learning and other settings where people learn a new
rguage
without instrucrion.
cst people would agree that learning a second language in a nonirructional setting is different from learning in the classroom. Many
iieve that learning'on the street' is more effective. This belief may be based
the fact that most successful learners have had exposure to the language
rside the classroom. \(hat is special about this 'natural'language learning?
in \ve create the same environment in the classroom? Should we? Or are
:re essential contributions that only instiuction and not natural exposure
e provide?
latural and instructional seftings
irural acquisition contexts should be understood as those in which the
irner is exposed to the language at work or in social interaction or, if the
Lrner is a child, in a school situation where most of the other children are
ri'e speakers of the target language and where the instruction is directed
,r-ard native speakers rather than toward learners of the language. In such a
Lisroom, much of a child's learning would take place in interaction with
ers as well as through instruction from the teacher.
structure-based instructional environments, the language is taught to a
oup ofsecond or foreign language learners. The focus is on the language
eli rather than on the messages carried by the language. The teacher's goal
ro see to it that students learn the vocabulary and grammatical rules of the
:get language. Some students in structure-based classes may have oppornities to continue learning the target language outside the classroom; for
hers, the classroom is the only contact with that language. In some cases,
110
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second langudge classroom
the learners' goal may be to pass an examination rather than to use the
language for daily communicative interaction beyond the classroom.
tI
j
Communicative, content-based, and task-based instructional environments
also involve learners whose goal is learning the language itself but the sryle of
instruction places the emphasis on interaction, conversation, and language use.
rather than on learning about the language. The topics that are discussed in
communicative and task-based instructional environments are often ofgeneral
interest to the learner, for example, how to reply to a classified advertisemenr
from a newspaper. In content-based instruction, the focus of a lesson is usualll'
on the subject mattet such
I
Lr
tir
Fr
history or mathematics, which students are
learning through the medium of the second language. In these classes, the
focus may occasionally be on the language itself, but the emphasis is on using
the language rather than talking about it. The language that teachers use for
teaching is not selected solely for the purpose of teaching a specific feature of
the language, but also to make sure learners have the language they need to
interact in a variety of contexts. Students' success in these courses is often
measured in terms of their ability to 'get things done' in the second language,
rather than on their accuracy in using certain grammatical features.
The chart in Thble
er
as.
similar to the one in Table 2. 1 in Chapter 2. In thar
chart, we compared the profiles of first and second language learners. In this
one, we compare natural and instructional contexts for second language
learning. Think about the characteristics of the four contexts represented breach column. For each context, decide whether the characteristics on the left
are present or absent. Mark a plus (+) in the table if the characteristic is
rypical of that context. Mark a minus (-) if it is somethingyou usually do nor
find in that context. Write'?' ifyou are nor sure. Note that the'Communicative instruction column has been subdivided into teacher-student and
student-student interaction. \X4rat happens when learners talk to each
other? Is that different from what happens in teacher-student interaction?
5. 1 is
Ar
;
sP,
Va
dis
Pr<
I
,\c(
Prot(
,;bl,
.II
-i6
Dv
.
t-L
tl
As you look at the pattern of + and * signs you have placed in the chart, yor-r
will probably find it matches the following descriptions.
!af
\7hen people learn languages at work, in social interactions, or in
lil
: io
-*:
playground, their experiences
in classrooms.
are often
th.=
quite different from those of learners
.fL
In natural acqaisition senings
.
Language is not presented step by step. In natural communicatir-e
interactions, the learner is exposed to a wide variery of vocabulary anc
structures.
.
Learners'errors are rarely corrected. Iftheir interlocutors can understani
what they are saying, they do not remark on the correctness of the learners'
speech. They would probably feel it was rude to do so.
-,ri)1
:
-:Ui
-,
-1
"tr
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second languttge
classroom
Structure-based
Characteristics
instruction
Learning one thing at a
time
Frequent feedback on
errors
Ample time for learning
High ratio of native
speakers to learners
Variety of language and
discourse types
Pressure to speak
Access
to modified input
Photocopiable
Tlble
'
@
Oxford University
5.1 Contexts for
Press
language learning
The learner is surrounded by the language for many hours each
day.
Sometimes the language is addressed to the learner; somerimes it is simply
overheard.
'
The learner usually encounrers
a
number of different people who use the
target language profi ciently.
'
Learners observe or parricipare in many different types oflanguage eyenrs:
brief greetings, commercial transacrions, exchanges of information,
arguments, instructions at school or in the workplace. Older children and
adults may also encounter the written language in the form of notices,
newspapers, posters, etc.
'
Learners must often use their limited second language abiliry to respond to
questions or get information. In these siruations, the emphasis is on getting
meaning across clearly, and more proficient speakers tend to be tolerant of
errors that do not interfere with meaning.
'
Modified input is available in many one-ro-one conversations. In
situations where many native speakers are involved in the conversation,
however, the learner may have difficulry getting access to language he or
she can understand.
I
1
I
I12
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
The events and activities that are typical of structure-based instruction differ
from those encounrered in natural acquisition settings. In cneulreR
Grarr
ASSUIT
TRANSLATIoN approaches, there is considerable use of reading and writing,
as learners translate texts from one language to another and grammar rules
langu
are taught explicitly. In
euororrNGUAL approaches there is little use of the
first language, and learners are expected to learn mainly through repetition
and habit formation, although they may be asked to figure o,-tt ih. gi"--",
rules for rhe sentences they have memorized.
.
'
cor,
'
Itp
Pro
Pfo
.
fn stuucture- based instructional settings
' Linguistic items are presented and practised in isolarion, one item ar a rime,
in a sequence from what teachers or textbook writers believe is 'simple' to
that which is 'complex'.
o
In
Thr
met
eacl
Bec;
serY
Errors are frequently corrected. Accuracy tends to be given prioriry over
meaningful interaction.
Learning is often limited to a few hours
a
stud
.
Lear
fron
week.
opp(
do, i
stud,
The teacher is often the only narive or proficient speaker the student
comes in contact with, especially in situations of roRrrcN LANGUAGE
LEARNING.
resp(
'
limited range of language discourse rypes. The most
rypical of these is the Initiation/Response/Evaluation (IRE) exchange
where the teacher asks a question, a student answers, and the teach.,
evaluares the response. The written language they encounter is selected
primarily to provide practice with specific grammatical features rather than
for its content.
'Asin
'
students often feel pressure to speak or write the second language and to
do so correctly from the very beginning.
langu
This r
'
Teachers often use the learners' native language to give instructions or in
classroom managemenr eyents. \7hen they uie the target language, they
rl'here
Students experience
a
subje
for la
rvher
are al
profi<
oppor
most I
tend to modi$. their language in order ro ensure .ompreh.nrion and
compliance.
,\ vari
qfoup
Language classrooms are not all alike. The conditions for learning differ in
,elevis
terms of the physical environment, the age and motivation of the students,
the amount of time available for learning, and many other variables.
Classrooms also differ in rerms of the principles that guide teachers in their
language teaching methods and techniques. Designers of communicative
language teaching programm€s have sought to replace some of the characreristics ofstructure-based instruction with those more rypical of natural acqui-
sition contexts.
In
communicative and content-based instruction, ih.
emphasis is on the communication of meaning, both berween teacher and
students and among the students themselves in group or pair work.
-is'COII(
-aftef
rleanir
,
'tuden
-
lrnctic
.here
'tren a
.r rhe e
Obseruing harning and teaching in the second language classroom
Grammatical forms are focused on only in order to clarify meaning. The
assumption is that, in focusing on meaning, learners will acquire the
language in a way that is similar to natural acquisition.
In cornmunicatiae irustructional settings
' Input is simplified and made comprehensible by the use ofconrextual cues,
props, and gestures, rather than through strucrural grading. Students
provide each other with simplified and sometimes erroneous input.
'
.
There is a limited amount oferror correction on the part ofthe teacher, and
meaning is emphasized over form. Students tend not to overtly correct
each other's errors when they are engaged in communicative practice.
Because the focus is on meaning, however, requests for clarification may
serve as implicit feedback. The need to negotiate for meaning may help
students see the need to say something in a differenr way.
Learners usually have only limited time for learning. In a typical teacherfronted classroom with25-30 students, individual students get very little
opportunity to produce language in a sixty-minute class, and when they
do, it's usually in the form ofa short response to a teacher's question. \7hen
students work in pairs or groups, they have opportunities to produce and
respond to a greater amount and variery oflanguage. Sometimes, however,
subject-matt€r courses taught through the second language can add time
for language learning. A good example of this is in immersion programmes
where most or all the subject marrer is taught to a group of students who
are all second language learners.
.
'
As in structure-based instruction, it is usually only the teacher who is a
proficient speaker. Learners have considerable exposure to the interlanguage of other learners, particularly in student-student interaction.
This naturally contains errors that would nor be heard in an environmenr
where the interlocutors are native speakers, but it provides many more
opportunities for students ro use the target language than is the case in
most structure-based instruction.
A variery of discourse rypes may be introduced through stories, peer- and
group-work, the use of 'authentic' materials such as newspapers and
television broadcasts. Text materials may include both those modified for
second language learners and those intended for native speakers. In the
latter case, teachers use instructional strategies to help learners get the
meaning, even iftheydo not know all thewords and sffuctures. In studentstudent interaction, learners may practise a range of sociolinguistic and
functional features o f lan guage thro ugh role-play.
.
There is little pressure to perform at high levels of accuracy, and there is
often a greater emphasis on comprehension than on producrion, especially
in the early stages oflearning.
r13
n4
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second. langudge classroom
.
Modified input is a defining feature of this approach to instruction. The
teacher makes every effort to speak to students in a level of language they
can understand. If students speak the same first language, they may have
little difficulty in understanding each other. If they come from differenr
language backgrounds, they may modify their language as they seek to
communicate successfully.
E"A-*
igrygt
;-heth<
-,assro,
:-cordi
'lne
e
:.assro,
:ori
'-995)
:r tern
un'L'aty
utlicL
nrryry aTpctrs afre *he
5t+D1€c*-, uuSC uy*" lo g.r,Cttrce
The
verb,
i4ab^.t- ?o\Eton...
,.rt
A
.:riviti
--:mo
*,- tol
::Lrdu(
.:gua
<=:
:WrvL"
a Erb ?
?)a
(_al
.,@$gr
General descriptions of classroom instruction such as those above cannor
capture the individual characteristics of particular classrooms. For this
reason, researchers have developed a number of ways to study classroom
learning and teaching. \7e will discuss two approaches to classroom research
in this chapter. 'We will look first at observarion schemes, in which
researchers anticipate the occurrence ofparticular events and behaviours and
make note of them within preplanned frameworks or checklists. Then we
will look at classroom ethnography, an approach that requires the observer to
describe what happens in the classroom, trylng not to limit the observation
to any predetermined categories or expectations.
Observation schemes
Many different obseryation schemes have been developed for use in second
language classrooms. They differ in several respects, including the number of
categories they contain, whether they focus on qualitative or quantitative
descriptions, and whether they are used throughout a lesson or on selected
samples of classroom interaction. The schemes also differ in relation to
any
..;cher
l-:
C
:.,.ted
:-.-".
a.]
::.':10
: :- -.-,\\
-..:-sor
-.
i.:
ire
- isrt
Clas,
:...;::p
:"=r:i.
-.:a:ac
- ":
::oa
: .::l
-r:
::Lla
'.
::- f
-
-
t'oli
-_-1
€
:'l ::is
-.t-
-
:
--i1;
.' . -,';j
..-r!
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
t,'hether they are used by observers in'real time'while they are in the
classroom, or used later outside the classroom to analyse audio or video
recordings or transcripts ofsuch recordings.
One example of a scheme developed specifically for second language
;lassrooms is the Communicative Orientation of Language Teaching
COLT) Observation Scheme described by Nina Spada and Maria Frcjhlich
' 1995). COLT is divided into tr,vo parts. Part A describes teaching practices
in terms of content, focus, and organization of activiry types. \When using
Part A, the observer can record, for example, whether the pedagogical
activities are teacher- or learner-centred, whether the focus is on language
iorm or meaning, and whether there are opportunities for students to choose
the topics for discussion. Part B describes specific aspects of the language
produced by teachers and students, for example, how much (or how little)
language students produce, whether their language production is restricted
in any walz, the kinds of questions teachers ask, and whether and how
teachers respond to learners' errors.
scheme and others like it have been used primarily in classroom
research that is intended to look at how differences in teaching practices are
The
COLT
related to differences
in second
language learning. Observation schemes
in the professional
have also been used in the training of new teachers and
development of experienced ones.
Below is an activity in which you are asked to use a set of pre-determined
categories similar to those used in the COLT scheme to characterize the
nature of interaction berween teachers and students and between students
and students.
C lassro om com?arisons
:
Tbac lter-student
interactions
Excerpts from four transcripts ofsecond language classroom interaction are
given in this and the following section. The first two present teacher-student
interaction. The transcripts come from classrooms that differ in their
approach to second language teaching; one of them represents structurebased instruction; the other, a communicative approach. Structure-based
approaches emphasize language form through either metalinguistic instruction (for example, grammar translation) or pattern practice (for example,
audiolingual).
\7ith
each transcript, there is a chart where you can indicate whether certain
things are happening in the interaction, from the point ofview of the teacher
and that of the students. Before you begin reading the transcripts, study the
following interpretations of the categories used in the grids:
I
Errors: are there errors in the language of either the teacher or the students?
115
116
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
2
Feedback on errors: when students make errors, do they receive feedback?
Fromwhom?
s
1
3 csNurNn euESTroNS: do teachers
and students ask questions to which
they don't know the answer in advance?
I
S
4 olsprey euESTroNs: do
teachers ask questions that they know the
answers to so that learners can display their knowledge of the language (or
lack of it)?
5 Negotiation of meaning:
do the teachers and students work to understand
what the other speakers are saying? \X/hat efforts are made by the teacher?
By the students?
6 Metalinguistic comments; do the teachers and students talk
language, in addition to using
s
I
s
1
s
I
s
1
about
it to transmit information?
In the following excerpts, T represents the teacher, s represents a student.
(The first two classroom examples in this chapter come from unpublished
data collected by P. M. Lightbown, N. Spada, and B. Barkman.)
s
'l
s
I
S
Classroom A: A structure-based approach
(Students in this class are fifteen-year-old French speakers.)
Teacher
1
S
'l
Student
S
Errors
'l
Feedbacl< on
S
errors
I
Genuine questions
s
Display questions
I
Negotiation of meaning
s
1
Metalinguistic comments
s
'l
Photocopiable
T
@
Oxford University
Press
OK, we finished the book-we finished in the book IJnit I , 2, 3.
Finished. \Workbook 1,2,3. So today we're going to start with Unit
4. Dont take your books yet, don'r take your books. In I, 2, 3 we
worked in what tense? \7hat tense did we work on? OK?
S Past.
T In the past-\What auxiliary in the past?
S
T
Did.
Did (writes on board 'I-2-3 Past'). IJnit 4,IJnit 4, we're going ro
work in the present, present progressive, present
continuous-OK? You dont know what it is?
S
'l
Classr
Stude
are tell
x'ritter
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
S
Yes
T
Yes?\fhat is it?
S
Little bit.
T Alinle bit.
S
T
Eh?
S
Uh, present continuous
Present continuous? Whatt that?
T
s e*n-g
T i-r*g
S
Yes.
T \fhat
does that mean, present continuous? You don't know?
fine.'What are you doing, Paul?
S
OK,
Rien fnothingl.
T Nothing?
S Rien-nothing.
T You're not doing anything? You're doing something!
S
Not doing anything.
T
You're doing somethingl
S
S
Not doing anything.
Youre doing something-Are, are you listening to
talking with Marc? \X4rat are you doing?
No, no-uh-listen-uh-
T
Eh?
T
S
to you.
T
You're you're listening to me.
S
Yes.
T Oh. (writes ''What
are you doing?
me? Are
you
I'm listening to you' on the
board).
s
je-[I...].
T \(hat
are
you-?
You're excited.
S
Yes.
T
You're playing with your eraser. (writes 'I'm playing with my eraser'
on the board). \Would you close the door please, Bernard? Claude,
what
S
T
is he doing?
Close the door.
He is closing the door. (writes 'He's closing the door' on the board).
\7hat are you doing, Mario?
Classroom B: A communicative approach
(Students in this class are ten-year-old French speakers. In this activiry they
are telling their teacher and their classnrates what 'bugs' them. They have
written 'what bugs them' on a card or paper that they hold while speaking.)
117
1
1B
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
Teacher
T
Student
S
Errors
T
Feedback on errors
T
S
S
Genuine questions
T
Display questions
S
T
Negotiation of meaning
S
T
Metalinguistic comments
S
Photocopiable
@
Oxford University
T
Press
S
It bugs me when a bee string me.
T Oh, when a bee stings me.
S
T
T
Stings me.
Do you get stung often? Does that happen ofteni The bee stinging
many times?
S
Yeah.
S
T
S
T
S
T
Often? (Teacher rurns to students u'ho arent paying atrenrion)
OK. Sandra and Benoit, you may begin working on a research
project, hey? (Teacher turns her attention back to '\Whar bugs me'
It bugs me (inaudible) and my sister pur on my clothes.
Ahl She borrows your clothes? \When you're older, you may
appreciate it because you can switch clothes, maybe. (Tirrns to
check another studentt written work) Milanie, this is yours, I wili
check.
good.
It bugs -OK.It's
me when I'm sick and my brother doesnt help
me-my-my brother, 'cause he-me-.
S
T
S
T Every day? Ah! Doesnt your bro-(inaudible)
his bicycle? Could
his brother lend his bicycle? Uh, your brother doesnt have a
bicycle?
T
S
Charac
,
- rmpar
.::rguag(
:;;h of
YeahlA new bicycle (inaudible) bicycle.
Ah, well. Talk to your mom and dad about it. Maybe negoriate a
new bicycle for your brother.
(inaudible)
I
:,ierent
,llassroo
-
Errors
some I
qr
:he
OK. You know-when (inaudible) sick, you're sick at home in bed
and you say, oh, ro your brother or youruisterr '\(orrld you please
get mea drink ofwater?'-Ah! Drop dead!'you know, 'Go play in
the ffafficl'You know, it's not very nice. Martin!
It bug ms 1s hxysIt bugs me. It 6ugzzme.
It bugs me when my brother takes my bicycle. Every day.
S
S
T
.ntona
nake
:rsuall;
-
:eedb:
:-acts.
j
ilenui
--,aSSfO
+ )ispla
.lnreres
.r
genl
:;acher
tiesen
*oublt
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
T
He has a new bicycle. But his brother needs
a
new one too.
S
Yes!
T
Hey, whoa, just a minute!Jean?
S
Martint brother hasMartin, who has a new bicycle? You or your brother?
Mybrother.
T
S
T And you have an old one.
S (inaudible)
T And your brother takes your old one?
S (inaudible)bicycle.
T His bicycle! How old is your brother?
S March 23.
T His birthday?
S
Yeah!
T Andhowoldwas he?
S Fourteen.
T Fourteen. tWell, why dont you tell your brother that when he takes
your bike you will take his bike? And he may have more scratches
than he figures for. OK?
Characteristics of input and interaction
Compare the two charts you have completed so far.-$7hat kinds of second
Ianguage input and opportunities for interaction are available to learners in
each of the environments that these transcripts exemplifir? How are they
different?
ChssroomA
1 Errors: Very few on the part of the teacher. However her speech does have
some peculiar characteristics rypical of this rype of teaching, for example,
the questions in statement form-often asked with dramatic rising
intonation (for example, 'You dont know what it is?'). Students dont
make too many errors because they say very little and what they say is
usually limited by the lesson.
2
Feedback on errors: Yes, whenever students do make errors, the teacher
reacts.
3 Genuine questions: Yes, a few, but they are almost always related to
classroom management. No questions from the students.
4
Display questions: Yes, almost all of the teacher's questions are of this rype.
Interestingly, however, the students sometimes interpret display questions
as genuine questions (T \(hat are you doing, Paul? S: Nothing.). The
teacher wants students to produce a sentence-any sentence-in the
'present continuous' but the student worries that het about to get in
trouble and asserts that he is doing 'nothing'. This is a good example of
119
120
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
how the teachert pragmatic intent can be misinterpreted by the student,
and of how strongly we seek to find genuine meaning in language.
5 Negotiation of meaning: Very little,
learners have no need to paraphrase
or request clarifications, and no opportuniry to determine the direction of
the discourse; the teacher is focused only on the formal aspects of the
learners' language. All the effort goes into getting students to produce a
sentence with the present continuous form of the verb.
6 Metalinguistic
comments: Yes, this is how the teacher begins the lesson
and lets the students know what really mattersl
Classroom B
Errors: Yes, students make errors.
I
And even the teacher says some odd
things sometimes. Her speech also contains incomplete sentences,
simplified ways of speaking, and an informal speech style.
2
Feedback on errors: Yes, sometimes the teacher repears what the student
with the correct form (for example, 'hebugzz me'-emphasizing
the third person singular ending). However, this correction is not
consistent or intrusive as the focus is primarily on letting students express
has said
their meanings.
3
Genuine questions: Yes, almost all of the teachert questions are focused on
getting information from the students. The students are not asking
questions in this exchange. However, they do sometimes intervene to
change the direction of the conversation.
conYe
brief
necess
Clas
Thiss
based
differe
Asint
indica
Comr
The fo
in thei
tiom I
task w
rable, s
iands
rudine
:rand.
--rackel
rnpubl
'l'eemar
Display questions: No, because there is a focus on meaning rather than on
accuacy in grammatical form.
Negotiation of meaning: Yes, from the teacher's side, especially in the long
exchange about who has a bicycle!
5 Metalinguistic
comments: No. Even though the teacher clearly hopes to
get students to use the third person ending, she does not say so in these
words.
You no doubt noticed how strikingly different these two transcripts are, eyen
though the activities in both are reacher-centred. In the transcript from
ClassroomA, the focus is on form (i.e. grammar) and in Classroom B, it is on
meaning. In Classroom A, the only purpose of the interaction is to practise
the present continuous. Although the teacher uses real classroom events and
some humour to accomplish this, there is no real interest in what students
are doing. Rather the teacher is highlighting their ability to say what they are
doing, using the correct verb form. There is a primary focus on correcr
grammar, display questions, and error correction in the transcript from
Classroom A. In the transcript from Classroom B, the focus is on meaning,
5rrors
ieedbz
Genuir
Displal
Negoti
Yetalin
Photocop
S1
S'
SI
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second langudge classroom
conversational interaction, and genuine questions, although there are some
brief references to grammatical accuracy when the teacher feels it
is
necessary.
C lassro om comp aris ons :
Student-student interactions
This section presents some student-student interactions. The transcripts are
based on the interactions between second language learners engaged in
different communicative tasks.
-{s in the previous section, there is a chart
with each transcript where you can
indicate whether certain things are happening in the interaction.
Communication task A: Picture description
The following transcript is of two girls aged ll-I2years, both ESL learners
in their first year of learning English in Australia. The first learner (S1) is
liom Hong Kong; the second (S2) is from Somalia. They are engaged in a
taskwhere S1 is describing a picture for 52 to draw. They are sitting at a
table, separated by a small barrier, so that they can see each other's faces and
hands (when they gesture), but not each othert work. The picture is a black
outline containing stick figures-a boy flying a kite and a girl holding his
hand. The stick figures are standing on some grass near a tree. Square
brackets indicate non-target pronunciation. (This transcript comes from
unpublished data collected by Alison Mackey, Rhonda Oliver and Jennifer
Leeman.)
Student
I
Student 2
Errors
Feedback on errors
Genuine questions
Display questions
Negotiation of meaning
Metalinguistic comments
Photocopiable
S
I
@
Oxford University
And o-on the right, there is a ltree]. Itt a- a, the ki-, the kite is up.
(Points up in the air) This is the kite. (Points up again)This is the
kite. (Points yet again) And the [tree] is up there.
52 Three bird?
Sl
Press
Huh?
t21
t22
second langua'ge classroom
Obseruing learning and' teaching in the
52 Is a three bird?
51 Huh?
52 UP, uP-uP the kite?
s1 Yeah, the kite is u-, the kite is up and the [tree]
L
r..o,
is down' (Points
directions)
52 The lbird] down?
51 The kite-, the ftree] is down'
52 \(hat's the iteel?
51 Huh?
's production)
52 \X4rat's the ltree] ? (Imitates Learner 1
51 Feel?
52 Fell?
Fell down? (Points down)
just at the bottom'
S1 No, it's not ih. ftlt down' No' it's
52 The bird?
S1 No' the tree.
52 The tree? (EmPhatic stress)
S1 Yes.
52 It is left and right?
51 kt right. (Points)
52 It's long? It's llittle]?
51 lt's-what?
52 lt's long and [litde]?
51 Um, a little-. It-' um, a middle size'
52 Middle size tree?
51 Yes.
52 It's little. (Says as drawing the tree)
Communication task B: Jigsaw
7 French immersion
The following transcript is of two students in a grade
of eight
based.9"
jigsaw activiry
lherseries
classroom. They are engaged in a
clock
alarm
bv
p.,"'* iJringih.;,;;;?; vgu"g.ii'l being awakened
other
the
s I' 3' 5 andZ ' and
early in the morning. 6* tt"at"in"as pitt"i-t
turns telling the story pory"y:d
student has pirrurr]z,-4, e , ""J a' rhty t"kt
ir"v aisplav all the pictures '.1::T:?* and write
".,i,r'*
the use of a number of
the story ,r,.y r,"u.lu" i"iJ' rtrri"g iht "o'y 'equires
the form 'se' is placed berween
reflexive verbs in e1*.t . i" rhe thlrd person,
('she gets up') and elle se
.n. *ui.* "rrd th. irrite verb. Thus, elle se lbue
Incorrect uses of ih.se reflexive verbs
souuient (,sh. ,.-.L[.rs') are correct.
;il;il;.,
areinitalics.irrttrirtr*rcriptthelearnersarecalledDara(D)andNina(N)'
ifn. d"," are from Swain and Lapkin 2002')
l
rt"",
]
|
""n.
I Displ
I Negc
Metal
Photoc,
I
I
I
l
T
I
T
I
T
I
f
h
I
I
N
D
N
D
N
D
N
Obseraing learning and teaching in the second langua'ge
Dara
classroom I23
Nina
Errors
Feedback on errors
Genuine questions
Display questions
Negotiation of meaning
Metalinguistic comments
Photocopiable
D
D
@
Oxford University
Press
Elle. [She]
Elle se souvient, non, Elle souuient
qi elle a un ... une pratique de
chorale, alors elle se lbve. I She remembers, no. She remembers that
she has a choir practice so she gets upJ
D Tout ) coup fSuddenly]
N Elle ssse [She ssse]
D Elle souuientlShe remembers]
N se souvie nt ov souaiezr ? lRemembers or remembers?]
D Elle souuient ... ahh, elle se souvient ... Elle souuient ... Elle se
souvient, no. [She remembers ... ahh, she remembers ... She
remembers ... She remembers, no]
N
D
pas... [not]
Elle souuient qi elle doit aller au band ... ? [She remembers that she
has to go to bandl
N Chorale. lChoir]
D Chorale. lChoir]
N Tout ) coup elle souuient qu'elle ... doit aller ) la chorale [All of
a
sudden she remembers that she has to go to choir]
D fvery softlyl elle se souvien ... non. lShe remembers . .. no]
D Alors, elle [So, she]
N non, wait. tout i coup elle ...
sudden she ... remembers?]
D Je pense pas que c'est se souvient. [I dont think it's remember]
N oh, souvient ... souvient. [oh remember ... remember]
D Elle souuientqu elle a le chorale. [She remembers that she has choir]
N Qu elle doit se prdparer. [that she has to get ready]
D Oui. [Yes]
N pour le chorale ... non, tout i coup elle souuientqu il ya une pratique
de chorale. lfor choir ... ro, all of a sudden she remembers that
there's a choir practice]
r24
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second langudge classroom
Characteristics of input and interaction
Compare the two charts you have completed. As before, what kinds of
second language input and opportunities for interacrion are available to
learners in each of the environments that these transcripts exemplify? How
are they different from each other and the teacher-student interaction you
looked at previously?
Commuruication tashA
1 Errors: There are many errors in the speech of both learners. This includes
grammatical and pronunciation errors. These errors are present in several
breakdowns in the learners' conversation.
2
Feedbackon errors: There is no error correction in terms of form
as the
learners struggle to understand each other's meaning. The difficulry they
are having in communication may serve as a kind of implicit feedback.
That is, the fact that the interlocutor does not understand may signal that
there is something wrong with what they have said.
3
Genuine questions: Yes, there are many genuine questions. Naturally,
Student 2 asks most of these questions because he needs to ger the information from Student 1 in order to draw the picture. Student 1 also asks some
genuine questions and these are almost always to ask for clarification.
4 Display
questions:
No, there are no display questions
because they
engaged in a real communication gap exchange. Student 2 cannotsee the
picture that student 1 possesses. Therefore all the questions asked are'real'
questions.
5 Negotiation of meaning:
Yes, indeed! Both learners are trying hard to
understand each other, even though they often fail to do so. Thii involves
many comprehension questions and clarification requesrs, as well as
repetitions of each other's utterances, often with emphasis, trying to
4 Dis
acti
que
5 Ne1
hav
ATI
cor
6Me
'vet
on
These
each
exclus
comp
hensir
the
se
both
r
explic
refex.
of the
In tht
reachr
categ(
broad
speci{
sectio
instru
and
f<
understand what the other learner has just said.
6 Metalinguistic
Cor
commenrs: None.
Conzmunication tash B
1 Errors: Both learners make several grammatical errors, most notably the
repeated failure to produce the refexive form of the verb
2
se
souuenir.
Feedback on errors: There is no actual error correction provided. Neither
learner
is really sure what the correcr form is.
Instead, there is
metalinguistic reflection and discussion as they try to figure out whether
they are using the correcr form of the verb se souuenir.
3
Genuine questions: The questions that are asked are genuine. The content
is language form, but the students are genuinely sharing information
about how to complete the task.
Studl
Roy
whicl
J
exami
This
r
langu
(i.e.
c,
secon
They
feedb
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second langadge
classroom I25
4 Display questions: There are no display questions. The students are
actively collaborating to reconstruct the story and are asking 'real'
questions of each other.
5 Negotiation of meaning: At this point in the interaction, the students
have agreed on the content of the story. Thus, there is more NEGoTIATroN oF FoRM, that is, more discussion of whether they are using the
correct forms to say what theyve agreed they want to say.
6 Metalinguistic comments: Although they are not using words such as
'verb' or 'pronoun', the students are talking about language as they focus
on trying to find the right form.
These two transcripts of student-student interaction are very different from
each other. In the first communication task, the children are focused
exclusively on meaning and on trying to understand each other in order to
complete the information gap activiry. They are constantly using comprehension and clarification requests as they negotiate meaning in this task. In
the second student-student transcript, however, the learners are focused on
both form and meaning. \X/hile reconstructing the story, they make several
explicit statements about whether they are using the correct form of the
refexive verb se souuenirand continually question the grammatical accuracy
of their use of this form as they continue to discuss the content of the story.
In the activities in the preceding pages, we have described and compared
teacher-student and student-student interaction in terms of six observation
categories. Some observation schemes use many more categories, covering a
broad range of instructional practices and procedures. Others focus on one
specific feature of classroom instruction and interaction. In the following
sections, we review eight studies in which one particular feature of
instruction has been examined. Four studies examine corrective feedback
and four investigate teachers' use ofquestions.
Correctiue feedbach
in
th e classro om
Study 1: Recasts in content-based classrootns
Roy Lyster and Leila Ranta (1997) developed an observational scheme
ivhich describes different types offeedback teachers give on errors and also
examines student uprerB-how they immediately respond to the feedback.
This scheme was developed in French immersion classrooms where second
language students learn the target language via subject-matter instruction
(i.e. content-based instruction). It may also be used to describe other types of
second language instruction as well.
They developed their scheme by observing the different types of corrective
feedback provided during interaction in four French immersion classrooms
r26
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second. langulge classroom
with 9-11 year-old students. They began their observations by using a
combination ofsome categories from Part B ofthe CoLT scheme and other
categories from models that had examined feedback in both first and second
language learning. They adjusted some of the categories to fit their data, and
they also developed additional categories. This resulted in the identification
met
:ern
t
:'
ilne
quer
lnfo
of six feedback types, defined below. The definitions are taken from Lyster
and Ranta (1997). The examples come from 10-1 1 year-old studenrs in an
ESL class.
Explicit correction refers to the explicit provision of the correct form. As the
teacher provides the correct form, he or she clearly indicates that what the
student had said was incorrect (for example, 'Oh, you mean . ..', 'You should
say
rhe
r
_t\Yn
' . .').
S
T
-llLt
The dog run fastly.
'Fastly doesnt exisr. 'Fast' does not take -fu.Thatb why i picked
'quickly'.
Recasts involve the teacher's reformulation of all or parr of a student's
utterance, minus the error. Recasts are generally implicit in that they are not
introduced by'You mean', 'Use this word', or'You should say.'
51 V&yyou dont like Marc?
T \Vhy don't you like Marc?
52 I dont know, I dont like him.
-'lici
Rept
utte
rhe,
In tl
Note that in this example the teacher does not seem ro expect uptake from
s 1. It seems she is merely reformulating the question S t has asked s2.
Clarifcation requests indicate to students either that their utterance has been
misunderstood by the teacher or that the utterance is incorrect in some way
and that a_repetition or a reformulation is required. A clarification requesr
includes phrases such as'Pardon me ...'It may also include a repetition of
the error
T
S
T
S
T
S
T
S
as
in '\7hat do you mean by ...
Howoftendoyouwash the
"rd
?'
dishes?
Fourteen.
Excuse me. (Clarification request)
Fourteen.
Fourteen what? (Clarification request)
Fourteen for aweek.
Fourteen times a week? (Recast)
Yes.
Int
Lunchanddinner.
Mualingwistic feedback contains comments, information, or quesrions
related to the correctness of the studentt utterance, without ixplicitly
providing the correc form. Metalinguistic comments generally indicate that
there is an error somewhere (for example, 'can you find your error?'). Also,
i YSt
imn
=ed
f ro\,
:act
=o,
---'De
I
::sul
IOIIT
vstr
-r?SS
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
metalinguistic information generally provides either some grammatical
terminology that refers to the nature of the error (for example, 'It's masculine') or a word definition in the case of lexical errors. Metalinguistic
questions also point to the nature of the error but attempt to elicit the
information from the student (for example, 'Is it feminine?').
S
.W'e
T
look at the people yesterday.
\7hat's the ending we put on verbs when we talk about the past?
S
e-d
Elicitationrefers to at least three techniques that teachers use to directly elicit
the correct form from the students. First, teachers elicit completion of their
own utterance (for example,'It's a...'). Second, teachers use questions to
elicit correct forms (for example, ... 'How do we say x in English?'). Third,
teachers occasionally ask students to reformulate their utterance.
S
T
S
My father cleans the plate.
Excuse me, he cleans the
???
Plates?
Repetition refers to the teachert repetition of the student's erroneous
utterance. In most cases, teachers adjust their intonation so as to highlight
the error.
In this example, the repetition is followed by a recast:
S He's in the bathroom.
T Bathroom? Bedroom. He's in the bedroom.
In the next example, the repetition is followed by metalinguistic comment
and explicit correction:
S
T
\We is ...
\We is? But it's
two people, right? You
error?'{/hen it's plural it's 'we are'.
see
your mistake? You
see
the
Lyster and Ranta found that all teachers in the content-based French
immersion classes they observed used recasts more than any other rype of
feedback. Indeed, recasts accounted for more than half of the total feedback
provided in the four classes. Repetition of error was the least frequent feedback rype provided. The other types of corrective feedback fell in between.
They also found that student uptake was least likely to occur after recasts and
more likely to occur after clarification requests, metalinguistic feedback, and
repetitions. Furthermore, elicitations and metalinguistic feedback not only
resulted in more uptake, they were also more likely to lead to a corrected
form ofthe original utterance.
Lyster (1998) has argued that students in content-based second language
classrooms (where the emphasis is on meaning not form) are less likely to
r27
128
obseruing learning and rcaching in the second langudge classroom
notice recasts than other forms of error correction. In this rype of
instruction, students may assume that the teacher is responding'to th.
content rather than the form of their speech. Indeed, the double chjlenge of
making the subject-matter comprehensible and enhancing knowledg. o?th.
second language itself within subject-matter instructioln h", leJMerrill
Swain (1988) and others to conclude thar 'nor all content teaching is
necessarily good_ language teaching' (p. 68). The challenges of content-based
instruction will be discussed more generally in Chapter Z.
Since Lyster and Ranta reported their findings, several other observation
studies of the rype of correcive feedback provided in second or foreign
language classrooms have been carried o,rt. so-. of them report simiLr
1s5uh5-*rst recasts are the most frequently occurring rype tf fe.dback
provided by teachers and that they appear to go .ttrn"ori..d by learners.
However, others repoft that learners do notice recasts in the classroom.
Below, rwo studies are described in which learners were observed to notice
and to respond ro recasts provided by their teachers.
Study 2: Recasts andpriuate speech
In a study with adult foreign language
learners of Japanese, Amy
ohta
(2000) examined the oral language that learne* adJresr.d to themselves
during classroom activities. She was able to obtain this pnrvern speBcn by
attaching microphones to individual students during classroom interaction.
The classroom interaction consisted of a fo.,r, o.-r grammar and metalinguistic instruction. In this context, ohta discov.redihat learners noticed
recasrs when they were provided by the instrucror. Furthermore,
learners
were more likely to react ro a recasr with private speech when it was
directed
to another learner or to the whole clasi rather ihan wh.., rhe recast was
directed to their own errors. on the basis of these findings, she concluded
that recasts do. get noticed in classroom interaction even if tley do not
lead to
uptake from the student who originally produced the error.
Study 3: Recasts and ulttahe
In a descriptive classroom.study
with adult learners of English as a second
language, Rod Ellis, Helen Basturkmen, and shawn io.*., (2001)
observed the rypes of corrective feedback provided by teachers and the
learners'immediate responses to it (i.e. uptak ). They observed that most of
the teachers' responses to the learners' .rror, a"*. in the form of recasts.
They also observed that learners immediately reacted. ro mosr of these
recasts. Both the frequency ofrecasrs and learners' responses to them led
the
researchers to conclude, like ohta, that learners notice and respond
to
r€casts in ways that may contribute positively to their second Lrrg,r"g.
development.
studies
r,2,
useful
mayh
3 wer,
childr,
gramn
by cor
of the
attenti
form.
langua
the Frr
were I
utteral
The in
learner
the cla
Study.
Rhond
an Aur
gated ,
differe,
identif
aonten
questic
in whi
approp
:he em
erplicir
and the
Oliverr
ihe
co:
qreatesl
".11
inst
]'anguag
inanagc
=edbac
.'rplicit
:ommu
:ngly,
tl
used at
and 3 used similar categories to describe feedback on error and
students' reaction to
it in different classroom environments. This permits
:ontent
.rplicit
obseruing learning and teaching in the second language
classroom r29
useful comparisons a1{ an insight into how the same teaching
behaviour
may have a different effect in a different situation. Learners in
Siudies 2 and
3 were adults in small classes. Those in the Lyster and Ranta study were
children. Furthermore, in the ESL class, learners received an hour
ofexplicit
grammatical instruction prior to the observation period. This was
followed
by communicative activities to practise the ,tructure taught in
the first part
of the lesson. Thus,- it is possible that this primed' tf;e learners to pay
attention to form and therefore led them to respond to recasts as feedback
on
form. similarly' students in. the Japanese foieign language crass received
langrrage-focused as opposed to th. cont.t t-b"r].d inri.r.?ior,
frovided in
the French immersion context in the Lyster and Ranta study. ThLs,
they too
were more likely to perceive recasrs as feedback on th. form of
their
utterances.
The importance of conrexr and how it contributes to different
ways in which
learners perceive and respond ro correcrive feedback is further
higilighted in
the classroom study described below.
Study 4: Correctiuefeedbach in context
Rhonda oliver andAlison_Mackey (2003) carried out a descriptive
study of
an Australian primary ESL classroom with 6-72 year-otds. They
investigated whether teachers' provision and learners' ure of correcrive
feedback
depending on varying contexrs for interaction in a resson. They
flff.t.d
identified four contexts in which teachers and learners interacted: (1)
content exchanges in which the teacher imparted knowledge
or asked
questions about the conrent of the curriculum; (2) managem.rit.".h"rrg.,
in which the teacher talked about the organization # th.
lesson and
appropriate classroom behaviour; (3) communication
exchanges in which
the_emphasis was on stt'den* using English in meaningf"i;;;,
and, (4)
exp,licit lang'rage-focused excharg.J*hJr. the emphasf
;", ;; grammar
and the use of metalinguistic termlnology.
oliver and Mackey found-that learners produced significantry more
errors in
the communication exchanges. Thus opportunlties for feedback were
greatest in this conrext. The researchers found that feedback
was provided
in
all instructional contexts but that it was most frequent i' ir. explicit
language-focused exchanges, followed by content, communication,
and.
management. \when they examined how learners reacted to
the correctiye
feedback, they found rhat learners modified their output
-o* of,.., within
explicit language-focused exchanges, only ,ome of the time in content
and
communication exchanges, and never in management exchanges.
Interestingly, the types ofcorrective feedback also varied-across .orrr."rri...asrs
were
at a consistently high rate in management, communication, and
content exchanges' but less so in explicii language-focused exchanges;
explicit corrective feedback was rarely provided?urlng .o.r,..rt, manageused
130
obseruing learning and teaching in the second rangudge crassroom
ment, and communication exchanges, an-d frequently during explicit
language-focused conrexts. The ,.rl,rl,, of this'rt,rdy .-fh"rir.
ho*
impo-rtant it is to keep in mind differences in the instruction"l
.ontert wh..,
we talk about teacher feedback and learner response to it.
other factors that may affect learners' reacions to different rypes offeedback
include age and Iearning goals. Adults are probably more fi(.iy
to recognize
recasts as feedback on language form p"rticularly if
high l.rr.i, of
and native-like perform"r..ln the second o, 'for.igi l"rrgr"g. "..,ir".y
are their
display
q
of
series
'Does he
this cont
purpose.
pedagogi
of displa;
Study 6:
goals.
In a case
Questions in tlte classroom
her instrt
Richard
Teachers'.questioning behaviour has been the focus ofa good
deal of research
in second language classrooms. euestions are fundatental in engaging
students in interaction and in exploring how much they
understand. Two
types of questions that have b.i.t .*ti.rsively examin.d
display and
genuine and the role theyplayin classroom interaction has "r.
been examined in
a number of studies.
Study 5: Teaclters'questions in ESL classrooms
Michael Long and charlene sato (1983) examined the forms
and functions
of questions asked by teachers in ESL classrooms and compared
them with
questioning behaviours observed outside the classroom b.n"..r,
non-native speakers. They were particularly interested
between the quantity
of 'display
in
native and
differences
(i.e. referentiar/genuine)
questions. Audio-recordings made"nd^'infor*ation'
of the interactions berween teachers and
students in six adult ESL ilasses reveared that teachers
asked more display
questions than information questions. In the native
speaker/non-native
speaker conversarions outside the classroom, referentiar
q;;;o;, *.re more
frequent than display questions. The research.r, .or.l,rded
that teacherlearner rnreraction is a 'greatly distorted version of its
equivalent in the real
world' (p.284), and they argued that the interactional structure
ofclassroom
conversarion should be changed.
chose th,
'mediatio
i.p. 184).
expert) s1
assistance
ior
exam
or ltmrtrn
rhe teach
contribut
example
'\X/ho
because ir
T
S1
S2
S3
T
Ss
T
Ss
S4
T
ing behaviour have also reported disproportionately higher numbers
of
"rrr*Jrr.
More recendy, however, a re-evaluation of display questions
has taken place.
This is based on the observation that th.r.
iiff.r.rr,
i"
"r.
-"y,
which
1
usr
display qr
Since the Long and Sato study, other classroom stud.ies
on teachers, question-
display to referential questions. In th. cont."t of .o-m.r"nic"ti
r. language
teaching, teachers have been urged to use fewer display questions
because
they are thought to lead to short,i_imple response, ,t r.quire
little cognitive
effort on the part of the learner. Insiead, they h"rre "ib...r'.r.our"g.d"to
more referential (or genuine) questions since ih.latrer are
"rk
tho,rghtio require
more cognitive processing and to generate more compl.,
I
\{cCormi,
ihe framer
qoals
in
a
tr
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
display questions can be asked in classrooms. One is for the teacher to ask a
series of questions in a drill-like format such as 'Do you have a brother?',
'Does he have a brother?', 'Do you have a sister?', 'Does she have a sister?' In
this context, display questions do not have a meaningful or communicative
purpose. In other contexts, however, display questions can serve important
pedagogic and interaction functions. The study below describes teachers' use
of display questions in a more positive light.
Study 6:
Scffi lding and display and referential
In a case study of one teacher's adult ESL
questions
class, Dawn
McCormick and
Richard Donato (2000) explored how the teachert questions were linked to
her instructional goals. \Torking within sociocultural theory, the researchers
chose the concept of scerrorDlNc to investigate teacher questions as
'mediational tools within the dialogue between the teacher and students'
(p. 184). Scaffolding refers to a process in which a more knowledgeable (or
expeft) speaker helps a less knowledgeable (or novice) learner by providing
assistance. McCormick and Donato identified six functions of scaffolding,
for example, drawing the novice's attention to the task, and simplifying
or limiting the task demands. The researchers examined another function-
the teacherk use of questions during scaffolded interactions-and how it
contributed to class participation and learner comprehension. In the
example below, they argue that the teacher's use of the display question
"Who usually lives in palaces?' serves an important pedagogic function
because it draws the learners' attention to the word 'palace' through the
display question and facilitates the learners' comprehension of the word.
T
Sl
Palace?
Like
castle?
52 Specialplace,verygood.
53 Very nice.
T
Castle, special place, very nice. til/ho usually lives in palaces?
T
Ss
Kings.
Kings, and queens, princes and princesses.
Ss
Yeah
54 M"yb. beautiful house?
T
Big, beautiful house, yeah, really big
McCormick and Donato suggest that questions should be examined within
the framework of scaffolded interaction and with reference to the teachert
goals in a particular lesson or interaction.
r3t
r32
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
Study 7: Olten and closed questions
Another distinction similar to the one between display and genuine
questions is that berween open and closed questions. closed questions
usuallylead to simple one-word responses, making them quick
to
".rd."ry
respond to. open questions lead to longer and more complex
answers,
including, for example, explanation and reasoning. In English-as-a-foreignlanguage classrooms in Austria, christiane Dalton-puffei (zooo) obseried
and audiorecorded the rype of questions asked by teachers, as well as the
responses students gave to them. In content and language-integrated learning (GLIL) classes, students produced a greater quantiry and quality of
output after open questions. Also, open-ended questions that askeJ learners
not just for facts but for reasons or explanations led to the most complex
linguistic ourcomes. Dalton-Puffer concluded that asking
-or. .o-pl.t
open-ended questions would benefit learners in these CLIL classrooms but
that this level of question/response interactions requires a high level of
competence in the foreign language on the part of the teacher.
Study 8:'W'ait time
Another aspect of teachers' questioning behaviour is 'wait dms'-drg
Eth
Anotl
langu
prede
activii
appro
simila
group
resear
classr<
some(
and re
Ethnc
qualit
above
not fo
politi<
amount of time the teacher pauses after having asked a question to give the
student time to respond. Joanna \white and Patsy Light-bown (1982) did a
quantitative analysis of wait time in ESL classes that were audiolingual in
social
their approach. They found that teachers rypically gave students no more
than a second or two before they directed the question to another student or
answered the question themselves. They also tended ro repeat or paraphrase
the question several times rather than silently wait for the studeni ,o for-,rla^te a response. Although such rapid question/answer patrerns were typical
of audiolingual classes, they also occur in communicative instruction.
Finding a balance between placing too much pressure on students to
respond quickly and creating awkrryard silences seems to be a real challenge.
In classrooms with students at different age levels and in different kinds"of
instruction, finding the right balance has been found to lead to students
providing fuller answers, expanding their ideas, and more successfully
processing the material to be learned (Tobin 1987).
silencr
languz
The classroom observation studies we have just described focus on specific
features of classroom interaction. In these studies, the feature of interest was
determined in advance of the observation on the basis of some hypothesis
about what kinds of classroom behaviours are important for learning. \7e
now turn to a different approach.
For e>
Inuit
<
misinr
Here
a
languz
Europ
Study
Karen
vears
\
practi<
enviro
cognit
in
sch,
and
va
childrr
restricr
issues,
the soc
Evider
includ
rng
fea
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second langudge
classroom I33
Ethnography
Another way of observing teaching and learning in second or foreign
language classrooms is to describe classroom behaviours without a set of
predetermined categories. Instead, the observer takes extensive notes of the
activities, practices, and interactions between teachers and learners. This
approach to classroom observation, often referred to as ETHNocRAlHY, is
similar to the way in which an anthropologist takes field notes in studying a
group of people in their natural surroundings. In doing ethnographic
research in classrooms, the obseryer can either be a participant in the
classroom activities, for example, as a teacher aid, or as a non-participant,
someone who sits quietly and unobtrusively in the background, observing
and recording.
Ethnographic approaches to understanding teaching and learning involve
qualitative studies that are much broader in scope than the studies described
above. That is, ethnographies in second or foreign language classrooms do
not focus solely on learning or on teaching but also on social, cultural, and
political realities and their impact on learners' cognitive, linguistic, and
social development.
For example, Martha Cragot (1992) language socialization research with
Inuit children led her to argue that if children come from a culture in which
silence is a respectful and effective way to learn from an adult, their second
language instructor needs to know this so that the children's behaviour is not
misinterpreted as refusal to participate or inabiliry to comprehend.
Here are summaries of three ethnographies carried out in second and foreign
language classrooms: one in the South Pacific, one in Canada, and one in
Europe.
Study 9: Language in the home and school
Karen \Tatson-Gegeo (1992) carried out a longitudinal study over several
years with nine families in the Solomon Islands. She explored language use
practices in the home and in the school. Observations in the homes revealed
environments that were rich and stimulating for both linguistic and
cognitive development. Nevertheless, a large number of the children failed
in school. A detailed analysis uncovered many differences in language use
and values between the home and school sefting. There was no use of the
childrens first language in school. Their first language was replaced with a
restricted and often incorrect version of English. Although these language
issues were contributing factors to the children's failure, a broader analysis of
the social and cultural context revealed other, more influential factors atplay.
Evidently, part of the children's language socialization experience at home
included parents negatively portraying their experiences at school, expressing fears about their children's ability to succeed and raising fundamental
134
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second langadge classroom
questions about the value of school in their lives. The researcher concludes
that these factors were central in contributing to the childrent lack of
continued cognitive and linguistic development in school.
Study 10: Separation ofsecond language learners inprimary schools
in a longitudinal study, KelleenToohey (1998) observed a group of children
age 5-7 in kindergarten, grade 1, and grade 2 in Vancouver, Canada. The
group included children who were native speakers of English, as well as
bilingual children who spoke both English and Polish, Tagalog, Cantonese.
Punjabi, or Hindi. All the children were in the same class, and English was
the medium of instruction. Toohey identified three classroom practices rhar
led to the separation of the ESL children in the classroom. First, the ESL
childrent desks were placed close to the teacher's desk, on the assumption
that they needed more direct help from the teacher. Some of them were also
removed from the classroom twice a week to obtain assistance from an ESL
teacher. Second, instances in which learners interacted more with each other
usually involved borrowing or lending materials but this had to be done
surreptitiously because the teacher did not always tolerate it. Finally, there
was a'rule' in the classroom thar children should nor copy one anothert oral
or written productions. This was parricularly problematic for the ESL
children because repeating the words of others was often the only way in
which they could participate in conversational interaction. According to
Toohey, these classroom practices led to the exclusion of ESL students from
activities and associations in school and also in the broader communiry in
which they were new members. Furthermore, such practices did not
contribute positively to the children's ESL development.
Study I 1: Socio-political change andforezgn language clnssroom discourse
In an ethnographic study of English-medium content classes in Hungarian
secondary schools, Patricia Duff (1995) examined the impact of sociopolitical changes on pedagogical practice. She compared the structure and
participation patterns of rwo classroom activities. One is a traditional
actiyity called a feleles which is a heavily ritualized recitation format closelrassociated with Soviet-oriented policies that were rejected after the fall of
communism in the late 1980s. As a result, in many English-medium classes
in Hungary, the fehlis was replaced by a more open-ended activity called
student lecture in which students prepared and presented material to rhe
class in a less ritualized way. In an examination of the kind of language
produced by students when participating in student lecrures, Duff observed
a large number of spontaneous comments and questions produced in
English rather than Hungarian. She also noted how students appeared to
incorporate feedback provided by the teacher (and other students) in their
subsequent production, how the teacher and students worked together ro
negotiate meaning and form, and how they developed their fuenq-,
accvracy, and comprehension skills in the process. On the basis of these
finding
classroc
Sum
In this
features
nave prr
of their
in whic
_.ummai
;eedbac
;ultural
-anguag
in the n
fest
leat
-hese po
Sourr
\llwrigl
Longma
.{llright
,-ittroduc
'lambri<
Chaudrr
*-niversi
Echevan
,:::nsible
,
learson.
Goldstei
l.Iahwah
Lrlch,
T
- niversit
\Ialamal
- niversit
Sinclair,
}ford:
(
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second
hngt'lg.
findings, Duff concluded that socio-political transformation impacts on
classroom practice and ultimately on second language learning.
Summary
have described some of the ways in which different
features ofsicond language instruction can be described and interpreted. \7e
have presented descriptions and examples of how classrooms differ in terms
of their overall instructional focus and provided examples of different ways
in which classroom observation has been carried out. \7e have included
summaries of studies examining specific pedagogical features (i.e. corrective
feedback and question Wpe) as well as those examining the broader social,
cultural, and political context and its relationship to second or foreign
In this chapter we
language learning.
In the next chapter, we will examine different views about how languages are
best learned in classroom settings and examine some research relevant to
these positions.
Sources and suggestions for further reading
Allwright, R. L. 1988. Obseruation in the Language Classroom. London:
Longman.
Allright, D. and K. M. Bailey. 1991. Focus on the Language Classroom: An
Introduction to Classroom Research for Language Teachers. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Chaudron, C. 1988. Second Language Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge
Universiry Press.
Echevarria, J., M. Vogt, and D. J. Short. 2004. Making Content Comprehensible for English Language Learners: The SIOP Model2nd edn. Boston:
Pearson.
Goldstein,
T. 2003. Teaching and Learning in a Muhilingual
School.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lynch, T.1995, Communication in
Universiry Press.
Malamah-Thomas,
Universiry Press.
the Language classroom.
A. l9B7. Classroom
oxford: oxford
Interaction. Oxford: Oxford
Sinclair, J. and M. Coulthard,. 1975. Towards an Analysis of Discourse.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
r36
Obseruing learning and teaching in the second language classroom
N. and M. Frithlich. 1995. The communicatiue orientation of
Lansuls1 Teaching obseruation scheme: coding conuentions and Appticiti o ns. sy dney: National centre for En glish Language Teachin g and R.iearch,
spada,
Macquarie Universiry.
van Lier,
L. 1988. The Classroom
and the Language Learner. London:
Longman.
'Wajnryb,
R. 1992. Classroom Obseruation Tasks: Resource Booh for
Language Teachers and 7lainer' cambridge: cambridge universiry press.
A
Six pr
\lany tl
ianguage
reen dev
:uestion
-s throu
:eaching
,:r this
c.
-=aching,
:roposal,
*rdings
-:
roposalr
1(
2J
3l
4',
5',
6(
- r assess
::proach
'"-- saw in
- nay al,
--:r- tnllr
::1-1rOnIT
-,'''olve la
-:usual
:
N
I
rclusio
i:-ralitati
:-.'olves
s
SECOND LANGUAGE
LEARNING IN THE
CLASSROOM
Six proposals for classroom teaching
Many theories have been proposed,for the L.d@to l."rn a second
language in the classroom. Even more teachi@qethodrld riaterials have
been developed to implement these theories. Buithe onlyway ro answer rhe
question '\7hat is the best way to promote language learning in classrooms?'
is through research that specifically investigates relationships between
teaching and learning.
In this chapter, we examine six proposals for second and foreign
language
teaching, provide examples from classroom interaction to illustrate how the
proposals get translated into classroom practice, and discuss research
findings that help to assess their effectiveness. The labels we have given these
proposals are:
I
Get it right from the beginning
Justlisten... and read
2
3 Let's talk
4 Two for one
5 Teach what is teachable
6 Get it right in the end
assess proposals for classroom practice, we need to use a range ofresearch
approaches, from large-scale quantitative to in-depth qualitative studies . As
we saw in Chapter 5, quanritative research may be essentia
it may also be e"pe.i
may
of quantitative reseaiCh is usually to
To
in@
,.-..,,_,.),,P..-,,...",,"olc5tnatmayarrectlearnlngSlmllarlylnqlrterent
environments and find ways of measuring these effects. These studies often
involve large numbers of learners in an effort to avoid the possibiliry that the
unusual behaviour of one or two individuals might lead to a misleading
conclusion about learners in general.
Qualitative research, including ethnographies and case studies, ofren
involves small numbers, perhaps one class or only one or two learners in that
138
Second lnnguage learning in tlte classroom
The emphasis is not on what is most general but rather on a thorous:
understanding of what is particular about whar is happening in rhr,
classroom. \X/hile quantitative and qualitative research are importanr -:
assessing theoretical proposals, ACTToN BISEABcH carried out by reacher-o
in their own.l"rrrooLr,ls also#rni"-specific
local questions. -:
is hardlv necessary ro tell experienced reachers that what 'works' in oi--=
co-ntext mav Fail in another.
.udiolir
rral lar
In this chapter we focus mainly on
,:mple
class.
EXIERTMENTAL sruDrEs that ue:;
designed to test hypotheses about how teaching affects second languae.
learning. Readers are encouraged to follow up with further reading but ai':
to explore related questions through
research activities
within their
le
"r"oid
-o makr
:revent
Examp,
-\ grou
sl
ou-r-
T
teaching and learning environments.
1
1
S]
T
Get
-'Get
it rightfrom
the beginning
S]
S'
it right from the beginning' is probably the proposal that characreriz..
more second and foreign language instruction than any other kinc
Although communicative language teaching has come to dominate in son:.environments, the structure based approaches discussed in Chapter I
-ct tha
especially grammar translation, remain widespread.
:o
The grammar translation approach has irs origin in the teaching of classic'
languages (for example, Greek and Latin). Students were presented u-ir
E.ramp
-{ grou
S]
,l1early,
rrect
vocabulary lists, often accompanied by translation equivalents, an:
grammar rules. The original purpose of this
studen:.
read literature rather than to develop fluency
fluencv in the spoken language. It.trhit
-;,"1 exerci t
"G61ffi "g[,,h"r
to ll.lp
d.r.lgp
,h.i
"ooro".h@
T
C
T
C
s
i.r.
In a typical activiry students read a text together line by line and are asked r:
translate it from the target language into their native language. Students ma',
answer comprehension questions based on the passage, often in their firs:
language. The teacher draws attention to a specific grammar rule that :.
illustrated by the text (for example, a cerrain verb form). Following this, th;
students are given an exercise in which they are asked to practise th;
grammatical rule by filling in the blanks with the appropriate verb form in .
series of sentences that may or may not be related to the texr they have reai
and translated.
Audiolingual instruction arose in part as a reaction ro the grammar translation approach. The argument was that, unlike grammar translatioi
teaching in which students learned about the language, audiolingui
teaching would lead students to actually speak the language (Brooks 196tr,
Lado 1954).In Chapter 2, we saw that the audiolingual approach was basec
on behaviourism and contrastive analysis. The examples below refleci
T
C
T
C
:';re re
itout v
::e senl
'',
ander
Resear
l. {any a
=heir
o'
earnet
::anslat
-aafileE
,:rlSfaC
Second language learning in the classroom
audiolingual teaching. It is evident that, even though the emphasis is on the
oral language, students rarely use the language spontaneously. Teachers
avoid letting beginning learners speak freely because this would allow them
to make errors. The errors, it is said, could become habits. So it is better to
prevent these bad habits before they happen.
Example
t
(A group offifteen-year-old students involved in an exercise based on the
simple present of English verbs.)
51
And uh, in the afternoon, uh, I come home and uh' uh, I uh,
washingmydog.
T
I wash.
S1
My dog.
Every dayyou wash your dog?
No. [ben]
Il n'a pas de chien!(= He doesnt have a dog!)
Non, mais on peut le dire!(= No, but we can say we dol)
T
SI
S2
S1
Clearly, in this case, the studentt real experience with his dog (or even the
fact that he did or did not have a dog) was irrelevant. \[hat mattered was the
correct use of the simple present verb!
Example2
(A group of twelve-year-old learners of English
as a
foreign language.)
T
Repeat after me. Is there any butter in the refrigerator?
Class Is there any butter in the refrigerator?
T
There's very little, Mom.
Class There's very little, Mom.
T Are there any tomatoes in the refrigerator?
Class Are there any tomatoes in the refrigerator?
T
There are very few, Mom.
are very few, Mom. (etc.)
Class There
Pure repetition.
students have no reason
n-ffif
to
get
ile their m
IITilGss full
the s ente
wander off to other thlngs.
Research findings
Many adult
those
with
Learners whose previous language learning experience was in grammar
translation classes may also prefer such instruction. As we saw in Chapter 3,
learnerd$eliefs ebout the kind of instruction that is best can influence their
g4iq|acliorl and-qgccess. The grammar translation approach is useful for the
r39
140
Second language learning in the classroom
- nfortun
:n form i
Ttt€
Soox ts
--ad to bt
.DESR
reaning
Pe?eAT:
ON'THE
:l compi
,:srructic
:rmparis
',']rere on
--reir em
\
THE BoT IS
Or.l THE DESK
.:curatel'
-,',-here
th,
,:ageS Of
:runicati
ithewo
:iimarily
_-5S, SOm(
rsess the
:
intensiye study of grammar and vocabulary and is valuable for uni.:
standing important cultural texts. The audioli
with i:,
ls
.Inotivated ad}{t learners in training programmes for government persor.'"=.
in the United Stltes. However, there is little classroom research to suppc..
sucEppro-ches for students in ordinary school programmes that musr ser.-=
the needs of students who bring different levels of motivation and aptitueto the classroom. In fact, it was the frequent failure of traditional gramrn":
translation and audiolingual methods to produce fluency and accurao-
-'
second language learners that led to the development of more communic-tive approaches to teaching in the first place.
Su
15
rtefs o
ha
not
ed that lan
anotner.
that errors are a
that the motivation of learners is ofte:
correctness in the earliest
of seconc
opponents of the 'Get it right from t
nnlng
is better to encourage learners to develop 'fuens,
on
lqlng.
proposal argue that
before 'accuracv'.
J
Some researchers and educators have reacted to the version of communicative language teaching that advocates an exclusive focus on meaning. The.,
argue that allowing learners too much 'freedom' without correcti;n ani
explicit instruction will lead to earlnflo;g.lg4ggg of errors. once again r-;r
hear the call for making sure that learners 'get it right from the beginning'.
-:om the
rterlang
r:ogramr
--nguage
:--mbina
Srudy 12
-r
the l:
,ngitudi
:lSrructi(
l:ench-s1
:rograml
:iactice (
-
re leal
.:rample,
::der' of
,xguage
-,'re 'natu
,:alnefs I
:-on stuc
-rills-rr
,'arners
-:structi(
:fammal
-
i
drillir
.xiliary
::rey also
Second language learning in the classroom
-nfortunately, it is difficult to test the hypothesis that a primary emphasis
will, in the long run,
-:ad to better results than those achieved when the primary emphasis is on
reaning in the early stages. To test that hypothesis, it would be necessary
:o compare groups that are similar in all respects except for the rype of
-lstruction they receive. However, it is not easy for researchers to find proper
:omparison groups. On the one hand, there are many parts of the world
..',-here one finds only structure-based approaches to language teaching, with
-heir emphasis on learning metalinguistic information and performing
:ccurately from the beginning. In these settings, there are no classrooms
rihere the teaching places the primary emphasis on meaning in the early
srages of learning. On the other hand, the widespread adoption of communicative language teaching in recent years has meant that, in other parts
of the world, it is very difficult to make comparisons with classrooms that are
primarily form-oriented because such classes simply do not exist. None the
less, some findings from second language classroom research do permit us to
assess the effect of instruction that is strongly oriented to the 'Get it right
irom the beginning' approach. These include descriptive studies of the
interlanguage development of second language learners in audiolingual
programmes (Study 12), and comparisons of the development of second
language proficiency between groups of students receiving different
combinations of form- and NlseNrNG-BASED INsrRUcrIoN (Study 13).
-'n form in the early stages of second language learning
Study 12: Aadiolingual pattern drill
1970s, Patsy Lightbown (i983a, b) carried out a series of
longitudinal and cross-sectional investigations into the effect ofaudiolingual
instruction on interlanguage development. The investigations focused on
French-speaking learners aged 11-16 in Quebec, Canada. Students in these
programmes typically participated in the rypes of rote repetition and pattern
practice drill we saw in Examples I and2.
In the late
The learners' acquisition of certain English grammatical morphemes (for
example, plural -s and the progressive -inj was compared with the 'natural
order' of acquisition observed in the interlanguage of uninstructed second
language learners (see Chapter 4). The resuits showed differences between
the 'natural order' and the relative accuracy with which these classroom
learners produced them. These findings suggested that the type of instruction students had experienced-a regular diet of isolated pattern practice
drills-resulted in a developmental sequence that was different from that of
learners in more natural learning environments. For a time after their
instruction had focused on it, learners reliably produced a particular
grammatical morpheme in its obligatory contexts. For example, after weetr<s
of drilling on present progressive, students usually supplied both the
auxiliary be and the -ingending (for example, 'He's playing ball'). However,
they also produced one or more of the morphemes in places where they did
t41
142
Second language
learningin the classroom
not belong ('He's want a cookie'). The same forms were produced wiil
considerably less accuracy in obligatory conrexts when they were no longe:
being practised in class and when the rhird person singular simple presenr -was being drilled instead. At this point, many students appeared to reverr rL
what looked like a developmentally earlier srage, using no tense marking a:
all (for example, 'He play ball'). tlgse findin
almost exclusive focus on accurac), andTEE*iee of frtticrilar grammaticaforms does not mean that learners .^.ill he eble to use the forms corr.cti,,
outs-i-FTEecl ass room drill seni
nor that
languag
student
second
from
tl
COInInU
iinguist
Interp
The
stu
reacherr
fiot
seem
Study
13
to
on repetition and drill o
favourrhe developrnent-of compreh.rgo",_ 45"gy,__g
: Grammar
1t
lus communicatia
In one of the earliest experimental
e
h*;i;
p ractice
studies
of communicative
languae;
teaching, Sandra Savignon (1972) studied the linguistic and communicatir'.
skills of forry-eight college students enrolled in French language courses a:
an American universiry. The students were divided into three groups: :
'communicative' group, a 'culture' group, and a coNTRoL cnoup, -L
groups received about four hours per week of ardiolirg.ral in
..the focus was on the practice and manioulation of sr
a special hour of diffe
m-ffii6ative' group had one hour per week devoted to ce
rn an errorr to encourage practrce rn usrng
rno spontaneous ways. I he 'culture
an nour
ed to activitie.
coprdueted*i*Jnglish,
oster an awareness of the Frenc:
langr*age and.ultttr. thronqh filmr, m,!rri., and irtlTE6ili
an hour in the language laboratory doing grammar
""d froru"crq
srmllar to
tnerr regular class peflo(
Tests
to
measure learners' linguistic and communicative abilities u-er.
administered before and after instruction. The tests oflinguistic comperencincluded
,a variety of grammar tests, teachers' evaluations of speakin:
skills, and course grades. The tests of communicative competence includec
measures
of fuency and of the abiliry to
understand and rransrrL:
information in a variery of tasks, which included: discussion with a nati.,';
speaker of French, interviewing a narive speaker of French, reporting facu
about oneself or onet recent activities, and describing ongoing activities.
At the e
DTFFERENCBs between
beginni
lingual,
:heir m
there were no sIGNrFrcA\t
;urstrc competence ngg4sures
ily-E-rEFer than the
commultlcatlYe grou
otner two groups on t
f communlcatlve tests
r the studr
vlgnon rnrerpret
results as support Til e argument that seconc
,e
achinl
.inguist
studentr
:.ccuracr
:hances
.rudies
aommu.
:nd exp
'i
is im
-ontinul
:rfrered
runicat
:he hyp,
irm-ba
:rld grar:
:o the'C
2
Jusi
Ihir-pg
:lace wh
;i-ffi
:iosely
ar
:rample
,.arning
-iqrtirem
:rehensil
Second language learning in tl.te
ianguage programmes that focus only
on
ckssroom
form do not give
students sufficient opportuniry to develop communication abilities in a
second language. Even more important in the context of the 'Get it right
from the beginning' approach was the evidence that opportunities for freer
communication did not cause learners to do less well on measures of
linguistic acaxacy.
accuracy and
Interpreting the research
The studies reviewed above provide evidence to support the intuitions of
teachers and learners that instruction based on the 'Get it right from the
beginning' proposal has important limitations. T,earners receiving audioli-ngqal
o
te
rheir messages and intentions effectivel)' in a second language. Experience
has also shown that primarily or exclusively structure-based approaches to
teaching do not guarantee that learners develop high levels ofaccuracy and
linguistic knowledge. In fact, it is often very difficult to determine what
students know about the target language. The classroom emphasis on
accuracy often leads learners to feel inhibited and reluctant to take
chances in using their knowledge for communication. The results from these
studies provide evidence that learners benefit from opportunities for
communicative practice in contexts where the emphasis is on understanding
and expressing meaning.
It is important to emphasize that in the Savignon study, all students
continued to receive their regular, grammar-focused instruction. They
differed only in terms of the presence or absence of an additional communicative practice componenr. In other words, this study offers support for
the hypothesis that meaning-based instruction is advanrageous, not that
form-based instruction is not. The conributions of communicative practice
and grammar-focused instruction will be discussed in more detail in relation
to the 'Get it right in the end' proposal.
2 Just listen ... and read
proposal is hesed o{r _!he h}'pothesis thet language acquisition take*
when learners are ex
to comorehensible i
tgg. As noted in Chapter Z;hs t"ai"ia"al whose name is most
closely associated with this proposal i
hen Krashe'g (1985, 1989). Read
Example 3 to get a feel for how this theory of classroom second language
learning can be implemented. Krashen's hypothesis thafdg_-g"e_gsg3nl
fhis
ace
lan
prg\.ry!,b]. bput is explored in the instructional setting described here.
143
r44
Second language learning in the classroorn
Exanrple 3
It
is time for English class at a primary school in a French-speaking
community in New Brunswick, Canada. The classroom looks like a
miniature language lab, with about thirty small desks, on each ofwhich there
fuound the room, shelves and
racks display scores of books. Each book is packaged with an audiocassette
that contains a recording of its content. The materials are not strictly graded,
but some sets of books are very simple, and other sets are grouped so that
they are gradually more challenging. There are pre-school childrens books
with a picture and a word or two on each page; illustrated stories with a few
is a cassette player and a set of large earphones.
per page; picture dictionaries; ESL textbooks for children;
illustrated science books about animals, weather, vehicles, etc. Students
(aged 8-10) enter the classroom, select the material theywant, and take it to
their individual workspace. They insert the cassette, put on their earphones,
and open their bootr<s. They hear and read English for the next thirty
minutes. For some of the time the teacher walks around the classroom,
checking that the machines are running smoothly, but she does not interact
with the students concerning what they are doing. Some of the students are
listening with closed eyes; others read actively, mouthing the words silently
as they follow each line with a finger. The classroom is almost silent except
for the sound of tapes being inserted and removed or chairs scraping as
students go to the shelves to select new tapes and books.
sentences
or lf
orgar
inpur
rhe te
or by
Occar
insidr
Patsy
devel<
gramr
ESL
studer
day sir
compt
cases
I
compr
experir
classes.
Lightb
... and read' is a controversial proposal for second language
It not only says that second language learners need not drill and
abilitie
continr
On cor
continr
practise language in order to learn it, but also that they do not need to speak
at all, except to get other people to provide input by speaking to them.
According to this view, it is enough to hear and understand the target
made g
rvhere t
'Just listen
teaching.
language. The classroom description above shows that one way to do this is
to provide learners with a steady diet oflistening and reading comprehension
activities with no (or very few) opportunities to speak or interact with the
teacher or other learners in the classroom.
Research relevant to this proposal includes studies of comprehension-based
teaching and extensive reading.'We will also look at some comprehension-
based instruction in which the input is manipulated in ways that are
intended to increase the likelihood that students will pay attention to
language form as well as meaning.
re h ens i o n - b as e d irus tr uc ti on
but
n
als<
classroo
Stud! I
Finding
ianguag,
second I
Research findings
Study 1 4 : C o mp
other
fo r
ch
i ldre
n
Example 3 was a description of a real programme implemented in
experimental classes in a French-speaking region in Canada. From the
beginning of their ESL instruction at age eight, students only listened and
read during their daily thirty-minute E SL period. There was no oral practice
cG*
1i@
age-appr
simple. I
iary
deve
program
-:articipa
-evels.
In
.implifie<
:eriod,
s
Second language learning
in the classroom
or interaction in English at all. Teachers did not 'teach' but provided
organizational and technical supporr. Thus, learners received native-speaker
input from tapes and books bur had virtuaily no inreraction in Englisir
with
the teacher or other learners. They guessed at meaning by using
thl pictures
br.reco.gnizing cognare words ih"t
similar in"French?nd English.
1
"r. equivalents ofa fewwordr,
occasionallytheycould refer to translation
inside a bookt back cover.
[p.d
Patsy Lightbown and her coll"ragu es (2002) investigated
the second language
development of hundreds of ihildren in this .o"mpr.hensiorr-bas.jprlq?3-. and compared.their learning with that of students in the ,.g.r1",
ESL programme, which was mainly an audiolingual approach. aI the
stude.nts in both programmes had had classes that lasted th;rty
-irr,rres per
day since they started their ESL instruction. A,fter two y."rr,
l."r.r.rs in the
comprehension-based programme knew as much Engiish as (and
in some
cases more than) learners in the regglar program. This'was
true not only for
comprehension but also for rp."ki"g, evln though the learners
in the
experimenral programme had never practised spiken English
in their
classes.
Lightbown and her colleagues reassessed the students' English
language
abilities three years later, when they were in grade B. some"students
had
continued in the comprehension-only progr"L*. throughout that
time.
on comprehension
measures and on ro-..ri."r,rres oforariroduction, they
continued to perform as well as students in the regular progr"--..
O,
other measures, some groups of students in the ..!,r1", pro!r"*,'.
n"a
made greater progress, .rp..i"lly in writing. Those ,,",rd.rrr,
i' classes
-!r.
yherq the regular programme included ,,It orrly audiolinguaiirrrrr,r.rio'
but also other speaking and writing componenrs, reacher feedback,
and
classroom
in
teraction.
Study 1 5 : Readingfor tuords
Finding reading material for primary school students learning
a second
language is challenging.
{inc!i"g reading material for adults in e"ily rtaee, of
second languase acouisiti
rs specially
desisned for adult,r.p
ESL
L_lsArnslr
le
are lncreasmqlv
nqlv available. These simplified
literary c
iographies, romances,
thrill.r, offer interesting and
".td
age-appropriate conrenr, while the vocabulary
and writing sryle re-main
simple. Marlise Horst (2005) used simplified .e"ders ir, rt,idy
of vocabu"
lary developmenr among adult immigiants who were enrollei
in an ESL
programme in a community centre in Montreal, canada. The
twenry-one
participants represented se11{ language backgrounds and
profi.i.r.y
levels. In addition to the activities of tleir-r.g,rl"r"ESL
.l"rr, ,ruj.rts chose
simplified readers that were made available in"a class library. over
a six-week
period, students took books home and read them or, ,h.i, own.
Horst
_
r45
146
Second language
learningin the classroom
developed individualized vocabulary measures so rhat learning could bt
assessed in terms of the books each student actually read. She found tha:
there was vocabulary growth attributable to reading, even oyer this shor:
period. Furthermore, the study's findings suggested that the more studenrs
read, the more words they learned. She concluded that s'hstanri:'t students muq reac!o
*g{ulav groyth through read
great deal.(more
to realrze
r"*
in
Chapter
4,
b*.4..,
Lersations, we tend to use mainly the 1,000 or 2,000 most.frgquen-t word_r"
Thus. r.a
who have reached an intermediate level of proficien cy may have fe..,
opportunities to learn new words in everyday conversation. It is in reading ;
variety of texts that students are most likely to encounter new vocabular
The benefit of simplified read,ers is that students are likely to encounter a
reasonable number of newwords. This increases the likelihood that they car
figure out the meaning of newwords (or perhaps be motivated to look then
up). If the new words occur often enough, students may remember then
aC.
reeling
lt.
r
Other rr
input f
srudies,
io langu
axposur(
=rd/or
p
gettir
:hem to
,,.n
-iis
reser
Study
l)
I'
Iartha
-
rench-s
,]uebec.
-ommur
when they encounter them in a new context.
..i'hether
Study 1 6: Tbtal p hysical resltonse
One of the best-known variations on the 'Just listen ... and read' proposal is
the second language teaching approach called 'Tbtal Physical Response'
_:1put
(TPR), developed by lames Asher (1972f. In TPR claqse) studentschildren s1 xcluh5-participate in activities in which they hear a series oi
commands in the target language, for example, 'stand up', 'put the book on
the table', walk to the door'. At a more advanced
t skir.
as the teacher provides a desclpiio" ofi"l
For a
tial number of hours of instruction, students are not required to saranything.
ion bv their
actlons.
students begin to speak, they take over the role ofthe teacher
and give commands as well as following them. Although Krashen has
expressed his enthusiasm for this approach to teaching, it differs from his
comprehensible input hypothesis in one important way. The comprehensible input hypothesis s
ts that no structural grading is necesil?iEt
t
teacners snou
I
lnstruct
Ed?xp6eTio
arE
lncreases ln com
v
ry so rnat
to ensure
, the vocabulary and srrucrures learner.
and orgaruzed. I he materral gradualli'
new lesson builds on the ones before.
Asher's research showed that students could develop quite advanced levels of
ton ln t
ing in oral practice. it is clearthere are limitations ro the kind of languige-t
rn to produce in
such an enyironment. Nevertheless, Asher's research shows that, forbeginnerqJthis kind of active listeninq qives learners a sood srart. It allou's
..-::
them ro burld up a considerable knowledge of-ihe new language withour
w(
The linl
Chapter
:ead a ser
-nstancel
:eferred
rlacemer
::ad the
- n them
*JrhougJ
.'l the c<
:reir accr
:ut not
:
'ntinue
:rglish
:=;ognizt
:itar the
.--:e rlang
.-:.rguage
:-tf
OSUfe
iiorma
:--, e
then
-,- sltlve (
_
_-- rarger
.-.:ir
firsr
Second language learning in the classroom
feeling the nervousness that often accompanies the first attempts to speak
it.
Other research that explores the 'Just listen ... and read' position includes
'input food', 'enhanced input', and 'processing instruction studies. In these
studies, efforts have been made to draw second language learners' attention
to language forms in different ways, for example, providing high-frequency
exposure to specific language features, enhancing the features in some way,
and/or providing explicit instruction. The emphasis in all cases, however, is
on getting the learners to notice language forms in the input, not on getting
them to practise producing the forms. The next two studies are examples of
this research.
Study 17: Inputfl.ood
Martha Thahey and Lydia \7hite (1993) carried out a study with young
French-speaking learners (aged 10-12) in rNrnNsrvE EsL classes in
Quebec. These students were in ESL classes in which instruction was
communicative and task-based. The goal of this research was to determine
whether high-frequency exposure to a particular form in the instructional
input would lead to better knowledge and use of that form by the students.
The linguistic form investigated was adverb placement in English (see
Chapter 4). For approximately ten hours over a two-week period, learners
read a series of short texts in which they were exposed to literally hundreds of
instances of adverbs in English sentences-so many that the investigators
referred to this study as an'input flood'. There was no teaching of adverb
placement, nor was any error correction provided. Instead, students simply
read the passages and completed a variety of comprehension activities based
on them.
Although learners benefited from this exposure to sentences with adverbs in
all the correct positions, their learning was incomplete. They improved in
their acceptance of sentences with word order that is grammatical in English
but not in French ('The children quickly leave school'). However, they
continued to accept sentences that are grammatical in French but not in
English ('The children leave quickly school'). The students' inability to
recognize that adverbs in this position are ungrammatical in English suggests
that the input flood could help them add something new to their
interlanguage, but did not lead them to get rid ofan error based on their first
language. As noted in Chapter 2,LydiaWhite (1991) agdlhat althoggh
with oositi
inout may orovide
re to lan
what is
matical in the second
ive them
tive evidence (information about
to permit learners to notice the absence in
Positive evidence is not
that
are present in their interlanguage (and
target
language
of
elements
the
their first language). Thus, more explicit information about what is not
r4-,
r48
Second language learning in the classroom
grammatical in the second language may be necessary for learners' continued
development. This is discussed in more detail in the section 'Get it right in
the end'.
Study 1 B : Enhanced input
Michael Sharwood Smith (1993) coined the term 'input enhancemenr' ro
refer to avariety of things that might draw learners' atrention to features in
the second language, thus increasing the chances that theywould be learned.
In a study involving enhanced input, Joanna \7hite (1998) examined the
acquisition of possessive determiners (specifically'his' and'her'; see Chapter
4) by French-speaking learners in intensive ESL classes aged ll-I2.
Students received approximately ten hours of exposure to hundreds of possessive determiners through a package of reading materials and comprehension activities provided over a two-week period. The major difference
between this study and tahey and'$7'hitet input flood is that rypographical
enhancement was added. That is, every time a possessive determiner
appeared in the texts, it was in bold rype, underlined, italicized, or written in
capital letters. The hypothesis was that this would lead the learners to notice
O7/t
lhey wt
literall
the possessive determiners as they read the texts.
\7hite compared the performance of learners who had read
tbllows
the
with that of learners who read the same
texts without enhancement. She found that both groups improved in their
knowledge and use of these forms but that there was little difFerence between
them. In interpreting these findings, \(hite questions whether the
enhancement was sufficiently explicit to draw the learners' attention to
possessive determiners. That is, even though the two forms were highlightec
by the use of bold type, capital letrers, etc., students did not learn how rc
choose the possessive determiner to match the gender qf the possessor. In
subsequent research, \White found lh4!_lgalnerq made more progress w[er:
they were given a si
rule and
her to find the correci
rm to complete stofles
where
ive determiners
typographically enhanced
_crocessi
passages
a, Lightbown, and
Vhite
2005).
Study 1 9: Processing insnucilon
In a series of studies, Bill VanPatten (2004) and his colleagues have invesdgated the effects of p
NSTRUCTION, another approach rc
comprehension-based learni
rocessing instruction, learners are pur ir
processi
fronoul
--ti notlcl
=I \.afietr
.:tentiol
:eanin6
.r-hich p
_LlrI€SpO
=:iqlieiI._
: --'moreh
Sducti
------JE__
:--- tnst.
::ocessir
.lnerS i
-r.L-<
sltuailons
i:rterpre
context,
,-
n
-'earcL
-, -
r shor,
ign language received instruction on different linguistic forms, fb:
".-red ex
example, object pronouns (VanPatten and Cadierno 1993). As noted i::
Chapter 2, YanPatten found that English-speaking learners of Spanis:
tended to treat the object pronouns, which precede the verb in Spanish, as :
"eY(9
--
: :;ilent'
.-arnin
Second language learning
in the classroom
'f::"izi,
Enhancing the input
they were subject pronouns. Thus, a sentence such as La sigue el sefior
(literally 'her (object) follows the man (subject)') was interpreted as 'She
follows the man. Two groups were compared in the study, one receiving
processing instruction, the other following a more traditional approach. !h1
processing instruction group received explicit explanations about obiect
pronouns and d'd som
r before the verb. Then, through
oF"o,i.i"gih", obi..
had to pay
a"@ers
i"r@"g
attention to how the target forms were used in order to understand the
meaning. For example, they heard or read La sigue el sefiorandhad to choose
which picture-a man following a woman or a woman following a manof learners also received
corresponded to the sentence. A second
s1Dli6i1 infnrrnation ehorrt the tarset for
co-mprehensign
prac
in
froduction pr@ice, doing exercises to practise the forms being taught. After
t6Elnr,rr'tfrl6-n,
, learners who had rqceived the comprehension-based
rocesslng rnstfuct
@
Interpreting the research
rch on comprehension-based
tion shows that learners can make con
lan
tarn
:!
e
of learnin
that
comprehension-
q!.'tq'.
gg
@sili- "d_lqgtegg::g-giliiffi'g
ment to ot
r49
1t0
Second knguage learning
in the classroom
Comprehension
of r
iJ".' ri,liiil;:?i,
".q
components
ui,ition.
oo,,,ia.,",tr.
ffi
of
classr
;;#:il'
:;
loundation of
f:*;ff ;:. ror
j
"Hr
. VanPatten,s research
showed rhat For.;,
:bh^-r^-,.
meanin
Exarni
":"'15 ^f
rncreased the chances
own sedoffilfr
InPut
language
meaning are valuable
pracdces. Nevertheless,
dagogi
Jl
(Stude
7
Irnother response. ro rh.
.o rpr.hETEil
's (1985)
thesis'. She a
aq it is when students f^lT?r"h.nr,ible output hypo_
nave- ro produce
languag. ai,r,
n to see
mltatlons
t.r____
rnrerlanguage
r lowever,
(sei
Clrrr,?F
_-- es\
lrr tjtc
|
", *. *iIiE
Lcr F
s rarK
talk' proposal, ji
ffi3re.ffugionsu,here their teachers and classmates
them *iunderstanci
S
S,
T
Sl
T
SI
How di
acdon o,
from tht
-
3 Letts talh
Advocates of ,Let,s
talk'
comprehensible i
ut and
othef s
ts.
--%h
engage rn interact
argue
r
enhang
'F&
Fo
n ve.rsa r ig n
ai
in
rera
when
lnten
ts,
cd
o"Gn-
1n
tuni
gle for meaning', rhat
opinions,ffi?
4q
q,
acquisitir
"
nat.
meant.
is.
Researcl
tnar
Most of
- rnteraction
' -^vvvrqurE, to tne
l;;;;
th,e language formslrammatical srrucrures-"-::qujtt
rrlPUtnesls, the nesotiation
leads
the words and rhe
artendi
n
g ro. rhis"i.,h.
;;;;;;;' ;;;
behaviour in the transc'rpr
non'
reraction in Comm
rn
uni*d
Negotiation of meani
ctJr;;; "j;::,1il!,l:Tee;:,1.,1ff;
B and frori*h. rrud.nt-sruden:
r" r*ila'ilti"p r., l.
that naturally arise i:g,::-tTplished-through
'
Look for
a variety of
modifications
ii'::m***"*.no'
negoriation.ofl.lnilqin
the .**pl.r,below
wirh rhe exampres given
and compare rh:,
for,rr. €ir^;. rigi,'rron-'
rhe beginning,proposal.
Examph4
(A group of rwelve-year-old
ESL studenrs are discussing
r|i..,,".:^^ a^ questionnairt
p.t, -lrtth.i.'r.acher.)
"bout
S And what is feed,?
T
Feed?Tofeedthedog?
descriptiv
classroom
cenffed.-g
rnteractior
modificati
frffi"'i'
In the mid
action on s
rhese studit
rvMichael
Chapter 3)
:onyeryatio
:rpdated ve
r@;
.re most likr
- one featul
,-onnections
s---
-
:hapteS rese
-ine with the
Second langwage learning in the ckssroom
S
T
butwhen I dont have a...
Ifyou dont have a dog, you skip the question.
Yes,
Example 5
(Students from Classroom B, as they settle in at the beginning of the day.)
T How are you doing this morning?
S1
I'm mad!
S2 \vhy?
T
sl
T
Oh boy. Yeah, why?
Because this morning, my father say no have job this morning.
Your father has no more job this morning? Or you have no job?
s1 My father.
How different these examples are from the essentially meaningless interaction often observed in classrooms where the emphasis is on'getting it right
from the beginning'. Such genuine exchanges of information must surely
enhance
ts'motii
lvltles.
Pate rn la
dlthgy,
as advocates
of this posiibn ctr-n1
, ror examp
way, the student
n
conversatron p
Example 4 never did find out what 'feed'
meant.
Research findings
Most of the early research that examined the 'Let's talli proposal
was
descriptiveinnature,focusingonsuchissuesas:H'@
in natural settings? How do tea.h..classrooms differ i
of conversational
classrooms differ fror., that observed
centred
interaction?
Do
task
'al studies also examined relationships between modimodifications?
fications in conversational interaction and comprehension.
In the mid-1990s researchers began to directly explore the effects of interaction on second language production and development orrer time. Most of
out in laboratory settings and are motivated
byMtpbaellong's (1996) updatedversion of the iryry@porhoit (r..
Chapter 3). Compared with the original version (Long 1983) stating that
cgryersational interaction promotes second [anguage development, rhe
version integrates learner capacities that contribute to second
teractlon tnat
uAiAglfor examp
these studies have been carried
are
as one feature that is believed to
c-6AG-ctions
bemffiIn
rrective feedback has been identi
a cruclal role ln
make
fact, as we will see later in this
interacrion hypothesis is more in
ttotheu
aptet researc
line with the 'Get it right in the end' position.
151
I52
Secondlanguage learninginthe classroom
Study 20: Learners talhingto learners
conslst
In one of the early descriptive studies on learner interaction, Michael Long
and Patricia Porter (1985) examined the language produced by adult
proficit
'sender
learners performing a task in pairs. There were eighteen participants: twelve
non-native speakers of English whose first language was Spanish, and six
native English speakers. The non-native speakers were intermediate or
advanced learners of English.
I
conside
were tl
'sendel
bution
Each individual participated in separate discussions with a speaker from
each of the three levels. For example, an intermediate-level speaker had a
conversation with another intermediatelevel speaker, another with an
advanced-level speaker, and another with a native speaker of English. Long
and Porter compared the speech ofnative and non-native speakers in conversations, analysing the differences across proficiency levels in conversation
pairs. They found that learners talked more with other learners than they did
with native
speakers. Also, learners produced more talk
with advancedlevel
with intermediate-level partners, pariy
because the conver-
learners than
1
$7hen
'receive
order tr
role, hc
interact
Yule an
advance
learners
Study
sations with advanced learners lasted longer.
and Porter examined the number of grammatical and vocabulary
-Long
errors and false starts and found that learner speech showed no differences
That is, intermediate-level learners did not make any more
errors with another intermediate-level speaker than they did with an
advanced or native speaker. This was an interesting result because it called
into question the argument that learners need to be exposed to a nativespeaking model (i.e. teacher) at all times ifwe are to ensure that they produce
fewer errors. Overall, Long and Porter concluded that although learners
cannot always provide each other with the accurate grammatical input, they
can offbr each other genuine communicative practice that includes negotiation of meaning. Supporters of the 'Let's talli proposal argue that it is precisely this negotiation of meaning that is essential for language acquisition.
-anguag
across contexts.
Study 21: Learner knguage andprofciency leuel
GeorgeYule and Doris Macdonald (1990) investigatedwhether the role that
different-level learners play in a two-way communication task led to differences in their interactive behaviour. They set up a task that required two
learners to communicate information about the location of different
buildings on a map and the route to get there. One learner, referred to as the
'sender', had a map with a delivery route on it, and this speakert job was to
describe the delivery route to the 'receiver' so that he or she could draw the
delivery route on a similar map. The task was made more challenging by the
fact that there were minor differences between the two maps.
To determine whether there would be any difference in the nature of the
interactions according to the relative proficiency of the forty adult
participants, different rypes of learners were paired together. One group
2
In a lon
2002)
lifferen
:nteracti
-ngaged
:haracte
inffom
;qe l.an
:jre olhs
..i'ho was
:
;-'--
rher rn
-:tIeIactl
::nities
r
-:nguage
,:'aInefS
::l.aintain
ln-ro parl
-raintain
-:
:
:
_
cogni
ilabofa
-;cessfu
lrudy 23
--rson lV
::;nmun
''.:e desi
Second language learning in the classroom
r53
consisted of high-proficiency learners in the 'sender' role and lowproficiency learners in the 'receiver' role. Another group had low-proficiency
'senders' paired with high-proficiency'receivers'.
tVhen low-proficiency learners were in the 'sender' role, interactions were
considerably longer and more varied than when high-proficiency learners
were the 'senders'. The explanation for this was that high-proficiency
'senders' tended to act as if the lower-level 'receiver' had very little contribution to make in the completion of the task. As a result, the lower-level
'receivers' were almost forced to play a very passive role and said very little in
-When
lower-level learners were in the 'sender'
order to complete the task.
role, however, much more negotiation of meaning and a greater variety of
interactions berween the two speakers took place. Based on these findings,
Yule and Macdonald suggest that teachers should sometimes place more
advanced students in less dominant roles in paired activities with lower-level
learners.
Study ZZ: The dynamics ofpairuorh
In a longitudinal study with adult ESL learners in Australia, Naomi Storch
(2002) investigated the patterns of pair interaction over time and whether
differences in the nature of the interactions led to differences in second
language learning. \Tithin her data, she identified four distinct patterns of
interaction. 'Collaborative' interaction consisted of two learners fully
tnant rnteractlon was
ert ideas; 'dominan
characterized bv an un
on rhe part of elther learner to en
of
or asree wrtn tne o
3 co;'ii'l6uiions:'dominant-passive' consis
to
was wllrnq to
,the orhersoeaker;
a
who was stronser than the other but actively encou
and
other in carrvins out the task. To investisate
t types-of
interaction led to different learning outcomes, she identified learning opportunities that arose during the interactions. Then she examined whether that
language knowledge was maintained in a subsequent task. Storch found that
Iearners who
in the collaborative and
'novlce palrs
over tlme.
who participated in the dominant-dominant and dominant-passive pairs
this as support for Vygotsky's theory
maintained the least. Storch in
arm tnat
of cosnitive develooment an
n
in the co-construction
of
Study 23: Interaction andsecond language d.euelopment
Alison Mackey (1999) asked adult learners of ESL to engage in different
communicative tasks with native speakers of the target language. The tasks
were designed to provide contexts for learners to produce question forms.
lr
l*
b4
Second language learning in the classroom
Group 1 learners interacted with native speakers, who modified the::
language as they sought to clarify meaning for the learners. Learners .:Group 2 did not engage in conversational interactions. Instead th;-"
observed the interactions between the learners and native speakers in Grou:
1. Group 3 included learners and native speakers who participated in rL.
same communicative tasks as Group 1. However, for Group 3learners, ti:.input was premodified. That is, the native speakers used language that hal
been simplified and scripted to match a level oflanguage that was assumed :':
be comprehensible to the learners. There was no negotiation of meanins
berween speakers in this group. On a post-test, learners who had engaged il
conversational interactions (Group 1) produced more advanced questiorforms than those in the two other experimental groups.
Study 24: Learner-learner interaction in aThai classroom
In a study relevant to the updated version of the interaction hypothesis, Kin
McDonough(2004) investigated the use ofpair and small group activities
English as a foreign language classes in Thailand. Students engaged
interactional activities in which they discussed environmental problems ir:
their country. The topic was chosen as one that would generate contexts for
h
ir
Certain
toabe
efTectiv,
exampl,
relation
study,
t
produo
conclus
Further
activitir
on
a
sin
rind of
relps t,
interact
Recentl
Cifferen
ing ove
for
exi
the use of conditional clauses such as 'If people didnt leave water running
while brushing their teeth, they would save an estimated 5-10 gallons each
time' (p. 213). Leaners were audio-recorded as they discussed the environ-
iecause
mental problems.
:eedbac
The recorded conversations were examined to see the extent to which
students used interactional features that are believed to facilitate second
language learning, for example, negawe feedb4k (i.e. clarification requests.
and modified output (i.e. a learner's
ers
reYtous utterance
accurate/comolex reformulation of his
e@ts),
'ffiryto
nditional
c
an
i
reca
\{cDor
Thai
le,
rot
alv
instrucl
instrucl
aftemp1
saving t
rhe'Ge
:rplicit
Learners who had used more negative feedback and modified output
significantly improved in the accuracy of their conditional clauses. Those
who made less use of these features did not. McDonough also explored
opinions about the usefulness of pair work and small group activities, asking
whether such activities contributed to learning. She found that the students
did not perceive pair and group activities as useful for learning English. This
was true both for students who seemed to have made effective use of the
interaction for learning and those who had not.
Interpreting the research
Research based on the interaction hypothesis has investigated factors that
contribute to the qualiry and quantity of interactions benveen second
language learners.
ls
It
has provided some useful information for teaching.
4Tu
trryP
rs one L
@
r:tstrucl
:rograr
r:ograr
lurop(
ianguap
:.Dproa(
:ontent
Second language learning in the classroom
Certainly, the studies by Porter, Yule and Macdonald, and Storch contribute
to a better understanding of how to organize group and pair work more
effectively in the classroom. The Mackey and McDonough studies are two
examples of research that have measured second language development in
relation to different aspects of conversational interaction. In the Mackey
study, the measure of second language learning was the learners' immediate
production following these interactions. Thus, it is difficult to draw any
conclusions as to the long-term benefits of conversational interacrion.
Furthermore, because this study was designed to use one-on-one pair-work
activities between trained natiye speakers and non-native speakers focusing
on a single grammatical feature, it is also difficult to relate the findings to the
kind of interactions that take place in classrooms. The McDonough study
helps to fill this gap because it is a classroom study and the effects of
interactional features on second language learning were measured over time.
Recently, a number of laboratory studies have also examined the effects of
different interactional features on specific aspects ofsecond language learning over time. Several studies have shown that implicit corrective feedback
(for example, recasts) in pair-work situations is beneficial. This may be
because recasts are more salient in pair work, particularly if only one form
is
recast consistently (Nicholas, Lightbown, and Spada 2001). In
McDonought classroom study, recasts (and other forms of corrective
feedback) were likely to have been more easily noticed as well because the
Thai learners were accustomed to traditional grammar instruction. This is
not always the case, however. As we learned in Chapter 5, when the
instructional focus is on expressing meaning through subject-matter
instruction, the teachers' recasts may not be perceived by the learners as an
attempt to correct their language form but rather as just anorher way of
saying the same thing. Later in this chapter we will look at studies related to
the 'Get it right in the end'position that have investigated the effects of more
explicit corrective feedback on second language learning.
4 Tuto for one
This aooroach to lansuase teachins referred to as content-based instruction
is onG in
or foreisn lansuas&s tEeyEudy
@cond
@e.
It is implemen@
instr[ctional settings including BILINGUAL EDUcATIoN and immersion
programmes and the 'content and language-integrated learning' (CLIL)
programmes in Europe. Other educational programmes such as the
tubi*t
'European school' extend this further by offering instruction in two or more
in addition to students' home language. The expectation of this
approach is that students can get'two for one', learning the subject matter
content and the language at the same time.
languages
t55
r55
Second language learning in the classroom
I+ immr""i.'n
ch o ose fo
anel QL_IL_
programmes*stg4tg-@ose (or their parenrs
r them ) tp-ecei.re-cao t".r t-b
".ed
iils
tilffi-i
n-aseco nd
I
an
gua ge.
In many educational situations, however, no other oprion is availablelFor
example, in some countries, the only language ofschooling is the language of
a previous colonial power. In others, educational materials are not available
in all local languages, so one language is chosen as the language of education.
In countries of immigration, students often have access to schooling oniv
through the majoriry language. Other students may have access to bilingual
education programmes that allow some use of a language they already kro*,
but the transition to the majority language is usually made within ayear or
requi.
tullv
{3s!
c!mn
speecJ
la-frlu-
ffi:a
3tffi
mean
learne
tvvo.
target
Research findings
A
In many contexts for content-based instruction, it is simply assumed that
students will develop both their academic skills and second language abilinr
In recent years, researchers have sought to examine this assumption more
featur
critically.
in Canada
on Canadian French immersion programmes is often cited in
support of the 'Jwo for one' position. Most immersion programmes are
offered in primary and secondary schools, but some universities also offer
content-based instruction that expands opportunities for studenrs to use
their second language in cognitively challenging and informative courses.
\fhat have the studies shown?
Study 25: French irnmersionprogrammes
Research
In terms of
success. Thousands
<,
optlon
sl
------F--:-
French immersion has been a grear
ofE
{Eanafi nTamilieTtra-vech-olilhis
both in areas where French is spoken in the wider communiry and
where French is rarely heard outside the classroom. Numerous studies har.e
shown that French immersion srudents develop nu.ncy,-tffile=E-"f
IiFening comprehe
Fthat
sec<
Swain
'histor
pick u
that tl
classes
approl
rvith
c
speecl
rhe sar
neede<
includ
learne,
leaturr
Study
In the
t'hich
in whi
'grader
is
Englisl
ron nas Deen ln
J
to express concern about
students' failure to achieve high levels of performance in some aspects of
French grammar, even after several years of full-day exposure to the second
language in these programmes (Harley and Swain 1984). Several possible
explanations have been offered for this.
i\?nte(
comoarable to that of their
years, however, educators and researchers
Some
that
n shows thar
that tbjlsgmerssngagd
the classes were larsely teacherrelatively little and were rarelr-
-
internz
i{ong
Englisl
In revir
Englisl
1997)
Jre de
lnglisl
tully. I
Second language learning
required to give extended answers. T
lete knowl
fullv with their i
rmitted them to
of the la
r57
in the classroom
rate successuse tneY were
n students
#
nll;erous errors in their
the same first
wefe lnnue
speech because the learners' interla
tAeiame Iimited con
tng envlronment,
tne same
H
tended to under-
@"g.,
ro there was rarely a need for negotiation
of
meaning. Such successful communication made it difficult for an individual
learner io *ork out how his or her use of the language differed from the
target language.
V
A second possible reason for students' lack ofprogress on certain language
features is their rarity in content-based instruction. For example, Merrill
Swain (1988) observed that even history lessons were often delivered in the
'historical present' (for example, 'The ships go down to the caribbean; they
they take it back to England . . .'). Roy Lys ter (1994) found
pick up sug"r
"ttd
ih"t th. polit. r..o.td person singular pfonoun 'vous' was used so rarely in
classes that even after years of immersion instruction, students did not use it
appropriately. Elaine Throne and Merrill Swain (1995) noted that learners
wiih only classroom exposure to the language did not have access to the
speech styles that would be rypical of interaction among nadve speakers of
th. r"-. age. Increasingly, it was suggested that subject matter instruction
needed to be complemented by instruction that focused gn language form'
inclgding pl4gq4lr!&lures of the langqage. In some experimental studies,
learnirs did benefit from form-focused instruction on particular language
features (see the'Get it right in the end' proposal).
Study 26: Late intntersion under stress in Hong Kong
1960s the educational system in Hong Kong moved from one in
which students studied either exclusively in English or in Cantonese to one
in which the majority of students studied in Cantonese in primary school
(grades 1-6) and in English at secondary school (grades 7-13). These late
English immersion programmes were popular with Chinese parents who
*"nted their children to succeed professionally and academically in the
international communiry. They were also seen as being consistent with the
In the
Hong Kong government's goal of maintaining a high level of chineseEnglish bilingualism.
In reviewing some of the research on teaching and learning behaviours in late
English immersion classes in Hong Kong secondary schools, Keith Johnson
(1i97) raised concerns about the abiliry of the educational system to meet
the demands for such programmes. He noted that students lacked the
English proficiency needed to follow the secondary level sgllisshrm successzutty. He also observed teachers' difficulties in effectively delivering the
CJ
158
Second language learning in the clasroom
content because of limitations in their own English proficiency. He argued
that several pedagogic behaviours contributed to the inabiliry of learners to
make adequate linguistic progress in these English immersion programmes.
'Mix' (a
One of them was tJacher talk that consisted of English, Chinese and
combination of the English and Chinese)' Observational classroom studies
Mi" predominated in the speech of teachers and
revealed that Chines.
"ltrd
that students interacted with the teacher and with each other in English onlr'
in minimal ways. Many students came to the first year of secondary school
lit.r".y rkilk i" English. To compensate for this, teachers
without
"rry
employed a variety of strategies to h.lp students comprehend texts. Ther,.a.r..d the vocabulary load,-simplifiedthe grammar, encouraged the use of
bilingual dictionaries,'and providid students with supplementary notes and
.h"ri in Chinese to asrist their comprehension. Johnsol obseryqdlfrat,
ts are
r"i",isht
not
time students come to read the tex
*fft.i.tttly familiar with the content to be
so that bv the
studenr
observz
had to
He
di<
checkir
rhat m
student
gender
perforn
ifor
exz
q'as evi
:reeded
Further
rnformr
discuss
to oear wr
"t.. Altnou
ilFnde rs tand the co n te nt
ese strategres
iir.y *"y not h-ave helped them learn to use the syntactic and discourse
,,r,r.,,rr., in the ,..o.td language to establish form-meaning relationships'
Therefore it is not surprising that the standards of reading in F'nglish at age
fifteen were reporred to be sftnificantly lower than those for Chinese. At the
same time, ho^werr.r, the educational outcomes for Hong Kong students in
conrent subjects continued to be high, comparable to, and in.some areas
superior to, achievements in other develo-ped countries. In addition, the
levels of first language Chinese reading proficiency remained high'
a
A new educational policy that includes mofe Chinese medium education in
secondary school has been implemented in recent years. The policy has been
contro,r.rsial, but early ,.r.rlis seem to suggest that there may have been
some decline in students' English proficiency. However, their performance
on subject matter examinations appears to have benefited from having more
of their instruction in Chinese, that is, when they have access to a more
'bilingual' educational opportunity (K. K. Luke, personal communication'
August 3,2005).
The stu
Solving
Cne
oe
.b".:
-angua€
+
-;1UKtrtr
!'Jggestl
, ould t
_
his
su
lncefr
.:e nch
*--e devt
,rthisl
Study 27: Inuit children in content-basedprogrammes
In ai aboriginal communiry in Quebec, Canada, Nina Spada and Patsr'
Lightbown
(Z.OOZ)
observed ihe teaching and learningofschool subjects and
children. The children are educated in their first
l"n!,r"!., Inuktitut, from kindergarten-to grade2 (age 5-7)' Then' except
for"o.clsional lessons in Inuit cultur.' their education is in one of Canadas
official languages, French or F,nglish. \7e found that nearly all students had
some diffiZultj, coping with subjecr marter instruction in their se.cond
language. In a'."s.-rtu?y of one French secondary level class, we observed
irrrir,r.Iion"l activities, analysed instructional materials, and assessed
l"igr"g. with Inuit
,'
.--: Inr
'- -,is
tl
j3uag
;t=g!l
- =,.',-
in
Second language learning
in the classroom
students' abiliry to understand and to produce written French. In the
observation data from a social studies lesson, it was evident that the teacher
had to work very hard to help students understand a text on beluga whales.
He did this in many ways-by
paraphrasing, repeating, simplifying,
checking for comprehension, gestures, etc. Despite these efforts it was clear
that most students understood very little of the text. In a French lesson,
students lacked the terminology they needed to talk about grammatical
gender in relation to adjective agreement. rvhen we examined the students'
performance on a wide range of measures to assess their knowledge of French
(for example, vocabulary recognition, reading comprehension, writing), it
was evident that the students did not have the French language skills they
needed to cope with the demands of rypical secondary level instruction.
Furthermore, even though many of the students were able to speak French
informally outside of class, their oral abilities were limited when they had to
discuss more complex academic subject matter.
The students' lack of age-appropriare academic French is a serious problem.
Solving it will involve complex educational, social and cultural questions.
belg"..
b.*..1
tt
l""gy"g
yJ
ranguage rnat tne stuclents neecl to succeed rn school.
i
ln additron, because
ocal communiry we
suggested that further development of the learners' first language literacy
Iguld
better prepare them for second language and subject
-itt.i
learning.
This suggestion has another important motivation. There are increasin--g
concerns that Inuktitut will be lost as future generations shift to English oi
French as their preferred language. An educational system that encourages
the development of both first and second languages may ensure the survival
of this heritage language (Thylor, Caron, and McAlpine 2000).
Interpreting the research
Content-based instruction has many advantages. In
amolnt oFE
order to u
>-
of-Eo
n te
It
creates a
to communlcate, motlvat
genulne
ni
r--.-
th
at iFdo-ln-i
riffryh
a I
I
e
D.h.1gj9lhqedu"n
ln a
ug.
lnterestrns
lnstructlon, especially where lessons are
designed around particular grammati
There are also some problems with content-based instruction. our research
with Inuit children adds further evidence to Jim Cummins' (1984) hypothesis that students may need several years before their abilirv to ,rr. ih.
language fbr cognitively challenging academic material has reached an
"g.S
delay in coming to grips with schooling can have lasting effecs,
as
we saw in
r59
160
Second language learning
in the classroom
the discussion of subtractive bilingualism in Chapter 1. Majoriry language
students in immersion programmes-in Canada and in Hong Kong-seem
to do well in learning subject mame! and it is noteworthy that they receive a
substantial amount of subject matter insrruction through their first language
over the full course of their academic careers. However, although they are
able to communicate with some fuency in the second language, students
often fall short of the high levels of linguistic accLLracy that their years of
schooling in the language might predict. In recent years, proponents of
content-based instruction have stressed the need to recall that content-based
language teaching is still language teaching. For example, Jana Echevarria,
MaryEllen Vogt, and Deborah Short (2004) have done research and
developed teacher education programmes rhat show the effectiveness of
lessons that have both content objectives and language objectives.
of developme
recasts as Sta
question or
Example 6
Students in ir
student who I
in Chapter 5.
Sl Myl
T \7h,
52 Inn
(two minutes
53
5
\7h,
54 My
The
researcher most closely associated with
Pienemann. I{e and hfr
h*. tti
"rro.i"t.r
this position is Manfred
,
n after
extenslye of lntenslve teachl
rlalrr unacqurreq. _t\s noteo ln
ter 2, their research provides ev
that some lrngulstrc structures,
for example, basic word order in sentences (6ofrFIex)
a predictable
'developmE
iil Chapter
attempt to
a
o
eatures
.
deve
ntal paih. These were labelled
stages of questions that we saw
this research. According to Pienemann, gL
-..
I will not
use learners have to
throu
2 and set to
3 befbre
are
As we saw in 'Get it right from
nlng, s Ients
have been
t them in gl"*l
them later because
are not
inEgrated into their
s. The underlying cause of the
@-el-Tffi'tr-Fen fully explained, but they may be based at least in pan on
learners' developing ability to notice and remember elements in the stream
ofspeech they hear.
In Example 7
Stage 3 quest
imitate
a Stag,
Example 7
(The same grc
S1 Is yc
T 'It y,
play
S1
'Is yr
S2 No.
In Example B,
provides the cr
followed by
ar
an answef.
Example B
(Inten'iewing
view also claim that certain other aspects of
lan
t at anv
me.
Sl
Is yo
52
Yes.
T
on factors
aptitude,
Bear
T
Tbach uiltat is teachable
@;--R"
a<
a
ties
n
the classroom.
.--u.
In Example 6 below, we see a teacher trying to help students with the word
order of questions. The students seem to know what the teacher means, bur
the level oflanguage the teacher is offering them is beyond their currenr srage
Youi
level
S1 Aspl
OK.
T
In Example 9
r
Stage 4 correct
Second language
learningin the cl.assroom
of development. Students are asking Stage 3 questions, which the teacher
recasts as Stage 5 questions. The students react by simply answering the
question or accepting the teachert formulation.
Example 6
Students in intensive ESL (1I-I2yeavold French speakers) interviewing a
student who had been in the same class in a previous year-see Classroom B
in Chapter 5.
51
T
Mylbne, where you put your 'Kid of the'Week' poster?
\(here did you put your poster when you got it?
52 In my room.
(two minutes later)
53
T
54
Beatrice, where you put your 'Kid of the \7eeli poster?
\(/here did you pur your poster?
My poster was on my wall and it fell down.
In Example 7, the student is using the 'fronting' strategy that is typical of
Stage 3 questions. The teachert corrective feedback leads the student to
imitate
a Stage 4 question.
Example
7
(The same group of students engaged in'Famous person interviews.)
51
T
51
52
Is your mother play piano?
'Is your mother play piano?' OK. \(ell, can you say'Is your mother
play piano?' or 'Is your mother a piano player?'
'Is your mother a piano player?'
No.
In Example B, the teacher draws the student's attention to the error and also
provides the correct Stage 4 question. This time, however, the feedback is not
followed by an imitation or a reformulation of the question, but simply by
an answer.
Example 8
(Inten iewing each other about house preferences.)
S1
Is
your favourite house is a splitJevel?
S2 Yes.
T
Youre saylng'is'two times dear. 'Is your favourite house a splitlevel?'
51 AsplitJevel.
T
OK.
In Example 9 the student asks a Stage 3 question, and the teacher provides a
Stage 4 correction that the student imitates. The interaction suggests that the
r6t
r62
Second language learning in the classroom
student is almost ready to begin producing Stage 4 questions. Note,
however, that the student does not imitate the possessive i something that
French speakers find very difficult.
Example
9
stages o
'readies'
S Do
T
the boy is beside the teacher desk?
Is the boy beside the teachert desk?
Is the boy beside the teacher desk?
Iffi
;ffi;
those
Research findings
Jhe'Teach what is teach
can be
t
ment, otner
develo
view
3
Nina Sp
tion of r
ro th
rnal
speakinl
forms
t
In a study of the acquisition of German
as a foreign language, \tan@l
ien=emann (1988) investigated whether instruction oermi-tteJ learners to
a Stage tn the natural sequence of rte.,eteBment. Two gfnrJ1c nl:
Australian universiry students who were at srage i in their
of
"cq,iisition
German word order were taught the rules associated with Stage
and
i
stage
4 respectively. The instruction_took_place over rwo weeks
d,rrirrg dL
"rid
time learners were provided with explicit grammatical rules and
exercises for
Stage 4 constructions. The learners who received instruction on Stage
3 ruie:
moved easily into this stage from Stage 2. However, those learners who
received instruction on stage 4 rules either continued to use Stage 2 rules
or
moved only into Stage 3Jhar is, rhey were
.l*ip' a stase in the
uence\Pienemann in
results as su
what they are not developmentall)' readv to
lear4
Alison Mackey and Jenefer Philp (1998) investigated whether adult ESL
learners who were at different stages in their acquisition of questions coujd
advance in their immediate
ion of these forms if thev received
ln conversatl
the incorrect
:"ith a.qrte.t
interested
in
f-orm
rests to I
5 questi,
signed tr
no studr
explicit i
ing whe
'ready') r
5 questi,
'ready'.
Learners
tbr the
S
However
,,r'ritten
c
of Stage
perform;
,i questic
-\reyou
subject c
*hil. -aintrining the-m.aninEffi,
iffi
-,ratching
:erforma
That is,
Chapter
uttefance
beyond
r
Study 29: Readies, unreadies and recasts
in
high-fre
The ma
Study 28: Ready to learn
develo
w
Study
su
change the
'develonm
course. The recommendation is to assess
the learners' developmental level and teach what would naturally come nexr.
Let us examine some studies that have tested this hypothesis.
P,
withour
were at
benefit
('Hide and seek'game.)
S
interact
questio
ir
lbr exam
ent5
were
discovering whether adult learners who received modifiec
lrench q
ibr exam
Second language learning in the
classroom 163
interaction with recasts were able to advance in their production of
question forms more than learners who received modified interaction
without recasts. Furthermore, they wanted to explore whether learners who
were at more advanced stages of question development ('readies') would
benefit more from interaction with recasts than learners at less advanced
stages of question development ('unreadies'). The results revealed that the
.readies,intheinteractionplusrecastsg'ou@
reacrles ln rne rnreractlon prus recasts group rmproveo more tnan tne
'readies' in t
iE rapid i
ment than
those who were not.
Study 30: Deaehprnentalstage andfrst knguage influence
Nina Spada and Patsy Lightbown (1999) have also investigated the acquisition of questions in relation to learners' developmental 'readiness'. French-
speaking students (aged 1I-I2) in intensive ESL classes received
high-frequency exposure to question forms that were one or rwo stages
beyond their developmental stage. Learners who were judged on oral pretests to be at Stage 2 or 3 were given high frequency exposure to Stage 4 and
5 questions in the instructional input.
The materials that contained the more advanced question forms were designed to engage the learners mainly in comprehension practice. There was
no student production and thus no corrective feedback, nor was there any
explicit instruction on question formation. \7e were interested in discovering whether Stage 3 learners (i.e. those considered to be developmentally
'ready') would benefit more from the high frequency exposure to Stage 4 and
5 questions than the Stage 2 learners, who were not yet developmentally
'ready'.
Learners' performance on an oral post-test measure indicated no advantage
for the Stage 3 learners. In fact, there was little progress for either group.
However, on a task that required learners to judge the grammaticality of
written questions there was evidence that all students had some knowledge
of Stage 4 and 5 questions. A more detailed examination of the learners'
performance on this task showed that studqnts
4and
5 auestions
or example,
'Areyou a good student?', '\fhen are
to eat
s ub ecio f tEE-ie nGn=c e was
t
r example, Are the students
watchingTV?', '\(hat is your brother doing?'). This pattern in the students'
performance appears to be related to a question rule in their first language.
That is, in French, questions with nouns in subject position are not inverted
(for example ,* Peut-Jean uenir chez moi? ='CanJohn come to my house?'). In
French questions with pronoun subjects, however, inversion is permitted
(for example , Peut-il aenir chez moi? ='Can he come to my house?').
i
164
Second language learning in the classroom
These results indicate that instruction timed to match learners'
developmental 'readiness' may move them into more advanced stages, but
their performance may still be affected by other factors. In this study firsr
language influence seems to be responsible for the learners' inability to
generalize their knowledge of inversion to all questions.
subject
(Dough
-
have
in
similar
t
rs taugh
Interpreting the research
indirect
The results ofthese studies
to learners when they are
-The'Tt
+,iiGEG
infuence canlnteract
NCSS
ways, If we compare the rypes of instructional
rnteract
input across the three studies, Pienemann's provided the mo
instruction to learners who
un
The
results
moved
the next
!.*dpr
d
ent
. The results of the
Mackey and Philp study also offer some support for the teachabiliry hypothesis but reveal that developmental readiness is not the onl)' predictor oj
success. The fact that ih. r.adies' respond.d
-or. pori,i'vJy6?."rm ttro
develo
T*.
'
the -unreadies'
suggesrs that the rype of insrructional/interactional input i.
also important. The Spada and Lightbown study shows hor11g[g-ftgS11Egg!figir
language nlay interact with developmental readiness in determilins
i@"1
Furthermore, in 6at st
w.s nFrylicir
"c$ag€s.
instruction on questions. Learners were simply exposed to a high frequenry oi
correctly formed higher stage questions in the input. Thus, they received
increased 'exposure' but no 'instruction', and, in the end, they did not perforrn
as well as learners who received focused instruction in previous studiei.
There is some research that may appear to offer counter-evidence to the
claim that it is h"neficial to
studies have used the Accessibiliw Hierarchy for relative clauses in English
a
(see Chapte r 4) to
acquisition of relative clauses.
level learners
#'-
DOSIITOn)
vo
once lea
@+hot.
i..i,
R.r"ltr of th.s. rtud
that when lorv*ho ,rt. r.l"tirr. .l^.rr., orrlyl.r-il6F*
afe ta
Hamiltonlgg4).
t
ow to use relative clauses bevond t
/
more .li
instructi
ffi
]]4dssc
notiryr[
-roo adv
--5!-.._
.languag(
'.}re timr
:onstder
rhis poir
right in
6
r
Get
Propone
:ole
fot.
basro-h€
listen ...
t"r"r.*
b;ffi
-.--iS CfUCta
i@
_aarnefs
At first glance, this
research seems to cor{iadict Pienemann's claim tha:
learners should be taught what is 'next'. However, it ilalso possible that the
ures are
different sorts
Chapter
notivati
also
theJwere-taugbt (Ammar and Lightbown 2005;
988;
only a sr
developr
:cquired
-
current level.
clause Dosrtl(
-In
some instances
s)'llabus
For example, it has been suggested thar
r
-'iprofic.
Propone
,-:achablr
:a]<e the
Second knguage learning in the
classroom
ive clauses in one position (usually the
ject position),
th.r. ir ro .onsrraint
on their
(Doughty I99I). \X/hat all the
tiye clause teaching and learning
have in common is that learners acquire the relative clauses in an oldgrygy
similar to the accessibiliry
t
l
arning subiect, then direct tb;.Jt, th..,
indirect obj.ct, and so on.
The 'Teach what is
f
teachable'
t must be emphasized thala:yUabus.There are numerous practical reasons for this, not least the fact that
only a small number of language features have been described in terms of a
developmental sequence. \fhile Pienemann's work
Iw\tSee
Chapter 2) provielesi:rsigh:s-i
some
more diffict'lt than others. those principles are not easily translated into
instructional
sequences. As Patsy Lightbown (1998)-E; suggested, the
.ffi=--i-i--i.
research is
important primaril
ins teachers
are
ruction on la
rs with
not immediatelv. The research also shows
4oo
srill be heloful bv r
"a"il-Aa<.,rrray
that thev will be a
t-at
least
that is
the time is right. However, many other facrors
consideration in choosing language features to focus on. \7e will return to
this point after we discuss the final proposal for language teaching 'Get it
right in the end.'
6 Get it right in the end
Proponents of the'Get irylght in
4+ered=+osi+ie*+eeeg{dze€n_i!0ps4qt
role for:brq-f.ocused
instrueiiqr
they do not assumelhareverlthing
talli, 'Two for one', and the Jusi
and read'positions, they have concluded that many language
has m-h" eaught. Like advocates of theLeCs
listen ...
{eatures-from front'tt.iation
acquit"d
.r."'all)'if
to
vocabular}' and
qranrnrarEillTd
lea.-ers har.. ,dequate exposure to the language and a
Thus,
rlhj
vlew
ion-based, contentrylI3ltg!.lsleambased, task-based, or other
of essentiallv meanin
lnstructron
as crucial for I
rnlng, t
rners will do better
ift
ve access to some formargue that
learners will benefitG terms
e
ency
ir learning and the level
of proficiency they will eventually reach.
Proponents of this position also agree with advocates of the '&fujarjs
t.".h"bl.' por+i"r
he teaching fails to
the student's
t. I hrs
165
r66
Second language learning in the classroom
proposal differs from the 'Teach what is teachable'
it ernphasizer the id."ih"GilEi
must be taught and marneed to
. There are a4grber of
ance_rorm_
or correctrve
l----------....2.....HDe espectally rmporran
nGTiffiffii
firstlanguagilGtwili-ffi"t
Examlt,
(The st
others
SI
T
p"rtlythe resurt of transfer from
that shared language. Because the errors "r. ,rot
"r.
c
i
s2
T
SI
Examples 10,
1 1 , and l2are taken from a classroom
where a group of twehryear-old French speakers,are learning English. t,.r"-pT.-i6,
rh.y
engaged in an activiry where scrambled ,.rr,.rr..,
"r.
are reordered to form
sensible ones' The following sentence has been praced
;;
;ir.
'Sometimes mymother makes good cakes'.
board:
As we si
with'hi
4s-eh,ir
selectint
briefy,
correct
Example 10
l
T Another place to put our adverb?
Sl Aftermakes?
T After makes.
(The stt
questior
52
Before good?
Mymother makes sometimes good cakes.
COff€Ct
No.
No, we cant do that. It sounds yucky.
languag
T
T
53
53
T.
Exampl
lively
r
ar
SI
Yucky!
T
Disgusting. Horrible. Right?
54 Horrible!
rdly
a
Proponents of 'Get
eventua
ical
it
lgqg"l And yet the students' arten
t in the end'
SI
is
that what learners focus on can
T
SI
Note thr
his pleas
tnat focuslns on
errors or that they will begin .rrirrq
suggest that the
the target
il$tol
"
nstrucuon
lvill allow learners to notice
r inpur and interaEffi
it is understood
fearu
as
nt leaffiiiTr-ohform as soon as it i
.
need.to
ren ts
interfere
Propone
driiliE
@
right f'.'
fodearn
rtlgil-ext
ac-q_qfrd
instructi
ITS
rt of the
Second language
learningin the ckssroom
Example 11
(The students are practising following instructions; one student instrucrs,
others colour.)
S1 Make her shoes
brown.
Now, her shoes. Are those Momt shoes or Dadt
T
shoes?
S2 Mom's.
T Momt. How do you know itt Momt?
S1 Because itt her shoes.
fu we saw in Chapter 4, French-speaking learners of English have difficulry
with'his' and'her' because French
ves use the
matical
rof
the obi
the natural
ive form. The teacher is aware o tnls
selectins the
briefly, wi
correct form.
t
ano-
interrupting the activiry-helps the learners notice the
Example 12
(The students are playing'hide and seek with a doll in a doll's house, asking
questions until they find out where 'George' is hiding. Although a model for
correct questions has been written on the board, the game becomes quite
lively and students spontaneously ask questions that refect their interlanguage stage.)
51
S1
Is George is in the living room?
You said'is' two times dear. Listen to you-you said'Is George is
in?' Look on th€ board. 'Is George in the' and then you say the
name of the room.
Is George in the living room?
T
Yeah.
T
sl Iwin!
Note that the teachert brief intervention does not disrract the student from
his pleasure in the game, demonstrating that focus on form does not have to
interfere with genuine interaction.
nts of 'Get it
draw
ri
rners attentlon
it is sometimes
to focus oniffi-fi
their
n
use
from the
They aqsume thagnuch
acoutsttlon wlll cleveloD naturallv o.llI-ot such
instrqcllo-n that focuses on the language itself.
of
t57
168
Second language learning in the classroom
Research findings
This ir
In recent years, there has been an increasing interest in examining issues
related to this proposal, leading to both descriptive and experimental
sguglu
cornp4!
b-ut-rhe
studies.
Study 31: Form-focus exqteriments in ESL
Since the 1980s, we have investigated the effects of form-focused instruction
and corrective feedback on the developing English of French-speaking
students participating in intensive ESL classes in Quebec. For five months
in either grade 5 or grade 6, students (aged 10-12) spent most of every
school day learning English through a variety of communicative interactive
activities.
In descriptive studies involving almost 1,000 students in thirry-three classes,
Form (Lightbown and
we found that teachers rarel
990, r994).
ln communlcatrYe lnteractlon.
lng rat
of the teaching was on activities
taneous lnteractlon,
nliles
. In these classes, learners
com
enl
3u.ncy, and commilnicaqyg-conficlgnce
ilfftr;ffi;,h.y-,',i
in
not the
could
b,
instruct
The stu
tests. Fc
-en+q
recerved
-Derform
immedir
-
sixweek
thclcarr
rhei&pe
(\X/hite
In th. 'l
eains th
followin
,.rrt"g
and comolexitv.
l_-__-/*
(
contribu
The experimental studies involved a smaller number of classes. In these
studies, we examined the effects of form-focused instruction and corrective
ttme.
feedback
The dif{
djff.tg"E
--
plffiilt
It. *i.it.J
"d].tb
fot
invesilgation because of the differences between English and French that
have been discussed (see Study 17 in'Just listen... and read'). The hypothesis was that learners would persist in using adverb placement rules
learners
:dverbs v
:unify tc
conSIstentwlth.hrench(the1rhrStlanguage)1rt@told
a\posure
Quistions were
rnd use q
selected for the second study because they have been extensively investigated
in the literature and considerable comparison data were available,
particularly with regard\o
".qttt,-1yly9lgl
Both the experimental \nd the comparison groups were tested before the
experiment began and again when the period of special instruction had
ended. Throughout the period of the experiments, all students continued to
participate in the regular communicative activities that were rypical of their
instruction. In addition, all students received instruction designed for the
experiment. The researchers gave each teacher a set of pedagogical materials
to be used for this purpose. The experimental groups received approximatelreight hours of instruction on adverbs or questions over a two-week period.
lome focr
iei?Tno
-991;Spi
-:
several
-'e have o
.:cond lar
:--scribed
w
:-uestions
:_qulsltlo.
-hapter
4
.:srructiol
Second language learning in the classroom
This included some explicit teaching of the rules associated with
r69
each
sgUtlulg-as *.11
comparisoq gro"
onal instruction,
but th.ir t.".h.rr *.r.
to t.a.h
"rk.d
",
not the focus #the expgirqgnt.
In this way, the comparison-gfiFG-*;
me
with the rypes of tasks and activities that were used for
instruction in the experimental groups and in the testing procedures.
The studies included immediate, delayed, and long-term/follow-up posrtests. For the adverb study the test tasks were writren, and in the queition
tion study the tests included both written and oral tasks. Learners who
recel
explicit instruction on adverb placement dramat-j
he adverb
ion had disa
that of unins
(\White 1991).
instructed
also made sisnificant
than the rrninstructed srouD o;
itten tasks im
ing instruction, and they maintained their level of knowledge on larer
testr
cont
(six weeks-i
uted to im
tructron
ment in oral
rmance
was sustalned over
ttrne.
The difference
di
due to a
room lnput
learners little
malntain their
continued 5&s4'6
. In contrasr, rhere were hundreds of opporruniti.r toTE, o,l,a6f
ahd use questionqevery day in the classroom. once learnets hjLdbee+ given 6 l.,tlsw9
toT. &.,qq.djnsrr-ucrion,_ir seerntrh_e)Lwsre alle tq continrre rc_aclvan.. in t
their knowledge and use of quesrions (vhite, spada, Lighrbown, and RaiE
1 991 ; Spada and Lightbown I 993).
v
In several of the studies we ha'e carried out in intensive ESL programmes,
we have observed the strong influence of the learner's first language on their
second language development. In Study 30 in 'Teach what is T.".h"bl.',
described the tendency of intensive ESL learners
quesrrons wnen rhe suDjecr rs a noun outffie
*.
to reject inversion in
ir
acquisition of the possessive dererminers 'his' and 'her' were discussed in
chapter 4 andin study 18. This led us to consider whether form-focused
instruction that includes explicir contrasrir.e information about how the first
170
Second language learning
in the classroom
and second language differ would help in their development of questior
explore this, we found
formation
iorrle*ive determin.tr. Lr a study to
"rd
determiners impror-ec
possessive
that learner, *ho r.c.i,ed instruction on
more in their knowledge and use of this feature than did learners rvhc
received instruction on q-rrestion forms. \7e related this finding to differences
is. ;
berween the form/m."ning connecrions of these two features. That
is
more
mother')
her
with
('He's
home
going
misused possessive determiner
than an ill-formed questior:
breakdown
a
communication
to
l."d
to
likely
(for e"ample, '\x/here he going?'). Results like these -point to the. importance
wat'
of .onridoing how instru"ction may affect language features in different
Study 3,
3.ov Lrvs
rnOlvle<
r lrencl
;ramint
,ingular
=nd farr
:o instr
:,.,-e
re tes
:o othel
(Spada, Lightbown, and \Mhite 2005).
.he ins
a growine
As we saw in the discussion of the 'Two for one' position, there is
immersioi
French
as
belief that learners in content-based programmes such
need more opportunities to focus on form and receive corrective feedback' -{
:e riod.
number or ,trrdi.r have explored the question of how this can best be
accomplished.
Study 32: Focusing on the conditional in French irnmersion
Elairie Day and Stan Shapson (1991) examined the effects of instruction on
the abiliry of French immersion students (aged about 12 ot 13) to use the
conditional mood of verbs in sentences such as Si je gagnais la loterie'
partirdis en uqlage('If I won the lottery, I would go away on a trip')'
jt
classes received several hours of focuseO
instruction on the ionditional over a period of five to seven weeks. The
students in the control group continued with their usual classroom routines'
that is, they continued to ett.ounter French mainly in the context of learnin-g
Students
in the experimental
their general school subjects (science, mathematics' history, etc.) through
the medium of French'
Special teaching materials were prepargd for the experimental classes by the
team of rese"r.L.rr. They consisted of: (1) group work that created oppor-
tunities for the use of the conditional in natural communicative situations:
(2) written and oral exercises to reinforce the use of the conditional in more
formal, structured situations; and (3) self-evaluation activities to encourage
,trrd.rrt, to develop conscious awareness of their language use. oral and
written rests were administered before the instructional treatment.
immediately after the instruction (five to seven weeks later), and at the end of
the school year.
Learners i
.xp:licit
:.tle ol:
=edbac
:ontinu
:uided
:nmedi
rificant
rrai pr,
--_._
lenefitr
Srudy 3
tsirgit
I
:rildrer
rr 8 yer
:rat is I
:ramm
;hildrer
I
tPr)
:isdnct
rascull
:roun
e
,or fem
::sted
:ests
c
b<
rmmed
-eceive<
:ccurat
lasses outperforme{
:eceive
:eneral
@-tettt
several months later'
:oo mu
:his mr
:han th
Second knguage learnirug in the classroom
Study 33: Focusing on sociolinguisticforms in French immersion
Roy Lyster (1994) examined the effects of form-focused instruction on the
know
and use of sgqiolinguistic sryle variations in three classes of
rmmersion students (about f3 yEais olcl). One of the main features
examined ii-hlT3t was
istinction befween the use of second person
singular pronouns tuand uous.The former is used to indicate informaliry
and familiarity while the latter is used as a formal marker of politeness. prior
to instruction, immediately after, and again one month later, the learners
were tested on their ability to produce and recognize these forms (in addition
to others) in appropriate contexrs.
The instruction took place for an average of twelve hours over a five-week
period. During this time, students in the
icit instruction and
contexts and corrective
o,.s ln a Yarrcty
ts rn tne two companson c
continued
r regular instruction without any focused instruction or
guided practice in using sociolinguistically appropriate forms. on the
urrrrrLurdts post-tesr,
immediate
lll flle
the experimental
experlmental CfaSSeS
peftofmect SIqclasses performed
sigPU5L-LC5L' ,rsalIlcrs
,learners in
nrhcantly better than learners rn the comparison classes on both writcen and
oral Dfocluctron
rmore, t
benefits were
miiniil
wnen learners were tes
ahonth
later.
Study 34: Focusing on gender in French imrnersion
Birgit Harley (1998) examined the effects of instruction with very young
children in French immersion programmes. six classes ofgrade z children (i
or B years of age) were given focused instruction on a grammatical feature
that is known to be a persistent problem for French imlersion 51udsn15grammatical gender. For twenry minutes a day over a five-week period these
children carried out many activities based on children's games (for e*ample,
'l rpr) that were modified to draw the children's aitention ,o g.nd.,
distinctions and which required them to choose between feminine and
masculine articles (une or un, la or /e). Students were also taught how certain
noun endings provide clues about gender (for example, -ettein la bicyclette
for feminine, and -eauin le bateau for miiculine). the students were pretested on their knowledge of grammatical gender via listening and speaking
tests before the instruction began and the same tests were administereJ
immediately after instruction and then again five months later. Learners who
received instruction were much better at recognizing and producing
accurate gender disdnctions for familiar nouns than those who did not
receive instruction. However, the instruction did not enable learners to
generalize their learning to new nouns. Harley's interpretation of this is that
too much newvocabularywas introduced in the later teaching activities and
this meant that teachers spent more time teaching the meaning of words
than the noun endings and their relationship to gender. Therefore, 'the input
17I
172
Second language learning in the classroom
on noun endings was simply not available in sufficient quantity and inten-'
iry for the majority of students to establish the predictive relevance of &e
noun endings in question (p. 169).
Prompts
llnsurstrc
*--.r1_*
ll'pes of t,
form in content-based
Lvsrerh-l
Study 35: Focusing on uerb
science cl.assrooms
Catherine Doughry and Elizabeth Varela (1998) carried out a study with ;
group of ESL learners who received second language instruction in contenrbased teaching. One class of middle-school students (II-I4 years old) fron
avariety of first language backgrounds received cgqggjE&g!!3ck on pa-.;
tense and conditional verb forms in English in their science class. For severaweeks, while students were engaged in oral and written work related to i
series ofscience reports, the teacher corrected their errors in past tense anc
conditional for*r-.bo,h ."pli.irlv.
ly. Students' abiliry to u5i
these forms was assessed before and after the experimental period and agair
rwo months later. Their performance was compared to that of a group o:
students who were in another science class doing the same science reporr
but who did not receive corrective feedback on the verb forms.
Student: who.received the correctiye feedback-made morg prgg.ress in,using
oasi and conditionalJorms than the comoarison srouo both immeFiatelt
and two months later. Their
ln tefms o
nts were doine more than re
ting forrrr.
had heard.
Study 36: fucasts andpromqtts in French immersion classroom.s
In Chapter 5, we described some of Roy Lystert descriptive research on th.
different rypes of corrective feedback provided by teachers in Canadian
French immersion and learners' immediate responses (uptake) ro thar
feedback. More recently, Lyster (2004) explored the effects of form-focused
insuuction (FFI) grd feedback type on second l"ngu"ge1e"r
studihTs who-frffi10-1l years old, in grade 5 French immersion classrooms. There were three experimental groups and one comparison group.
Learners in the experimental groups received explicit FFI on grammatical
gender. The instruction drew their attention to the fact that some noun
endings reliably predict grammatical gender in French. For example, it is safe
to assume that words that end in -ette are feminine, while those that end in
-age are masculine. After this information had been presented, students
participated in approximately eight hours of instructional activities in which
their attention was drawn to this language feature while they were working
internaliz
learners t
not use r
e@iih?
did not
received
qrouPs c(
based ins
On the p
rhe comt
-*-'4
F
FV+pror
.......e
'K@s
rhe exper
rasks. Lyr
ii66-con
students
researche
order to
<
to speak
learners
s
masculin
provided
rarget fea
groups or
compone
Study
37
Motivate
!d'Tc
develoon
collabora
@
-
ros!_p.gll
were tnt(
on their regular subject-matter instruction. Two of the experimental
groups also received corrective feedback in the form of either recasts or
between
made rev
prompts. These two rFpes of feedback differ primarily in that recasts sive
learnerg thp corr.... targ:t forr-n whergas @dgnal
the need for, a
the refon
the
resea
episodes-
Second language learning in the
classroom
173
Prompts include clarification requests, repetitions, elicitation, and metaof these different
ffiplei
ues (see Lhapter
back).
v'
hy
internalizedform.' (p. 405). T
ted that
that t
Iearners
mpts can push
have some
it to their inter
. The third
but
ictivities,
expeflmental group rece
learners
of
did not receive consistent feedback. The comparison group
received neither FFI nor corrective feedback on grammatical gender. All
groups continued their regular French immersion proglamme of contentbased instruction.
not use reliablv and to com
re
and the relatecl lnstruc
f:Fr* p,o^fi
On the post-tests
rhe comoarison sroup in assigninq grammatical gender. Iq-addition, the
up on
tter tnan
p did signi
among
r, there were no srgnlhcant
t
(FFI, FFI + prompts, andFFl + recasts) on the ora
tFe experimental
of the
is finding as a
TASKS. LVSTET
rlme-consumlng nature of oral taikt, o.tly_" randomly selected subsample of
" f"1.-ly':t:.,
-"{ystudv.
'he
with the
I hese stuclents met wltn
tfris parr of thffimet
students
srudents participated in thi
in
ln
sessions,
SeSSlOnS,
these
tnese
During
l-Jurlng
sessions.
Sesslons.
onq-on-one
tntensive
tntenslve
rn
tn
researcher
three
rlsearCher
students
r.h.,
eriotrraged
order to .nrrrr
to speak as clearly as possible because previous research had shown that
learners sometimes used a'hybrid article' that could be interpreted as either
masculine or feminine. This emphasis on clear articulation of articles
provided learners in all three groups with individualized attention on the
target feature and thus may have contributed to the performance of all three
groups on the oral measures, regardless of their experiences in the classroom
component of the research.
Fr-t *
FFi{ t<t4
c,:\t
tr,l 1"
g
idea that
11-
Swain and Sharon
learnins occurs
(2002)€bserved the language*
srrrdents as thev wrore
a
storv
they had written with a reform
tt i"
a
tttm"
swain and LaPkin
out what students noiiid about differences
and the reformulated one and whether they
version
original
berween their
made revisions to their original stories based on their collaborative talk about
rhe reformulated version. The talk that learners produced in all phases of
the research was recorded, transcribed and coded for language-related
episodes-'any part of the dialogue where learners talk about the language
v,.@
*,(
T.-',e
llcr,
^tt
orr,v\
Study 37: Focus onform through collaboratiue dialogue
Motivated
.(+r-c.rF
-
I
174
Second language learning
in the classroom
they produced, and reflect on their language use' (p. 292). An excerpt of the
learners' collaborative talk from this study is presented in Chapter 5. The
language related episodes were coded in terms of whether they focused on
lexical, grammatical, or discourse features. The researchers used the original
the two learners created
re-test
e storles tnat
each learner const
lea
much
rv. The researchers conclude rhar
th. -"liip
ge in collaborative talk on the
language features in question led them to a greater understanding of their
correct use.
Study 38: Focus onform in task-based instruction
In a study investigating the importance of the teacher's role
in
task-based
,-) a@ersthrougha'@
progression', learners were !rq! eqkgl to \Mqrk in groups to speculate on thE
'a i&;fiFf an unknown p.rion (for e"ample;age, ge"aei, occ"p"tio"fU.
lo@ght
to come from that persont pocket. In
carrying out this task, learners were observed to produce expressions of
probabiliry and possibility such as 'It's possible that he smokes' and 'maybe
itt a girl', b", ftry instances of modal arxiliaries (for example, 'Tg!!_:_.m3;,')
were used.
students *ere asE[ to .o-.
to@H6r as a whole group to tell each other what they had decided. During
this phase, t
her acted as a co-communicator and maintained the focus
on meanind but
lv shifted to form by usins the lan
that the
learners
on tnelr own and proudln
with alternative
yr ot %pl.rjgl!t".gl3!nry. Initially, thir * dq4b i*pliqitly. For
example ilTlearner said something like'\7e think uh 50 per cent he smokes'.
the teacher said'So you're not certain that he smokes?'After each group had
presented, the teacher provided a more explicit focus. She drew the learners'
attention to other ways of expressing possibiliry and probabiliry by oveitlr
ta ng
age rorm as
m'TfieffiF below(p. 13
T
,fii;
\X4ren
probaL
and co
that m
soeech
Interp
The or
instruction, Virginia Samuda (2001) explored ways of guiding adult ESL
learners' attention to form-meanins relationshi
focusing on expressions
posslDlllty a
illty (ror example, mrgnt , coulcl , rts posslble r. In
ST
whole
hypoth
aommt
mes cal
matlca,
lnstruc
15
Contl
aher th
-)waln
i
Drogral
providr
otner ti
."tt";
I
_earnerl
:orm v
also
in.
rhat pu
iorq.
We hav
some
lr
,y/uous
-earninr
','ocabu
Businessman
:!aturer
Businessman ninety? OK So youre 90 per cent certain he's a
businessman, right? Heret another way to say this. You think it\
90 per cent certain, so you think he must be a businessman. He
must be a businessman (writes it on the board). So this (points to
'must be' on board) is showing how crnrerN how suRE you are .
Not 100 per cent, but almost 100 per cent. 90 per cent.
'eafneft
In the final
stage
of the task, the studentr+repared and Fr.".
based on their conclusions
possessi
neanin
suggestr
-,r-hen
t
Tleanrn
nave a l
necessal
n to the
9rrecil
\---::
Second language learning
in the c/assroom
175
\When the
researcher
probabiliry ard oo.r.;1,;ri,1lf1::l^:lt ,differences between expressions
ff :'.*jil,il1n#li:y
ii wrth *J
ll1
tlla{- ::t_",ed
nLrn gle ngtanc_es
Ta
spqech.
j::l$".',a;il::::::"ff :ffi ::?:i,:l:",lof
n,J,*e.,
n
i
Interpreting the research
Jhe
ole11l]1esglrs of the
studies
for the
.ommunic3lyg and contenr
mes can
wlthrn
Wrheirknowled
ow,
Thi
rs con
rc9_exposure to a liner
'"_"""11
atrer
th e ex
and
"nd,..r.
ure rn the
perGn tdlTre"atffi
ofp"rrGtGhil_
!
to wherher there
ar ctaJ-sro=E'Eput
Lapkin,s,;dy;f
.;mtive interaction in
lwain
Prog:p.m:s points to the fact that
provide information
about
and
each
*
r,Jdy
can !$Samud"r
clrccuvely drrect students,
form within. task_based
attention to
inrr.u.dJrr.
also in French immersion,
that
ts that I
ghem to
fi
that
ldes the co
\7e
have also seen that form-focused
instruclign may be more
some language features
effective with
,h"r
tu/uous distinction
learning
tu
in Lyrt.r,
and ,or,
;il-;,h.rl rr,. #;;cii:;;-*
(fi;4;'ily
.is.rr.,r;idi';
;;;';;
that affect meaning
voca b u r ary i tems
a n d,h,,
could_ be due
of the
to the fact that
,,lr*.r., of learnins two
b.J [:, d trfi .;i;;; ilil HJT'",,"*
fearures
i" r.*
In our srudy with intensive
learners, Iearners
"s#u, *rrr.
rr":* l..ri;;
-"y
lu...rrfrl
possessive determiners
irrriruction on
*'"n qu.r,i*lL."ur. there"t.,
is ,trong.. for-ffi::f ;:: T;ilil::l J ?:Yr+ ;;'J'i"""'lhan wi,h" q u es,i o n s rhi,
meanrng. Vfhen
r-
ffi
nave a major im
t on the
necessary to sustalE
ofm-
fes that do
g1f-c"nnJ)Tu,-l
176
Second language learning in the classroom
The implications of classroom research
for teaching
ln splt
to\ sln
studer
Many questions have been raised by the research that has been done to tesr
the hypotheses that the different proposals
t. Although there is stili
much work to do, it seems evident
exclusive focus on meanins or those
resentln an al
exclusive focus
rm alone are not recom
form within communicati
support trom classroom
\We
know that
learners
will
succeed
rdless of the teachins methodlf,n
lansuase learni
second
s
ffitd.l
worIo,
in
of the
m
trave-E€t-Individuals whose mastery of a foreign language
of their experience in such classes. Similarly, agdjdi:,€l,al
instruction has produced hi
us
-16]
rnterpl
TE"r,
own, i
q"-b
It
is in
focuse,
'G;it
d;54
that
ir
conten
developed out
leiffi
Howffialsci
m;i;;
that these met
leave man]
in ordinarv conversa
dsrdd
rsses. Grammai iransTation
and audiolingual approaches will continue to be used, but the evidence
suggests that'Get ir right fi
way the mai
I second lan
their
rnstruc
Resear,
point r
antlfH
o'dfl
mental
,hsrel
mental
There is i
basic
strui
ing evidence that learners conrinue to
oft
mmes that
haLve
difficulry with
or no lorm-
lons-t(
propos
rvavs in
The in
I
evidence that learners make considerable
n ln com
ypomesls
[earners si
focus on meanin
nsion-based
tlct
in both
find
mmes
care of itself i
ible inour.
when they include
ided attention to
features as a
&uq--o-afgJst. This
anguage I
dri;
also
es
sai*
acl]Y!!l1
;haract
rion. p
is
language and learning backgrou
-{ccord
should
and un
The 'Lett talld proposal raises similar concerns. Opportunities for learnen
-..=+-Yersational i
and parred actrvltres can
and the abiliw to manage cffi
rnefs ma
-gwge
th
measul
langual
allow them to communicate
s
@
Second language
Because 'Let's
in soite of their
when
talli
ln
mmu lcailon
simula
students
learningin the classroom
dat
n,
conversatlon
o
SISNA
as a
v
a
--
learners may
contlnuatlon
us, programmes
..,.
conversatlon
s
cus on
tark app
are lncom
own, and learners' gains in fuen
matched by their
ment of more accurate and com
It is important to emphasize that the evidence
focused instruction and corrective feedback
roach.
16
on tnelr
not
be
lan
has shown that learners
fromcirffi
that is meaning-based The results of
form.
to support a role for for{n-
@
that certain
the
urthe rmore,
back on error
It
177
researcE--in
performance afe not
as
Research investigating rhe
feach what
is
rriler].
-rn expefl---.-..._t- The number of features that research.rs harri ilvesti
mental studies within this framework is.far too small. On the other hand,,
t
mental sequences is necessary or even desirable or thaiit wilfimpro',re the
age
at rs most yalua
lt Serves to
in which learners'inte
age may change in response to instruction.
The implications of 'Te--il
t 15 teac
may De seen prlmarily ln the
fact that genuine progress in second language development must be
measured in ways that include, but are not limited to, increased accuracy in
language production.
According to the Qe=t-!Lgbfgt:f!g_ end' pro,posal, classroom activities
-o-uld be built primarily on creati
ities f'or students to
PF}
an
CF
the h
also essential
for
ls to
characteristics of the
contlnu
ce between
The lea(oe.rs'agE)
tion, prior pdus=L_li-o=g?lj=xpsgqnces, mffia,
and form-focused
tng to tne
me
talhguisric-sophisrie*
gga!9.,.as-tue!
€
"rd
-_==:
"-tuly:glS:_r-q.,l""gj'"g:..!911qlgy"gq"!'.e4y@-!""qesdlq_b..
-==::-:;:'
T7B
Second language learning in the classroom
taken into account when decisions are made about the amount and rype
_:5
form-focus to offer.
One impo
on is that of choosin the lan
ts
'incidentallv'-that
H
exposurei These incl
of
oi
features that are to
age are lear
th
slm
features that are
oatterns rhar are consruenr
vocabula
-E'ho-A3losicallv salienr.(and-lomlEramrnatical
Fith the learnerst first language..Other features, however, are
moreTi[Elio
more
with
the
help
of
instruction.
Catherine
be
efficiently acquired
classror
import
may be
and thr
-\s we
comple
comoli
bb bett
ffie;
it plura
also oc
>-l-
dlscov€
form that occurs
in order to understand
ally redundant (i.e. n6t
her a speaker says 'Keiko live in
tE-rid
thrrd person
penson srngular
singular -s.
-s. ThEefore, whether
i
Tolryo
Iolryo no# or 'Keiko lives in Tokyo now', the listener will understand the
tl
meaning. For this reason, the -s may be difficult to notice and may not be
acquired unless the learner's attention is drawn to it through form-focused
instruction.
r
,a
for which form-focused instruction
Other
crucial role are those that are influe
ers nrst
are misleadins simi
e first a
secono
n seconct
rv be incre?sed in
second languase
la
:j::'classrooms where learneETh-are the same firstq -langu
lan age asdreinfo-rclteach
o
example, rrJd.ilrr in French immersion may need guidance indisiinguishing berween the French auoir/?treand
English 'have/be'. Form-focused instruction may also help in those cases
where learners have developed an interlanguage rule, based on the firsr
language, that is more general than the rule in the second language, for
example, the problem that French-speaking students had with adverb
placement in English.
Language forms that affect meaning in ways that can lead
as learners en
in nesotiation to solve those
oroblems.
r
r maKes an error wrtn a
connections-th:an others. For example, if a
€
ffi.
p6Tsessive determiner in English and says 'John took
her money' instead of
'John took his money communication is likely to be affected. The forms 'his'
and'her' are crucial to understanding the meaning. Ifhowever, a speaker sar-s
'John take his mo ney?' aclqqp anrjd-tyllb4ll4gjllggdgn, instead of ' Did
John take his money?', it is likely that both utterances will be understood a-.
questions. The absence of inversion does not interfere with communication
in the same way that choosing the wrong word does. Evidence from
if the
i
'GfrSum
Classrc
grei
JOntexl
:ffectir,
..<-i
-rmrted
{luraJ
and
s
:-edba(
:oG?ii
amenti(
ri learr
-.,1G;
classroorn
Second knguage learning in the
179
classroom research suqqests that form-focused instruction misht be more
important for featureJ with weat
.
.
at they say
and the correct
*"y
to say what rhey mean.
As we know, the rules associated with some language features are more
complex than others. For example, the artl
complex and abrq4l! and notoriously difficult to reac,h. Thus, learners may
b
input. On the other
'put
hand, a simple'rule oftEl
as
an -.rat the end of a noun to make
if the '
this
form that is hard to
on successful communication.
rule
s@effect
Summary
Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that
lorm-foo'sfd instruction and corrective feedb.ack provided within the,
arguethatse@
(and should)
uided, form'
ln certaln cl
instruction and cor
r examDlel t
*i
to,correcr.psrsistent errors that leainEn-Gemnot to notiie
attention. Teachers should also
of learners in a class are
the
TeachGFh-rffilGTfTo
nrng to eme
t
more aware o
L
tures that are
guage
the second
their
students and orovide some
activities that draw
activiliFs*by devel
@j!g
account
k
rners to
can also
iwh
r attentlon
contexts in whi
ins them to ask
crea
communrcatlve
t
ions about lan
ide form focus must
differenHn leariEr-ch4lacterilii6l6f coFse. Quite different
ts
l.Jrrring
a
?
v
lan
int out how a
beglgleuod'
structure in a learnert first
just
not hesirate
fourth
or fifth language, young children beginning their schooling in a second
language environment, both younger and older immigrants who cannot
180
Second language learning in the classroorn
read and write their own language, and adolescent!
language for a few hours a week at school.
teachers are
studfng a foreign
Brown,
Oxford
the need to balance
research sim
fifirm their
assroom
. Although this may be true to some
is hardlv the case that all teachers have a clear sense of how best to
ilntit
glgplish
Burns,.
Cambri
Christi
rheti-&oal. It is not always easy to step back from familiar
practices and say, 'I wonder if this is really the most effective way to go about
this?' Furthermore, it can be difficult to try out classroom practices that
ntexts. rylanv teacners
inst the orevailinsliE
in env[onments where there is an emphasis on accuracy that
virtually excludes spontaneous language use in the classroom. At the same
qa! of communicative language teaching methods has
sometimes resulted in a comolete reiecii6A-o
teaching. But it is not necessary to
E6Ftion in second lan
meanrnginstruction. Rather, tE:!419!9.
is to find t
bal
VA:TI
Classroom-based research on second language learning and teaching has
glven us partlal answers to many questlons. I hrough contlnulng researcn
and experience, research?FTfitl teachers will 6ll in more details, always
recognizing that no single answer will be adequate for all learning environments. Among the questions we will continue to ask are these: How can
classroom instruction provide the right balance ofmeaning-based and formFocused instruction? \71f&h features of language will respond best to Qgrfocused instruction, and
wrtnout expllclt
Gass,
leArn6sfaYe tdequate access to
well to metallnsulsuc ln
attentlon on
\7hich learners will_Iesfgd
will reauire some orher wav of
?
rm?
to oraw
ers
,
attention to form-before, after, or during communicative practice? How
should corrective feedback be offered
*h.n should learners be alliled
"nd
utterancesi Lontln
s their attention
classroom-centred research, including the action research by teachers in
their own classrooms, will provide further insights into these and other
important issues in second language teaching and learning.
Crand
Higher
Cumn
Crossfi
Dougl
Seconc
Ellis,
l
learnir
interar
Specia
Hedgr
Oxfor
Hinke
and Lt
Hink
Secont
Associ
Howa
Oxfor
Lado,
McGr
Larser
2nd
e<
Sources and suggestions for further reading
Lightl
Baetens Beardsmore, H. (ed.). 1993. European Models
Educati on. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Mack
Brown, H.
D. 2001. Teaching by Principles: An Interactiue
Language Pedagoglt.
of Bilingual
Approach to
-Wesley
Longman Inc.
\X/hite Plains, NY Addison
secon(
and
L
Second language
learningin the classroom
D. and T. S. Rodgers. 2002. Doing Second Language
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Brown, J.
Research.
Burns, A. 1999. Collaboratiue Action Research for English Language
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Teachers.
Christian, D. and F. Genesee (eds.). 2001. BilingualEducation Alexandria,
VA: TESOL.
Crandall, J. A. and D. Kaufman (eds"). 2002. Content-Based Instruction in
High e r E duc ati o n S e ttings. Alexandria, VA: TES OL.
Cummins, J. 2000. Language, Power and Pedagoglt: Bilingual Children in the
Crossfre. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Doughry,
Seco
C. and J.'sTilliams (eds.). 1998. Focus on Form in
Ckssroom
nd Language Acquisition. Cambridse: Cambridge University Press.
Ellis,R. (ed.). 2001. 'Form-focussed instruction and second
learning'. Language Learning 5l I Supplement
language
1.
Gass, S., A. Mackey, and T Pica (eds.). 1998. 'The role of input and
interaction in second language acquisition . Modern Language Journal\2l3,
Special Issue.
Hedge, T. 2000. Teaching and Learning in the Language Ckssroom. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Hinkel, E. (ed.). 2005. Handbook of Research in
Second Language Teaching
and Learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
Hinkel, E. and S. Fotos. 2002. New Perspectiues on Grammar Tbaching in
Second Language Classrooms. Mahwah, NJr Lawrence Erlbaum and
Associates.
Howatt, A. P. R. 2004. A History of English Language Tbaching.2nd edn.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lado, R. 1964. Language Teaching
McGraw-Hill.
A
Scientifc Approach. New York:
Larsen-Freeman, D. 2000. Techniques and Principles in Language Tbaching
2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Lightbown, P M. 2000. 'Anniversary article: Classroom SLA research and
second language teaching'. App lied Linguistics 2l I 4 : 43 l-52.
Mackey, A. and S.
M. Gass. 2005.
Second Language Research: Methodology
and Design. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
181
r82
Second language learning
in the classroom
Norris, J. M. and L. Ortega. 2000. 'Effectiveness of second language
instruction: A research synthesis and quantitative meta-analysis'. Language
Learning
50
I
3: 417 -528,
Pinter, A. 2006. Tbaching Young Language Learners. Oxford: Oxford
Universiry Press.
Snow, M. A. 2001. 'Content-based and immersion models for second and
foreign language teaching' in M. Celce-Murcia (ed.): Teaching English as a
Second
or
Foreign Language
7
3rd edn. Boston, MA: Heinle &
Heinle/Thomson Learnirg. pp. 303-1 8.
N. 1997. 'Form-focussed instruction and second language
acquisition: A review of classroom and laboratory researcti. Language
Teaching30l2:73-87 .
Spada,
B. (ed.). 2004. Processing Instruction: Theory, Research and
Commentary. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
VanPatten,
VanPatten, B.,J. \Williams, S. Rort, andM. Oversrreet (eds.). 2004. FormMeaning Connections in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
\7ong, W: 2005. Input Enhancement: From Theory and Research to the
C lass ro o m. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
In th
opinir
stron[
some
look z
imitar
instru
S€COIlt
you'vr
To co:
are
sc
learni
TL
Itisd
mainl
senter
ofi-lR
rs
evrd
'It
war
learne
him i
langur
memc
Thix
iearnir
-L!
---PJ.tt
tl
certalr
Tessrh
well
as
strateg
POPULAR IDEAS ABOUT
LANGUAGE LEARNING
REVISITED
In the Introduction, we presented a number of commonly expressed
opinions about how languages are learned. \7e asked you to indicate how
strongly you agreed with these opinions. Now that you have read about
some of the theory and research in second language acquisition, take another
look at those ideas. Have you changed your mind about the importance of
imitation or feedback on errors, or whether starting second language
instruction early is the best approach? Do you feel that your views about
second language acquisition have been changed or only confirmed by what
you've read in the preceding chapters?
To conclude this introducdon to second language acquisition research, here
are some of our own reflections on these popular ideas about language
learning.
1
Languages are learned mainly through
imitation
It is difficult to
are learned
m"ltlilthtqqgh imitaflqn. For one thing, learner. {'".'d''ie man}' noGl
sentences that thev could not have heard befbre. These sentences are based
of how the I
t in childrent sentences
as 'I'm hiccing up and I cant stop', and
'It was upside down but I turned it upside right', and with second language
learners who say'The cowboy rided into town', or'The man that I spoke to
him is angry. These examples and many others provide evidence that
language learners do not simply internalize a great list of imitated and
memorized sentences.
Thi
not mea
Iearning. Some children imitate
a
great deal as they
(eThot doloTimltaffirything
role to
in lan
acqqt. t!441qt lan€Uege,
they hear. Instead, ihey selectively imitate
certaln words or structures that they are rn the process of learnrng. lt rs also the
itation learn language as ouickllr alld as
wellas thoseFho i*it"t.
stratesv but it is not a uiTilffiil characteristic of lansuase learners.
earmns
tB4
Popular ideas about language learning reuisited
Like first language learners, second language learners produce many
sentences that they could not have heard. Some-may lqllIbj{lhglbg4qfit
from opportunities to imitate samples of the new-lt;zuase, and i,rrirarion is
clearly important in developing pronunciation and intonition. For some
;a"anGl learneri
pronunciation,
careful listening and imitation in a language laboratory can be veryvaluable.
But for beginning learners, the slavish imitation and rote memorization that
characterized audiolingual language approaches to language teaching can
lead to a dead end. Learners need to do more than recite bits of perfectly
accurate language. They learn as they make the effort needed to understand
and make themselves understood in genuinely meaningful interaction.
Otherwise, theymayhave acquired limle more than a collection ofsentences,
waiting for the moment when those sentences will be useful!
3L
la
The I
predic
fieta
;fi.4
ln nal
lansu:
varlet.
especl
metali
guage
nor
2
Parents usually correct young children uilten they
mahe grammatical errors
There is considerable variation in the extent to which parenrs correct their
's age-andchidi.n'r speech.
vaiiation is
on
on the oarerGGZiIllfi
und. tX/hen
chilclren are very young, parents rarely comment on grammatical errors,
Tl
altnougn thev mav correct lapses rn poltteness or the chorce of a word thar
iloesnt make sensc, Al ch-ildien reafh-tEliooi age, parenrs may corrc.t th.
kindi ofnon-standard speech that they hope their children will outgrow, for
example, 'Me and Fred are going outside now'.
f[t
Extensive observations of parents and children show that, as a r,rle, parent"
tend to focus on meaning rather than form when rhey correcr childrent
speech. Thus,Tflf
correct an incorrect word choice, an incorrect
staiE?nent of the facts, or a rude remark, but they do not often reactJo_effors_
b
sroun
catlon
choicr
enSag
envirc
47
d'r
Every,
who
<
highh
\We
kr
adul$
f<
langur
thatdonotinterferewithcommunication.ffichildren
cln
tive feedback in order to learn the basic
that
structure (the word order, the grammatical morphemes, the intonation
learnir
patterns) of their language. Fortunately, they appear to be able to acquire the
adult form of the language with little or no explicit feedback.
sryles
The case for second language learners is more complex. On the one hand,
both children and adults can acquire a great deal of language without any
formal instruction or feedback on error-On the orher haqd, the evidene
suggests that, without corrective feedback and guidance, ,..oil
age
ln qstlaTljg_ gng-ramma!]l1l ror4s ror years.
langul
c
Teach
secon(
ffi-r
learn
rvhich
motiv
studer
age,
--"-
1:
stuoer
motlv
---
18i
Popular ideas about language learning reuisited
3 Highly intelligentpeople are good
hngaage learners
The kind of intelligence that is measured
by(q- GDis
often a good
,
predictorofsuccessr,,classroo-swheretheemp@utularv rte
(ror exam
the lansuase
.,
s I .
*ho do wqll on IQ
in natural la
on
tests
settlnss
re ln
a
ers
researc
with
a wide
ofi
ltles can
ttue if the emphasis is on oral communication skills rather than
"$Ei"lly
metalinguistic knowledge. Most important, perhaps, is the fact that language learning involves a great variety of skills and abilities. S;udenushSgl{
not-be excluded from opportunities to learn another language on the
. In manyeoulc ablllty to
I"tion"l .ont.xts, students from immigrant or rru-nority groups have no
choice about learning a second language' \'X/hat is essendal is findingways to
engage the different kinds of ability that students bring to the learning
environment.
4
The bestpredictor ofsuccess in second language
acquisition is motiuation
Everyone aere€{that learners who want to learn tend to do better than those
.yho-a"q&"r
\=
,
example, that learners
ieve the Huencv and accuracv
a
tng.
secono language as
that chtldren do rn hrst
Effiisition'Thisshouldnotbetakenasevidencethatadultsecond
,-i-i-__'_12nguase learners are n,
?hilome with age or of other individual differences such as language
learning aptitude or how the instruction interacts with individual learners'
styles and preferences for learning.
-
Teachers have no infuence over learners' intrinsic motivation for learnirlg q.
s
rounds
ffiT-if.-.*periences, all of which have contributed to their motivation to
learn and attitudes toward the target language and the community with
which it is associated. The princ:Sal way that teachers can influence learners'
ruve envlfonment ln w
classroom a
motivation is by maki
ln
'€n
i@opriatetotheir
cultural
This in turn can contr
fiotivation, leading to still greater
success.
te to posrtrve
186
Popular ideas about language learning reuisited
5
The earlier a second language is introduced in
schoolprogrammes, the greater the lihelihood of
success
in learning
language
instruction must depend on the objectives of the language programme in the
particular social context of the school. 'when the objective is native-like
performance in the second language, then it may be desirable to begin
exposure to the language as early as possible. The research evidence is fairly
strong that those who begin second language learning at an early age arc
most likely to eventually be indistinguishable from native speakers.
However, even in cases where nativelike proficiency is targeted, it is important to recognize certain disadvantages ofan early start for second language
learning. \7hen an early srart means th"t cb4dEg h"* littleppport,tnity io
, the resulting subtractive bili
rsm ma
bas
On
The decision about when to introduce second or foreign
conttnue to
esp
intr
tlve conseouences. .bor chr
rom mlnofltvJ
aov
frur
Ti
6
Firs
con
fi
Ger
lear
sofi
On
,h9
know will have
more self-confidence, will be able to [ean: more
years, and will not lose valuable time in a
just to unders
at ls
tns ln
earlv school
e ctassroom,
For many children, there is no opportqnity to have their early schooling in
their first language. They are members of small minoriry groups where It is
not practical for schools to offer them an educational programme in their
- language, or they live in jurisdictions where legislatlon has mandated a
first
single language of education for all children, regardless of their background.
For these children, it is crucial to have sensitive educators who resfect the
children's difficulry who encourage parents to maintain the home language,
and who understand that second language learning takes time and effort.-
For foreign language instruction or for second language instruction where
the level of proficiency that is targeted is not native-like performance by all
students, the situation is quite different. rMhen the goal of the educational
programme is basic communicative skill for all students, and where there is a
strong commitment
to
mainraining and developing the childt first
lalguage, it can be more efficient to begin second language teaching later.
olds) are able to catch up quickly to
Qld*.Sb:ldren (for example, ren
those w
prglammgq o
only a rew
rnstructton.
Iear
lanE
freq
Arp
not
that
reE
not
out
take
that
Hol
leal
erfo
tes
learr
thar
Popular idras about language learning reuisited
t87
especially true if the foreign language course includes a period of more
intensive exposufe to the new language. All school programmes should be
based on realistic estimates of how long it takes to learn a second language.
a week-even
One or
frustration
ffi
as learners
I
uce
for seven or eieht vears-will not
to
fhis'drip-feed' approach
rs wttnout
havE
stuoyln
ts,
are sometimes right a
6 Most ofthe mistahes that second language
learners mahe are due to interferencefrom tlteir
firtt language
First, we should recognize that
languages are relatively close cousins (for example,
Germ
*r*^^^"--
ish and
ihere is much that
-- - f
learners already'knov/-including the alphabet, cognate w6Jds, as well
synrix.
some basic princi
o"6;;[=;;L""d,
thetra
l*".r
as
the nat
l""g""ge. \(hen egglqjr€-causgd-by
th. *rj." "n"*o**"e.iiorr i"
leirners' oerceotion ofsomer.'rrtial similarity bcnareen the first and second
learners are
lan
fr{Cfriltln contact with othlt lgqrytt *ho
fupects of the second language that are different
same errors.
froglbt
!
,h",--4Exjeil"r. Second language learning is not simply a process of putting
<
..
r
a
I
set5ndJanguage words into first-language sentences. In fact, learners may
not always be able to take advantage of similarities unless they are pointed
can be
out to them. \7e saw
assl{rl] eL mcogq ctll r o_nqetlmes'
Similarities because
take ad
tliat the lansuases must be different.
However, the first language is not the only influence on second language
often make the same kinds of
learning. Le-arngry!9191!@
similar to those
errors, and some of these errors are rem
Iffiage le"rnetr Itt tuch cases, second-language errors are evidence of the
l;;;GFoilr to discover the structure of the target language itself rather
than attempts to transfer patterns from their first language.
v
1BB
Popular ideas about langudge learning reuisited
7
The best uay to learn neu aocAbukry is
in th
through reading
orm
This statement is absolutely true. But
it
corr(
does nor tell the whole story.
dililr l
,ar"-ffi".;,
d ram
t i c al
a
ly
g
'e
r
yeafs,
r-
and reading is the major source of this growth. Secopd language learners can
also increase their vocab
e throu
rners will read the amount of tarse
reads throughout more itran a
ial that is-interesting and important
text that a child
1n
suggests that seco
t@
I lrom
rcn
evl
read
op good strategies for learning
and remembering words will benefit more than those who simply focus on
getting the main ideas from a rexr. -il/hat is perhaps most striking in the
research is the evidence that in order to successfully guess the meanings of
new words in a text, a reader usually needs to know 90 per cent or moie of
the words in that text.
8 It is essentialfor
learners to be able to pronounce
all the indiaidual sounds in the secon; language
R.:.lr.h l" p*gtation has shown that second languagespe* liry
,p@.tr
t@-the
t0
d.p.rrd*6
Secor
may
learn
atten
in
ac<
tion
i
learn,
do yc
doing
one
I
langu
restru
Some
"'. o..thet "6'iliE' r.p ro drr..
melody' of the language-iq,g
.h.i,
.4.tt_rtrdryrdud-so'n.J. nnotr,ei important
emffi"bili.y_lg "r.i.ul"tg_ rh.
fact that most languages of
""d*r.ble
the world are spoken in many different
varieties. Thus, it no longer seems
to pr
appropriate to insist that learners be taught only one language variery or that
practi
ro
o
d
only native speakers of a particular variery are rhe beit teachers. Rather,
learners need to learn to understand and produce language varieties that will
permit them to engage in communicative interaction wiih the interlocutors
tron t
lhrs(
langu
and
tl
unirl
norm:
they are most likely to encounter.
hearir
unit r.r
9
1I
Once learners hnout ,oughly 1,000 uor^ and the
basic structure of a second knguage, they can easily
participate in conaersAtions with natiue speahers
It is true that most conversational lan
i"yglf" only a relatively limited
number ofwords and sentenFF6dR
rt easler to
unoerstand ancl to make themselves
they also have
rstanclrng ot some of the pragmatrc features
new
fr
so6EiifrE-iiGT[ffor them to focus their
ait@oro-
speakers show respect, apologize, or make requesrs. The cultural dlfferences
I
Rsel
certair
necess
Percelr
-ffi;
Ieaturc
Popular ideas about language learning
reaisited
in these types of interactions sometimes lead to communication breakdown
or misunderstandings, even when the words and the sentence structures are
correct.
10
Tbachers should present
grammatical rules one
at a time, and learners shouldpractise examples
of each one before going on to another
Second lan
learnin
I-ea
tner",
ting that they have
that form), fail to
the form (or make errors when they
attempt i,)g r,3g94
The decline
inacnracy at stageymay show that learners are incorporating new information about the language into their interlanguage. \7e saw, for example, how
learners may ask correct formulaic questions such as '\Zhatt that?', or'How
do you say procheinEnglish?', and then produce questions like qil/hat you're
doing with that?' at a later time. Language development is not just adding
one rule after another. Rather, it involves processes of integrating new
language forms and patterns inro an existing interlanguage, readjusting and
restructuring until all the pieces fit.
Some st
tlon
are based on the false assum
second
tlsasorto
on or ru
can be seen in the organization oftex
language feature in the first unit and reinforce it in several subsequint units,
and then mor'e on the next feature, with only rare opporrunities for learners
rs
to practise the ones previously taught. This isolated presentation and
practice of one sffucture at a time does not provide learnei, with an opportunity to discover how different language features compare and contrast in
normal language use. It is also likely that, without opportunities to continue
hearing, seeing, and using them, the language feaiures learned in the first
unit will have been forgotten long before the last.
11
Teaclters should teaclt simple knguage structures
before complex ones
that no matter how lan
is presented to learners,
certain structures are acquired before others.
is suggests thatl!_t!
"g are
n6c€s&Lqr nor desirable to restricllearners'
to structures that
perceived in linguisti- terms
n tnls lnvo
isolated-pffiTltion, orderi
practrce of slmple to com
-,------i--
189
190
Popular ideas about language learning reuisited
At the same time, there is no doubt that second language learners benefit
from the efforts of native speakers and fuent bilin[ual to modify their
speech to help them understand. The language used in modified interacion
may contain a variety of linguistic structures, some 'simple' and some
'complex'. However, it also includes a range of adjustments that enable
second language learners to engage in interactions with native and more
advanced speakers of the second language more easily-more repetition,
Immr
emba
othert
persls
13
slower rate of delive ry, paraphrasing, etc.
Teachers must
in' classroom la
forms are so rare
to
use, and
that learners ha
ifthe
int of providi tnem.
not necessarily difficult or complex forms. As we saw in
r 6 (Study
31) some common language forms turn out to be extremely rare in
learn them
classroom language.
Such
or wrl
maste
This
I
cuenc(
#
as soon as
!umcl(
tltey are made in order to preaent theformation
of bad habits
a
a
------=
meanl
,E"t
12 Learners'ercors should be corrected
Errors
I
ofthe development
a childt first lang'nge as well as of second language learning by children
is true
"-ilffi
rcr lanl
zuidan
-\posu
.Fhen
r
-;els th,
systemf-showing where
or where
tter-n to the
":fi'fi
-.f+ngs
:raterla
Teachers have a
tne Drovrslon
n errors are perslstent,
students rn a class, lt lS lfllpr
to hel learners do their best, and this includes
lnstructlc)n
on error.
e@
form or structure immediately or consistentty.
f
many trmes.
Excessive feedback
ive effect on motivation. of
course,
a
to
their
reactlons to
coirectlon. The amount and rype of correction that is offeredii
il ;;;; ;.'.j;ffi;#:':;
",ii
:ffi.:il'::
:enainl
::laStere
the error is based on a
developmental pattern, the instruction or feedback may be useful onlywhen
the learner is ready for it. It may be necessary to ,.p."t feedback on error
;#ffi',;
--a-rners
;T
J'
;:11 1"'
relationship with the teacher and with each other. Children and adults with
little education in their first lan
will not benifii
atly from
tl
nrversrty students
adv
learners of the lan
ma find
explanations ofgreat
14u
(t
c(
Popular ideas about language learning reuisited
to
errors
in an oral
nlcatlon
some studenG
se
m
ile for
helo them notlce a
ng.
ion is exactlv what
persistent error at iust the moment when it occurs.
13
Teacbers should use materials
that expose
students onb to hnguage structures tltey haue
already been taught
Such a
can provide
meanl
ensible i
t ofcou
ne
or wntten texts t
Jhus, restrictin classroom second
materials to t
(*-=.-*7..''
tnat contaln trttt. o, ,r1fr that is new m
quences. I here wlll undouQted.ly be 4 loss of motivation if students aie not
#,..:ry
t@
Students also need to develop strategies for diiling
with 'real' or 'aurhentic' material if they are eventually goingto b. prep"r.J
for language use outside the classroom. They do this first with the teaiher's
guidance and then independently. Restricti
exposure to the
extends their
'mastered'
\iMhen a particular form is introduced for the first time, or when the teacher
feels there is a need for!olI-=.,tp" of a persistent problem, it iFippr6p*riaiJto
t ln a context
things.seern easy, But it would be a* s
ts to use
.--!- :-i'
-r-s:_.----materials
exc_lu-ively 9r jIgn_ppdglsinqntly. \7e
remember that
lefun6rs who succesrfully
a second'language outside classrooms
".q,rir.
certainly are exposed to a great
variery of forms and structures they have not
mastered.
14
Wlten learners are allouted to interactfreely
(for example, in group or pair actiuities), tbey
cap! each otlter's mistahes
If the activi4gs lle ryellJlqsjgqed aryl learners al_e
1p__prop_1i1telymatch,e1lp4r
and group work provides far more practice in speakins and particioatins in
conversatlons
mewhat surprisifi-fly, research has shown that lg,allersdo-nor+Joduce3ny more errors in
their speech when talking to learners at similar levels of proficien
or to natrYe
research also shows, however, that
r91
192
Popular idras about language learning reuisited
way that learners working together can discover how to express or interpret
meaning in the second language. In order for this to happen, the tasks must
be carefully planned to give learners access to new language they need.
Group and pair wqfhis a valuable addit&n to the
of activi
nt.
e
Used
comtination with individual work and teacher-centred activities, it
rmportant
lng
nlng.
Res
qrai
thrs
rnt€
CVCI
tF
Inc
mut
15 Students learn what tltey are taught
feed
Gachers furorlfrom experience that students dont learn everything they are
hear
ht directlv.
teaching methods typically give
t opportfiry to learn only a restricted number of words and
sentence types. Even when the language teaching method provides much
richer language input, the fact that something is taught or made available in
the input does not mean learners will acquire it right away. For example,
has
imn
thes
more tnan
some aspects of the second language emerge and evolve according to 'natural'
sequences of development and learners may be more likely to learn certain
language features when they are developmentally 'ready'.Thuq ?ttempts to
teach aspects oflanguage that are too far away from the learnert currenr srase
ot development wlll usua.lly be frustratrng.
*her *.@mple,
vocabulary, can be taught
at any time, as long as the learners are interested in the opportunity to learn
and the teaching methods are appropriate to the learner's age, interesrs,
needs, experiences, and learning sryles. p6-nul@yJgrners can learn a grear
deal that no one ever teaches them. Thdy qre able to use their own lnlglrEJ
[earning mechanisms to discover many of the
rules and relationrnifrfi-Ifr-this senG, students
leilrnmuch more than t
16
Tbachers sltould respond to stufunts' errors by
correctb rephrasing uthat they haue said ratlter
than by explicitly pointing out tlte ercor
This kind of feedback, referred to a
has been foua{g' be by far-rhe
most common
. This has
been
to be true for learners at different ages and in different
instructional models-from audiolingual to communicative and contentbased instruction. It has the advantage of not interrupting the flow of
interactioq. It ir r..
.--information they need without embarrassing them.
ffidEGTE-.
expr
sayli
I7
The
lncr(
.-:-
rnhe
;oG
,kt
.losln
"nI
subjr
langr
Reser
classt
tive
r
foun,
clifr
ln
cei
rangr
exam
accul
studi
contc
Popular ideas about language
Research
with adult
learners,
classes
with
a
that
learningreuisited 193
general focus on
are
th-is kind of feedback. Research in which learners in
-ifrterlocutors has also shown?hat
may not
exactly which
tiie
on.
In content-based instruction (for example, immersion classes) and in communicative instruction with younger learners, more explicit forms of
feedback have been found to be more effective in getting learners to respond
immediately. Recasts often
to be misin
Learners seem to
hear them as cohffiati
n as cofrectlon
3i-tuations, recasts have been found to be moreEffiEiiGl
teacher
has a method of signalling to the student-rone of voice, gesture, or facial
'I think I
expression-that says to rhe student,
understand what you are
sayrng, and I'm telling you how you can say it better'.
17
Studcnts can learn both language and academic
content (for txample, science and history)
simubaneousb in cksses ultere the subject matter
is taught in their second hnguage
The advantages of content-based instruction are numerous. Motrvation ist
^-----
inh
i6-learn the
. Co-nEn r-based
I
i
nrcrilrio-ffi
to spend d6iFiimeln
lghgut
out on ln
ect matter. The ranse
of vocabularv
a!
uage sffucture that students encounter in learning academic
subjects is more varied than that which is rypically available in foreign
language classes.
Research has confirmed
that students in content-based and immersion
classes develop comprehension skills, vocabulary, and general communica-
tive competence in the new language. Teachers and researchers have also
hs\a'tygSlbC[he ab iliry tJun-ders taffi
tr.,Oi i.t
fo und,
.1"
i
especially in areas ofaccuracy on
that do not usually interfere with meaning. Thus, for
example, students can spend years in French immersion without achieving
accuracy in marking nouns for gender or verbs for tense. Experimental
studies in which an elemenr of form-focused instruction was added to the
content-based instruction have shown that, with guidance, students can
l-anguage fbatures
r94
Popu
lar iden
ab
out language learning reuisited
improve in these areas as well. Both students and teachers need to keep in
mind that content-based language teaching is also language teaching.
GI
Conclusion
Knowing more about second language acquisition research will not tell you
what to do in your classroom tomorrow morning. -We hope, however, thar
this book has provided you with information that encourages you to reflest
on your experience in teaching. \7e hope, in addition, that this refection will
contribute to a better understanding ofyour responsibilities as a teacher and
Theg
langu
are
in
not
ir
a
dict
those ofyour students as language learners.
ACCU
As we have seen, language learning is affected by many factors. Among these
are the personal characteristics and experiences ofthe learner, the social and
langu
cultural environment both inside and outside the classroom, the srructure of
the native and target languages, opportunities for interaction with speakers
of the target language, and access to correction and form-focused instruction. It is clear that teachers do not have control over all these factors.
Nevertheless, a befter understanding of them will permit teachers and
learners to make the most of the time they spend together in the twin
processes ofteaching and learning a second language.
order
than
i
ACTI(
classrr
questi
.{CTI\
but al,
ADDI:
6rst.
.{MER
North
langu:
rhroul
NUDI(
reachi
structl
ilstrur
repetil
each o
.{UDIT
tbr exa
BEHA\
ilon-Y(
1.
tnls vlt
surrou
tormat
GLOSSARY
The glossarycontains items thathave aspecial or technical meaningin second
language acquisition research and second language teaching. The definitions
are intended to refect the terms as we use theminthis book. As a rule, we have
not included words for which appropriate definitions can readily be found in
a
dictionary.
ACCURACv oRDER: The relative acs)racy of grammatical forms in learner
language. For example, learners are often more accurate in using plural -s
than in using possessive 's. Some researchers have inferred that an accuracy
order is equivalent to a developmental sequence.
ACTIoN REsEARCH: Research carried out by teachers, often in their own
classrooms or in collaboration with other teachers. The research goals and
questions are local and specific to their own teaching environment.
ACTIVE LrsrENrNG: A teaching technique in which students not only listen
but also show their comprehension by their responses.
ADDITIVE BrLrNGUALrsru: Learning a second language without losing the
first.
AMERIcAN sIGN LANGUacB (ASL): The gestural language used by many
North Americans who are deaf or who interact with deaf persons. It is a true
language, with complex rules of structure and a rich vocabulary, all expressed
through motions of the hands and body.
AUDIoLINGUAL AppRoecH: An approach to second or foreign language
teaching that is based on the behaviourist theory of learning and on
structural linguistics, especially the contrastive analysis hypothesis. This
instructional approach emphasizes the formation of habits through the
repetition, practice, and memorizationof sentence patterns in isolation from
each other and from contexts of meaningful use.
AUDIToRv DIscRIMINATIoN: The ability to distinguish language sounds,
for example minimal pairs such as 'ship/sheep'.
BEHAVrouRrsu: A psychological theory that all learning, whether verbal or
non-verbal, takes place through the establishment of habits. According to
this view, when learners imitate and repeat the language they hear in their
surrounding environment and are positively reinforced for doing so, habit
formation (or learning) occurs.
196
Glossary
BTLTNGUAL EDUcATIoN: Schooling in which students receive instruction
in
COMP
two (or more) languages, usually their home language and a second language.
to lanE
BrLrNGUALrsvr: The ability to use more than one language. The word itself
does not specify the degree of proficiency in either language.
becaus
cHrLD-DrREcrED spEECH: The languag€ that caretakers address to
children. In some cases, this language is simpler than that which is addressed
to adults. In some cultures, it is also slower, higher pitched, more repetitive,
and includes a large number of questions.
COMP,
seconc
or writ
(1e85)
COMP]
cHUNK: A unit of language that is often perceived or used as a single unit.
Chunks include formulaic expressions such as 'thank you or 'Hi, how are
you?'but also bits of language that frequently occur together, for example,
'ice cream cone' or 'bread and butter'.
variety
on col
cLASSRooM oBsERVATToN scHEME: A tool (often in the form of a grid)
that consists of a set of predetermined categories used to record and describe
teaching and learning behaviours.
complt
encoul
more I
other.
cocNATE: A word in one language that resembles a word in another
language and has the same meaning, for example, 'nation' and nation in
English and French or uacaand uache (cow) in Spanish and French.
CONTI
cocNrrrvrst: A
research approach that emphasizes how the human mind
receives, processes, stores, and retrieves information in learning and retrieving
information. The focus is on internal learning mechanisms that are believed to
be used for learning in general, not just language learning alone .
cocNITrvE MATURTTv: The abiliry to engage in problem-solving, deduction, and complex memory tasks.
coMMUNICATIVE coMIETENcn: The ability to use language in a variety of
settings, taking into account relationships between speakers and differences
in situations. The term has sometimes been interpreted as the ability to
convey messages in spite of a lack of grammatical accuracy.
Respor
CONN]
lessons
exampJ
matics,
CONTF
learner
similar
CONTR
from tl
researcl
that tht
the exp
CORRE
coMMUNrcATrvE LANGUAGE TEACHTNG (CLT): CLT is based on the
premise that successful language learning involves not only a knowledge of
the structures and forms of a language, but also the functions and purposes
that a language serves in different communicative settings. This approach to
teaching emphasizes the communication of meaning in interaction rather
than the practice and manipulation of grammatical forms in isolation.
coMIETENCT: Linguist Noam Chomsky used this term to refer to knowledge of language. This is contrasted with performance, which is the way a
person actually uses language-whether for speaking, listening, reading, or
writing. Because we cannot observe competence directly, we have to infer its
nature from performance.
*re tarl
exampl
"goes",
andma
forget
t
CORRE-
or
size
relation
increase
highest
homew
Glossary
coMpREHENSIBLE INpuT: A term introduced by Stephen Krashen to refer
to language that a learner can understand. It may be comprehensible in part
because of gestures, situations, or prior information.
COMpREHENsIBLE OUTpUT HypOTHESIs: The hypothesis that successful
second language acquisition depends on learners producing language (oral
or written). Swain (1985) proposed this hypothesis in response to Krashens
(1985) comprehensible input hypothesis.
coMIREHENSIoN-BASED INSTRUCTIoN: A general term to describe a
variety of second language programmes in which the focus of instruction is
on comprehension rarher than production (for example, Total Physical
Response).
coNNECTIoNISM: A theory of knowledge (including language) as a
complex system of units that become interconnected in the mind as they are
encountered together. The more often units are heard or seen together, the
more likely it is that the presence of one will lead to the activation of the
other.
coNTENT-BASED INSTRUCTIoN: Second language programmes in which
lessons are organized around subject matter rather than language points. For
example, in immersion programmes students study science, history, mathematics, etc. in their second language.
CONTRASTIVE ANALYSIS HYPoTHESIS
(CAH): The expectation that
have less difficulry acquiring target language patterns that are
similar to those of the first language than those that are different.
learners
will
coNTRoL GRoup: In experimental studies, a group of learners that differs
from the experimental group only in terms of the single variable that the
researcher is investigating. Performance of the control group is used to show
that the variable in question is the best (or only) explanation for changes in
the experimental group. Also called'comparison group'.
coRRECTIVE FEEDBACK: An indication to a learner that his or her use of
the target language is incorrect. Corrective feedback can be explicit (for
example, in response to the learner error 'He ge'-'No, you should say
"goest, not "go"') or implicit (for example, 'Yes, he goes to school every day),
and may or may not include metalinguistic information (for example, 'Don't
forget to make the verb agree with the subject').
CoRRELATIoN: A statistical procedure that compares the relative frequency
or size of different variables in order to determine whether there is
a
relationship between them. In a positive correlation, both variables tend to
increase or decrease in a similar pattern. For example, if the students with the
highest grades in French also spend the greatest number of hours doing their
homework, this would be a positive correlation, suggesting that as one
197
198
Glossary
variable increases, the other does as well. Howeve! it does not prove that one
of the variables caused the other. In a negative correlation, one variable
increases as the other decreases. For example, lower scores in a speaking task
may be associated with higher levels of anxiery.
CRTTICAL pERIoD HYPoTHESIs (CPH): The proposal that there is a
limited period during which language acquisition can occur. The strong
version of the CPH is that there are biological mechanisms specifically
designed for language acquisition and that these cease to be available at or
even before puberry. Thus an older learner has to use general learning
mechanisms that are not designed for-and thus not as effective forlanguage acquisition. The weak version (sometimes called the 'sensitive
period hypothesis') is that, erren though the same learning mechanisms are
involved, second language learning will be more difficult for older learners.
cRoss-sECTToNAL
sruDy: A study in which participants at different
ages
and/or stages of development are studied. Inferences about sequences that
would apply to the development of individual learners are sometimes drawn
from cross-sectional studies. This contrasts with longitudinal studies.
DEcLARATIVE KNov'LEDcs: Information that we have and know we have.
An example would be a rule such as 'the verb must agree with the subject to
form a correct sentence'. In some skill learning theories, it has been hypothesized that all learning begins with declarative knowledge. It is sometimes
referred to as 'knowledge that'. Contrast with procedural knowledge.
DEscRrprrvE sruDy: Research that does not involve any manipulation,
change, or intervention in the phenomenon being studied. The researcher's
goal is to observe and record what is happening. This contrasts with
experimental study.
DEVELoIMENTAL ERRor: An error in learner language that does not result
from first language influence but rather reflects the learner's gradual discoyery of the second language system. These errors are often similar to those
made by children learning the language as their mother tongue.
ENHr
featu
explir
of un
ETH}
unde
reseal
with
1
EXPE
impa,
exPer
differ
intere
tions
this b
whicl
educa
FIELf
to des
Thati
hidde
more
parts.
TIRST
first le
mayb
FORE]
speakr
taik is
FORE]
usuall'
ENTAL F EATU ns s : Those aspects of a language which, according
to Pienemann and his colleagues, develop in a particular sequence, regardless
s.idell
of input variation, learner motiyation, or instructional intervention.
is som
D EVE
Lo
pM
DEVELoIMENTAL sEeuENcE: The order in which certain features of a
language (for example, negation) are acquired in language learning' Also
called developmental stages.
DrspLAy euEsrloN: A question to which the asker already knows the
answer. Teachers often ask these questions (for example, '\(/hat colour is
your shirt?') to get the learner to display his or her knowledge of the
language.
being
Floren
FORM.
tbrms
intera<
highli{
Glossary
ENHANcED rNpur: Input that is altered in an effort to make some language
features more salient to learners. It can be more or less explicit, ranging from
explicit metalinguistic comments to typographical enhancement (bold type
or underlining) or exaggerated stress in speaking.
ETHNoGRApsv: Descriptive research in which the observer seeks to
understand a group or community from within its own perspective. The
research requires extensive periods of observation as well as consultation
with group members to validate the observer's descriptions.
sruDy: Research designed to test a hypothesis about the
impact of one or more very specific variables on another variable. A strictly
experimental study would have 'experimental' and 'control' groups that
differ from each other only in the presence or absence of the variable(s) of
interest. In educational research, it is often difficult to create all ofthe conditions that permit a study to be termed as a 'genuine' experimental study. In
this book, the term is used in a non-technical sense to refer to research in
which an attempt has been made to investigate a single variable in an
EXIERTMENTAL
educational setting. See also quasi-experimental.
FrELD TNDEpENDENI/rIBrr DEpENDENT: This distinction has been used
to describe people who differ in their tendency to see the forest or the trees.
That is, some people (called field independent) are very quick to pick out the
hidden figures in a complicated drawing. Others (called field dependent) are
more inclined to see the whole drawing and have difficulry separating it into
parts.
FrRsr LANGuece (uorHER ToNGUE, NATTvE reNcuecr): The language
first learned. Many children learn more than one language from birth and
may be said to have more than one 'first'language.
FoREIGNER rarr<: The modified or simplified language that some native
speakers address to second language learners. A special category offoreigner
talk is teacher talk.
FoREIGN LANGUAGE LEARNING: This refers to the learning of a language,
usually in a classroom setting, in a context where the target language is not
widely used in the communiry (for example, learning French in China). This
is sometimes contrasted with 'second language learning', where the language
being learned is used in the community (for example, learning Italian in
Florence).
FoRM-FocusED TNSTRUCTIoN: Instruction that draws attention to the
forms and structures of the language within the context of communicative
interaction. This may be done by giving metalinguistic information, simply
highlighting the form in question, or by providing corrective feedback.
r99
200
Glossary
FORMULATC LANGUAGE: Expressions
or phrases that are often perceived
and learned as unanalysed wholes. For example, a child or secondlanguage
learner may first hear '\whatb that?' as a single unit of language ratheruh"nt
three units.
expefl
auton
INNA:
are de
FossrlrzATroN: This term is used to describe
persistent lack of change in
interlanguage patterns, even after extended exposure to or instruction in the
target language.
a
FUNCTToN !?'oRDS: t$7ords that are used mainly as linking or supporting
words for nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. For example, prepositioni
('to', 'for', 'by') and articles ('d, 'the') are two rypes of function woids. They
have little or no meaning when they occur alone, but they have an important
effect on the meanings of the words they accompany.
cENUTNE euESTroN: A question to which the asker does not know the
answer in advance (for example, 'v{hat did you do last weekend?'). Also called
'referential' or 'information' questions. contrasts with 'display question'.
GRAMMAR TRANSLATTON: An approach to second language teaching
characterized by the explicit teaching of grammar rules and th. ,rr. of transl
lation exercises.
INPU:
spoke
INSTF
as
the
INTE(
is basr
target
group
INTE}
instru
old
Fr
Queb,
instru
Most
time i
r
GRAMMATTCAL MORPHEMES: Morphemes
are the smallest units of
l1gylg._ that carry meaning. A simple word is a morpheme (for example,
'book), but when we talk about 'grammatical morphemes' we are usually
referring to smaller units that are added to words to
their meaning (for
"lt.rwords (for
the -s in'books' indicates plural) or function
exairple,
91ample,
'the') which are ordinarily attached to another word.
GRAMMATTCALTTY JUDGEMENT: A test or task in which participants are
asked to make a decision about whether a sentence is correct (or appropriate)
or not.
HyporHEsrs: A statement of a possible fact that can be tested through
perioc
domir
progr€
instru
iNTEF
is base
conver
rheir i
ments
research. Most empirical research starts from one or more hypoth.res
"id
involves the design of a study that can either show support for the hypothesis
languz
or disprove it.
may h
IMMERSIoN pRocRAMMn: An educational programme in which a second
language is taught via contenr-based instruction. That is, students study
subjects such as mathematics and social studies in their second language.
Typically, students in immersion programmes share the same fi^t la.rguag..
secon(
INTER
rend
t
S\TStCIT
input
r
TNFoRMATTON pROCESSTuc: A psychological theory that uses a computer
INTER
metaphor for the human brain. It includes the idea that the brain has a
very large capacity to store information for the long term, but a more
limited capacity for information that requires our attention. \x/ith repeated
ri'ith
LANGI
I
Stephr
Glossary
experience and practice, things which at first required attention become
automatic, leaving more attention available for focus on something else.
TNNATTsM: A theory that human beings are born with mental sffuctures
are designed specifically for the acquisition of language.
that
INpur: The language that the learner is exposed to (either written or
spoken) in the environment.
ENTAL M STIVATT o N : Motivation that is essentially practical, such
as the need to learn the language in order to get a better job.
r
N s T RUM
Motivation for second language learning that
is based on a desire to know more about the culture and community of the
target language group and even a desire to be more like members of that
TNTEGRATTvE MoTIVATIoN:
group.
rNTENsrvE ESL: In this book, 'intensive' ESL is used to refer to an
instructional approach that we have observed in Quebec where l0 -12 yearold French-speaking students learn English as a second language' Most
Quebec students in this age group have only an hour or t\vo of ESL
instruction each week. 'Intensive ESL classes provide much more time.
Most of the classes observed in our research set aside one five-month block of
time in one school year and devote full days to ESL instruction during that
period. The pedagogical approach we observed in these classes was predominantly communicative language teaching. In contrast to immersion
programmes, intensive ESL classes do not usually include content-based
instruction.
sr HyporHEs
: The hypothesis that language acquisition
is based both on learners' innate abilities and on opportunities to engage in
conversations, often those in which other speakers modify their speech and
their interaction patterns to match the learners' communication requirements. The innate abilities are not seen as being specific to language or
language acquisition.
TNTERACTT o Nr
Is
INTERLANGUAGE: A learner's developing second language knowledge. It
may have characteristics of the learnert first language, characteristics of the
second language, and some characteristics that seem to be very general and
tend to occur in all or most interlanguage systems' Interlanguages are
systematic, but they are also dynamic. They change as learners receive more
input and revise their hypotheses about the second language'
rNTERLocuron: A participant in a conversation.
This term is most often used interchangeably
with language learning. However, for some researchers, most notably
Stephen Krashen, acquisition is contrasted with learning' According to
LANGUAGE ACeuISITIor.r:
201
202
Glossary
IGashen, acquisition represents 'unconscious' learning, which takes place
when attention is focused on meaning rather than language form.
NEGC
In this book, this term is a general one, referring
simply to an individual's developing knowledge of th. t"rg.t language. Ii
stephen Krashen's terms, however, 'learning' is contrasted *ith '"cquirition,
and is described as a 'conscious' process that occurs when the learnert
objective is to learn about the language itself, rather than to understand
in cor
messages conveyed through the language.
used
LANGUAGE LEARNTNG:
LoNGrruDrNAL sruDy: A study in which the same learners
period of time. This contrasts with a cross-sectional study.
are studied over
adjusr
OBLI(
matic
senter
obligr
I
ORDI
a
OVER
MEANTNG-BASED TNSTRUCTTON: See communicative language teaching.
use a
METALrNGUrsrrc AVARENESS: The ability ro rreat language as an object, for
example, being able to define a word, or to saywhar ro.tndr make up that word.
regulr
MITTcATToN: Softening. In pragmatics, a phrase or tone of voice to reduce
the possible negative impact ofwhat is said.
MoDTFTED rNpur: Adapted speech that adults use to address children and
native speakers use to address language learners so that they will be able to
understand. Examples of modified input include shorrer, simpler sentences,
and basic vocabulary.
PATT.
asked
Typi,
PERF
writir
knorn
to
var
the
MODTFTED TNTERACTTON: Adapted conversation patterns that proficient
speakers use in addressing language learners so that the learner *ill^be able to
understand.
_Examples
of
interactional modifications include compre-
n
PRIV
not
e
hension checks, clarifi cation requests, and self-repetitions.
PRO(
Mo RIHEME : See grammatical morphemes.
perfo
decla
NATIvE-LrKB: The abiliry to comprehend and produce a second language at
a level of performance that is indistinguishablehom that of a ,r"tirr. ip.ik.r.
NATIVE spEAKER: A person who has learned a language from an early age
and who has full mastery of that language. Native spiakers may differ In
terms of vocabulary and srylistic aspects of language use, but they tend to
agree on the ttasic grammar of the language. The notion 'native speaker'
must always be understood within a specific geographic region or socioeconomic group because there is wide variation among 'native speakers' of
most languages.
PRO(
are g)
their
sente
itself
QUA]
the p
amot
exter
NATURAL oRDER: See developmental sequence.
NEGorrATroN oF FORM: An interaction in which language learners work
toward the correct form in a context where meaning is understood. If a
teacher is involved in the interaction, he or she seeks to guide students to find
the right form instead of providing it for them.
QUA]
num
varia
or
'c
meas
Glossary
NEGorrATroN oF MEANING: Interaction between speakers who make
adjustments to their speech and use other techniques to repair a breakdown
in communication. See also modified interaction.
oBLrcAToRy coNTExrs: Places in a sentence where a particular grammatical form is required if the sentence is to be correct. For example, in the
sentence 'Last week, my brother rent a car', the speaker has created an
obligatory context for the past tense by the use of 'Last week', but has not
used the required form ofthe verb in that context.
oRDER oF ACQUISITIoN: See developmental sequence.
ovERGENERALIZATIoN ERRoR: This type of error is the result of trying to
use a rule in a context where it does not belong, for example, putting a
regular -edendingon an irregular verb, as in 'buyed' instead of 'bought'.
A teaching technique in which learners are
to practise sentences chosen to represent particular linguistic forms.
IATTERN IRACTTcE DRrLL:
asked
Typical of the audiolingual approach.
pERFoRMANcs: The way we use language in listening, speaking, reading,
writing. Performance is usually contrasted with competence, which is the
knowledge that underlies our abiliry to use language. Performance is subject
to variations due to inattention or fatigue whereas competence, at least for
the mature native speaker, is more stable.
IRIVATE spEEcH: The language we use when we are talking to ourselves,
not expecting anyone to hear or respond.
pROCEDURAL KNow'LEDcB: Knowledge that underlies
performance. Also referred
to
as 'knowledge how',
fuent or automatic
it is contrasted with
declarative knowledge.
pRocEssING INSTRUCTIoN: An approach to instruction in which learners
are given explicit information about the language feature to be learned and
their practice activities involve the comprehension (not production) of
understoodwithout a focus on the language
itself. The approach was developed by Bill VanPatten.
sentences or texts that cannot be
euALITATIVE REsEARCH: An approach that uses detailed descriptions of
the phenomena being studied rather than counting or measuring the exact
amount of some specific variable or variables. Qualitative research requires
extensive observation and insightfu I interpretation.
quANTTTATIVE RESEARcH: An approach that requires precise counts or
numeric measurements of variables. In a quantitative study, both the
variable that is believed to affect learning and the learning itself are measured
or 'quantified'. Quantitative research requires careful selection of the
measurements that will be used to represent the variables being studied.
203
204
Glossary
RATE oF DEVELoIMENT:
language development.
RECAST:
The speed at which learners progress in their
SUBS
sente
To repeat a learnert incorrect utrerance, making
changes that
conyeft it to
a correct phrase or sentence. 'Recast'is
also used a, a
read
noun, rhat
SUBT
is, a recasr is the modified/corrected form
of the learnert utterance.
REGISTER: A swle
gr
*?
for a particular setting.
of using language.that
io, o-;;'t;,^3f."r.i"g
different registers; the register ur.d in *ritirg""
from that used writing a l-etter to a friend.
r
langu
typical of or appropriate
SUPR
and writing usually require
rhyth
is
,.r."J
ripo6 i, airr.r..r,
TARG
langu
S.AFFOLDTNC: The language_that an interlocutor
uses to support the
communicative success of another speaker. It
may include tlr. prorririo., of
missing vocabulary or the expanrio' of the
,p."k.rt i".;;p;;;.:;;;;;;. "'
TASK-
SECOND LANGUAGE:
langur
newsp
'tasks'
In this book, the term refers ro any language
other
than the first language rearned. Thus, it
may actuall y refer to the third or
fourth
train
language.
TEACI
SEGMENTALs: The individuar sounds
of a ranguage. contrasted with ,supra_
segmentals', which are patterns of intonation.
TRANI
secon(
srGNrFr.ANT DTFFERENc.: This
is a technical term that refers to
differences
according ,o ,,,"ri.ry of statistical ,*,s,
unfikety to
|;.:e;1^*:::g Tl,:i,
chance. Such differences can be small ";;
or large. Their
lil:,111p^.^l1d,,b),
stgnthcance' is due to the consistency
of the differences as well th.ir rir..
ence'i
ways i
ledge
hypotl
term is
go yesterday').
UPTAK
socrocuLTURAL THEonv: An explanation
for^knowledge and learning
that is based on the assumption that alr
learning is first social then individuar.
Learning is viewed as a process that is
sociaily mediated, that is, it is
dependent on dialogue in iace-to-face interaction.
The claim is that during
communication, learners jointry constru*
knowledge *ii.r, Ji",ernarized
by the individual.
STRUCTURAL GRADTNG:
A technique for organizing or sequencing material
in a texrbook or lessons..The basis fo, rh.
orginir"ri#
i, , girdu"l i'r,.r."r. in
a
complexiry of grammarical features.
a
UNIVE
",
SIMILIFICATToN: Leaving out elements
of a sentence, for example, using
the same form of a verb relardl.r, of p.rror,
number, tense (,I go today. He
STANDARD vARrETy: Thevariety
of a given language that is typicaily used in
formal writing and formal pubir. rp."rH"g (including
broadcasting). The
standard variery of widely spoken l"ngurg'.,
may be different in different
pl":.-r. For example, American English] Br'itish
E'glrrh, i";;il;
English.
and Indian English each has it, o#.r siandard
*.iilr*rr.,-.ro,r,
ethnic, regional, and socioeconomic varieties. ""riEry, ",
t
notices
Ranta's
to corf(
VARIAB
Variabl
individr
languag
learninp
measurc
VARIAT]
iramew<
:
for
exar
any poir
\NRIETl
regional,
Glossary
suBSTITUTIoN DRILL: A teaching technique in which learners practise
sentences, changing one element at a time, for example, 'I read a book';
read a newspaper'; 'I read a story'. Typical of the audiolingual approach.
suBTRACTIVE BILINGUALIsM: Partially
'I
or completely losing the first
language as a second language is acquired.
suIRASEGMENTALs: The sounds of a language that involve the melody and
rhythm of the language, rather than the pronunciation of individual sounds.
TARGET LANGUAGE: The language being learned, whether
language or a second (or third or fourth) language.
it
is the first
TAsK-BASED INSTRUcTIoN: Instruction in which classroom activities are
'tasks'similar to those learners might engage in outside the second or foreign
language classroom. Thsks may be complex, for example, creating a school
newspaper, or more limited, for example, making a phone call to reserve a
train ticket.
TEAcHER
tarr:
See
modified input and foreigner talk.
TRANsFER: The infuence of a learner's first language knowledge in the
second language. Also called 'interference'. The term 'first language infuence' is now preferred by many researchers. It better reflects the complex
ways in which knowledge of the first language may afFect learners' knowledge and use ofa second language.
uNrvERsAL GRAMMAR (UG): Innate linguistic knowledge which, it is
hypothesized, consists of a set of principles common to all languages. This
term is associated with Chomsky's theory of language acquisition.
uprAKE: This term is sometimes used generally to refer to what a learner
notices and/or retains in second language input or instruction. Lyster and
Ranta's (L997) definition refers to a learner's observable immediate response
to corrective feedback on his/her utterances.
An element or characteristic that can be measured or defined.
within a group or
individual. Some examples of variables that are commonly examined in
vARIABLE:
Variables can differ in different groups or change over time
language acquisition research include the amount of time a person has been
learning the language, scores on aptitude tests, and performance on
measures of language knowledge.
vARIATIoNAL FEATURES: In contrast to the developmental features in the
framework developed by Pienemann and his colleagues, variational features
(for example, vocabulary, some grammatical morphemes) can be learned at
any point in the learner's development.
vARIETy: A way of speaking and using language that is typical of a particular
regional, socioeconomic, or ethnic group. The term 'dialect' is sometimes
205
206
Glossary
used. Some language varieties are stigmatized as 'uneducated' but each
language variety has its own rules and patterns that are as complex and
systematic as those of the so-called 'standard' language. Among the most
studied non-standard varieties of English are British cockney and AfricanAmerican Vernacular English.
BI
\roRKrNG MEMoRy: The cognitive 'space' in which we actively process new
information or information that is currently in focus. Also called'short-term
This
memory.
have
zoNE oF pRoxrMAL DEVELoIMENT (ZPD): The metaphorical 'place' in
which a learner is capable of a higher level of performance because there is
support from interaction with an interlocutor. In Vygotskyt theory, learning
takes place through and during interaction in the learne is ZPD .
Allw
Long
AIlw
Inffo
Cam
Alter
cons(
Lingt
Amn
struc'
ofEn
Secon
Ande
York:
Asher
learni
Aver;
Oxfo
Baete
Educt
Baker
edn.
(
Bard<
pragn
Learn
Bardc
Form,
Bardc
acade
Secon,
BIBLIOGRAPHY
This list brings together all the works cited in this book
have been suggested for further reading.
Allwright,
R. 1988.
Obseruation
as
well
as those
that
in the Language Classroom. London:
Longman.
Allwright, R. and K. M. Bailey. 199I. Focus on the Language Ckssroom: An
Innoduction to Classroom Research for Language Teachers. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Altenberg, E. P. 2005. 'The judgment, perception, and production of
consonant clusters in a second language'. International Reuieu of Applied
Linguistics 431 l: 53-80 .
A. and P. M. Lightbown.
2005. 'Teaching marked linguistic
structures-more about the acquisition of relative clauses by Arab learners
of English in A. Housen and M. Pierrard (eds.): Inuestigations in Instructed
Second Langu.age Acquisition. Amsterdam: Mouton de Grulter. pp. 167-98.
Ammar,
Anderson, J. R. 1995. Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach.New
York \Wiley.
Asher,
I.
1972.'Childrent first language as a model for second language
56 I 3: L33-39 .
learning.' Modern Language Journal
P and S. Ehrlich. 1992. Tbaching American English Pronunciation.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Avery,
Baetens Beardsmore, H. (.d.). 1993. European Models
Education. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
of Bilingual
Baker, C. 2001. Foundations of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism3rd
edn. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 1999. 'Exploring the interlanguage of interlanguage
pragmatics: A research agenda for acquisitional pragmatics.' Language
Learning 49 I 4: 577 -7 13.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2000. Tbnse and Aspect in Second Language Acquisition:
Form, Meaning and Ue.Oxford: Blackwell.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and B. S. Hartford. 1993. 'Learning the rules of
academic talk A longitudinal study of pragmatic development.' Studies in
Second Language Acquisition 15l 3: 279-304.
Bibliography
A. and B. Mac\(rhinney. 1981. 'second-language acquisition
from a functionalist perspective: Pragmatic, semantic, and perceptual
strategies' in H. \Tinitz (ed.): Natiue Language and Foreign Language
Bates, E.
Bur
Car
Acquisition, Annals ofthe NewYorkAcademy of Science. NewYork NewYork
Academy of Science, YoL 379. pp. I90-2I4.
Bur
Berko Gleason,J.(ed.). 2005. The Deuehpment of Language,6th edn.
London: Allyn and Bacon and Longman Publishers.
Car
nov
Car
Et€
Bialystok, E. 1991. 'Metalinguistic dimensions of bilingual language
proficiency' in E. Bialystok (ed.): Language Processing in Bilingual Children.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 11340.
Ant
Bialystok, E. 2001. Bilingualism in Deuelopment: Language, Literacy, and
Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry Press.
Cel,
Proi
Blaxton, T. A. 1989. 'Investigating dissociations among memory measures:
Support for a transfer-appropriare processing framewo rk.' Journal of
Exp erimental Psycho logy : Learning Memorl and Cognition | 5 I 4: 657
-68.
Car
Lan
Cha
Uni
Bley-Vroman, R. 1983. 'The comparative fallacy in interlanguage studies:
The case of systemati city.' Language Learning33ll: l-I7 .
Lan,
Block, D. 2003. The Social Turn in
Edinburgh Universiry Press.
VA:
Bloom,
Second Language Acquisition. Edinburgh:
L.
1991. Language Deuehpment from Two to Three. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Bloom,
L. and M. Lahey. 1978.
Language Deuelopment and Language
Chc
Chr
Coll
achi
Coll
Disorders. NewYork John \7iley.
acqr
Bongaerts, T.1999. 'ultimate atainment in L2 pronunciation: The case of
very advanced late L2 learners' in D. Birdsong (ed.): Second Language
Acquisition and the Critical Period Hypothesis. Mahwah, NJ: Lawien-e
Erlbaum and Associates. pp. 133-59.
Coo
Breen, M. P. (ed.) 200L Learner Contributions to Language Learning: New
Directions in Research. Harlow: Pearson Education.
Brooks,
N.
Brown, H.
1960. Language and Language Learning. NewYork Harcourt.
D. 200I.
Language Pedagogy.
An Interactiue Approach
NY Addison \Tesley Longman Inc.
Teaching by Principles:
Vhite
Plains,
Brown, J. D. and T. S. Rodgerc. 2002. Doing Second Language
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
to
Research.
Brown, R. 1973. A First Language: The Early Saga. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
depr
Ling
Con
Reui,
Crag
acqu
Cran
Higl
Croc
agen'
Cum
and
l
Bibliography
Burns, A. 1999. collaboratiue Action Researchfor English Language
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Teachers.
c. r975.'French in the primary school: The British experiment.'
Canadian Modern Language Reuiew 3 I I 5 : 388402.
Burstall,
carroll, J. 1991. 'cognitive abilities in foreign language aptitude: Then and
now' in T. Parry and c. Stansfield (eds.): Language Aptitudr Reconsidered.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
carroll, J. and s. sapon. 1959. The Modern Languages Aptitude Test. san
Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Celce-Murcia, M., D, M. Brinton, and J. M. Goodwin. 199G. Teaching
Pronunciation: A Reference for Teachers of English to speahers of othi
Languages. Cambridge : Cambridge Universiry Press.
chaudron,
Universiry
c.
1988.
secorud Language classrooms.
cambridge: cambridge
Press.
N. 1959. 'Review
Language35lI:25-58.
Chomslcy,
of
"Verbal Behavior" by B.F. Skinner'.
christian, D. and F. Genesee (eds.). 2001. Bilingual Education Alexandria,
VA: TESOL.
collier,
v P. 1989. 'How long? A synthesis of research on academic
achievement in a second language.'
TES)L Quarterljt23l3:509-31.
collins, L. 2002. 'The roles of Ll influence and lexical aspect in the
acquisition of temporal morphology.' Language Learning 52r r, 4l-g +.
Cooh V. 2003. 'The poverry-of-the-stimulus argument and structurein L2 users of English.' Internatiinal Reuieta of Applied
Linguistics 4l I 3: 201-21.
dependency
Corder, S. P. 1967. 'The significance
Reaiew ofApplied Linguistics
5
of
l2-3: t6I-69
learners' errors.' lnternational
.
M.
1992. 'Communicarive interaction and second language
acquisition: An Inuit example.' TE S O L Quarterly 26 I 3 : 487
-505.
Crago,
crandall, J. A. and D. Kaufman (eds.). 2002. content-Based Instruction in
Higher Education Senings. Alexandria, VA: TESOL.
crookes, G. and R. schmidt. r99r.'Motivation: Reopening the research
agenda.' Language Learning 4l I 4: 469-5 12.
cummins, J. 1984. Bilingualism and special Education:
and Pedagogy. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Issues in Assessment
209
210
Bibliography
Cummins, J. 2000. Language, Power and Pedagogt: Bilingual Children in the
Cro ssf.re. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
D6
'Wild
Dd.
Curtiss, S. 1977. Genie: A
Child.' New York Academic
Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day
D6
and
Press.
Dalton, C. and B. Seidlhofer. 1994. Pronunciation. Oxford: Oxford University
D6
Press.
lani
ofs
to encourage speaking in
E. Uso-Juan and A.
in
classrooms'
content-andJanguage-integrated
Martinez-Flor (eds.): Current Tiends in the Deuelopment and Tbaching of the
Four Language Skilk.Berlin: Mouton de Grulter.
Evi
Lat
E. and S. Shapson. 199I. 'Integrating formal and functional
Do
Dalton-Puffet C. 2006.'Questions
Day,
as strategies
Do
approaches to language teaching in French immersion: An experimental
approach.' Language Learning 4l I l: 25-58.
C.
DeKeyser, R. M. 1998. 'Beyond focus on form: Cognitive perspectives on
learning and practicing second language grammar' in C. J' Doughry and J.
\filliams (eds.): Focus on Forn't in Classroom Second Language Acquisition.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp 42-63.
Do
C.r
Lat
Cat
Do
Lat
DeKeyser, R. M. 2000. 'The robustness of critical period effbcts in second
language acquisition.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 2214:
493-533.
Du
DeKeyser, R. M. 2001. Automaticity and automatization'in P. Robinson
(ed.): Cognition and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge
Du
University
Press.
pp. 125-51.
clas
and
Laz
'Wiebe.
1998. 'Evidence in favor ofa
Derwing, T. M., M. J. Munro, and G.
broad framework for pronunciation instruction . Language Learning 4Bl3:
Ecl
393410.
Pea
Derwing, T. M. and M. Rossitel 2003. 'The effbcts of pronunciation
instruction on the accuracy,fluency, and complexity of L2 accented speech'.
Ecl
App lied Language Learning 13 I | :
t-I7
.
Cot
clat
Apt
Villiers, J. G. and P. A. de Villiers. 1973.'A cross-sectional study of the
acquisition of grammatical morphemes.' Journal of Psycholingaistic Research
213:257-78.
Ein
Donato, R. 1994. 'Collective scaffolding in second language learning' in J.
Lantolf and G. Appel (eds.): Vygotskian Approaches to Second Language
Research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. pp.33-55.
4na
de
Diirnyei, Z. 200Ia. Motiuational Strategies
Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry Press.
in
the Language Ckssroom.
pefr
Elli
pp'
Elli
i-I
Stu,
Bibliography
Diirnyei, Z. 200 lb.
Te ac h
ing
an
d Ru e arc h ing
M o tiu atio n. Harlow: Longman.
Z. 2005. The Psychologlt of the Language Learner: Indiuidual
D
rences in S eco nd Language Ac quisition. Mahwah, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum
and Associates.
Diirnyei,
ffi
Skehan. 2003. 'Individual differences in second
language learning' in C. J. Doughry and M. H. Long (eds.): The Handbook
of Second Language Acquisition. Malden, MA: Blackwell. pp. 5 89-630.
Dbrnye|Z. and
P.
Doughty, C. 1991. 'second language instruction does make a difference:
Evidence from an empirical study of SL relativization.' Studies in Second
Language Acquisition 13l 4: 431-59 .
Doughty, C. and E. Varela. 1998. 'Communicative focus on form in
C. Doughry and J.'Williams (eds.): Focus on Form in Classroom SLA.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. II4-38.
Doughty, C. andJ.'Williams. 1998. 'Pedagogical choices in focus on form' in
C. Doughry and J. \filliams (eds.): Focus on Form in Cl.assroom Second
Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 197-261.
Doughry,
C. and M. H. Long (eds.). 2003. The Handbook of Second
Language Acquisition.
Duff,
Oxford: Blackwell.
P. 1995. An
ethnography
of
communication
classrooms in Hungary.' T E S O L Quarterly 29 I 3: 505-37
in
immersion
.
Dunn,'W andJ. P. Lantolf. 1998.'Vygotsky's zone of proximal development
and Krashent i+1: Incommensurable constructs, incommensurable theories.'
Language Learning 48: 41142.
M. Vogt, and D. J. Short. 2004. Mabing Content
Comprehensibk for English Language Learners: The SIOP Modtl. Boston:
Echevarria,
J.,
Pearson.
Eckman,
F.,
L. Bell, and D. Nelson. 1988. 'On the generalization ofrelative
in the acquisition of English as a second language.'
clause instruction
App lied Linguistics
9I
I:
I-20.
D., E. R. Siqueland, P. Juscryk, and
perception in infants.' Science 77l: 303-5.
Eimas,
P.
J.
Vigorito. 1971.
'Speech
Ellis, N. C. 2001. 'Memory for language' in P Robinson (ed.): Cognition
and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry Press.
pp.33-68.
Ellis, N. C. 2002.'Frequency effbcts in language acquisition: A review with
implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition.'
Studies in Second Language Acquisition24l2: I43-BB.
2t7
212
Bibliography
C. 2003 'Constructions,
chunking, and connectionism: The
emergence of second language structure' in C. J. Doughty and M. H. Long
Ellis, N.
Gz
Mr
(eds.): The Handbook ofsecond LanguageAcquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. pp.
Gz
63-t03.
int
Ellis, N. C. 2005. At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and
implicit language knowledge.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition2712:
305-52.
82
Ga
Ini
Ellis, R. 1994. The Study of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
fur
Ellis,R. (ed.). 2001. 'Form-focused instruction and
for
22
learning.' Language Learning
5II
Supplement
second language
1.
Ellis, R., H. Basturkmen, and S. Loewen. 200I. 'Learner uptake in
communicative ES L lessons.' Language Learning 5 | I 2: 281-3 18.
Elman, J. L., E.A. Bates, M. H. Johnson, A. Karmiloff-Smith, D. Parisi,
and K. Plunkett. 1996. Rethinhinglnnateness:
Deuehpmenr. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
A Connectionist
Ga
Ga
lar
Re
Perspectiue on
Ga
Favreau, M. and N. Segalowitz. 1983. Automatic and controlled processes
in the first- and second-language reading of fluent bilinguals.' Memory and
Cognition Il I l: 56-7 4.
inr
8r(
Ge
La
Ganschow, L. and R. L. Sparks. 2001. 'Learning difficulties and foreign
language learning: A review of research and insmr,rction.' Language Teaching
Ge
3412:79-98.
Ur
Garcla Mayo, M. del P. and M. L. Garcia Lecumberri (eds.). 2003. Age and
the Acquisition of English as a Foreign Language. Clevedon: Multilingual
Ge
Matters.
Ba
Gardner,D.2004. 'Vocabulary input through extensive reading: A
comparison of words found in childrent narratiye and expository reading
materials.' App lied Linguistics 25 I l: l-37 .
Gi
Gardner,
H.
1993. Mubiple Intelligences: The Theory in Practice. New York:
cl.
AN
De
Gc
or(
Basic Books.
der
'W E. Lambefi. 1972. Attitudes and Motiuation in
Gardner, R. C. and
Second Language Learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Gc
Gass, S. 1982. 'From theory
Mi
to practice' in M. Hines and'W'. Rutherford
TESOL. pp.129-39.
Gt
'Tl
Gass, S. 1988. 'Integrating research areas: A framework for second language
studies.' App lied Linguistics 9 I 2: l9B-217 .
pr(
(eds.): OnTESOL 81. W'ashington, DC:
Co
Bibliography
M.
1997. Input, Interaction, and the Second Language Learner.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
Gass, S.
Pica (eds.). 1998. 'The role of input and
second language acquisition. Modern Language Journal
Gass, S., A. Mackey, and T.
interaction
in
8213.
Gass, S. M. and L. Selinker. 2001. Second Langaage Acquisition: An
Introductory Course 2nd edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and
Associates.
Gatbonton, E. and N. Segalowitz. 1988. 'Creative automatization: Principles
for promoting fuencywithin a communicative framework.' TESOL Quarterly
2213:473-92.
Gatbonton,
E. and N. Segalowitz. 2005. 'Rethinking
communicative
language teaching: A focus on access to fluency.' Canadian Modern Language
Reuiew6Il3:325-53.
Gatbonton, E., P. Tiofimovich, and M. Magid. 2005.'Learners' ethnic
group affiliation and L2 pronunciation accuracy: A sociolinguistic
investigation' . T E S O L Quarurly 39 I 3 : 489-5 I I .
Genesee, F.
L
1976.'The role of intelligence in second language learning.'
anguage L e ar ning 26 I 2: 267 -80 .
Genesee, F. (ed.). 1995. Educating Second Language Children: The Whole
Child, the Who le Curriculum, the Whole Community.Cambridge: Cambridge
University
Press.
M. Crago, and J. Paradis. 2004. Dual Language Deuelopment
and Disordrrs: A Handbook on Bilingualism and Second Language Learning.
Genesee, F.,
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
H. and S. Opper.
1969. Piaga's Theory
of
Intellectual
D eu e lop m e nt : An In tro ductio n. Englewood Cliffs, NJ : Prentice-Hall.
Ginsburg,
Goldschneider, J. M. and R. M. DeKeyser. 2001. 'Explaining the natural
order of L2 morpheme acquisition in English: A meta-analysis of multiple
determinant s.' Language Learning 5l I 1 : 1-50.
Goldstein, T. 2003. Tbaching and Learning
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
in a Multilingual School.
Guiora, A., B. Beit-Hatrlahami, R. Brannon, C. Dull, and T. Scovel.1972.
'The effects of experimentally induced changes in ego states
pronunciation abiliry
in a
Co mp re h ens iue Psy c hiatry
I
3I5
second language:
: 421-8.
An
on
exploratory study.'
213
214
Bibliography
Hahn, L. D. 2004.'Primary stress and intelligibility: Research to motivate
the teaching of suprasegmentals.' TE S O L Quarterly 38 I 2: 201-23.
Hy
SL
La
Hamilton, R. 1994. 'Is implicational generalization unidirectional and
maximal? Evidence from relativization instruction in a second language.'
Ita
Language Learning 44 I I : 123-57
Me
.
Harley, B. 1998. 'The role of focus-on-form tasks in promoting child L2
acquisition in C. Doughty and J. \Tilliams (eds.): Focus on Form in
C las s ro o m S e c o n d Language Ac quis i ti o n. Cambridge : Cambridge University
Press. pp. 156-74.
Jeo
Mt
Jeo
An,
Harley, B. and M. Swain. 1984. 'The interlanguage of immersion students
and its implications for second language teaching' in A. Davies, C. Criper,
and A. Howatt (eds.): Interlanguage. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University
Press. pp.
29I-311.
Hatch, E. 1978.'Discourse analysis and second language acquisition in E.
Hatch (ed.): Second Language Acquisition: A Boob of Readings. Rowley, MA:
Newbury House. pp. 40I-35.
Heath, S. B. I 983.
Ways
with
Words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hedge, T. 2000. Tbaching and Learning in the Language Classroom. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Hinkel, F,. 2002.
Second Language.Writers' Ti:xt: Linguistic and Rhetorical
Joh
l*r
Etr
Jol,
un(
Intr
Kar
1*r
Kee
Gra
Features. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
KeI
Hinkel, E. (ed.). 2005. Handbook of Research on Second Langudge
dev,
Teaching
and Learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
edr
Pe
Hinkel, E. and S. Fotos. 2002. Neu Perspectiues on Grammar Tbaching in
Second Language Classrooml Mahwah, NJ' Lawrence Erlbaum and
Associates.
Horst, M. 2005. 'Learning L2 vocabulary through extensive reading: A
measurement study.' Canadian Mo dern Language Reuiew 6l I 3: 35 5-82.
Horwitz, E. K. and D. J. Young.l99l. LanguageAnxiety: FromTheory and
Research to Classroom Implications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Horwitz, E. K., M. B. Horwitz, and J. Cope. 1986. 'Foreign language
classroom anxiety.' Modern Language Journal 7012: 125-32.
Howatt, A. P. R. 2004. The History of ELT. 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford
University
11
Klei
Een
Koj
1-Oq
icttt
Kra
Erot
)Cr
Kra
{}J
Press.
Hulstijn,J. and B.Laufer. 2001. 'Some empirical evidence for
Krar
the
involvement load hypothesis in vocabulary acquisition.' Language Learning
5rl3:539-58.
i on,
Bibliography
Hyltenstam, K. and N. Abrahamsson. 2003. 'Maturational constraints in
SLA in C. J. Doughry and M. H. Long (eds.): The Handbooh of Second
Language Ac quis itio n. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 5 4 0-B B.
Itard, J.-M. -G.1962. TheWild Boy ofAueyon (L'Enfant sauuag).NewYork:
Meredith.
Jenkins, J. 2000. The Phonology ofEruglish as an International Language: New
Modrls, New Norms, New Goak. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
in teaching pronunciation and intonation.'
Annual Reuiew ofApp lied Linguistics 24:109-25.
Jenkins,
J. 2004.
'Research
J. and E. Newport. 1989. 'Critical period effects in second
language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of
English as a Second Language.' Cognitiae Psychology 2I I l: 50-99 .
Johnson,
Johnson, R. K. 1997. 'The Hong Kong education system: Late immersion
under stress' in R. K. Johnson and M. Swain (eds.): Immersion Education:
Internati o na I Persp e c tiu e s. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
Kasper, G. and K. R. Rose. 2002. 'Pngmatic development
language.' Language Learning 52,Supplement 1.
in a second
Keenan, E. and B. Comrie. 1977. 'Noun phrase accessibility and Universal
Grammar.' Linguistic Inquiry
Bl
l: 63-99.
Kellerman, E. 1986. An eye for an eye: Crosslinguistic constraints on the
development of the L2 lexicon in E. Kellerman and M. Sharwood Smith
(eds.): Crosslinguistic Influence in Second Language Acquisition. New York:
Pergamon. pp.35-48,
Klein,'W and C. Perdue. 1993.
Utterance Structure. Amsterdam: John
Benj amins. http ://www.mpi. nl/worl dl tgl lapp I esf/esf.html
Kojic-Sabo,I. and P M. Lightbown. 1999. 'Students' approaches to
to success.' Modern Language
vocabulary learning and their relationship
JournalS3l2: 175-92.
Krashen, S. 1977. 'Some issues relating to the Monitor Model' in H. D.
Brown, C. A. Yorio, and R. H. Crymes (eds.): On TESOL '77.Washington,
DC:TESOL.pp. t44-58.
Krashen, S. 1982. Principles and Practice
Oxford: Pergamon.
Krashen, S. 1985. The Input Hypothesis:
Longman.
in
Second Language Acquisition.
Issues
and Implications. London:
215
216
BibliograPhy
Krashen,
S. 1989. '\7e acquire vocabulary and spelling by
iaaiil""A
evidence
fo.,h. i'p'.rt
reading:
Li
hypothesis.' Mofurn Language Journal
fer
let
ft|4:440-54.
New York:
1964. Language Teaching: A Scientifc Approach'
McGraw-Hill.
bicultural experiences
Lambert,'w: E. 1987. 'The effects of bilingual and
Homel' M' Palij' and
on children's attitude, and 'ocial perspecti;es' in P'
and Associates'
D. A"ronro.t (eds.): Mahwah, Nit f"*tttce Erlbaum
pp. t97-221.
Education of children:
Lambert,'w'. E. and G. R. Tucke r. 1972. Bilinguat
Newbury'
The St. Lambert Experiment' Rowley, MA:
Lado,
R.
Lantolf, J. P. (.d'). 2000' Sociocubural Theorlt and
Press'
Le arning. Oxford: Oxford Universiry
D. 2000. Tbchniques and Principles in Language Ti:aching
Second Language
Larsen-Freeman,
2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press'
B. 1'992.'How much lexis is necessary- fo-r reading.comprehension?'
l" p. i.'L funaud and H. Bdjoint (eds.): vocabularL andApplied Linguistics.
Laufer,
London: Macmillan. PP. 125-32'
language behavior.'
Leow, R. p. 1997. 'Attention, awareness, and foreign
Language Learning 47 I 3: 467 -505.
Lir
EI
Lii
he
La
Lc
ne
Lo
ac(
Ac
Lo
lar
Lo
2flr
(e.
IVI,
Ly
O)
classrooms' in
Lightbown, P. M. 1983a. Acquiring English L2 in Quebec
at the crossroads.
s. Felix and H..wode (eds.)i Laiguage Deuelopment
Tiibingen: GunterNarr. PP. 151-75'
Lyt
Lightbown, M. 1 9B3b.'Exploring relationship: ltyt:i.developmental
and M' H'
inr.r.r.rional sequences in L2 aJquisition in H' \7' Seliger
"n?
i."g i.ar.l, Classroin, Oriented Ruiarch in Second Language Acquisition'
Lyr
Fre
T5
P.
Rowiey, MA: Newbury House'
pp' 2t745'
Lightbown,P.M.lg85..Greatexpectations:Secondlanguageacquisition
17 3-89 .
an4 classroom teaching.' App lie d Linguistics 6 l 2:
dis
Lyr
ins
,.J."r.h
Ly
in focus on form'
Lightbown, P. M. 1998.'The importance of timing
in Classroom
Form
J.'\Tilliams^ (.td:)' Focus on
in-C. Doughty
"ni
University Press'
Language Arqi;,itlon' Cambridge: Cambridge
Ne
Second.
Ma
pp. r77-95.
rep
Lightbown,P.M.2000.'Anniversaryarticle:ClassroomsLAresearchand
Ma
aliz
2l I 4: 43 | -52'
,.Jo nd language teachin g.' App li e d Linguis ti c s
La;
Bibliography
Lightbown, P. M. and N. Spada. 1990. 'Focus on form and corrective
feedback in communicative language teaching: Effects on second language
learning.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition l2l4: 42948.
Lightbown, P. M. and N. Spada. 1994.'An innovative program for primary
ESL in Quebec.' TESOL Quarterly2Sl3:563-79.
M., R. Halter, J. L. Vhite, and M. Horst. 2002. 'Compreit yourself"' . Caruadian Modern
Language Reuiew 58 I 3: 427 -64.
Lightbown,
P.
hension-based learning: The limits of "do
Long, M. 1933. 'Native speaker/non-native speaker conversation and the
negotiation of comprehetrribl. input.' App lied Linguistics 41 2: 1264I.
Long, M.1996. 'The role of the linguistic environment in second language
acquisition' in \( Ritchie andT. Bhatia (eds.): Handbook ofSecond Language
Acquisition. NewYork Academic Press. pp. 413-68.
Long, M. and
P.
Porter. 1985. 'Group work, interlanguage talk, and second
language acquisition.' T E S O L Quarterly
19 I 2:
207
-28.
Long, M. and C. Sato. 1983. 'Classroom foreigner talk discourse: Forms
and functions of teachers' questions' in H.'\(/. Seliger and M. H. Long
(eds.): Classroom-Oriented Research in Second Language Acquisition. Rowley,
MA: Newbury House. pp. 268-85.
Lynch,
T. 1996. Comrnunication in
Oxford University
the Language Classroom. Oxford:
Press.
Lyster, R. 1994. 'The effect of functional-analytic teaching on aspects of
French immersion students' sociolinguistic competence.' Applied Linguistics
1513:263-87.
Lyster,R. l998.lRecasts, repetition and ambiguity in L2
discourse.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition2\ll: 5 1-8 1 .
classroom
Lyster R.2004. 'Differential effbcts of prompts and recasts in form-focused
instruction.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26 I 3: 399432.
L. Ranta. 1997.'Conective feedback and learner uptake:
Negotiation of form in communicative classrooms.' Studies in Second
Language Acquisition 19l l: 37-61 .
Lyster, R. and
Maclntyre, P. 1995.'How does anxiety affect second language learning? A
reply to Sparks and Gansch ow.' Modern Language Journal 791 l: 90-9 .
Maclntyre, P., R. Climent, Z. Diirnyei, and K. Noels. 1998. 'Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in a L2: A situational model of L2
confi dence and affi liatio n.' Mo drrn Language Journa I 82 I 4: 5 45-62.
217
2lB
Bibliography
Mackey, A. 1999. 'Input, interaction and second language development: An
empirical study of question formation in ESL.' Studies in Second Language
Acquisition 2I I 4: 557 -87 .
A.
N
Philp. 1998. 'Conversational interaction and second
language development: Recasts, responses and red herrings.' Modern
Mackey,
and J.
Language Journa I 82
I3
h
I,
: 338-5 6.
ol
M. Gass. 2005. Second Language Research: Method.ologt
and Design. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
Mackey, A. and S.
Mackey,
A.,
S.
M. Gass, and K. McDonough. 2000. 'How do
perceive interactional feedback?' Studies
in
S.
L
learners
N/
Second Language Acquisition
ol
2214:471-97.
L,
ey, B. I99 5 . Th e C H I L D E S Proj e ct: Tb o k fo r Ana ly zing Ta I b 2nd
edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates. http://childes.
M
de
psy.cmu.edu/
Se
MacWhinn ey, B. 1997 .'Second language acquisition and the competition
model' inA. M. B. de Groot andJ. F. Kroll (eds.): Tutorials in Bilingualism:
Psycholinguistic Perspectiu€s. Mahwah, NJ' Lawrence Erlbaum and
ed
MacW.hinn
M
M,
Associates. pp. 113-44.
he
Malamah-Thomas, A.
I 9 87
. C las s r o o m In terac ti o n. Oxford : Oxford Universiw
La
Press.
Masgoret,
A.-M. and R. C. Gardner. 2003. Attitudes, motivarion,
Na
and
JOI
second language learning: A meta-analysis of studies conducted by Gardner
and associat es.' Language Learning 53 I I : 123-63.
)ia
McCormick, D. E. and R. Donato. 2000. 'Teacher questions as scaffolded
assistance in an ESL classroom'inJ. K. Hall and L. Verplaetse (eds.): Secorud
and Foreign Langwage Learning through Classroom Interaction. Mahwah,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates. pp. lB3-20I
I tl;
\a
Car
\e.
'lo!
McDonough, K.
group activities in aThai EFL context.' System32:207-24.
2004. 'Learner-learner interaction during pair and small
Mclaughlin,
B.
1978. 'The Monitor model: Some methodological
consideratio ns.' Language Learning 28 I 2: 309-32.
Mclaughlin, B. 1990. 'Restructuring.' Applied Linguistics 11/1: 113-28.
Mclaughlin, B. and R. Heredia. 1995.'Information-processing approaches
to research on second language acquisition and use' in \X1 C. Ritchie and T. K.
Bhatia (eds.): Handbooh of Second Language Acqaisition. New York Academic
Press. pp. 213-28.
\or
-
-:-
:-
llor
: -'.
\:rr
:I:
Bibliography
Meara, P. M. 1980. 'vocabulary_acquisition: A neglected aspect of language
learning.' Language Tbaching and Liiguixics Abstrirr lZ: ZZi_46.
M. 2005a
ll!ryo Language Aptitude zsrs. swansea; Lognostics.
http //www. swan. ac. uk/cals/calsris/lognostics. htm
Meara,
P.
:
M. 2005b. 'Designing vocaburary tests for English, spanish and
in c. Butler, M. dos Angeles G6#z-Gonzales and
s. Doval-Sudrez (eds.): The Dynamics of Linguage (Jse: Functional and
Dynamic Perspectiues.Amsterdam: John Blnjamin s. pp 269_53.
Meara,
P.
other
lan_guages'
Meisel,
I.M.
r9B7. 'Reference to pasr events and actions in the developmenr
of natural second language acquiiition in c. pfaff (ed,.): First and second
Language Acquisition Processes. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.
Meisel,J. M., H. clahsen, and M. pienemann. 1981. 'on determining
in natural second language acquisition.' studies in
S e co nd Language Acquis iti on 3 I 2: 109
-3 5.
developmental stages
Mitchell, R. and F. Myles. 2004.
edn. London: Arnold.
Second Language Learning Theories
2nd
llunro, M. J. and T. M. Derwing. 1995. 'Foreign accent, comprehensibiliry and intelligibllity in the ipeech of ,..o'j hrg.r"g. learners.'
Language Learning 45 I I : 7 3-97 .
Nagy, 'wl E., P. A. Herman, and R. Anderson. 19g5. 'Learningwords
from
context.' Reading Res earch euarterly 20 I 2: 233_5 3.
Naiman, N., M. Friihlich, H. H. Stern, andA. Todesco. 1995. The
Good
L anguage L e arn e r. Clev edon : Multilingual Matters.
Nation, I. s.
P. 200 r - Lea rn i ng voca bu rary in Anoth er
Cambridge Un iversiry Press.
Newport,
E.
1990- 'Marurational consrraints
Cognitiue Science
74lI: II-28.
Language.cambridge:
on
ranguage rearning.'
P. M. Lightbown, and N. Spada. 2001. ,Recasts as feedback
to language learners.' Language Learning 5il4:719_58.
Nicholas, H.,
Norris, J.
M. and L. ortega. 2000.
'Effectiveness
of L2 instruction: A
research synthesis and quantitative mera-analysis.' Language
Learning5013:
417-528.
Norton, B. 2000. Identity and Language Learning: Gender, Ethnicity, and
Educational C hange. London: Longma.rTpearson Education.
Norton, B. and K Toohey. 2001. 'Changing perspectives on good language
learners.' TE S O L Quarterly 35 I 2: 307
-2j.
2t9
220
Bibliography
Norton
Peirce,
B.
1995. 'social identiry investment, and
learning'. TE S O L Quarterly
l
language
a
29 I 1': 9-31.
Obler, L. 1939. 'Exceptional second language learners' in S. Gass,
C. Madden, D. Preston, and L. Selinker (eds.): Variation in Second
Language Acquisition, Vol. II: Psycholinguistic Issues. Clevedon: Multilingual
Matters. pp.14l-59.
odlin, T. i989.
Language Tiansfer. cambridge: cambridge University
J
P
F
P
P
Press.
L
Odlin, T. 2003.'Cross-linguistic infuence' in C. J. Doughry and M' H'
Long (eds.): The Handbioh of second Language Acquisition. oxford:
Blackwell. pp.436-85.
L
ohta, A. 2000. 'Rethinking recasts:
A
learner-centered examination
of
corrective feedbackin theJapanese classroom' inJ. K. Hall and L' Verplaetse
(eds.): Second and Foreign Language Learning through Classroorn Interaction.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates. pp. 47-71.
oliver, R. and A. Mackey. 2003. 'Interactional context and feedback in
child ES L classrooms.' The Modern Language Journal 87 I 4: 5 19-33.
Oller, D.
K. and R. E. Eilers (eds.). 2002. Language and Literacy
Deuelopment in Bilingual Children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
P
P
ol
R
c(
Ir
R
H
R
ler
Oxford, R. 1990. Language Learning Strategies: What Euery Tbacher Should
Know. New York: Newbury House.
Pe
Patkowski, M. 1980. 'The sensitive period for the acquisition of syntax in
Ri
a
second language.' Language Learning 30 I 2: 449-7 2.
C;
Lu
Piaget, J.195111946. Plo! Dreamt and Imitation in Childhood. New York:
Norton. (Originally published as La formation du symbole chez I'enfant.
R(
NeuchAtel: Delchaux et Niestl6.)
Ur
Pica, T. 1994. 'Research on negotiation:'W'hat does it reveal about second
language acquisition? Conditions, processes, and outcomes'. Language
Learning 441 3 : 493-527 .
1988. 'Determining the infuence of instruction on L2
speech processing.' AILA Reuiew 5ll: 40-72.
Pienemann,
M.
Pienemann,
M. 1999. Language Processing and
Ct
RC
Ca
Rc
Le,
Ry
Second
Language
Deue lopment: Pro cessabi lity Th eor4'Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Pienemann, M. 2003. 'Language processing capacity in C. J. Doughry and
M. H. Long (eds.): The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford:
Blackwell. pp. 57 9-7 14.
Sar
res
Ap.
Bibliography
Pienemann, M., M.Johnston, and G. Brindley. 1988. 'Constructing an
acquisition-based procedure for second language assessment.' Studies in
S e co
nd Language Acquisitio n I 0 I 2: 217 43.
Pimsleur, P. 1966. The Pimsleur Language Aptitude Banery. New York:
Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovic.
Pinker, S. 1994. The Language Instinct. NewYork: \Tilliam Morrow.
Pinter, A. 2005. Teaching Young Language Learners. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Pipe6 T. 1 998. Language and Learning: The Home and
Upper Saddle fuver, NJ: Merrill/Prentice-Hall.
Sch oo
I Years2nd edn.
Piske, T., I. R. A. MacKay, andJ. E. Flege. 2001. 'Factors affecting degree
of foreign accent tnanL2: Areview.' Journal of Phonetics2g: l9I-2I5.
Ranta, L. 2002. 'The role of learners' language analytic abiliry in the
communicative classroom in P. Robinson (ed.): Indiuidual Dffirences arud
Insnucted Language Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 159-80.
Reid, J. (ed.) 1995. Learning Styles in the ESL/EFL Classroom. New York:
Heinle and Heinle.
Ringbom, H. i985. 'Crosslinguistic influence and the foreign language
learning process' in E. Kellerman and M. Sharwood Smith (eds.):
Crosslinguistic Influence in Second Language Acquisition. New York:
Pergamon Press. pp. 150-72.
.W:
C. and T
K. Bhatia. 1996. (eds.). Handbooh
Language Acquisition. New York Academic Press.
Ritchie,
of
Second
Robinett, B.'W andJ. Schachter (eds.). 1983. Second Language Learning:
Contrastiue An4lysis, Error Analysis, and Related Aspects. Ann Arbor: The
Universiry of Michigan Press.
Robinson, P. (ed.). 2001. Cognition and Second Language Instruction.
Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry Press.
Robinson, P. (ed.). 2002. Indiuidual Dffirences and Instructed Language
Learning.Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Rlnner, R. 1 993.
Genie
:
A Scienffic Ti,age$t New York HarperCollins.
Sachs,J., B.Bard, and M.Johnson. 1981.'Language learning with
restricted input: Case studies of two hearing children of deaf parents.'
App lied Psycho linguistics 2 I I : 33-54.
221
222
Bibliography
Samuda,v. 2001. 'Guiding relationships between form and meaning
during task performance: The role of the teacher' in M. Bygate, P. Skehan
M. Swain (eds.): Researching Pedagogic Tashs: Second Language Learning,
"nd
Tb ac h ing and Tes ting. London: Longman' pp. I 19 -40.
in ForeignDevelopment.
for
curriculum
PA:
center
languag, Tbaching. Philadelphia,
Savignon, 5.1972. Communicatiue
schachter,
Competence: An Experiment
1974. 'An error in error analysis.' Language Learning 2412:
I.
205-14.
schachter,
s
1
S
v
h
SJ
*
Si
J. 1990. 'on the issue of
acquisition.'
B. 1990.
Schieffelin,
completeness
Second Language Researc h 6 I 1 :
in
second language
93-124.
Si
The Giue and Thke of Euerydait Life: Language
Socialization of Kaluli Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University
S1
Schmidt, R. 1990. 'The role of consciousness in second language learning.'
App lied Linguistics 11 I I : 17 -46.
schmidt, R. 2001. Attention in P. Robinson (ed.): cognition and second
Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry Press. pp. 3-32.
Schmitt, N. 2000. wcabular\ in Language Tbaching. cambridge: cambridge
University Press.
Schulz, R. A. 2001. 'Cultural differences in student and teacher perceptions
concerning the role of grammar teaching and corrective feedback: USA85
I
ec
Press.
Schieffelin, B. and E. Ochs (eds.). 1986. Language Socialization Across
Cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry Press'
Colombia.' Mo dern Language Journal
C
2: 244-58.
L(
ST
o
SK
Sl'
in
Dt
Sl(
Mr
Sn
Le,
1979.'The acquisition of English negation by speakers of
'$7.
Andersen (ed.): The Acquisition
Spanish: a review of the literature' in R.
'w'ashington,
inc
DC:TESOL.
Ox
Schumann,
ind
I.
Use of spanish and English as
First and second Languages.
B. 1993. 'On explicit and negative
Sn,
PI
data effecting and affecting
Snr
comperence and linguistic behavior.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition
acq
15l2: t47-63.
49t
H' Long
Segalowitz, N. 2003. Automaticity' in C. J. Doughry
"id y.
(.dr.)' The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell.
fort
Schwartz,
pp.382-408.
seidlhofer, 8.2004. 'Teaching English
App li e d Lingais tics 24: 209 -39 .
Snr
Secr
as a
linguaiianca..' AnnuAl Reuiew
of
Hei
BibliograPhy
Selinker, L.1972. 'Interlanguage.' International Reuieu ofApplied Linguistics
l0l2:209-31.
N. and B. Z. Pearson. (eds.)' 2004. Eualuating.Language
Deuelopment and Dialect from Dinrder. Special
Distinguishing
Viriation:
issue of seminnrc i, speerh and Language,NY: Thieme Medical Publishers.
Seymour, H.
sharwood smith,
M. 1993. 'Input enhancement in instructed SLA:
theoretical bases.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 1512: 165-79.
Sinclair, J. and M. Coulthard. 1975. Tbwards an Analysis of Discourse.
Oxford: Oxford University Press'
Singleton, D. and L. Ryan. 2004. LangaageAcquisition: TheAge Factor2nd
edn. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
skehan, P. 1989. Indiuidual Dffirences
London: EdwardArnold.
skehan, P. 1993.
Oxford University
A
in
second Language Learning.
cognitiue Approach to Language Learning. oxford:
Press.
skinner, B. F. 19 57 . Wrbal Behaulan New York Appleton-century-crofts.
Slobin, D.1973. 'Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar'
in C. A. Ferguson "ttd O. I. Slobin (eds.): Studies of Child Language
Deuelopmenr. NewYork: Holt, Rinehart, and\7inston. pp 175-2OB'
slobin, D. I. 19s5-1997. The crosslinguistic study of Language. vols 1-5.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
N. and I.-M. Tsimpli. 1995.
The
Blackwell'
Learning and Mo du larity. Oxford:
Smith,
Mind of a Sauant: Language
c.
1995.'Issues in the study of input: Finetuning, universality,
individual and developmental diffbrences, and necessary causes' in
p. Fletcher and B. Mac\whinney (eds.): The Handbooh of child Language.
Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 180-93.
snow,
Snow, C. and M. Hoefnagel-Hithle. 1978.'Thecritical period for language
acquisition: Evidence from second language learning.' Child Deuelopment
4914: rtI4-28.
Snow, M. A. 2001. 'content-based and immersion models for second and
foreign language teaching' in M. celce-Murcia (ed.): Teaching English as a
Seroid
oi fortlgn Linguage 3rd edn. Boston, MA: Heinle &
HeinleiThomson Learnitg. pp 303-1 8.
223
224
Bibliography
Spada,
N.
1997.
'Form-focussed
acquisition: A review
Teaching3\l2:73-87.
of
classroom
instruction and second language
and laboratory research.' Language
Et
N. and M. Friihlich. 1995. The Communicatiue
Orientation of
Language Tbaching Obseruation Scheme: Coding Conuentions and
Applications. Sydney: National Centre for English Language Teaching and
Spada,
Sr
in
Ta
lar
IC
Research, Macquarie University.
Ta
N.
and P. M. Lightbown. 1993. 'Instruction and the development
of questions in L2 classrooms.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 1512:
Spada,
lar
Bi
205*24.
Spada,
N. and P. M. Lightbown. 1999. 'Instruction, Ll
T0
influence and
developmental readiness in second language acquisition.' Modern Language
Re
Journal B3lI: l-22.
T0
M. Lightb own. 2002.'L1 and L2 in the education of Inuit
children in northern Quebec: Abilities and perceptions.' Language and
To
grl
Spada, N. and P.
Education
16 I 3:
212-40.
N., P. M. Lightbown, and J. L.'White. 2005. 'The importance of
form/meaning mappings in explicit form-focussed instruction' in
A. Housen and M. Pierrard (eds.): Inuestigations in Instructed Second
L angu age Ac qu is iti o n. Amsterdam : Mouton de Gru;.ter. pp. I 9 9 a3 4.
Spada,
Spielmann,G. and M.J.Radnofsky. 2001. 'Learning language under
tension: New directions from a qualitative study.' Modern LanguageJournal
8512:259-78.
Storch,
N. 2002. 'Patterns of interaction in ESL pair work.'
M. 1985. 'Communicative
competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its developmenr' in S. Gass
and C. Madden (eds.): Input in Second Language Acquisitioz. Rowley, MA:
Newbury House. pp. 235-53.
M.
1988. 'Manipulating and complementing content teaching to
maximize second language learning.' TE S L Canada Journal 6 I I : 68-83.
Swain,
To
Cl,
Tii
sec
18
var
Va
Language
Learning52ll: I19-58.
Swain,
CI
Swain, M. 2000. 'The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition
through collaborative dialogue' in J. P. Lantolf (ed.): Sociocuhural Theory and
Second Language Learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Swain, M. and S. Lapkin. 1998. ' Interaction and second language learning:
Two adolescent French immersion students working rogether.' Modern
Language Jo urna I 82 I 3 : 338-5 6.
Co
Va
pr(
Va
Mt
Lat
vy1
Pre
'Wa
Lai
W'a
of<
Kw
(ed
Dil
Bibliography
Lapkin. 2002. 'Thlking it through: Two French
immersion learners' response to reformuladon.' International Journal of
Swain,
M.
and
S.
Educational Research
37 |
34: 285-304.
Throne, E. and M. Swain. 1995.'A sociolinguistic perspective on secondlanguage use in immersion classrooms.' Modern Language Journal T9l2:
t56-78.
Thylor, D., M.Caron, and L. McAlpine. 2000. 'Patterns of perceived
language ability and use in Arctic Quebec.' Bilingual Education and
Bi lingualisrn 3 I 4: 283-9 6.
Tobin, K.1987. 'The role of wait time in higher cognitive level learning.'
Reuiew of Educational Research 57: 59-95 .
K. 1998. 'Breaking them up, taking them away: ESL
1.' TES OL Quarterfu 321 l: 6I-84.
Toohey,
grade
students in
Toohey, IC 2000. Learning English at School: IdentitT, Social Relations and
Classroorn Practice. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Towell, R. and R. Hawkins .1994. Approaches
to Second LanguageAcquisition.
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Tiahey, M. and L.'White. 1993 'Positive evidence and preemption in the
second language classroom.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition l5l2:
t8t-:204.
van Lier, L. 1988. The Clnssroom and the Language Learner. London: Longman.
VanPatten,
B. (ed.). 2004. Processing Instruction:
Theory, Research, and
Commentary. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum andAssociates.
T. Cadierno. 1993. 'Explicit instruction and input
processing.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition l5l2:2254I '
VanPatten,
B. and
'rWilliams, S. Rott, and M. Overstreet (eds.). 2004. FormVanPatten, B.,J.
Meaning Connections in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Vygotslcy, L. S. 1978. Mind in Society.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press.
'Wajnryb,
R.
Language
Te a c h ers
1992. Classroom Obseruation Tasks: A Resource Book for
and Tiain
e rs.
Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
'Watson'Gegeo, K. A.1992. 'Thick explanation in the ethnographic study
of child socialization: A longitudinal study of the problem of schooling for
Kwara'ae (Solomon Islands) children in \( A. Corsaro and P J. Miller
(eds.): Interpretiae Approaches to Childrenls Socialization. Special issue of New
D ire ctio n fo r C h i ld D eue loprnent 58 ; 5 | -66.
225
225
Bibliography
'wells, G. 1986.
children Learning Language and using Language to Learn.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
'wertsch,
J. v 1985. vygotsky and the social Formation ofMind. cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press.
'wesche,
M. B. 1981. 'Language aptitude measures in streaming, matching
students with methods, and diagnosis of learning problems' in K. Dillei
Gd.): Indiuidual Dffirences and [Jniuersak in Laigiage Learning Aptitude.
Rowley, MA: Newbury House. pp. 119-39.
'w'esche,
\fhite, J. 1998. 'Getting the learners' attention: A rypographical input
enhancement study' in c. Doughry andJ. \filliams (.ds.i, For), on Form in
Ckssroom SLA. Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry press. pp. g5-113.
J. and P. M. Lightbown. 1984. fuking and answering in
ESL
'white, L.,19s7.
Against comprehensible input: The input hypothesis and
the development of secondJanguage cornperence.'- srroid Language
ResearchT l2: 133-61.
'white
L. 1989. (Jniaersal Grammar and second Language Acquisition.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
L. r99l. Adverb placement in second language acquisition: Some
oJ^.' slrrond. Language
ksearch712: t33-61.
\7hite, L.2003a. second Language Acquisition and [Jniuersal Grammar
Press.
\vhite, L. 2003b.'on the nature of interlanguage
representation' in c. J.
Doughty and M. H._Long (eds.): The Hindbooh-of second Languig,
Acquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 20-42.
'White,
L.,
N. Spada,
P.
M. Lightbown, and L. Ranta. 1991. 'Input
enhancemenr and syntactic accuracy in L2 acquisition.' Applied Linguitics
l2l4:416-32.
'wode,
E.
H. r97B. 'Developmental sequences in naturalisticL2acquisition in
M. Hatch (ed.): second LanguageAcquisition: A Book ofReadizgs. Rowrey,
MA: Newbury House. pp. 101-12.
\
(
\
It
A
\u
ti
Y
Y
in
L,
Z,
ol
Z,
sy
effects of positive and negative evidence in the classr
Cambridge: Cambridge Universiry
I
S
C
classes.' Canadian Modern Language Reuiew4012, ZZA-++.
'white,
l:
at
M. and r. s. Paribakht
(eds.). 1999. 'rncidentil. L2 vocabulary
acquisition: Theory, currenr research, and instructional implications.'
Studies in Second Language Acquisition2I12, Special Issue.
'White,
\
Bibliography
'Wode,
H. 1981. 'Language acquisitional universals: A unified view of
language acquisition in H.'$Tinitz (ed.): Natiue Language and Foreign
Language Acquisition. New York: Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences, YoL 37 9 : 218-34.
'W'ong,'W:
to the
C lassro
2005. Input Enhancement: From Theory and Research
om. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
'Wong-Fillmore,
acquisition
in
1979. 'Individual differences in second language
'W.-S.
Y. \Vang, and D. Kempler (eds.):
C. J. Fillmore,
L.
Indiuidual Dffirences in LanguageAbility and Language Behaziar NewYork:
Academic Press. pp. 203-28.
'Wong-Fillmore,
L. 1991. '\7hen learning a second language means losing
the first.' Early Chi ldhood Research Quarterfu 6 I 3: 32345.
Yorio, C. 1985. 'Consumerism in second language learning and teaching.'
Canadian Modern Language fuaiew 421 3: 658-87 .
Yule, G. and D. Macdonald. L990.'Resolving referential conficts
interaction: The effect
Learning 4014:539-56.
of
proficiency and interactive role.' Language
Zobl,H.19B0. 'The formal and developmental selectivity of
on L2 acquisition.' Language Learning 30 I I : 43-57 .
Znbl,H.
in L2
Ll
influence
1984. 'The wave model of linguistic change and interlanguage
systems.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 612: 160-85
.
227
INDEX
Entries are arranged in letter-byJetter alphabetical order, in which spaces between words are
ignored.
Glossary entries are shown by'g' after the page number.
21J,
69
accent69-70,106
child-directed
accessibiliry hi erarchy 9A, 164-5
accuracy order 83, l95g
acquisitionJearning hypothesis 36
action research 138, l95g
active listening 146-7, l95g
additive bilingualism 2 6, 19 5 g
adolescent learners 3 1, 61,72-3
childhood bilingualism 25-6
Child Language Data Exchange System
see
also
4-5,
first language 1-27, 1834
second langua ge 29-32, 58-9,
-8, l68J
0
t95g
6l-2
aptitude5T-9
attention 45
62-5
audiolingual approach 34, 112, 132,
t38-9, r40,143, t95g
see also formdaic language
clarification requests 44, 126, 127, 154
see
ahoPrcmpts
classroom comparisons I I 5-25
classroom ethn ography I 33-5
classroom lear ning 137 -82
classroom motivation 64-5
classroom observation 109-36
schemes lI4-32,1969
CLIL (content and language-integrated
Iearning) 132,155-6
closedquestions 132
CLI (communicative language teaching)
38,140, r4r, t96g
audiolingual pattern drills 14l*2
auditory discrimination 7 2, 19 5 g
'aural'learners 59
authentic materials I 13
auxiliaries, question formadon 7
cognates 99, 1969
cognitive development 19-20, 153
cognitive maturity 30-1, 1969
cognitivist app roach 3846, 1969
collaborative dialogues 48, 17 34
collaborative interaction I 53
communicative competence 96, I42,
avoidance82,94
awareness 45
metalinguistic 8-9, 26,
24,
chunks 5,6,93,1969
I 47
affective filter hypothesis 37
age of acquisition 57 -7 4, I 86-7
alcohol, effect on pronunciation 61
Ame rican Sign Language (ASL) l8-19,
aff itudes, lear ner s'
7
r86_7
choice motivation 64
affect3T
anxiety
19
(CHILDES) 21
prberty
9
peech
children
adult learners 30-1, 68-7 3
adverb placem ent
s
30-l
t969
babies' language
l,
behaviourism 10-15 , 34-5 , 49 , 93,
see abo ardiolingual approach
beliefs of learn
ers
communicative instruction 110, 113-14,
2
l95g
66-7
bilingual education | 55, 19 69
bilingualism 25-6, 134, 186, 1969
biological programming I 5-17
117-19, 147-8, r79
communicative language teaching
Communicative Orientation of Language
Teaching (COLT) 1 15, 126
brain activity 40
competence 19,36,1969
communicative 96, 142
CAH (Contrastive Analysis Hypothesis)
linguistic 142
competition model42-3
34,78-82,93,197g
caretaker talk rct child-directed speech;
modified input
(CLI)
38,140, 141.1969
competitive goals 65
comprehensible input 37 -8, 43, 48,
168, t97g
1
434,
230
Ind.ex
comprehensible output hypothesis 44, 48,
150, t97g
comprehension-based instruction
144-50,
176,197g
error analysis 79-82
erfor cofrection
first language 1 84-5
second language 32, 190-1, 192-3
classroom observation 1 10-13, 1 16,
comprehension checks 44
conditional mood 170
connectionism
234, 4l, l97g
see
content-based instruction 38,
125-8,
155-50,172, 1934, t97g
s ee als o immersion programmes
content and language-integrated learning
(cLtL) 132,155_6
119,120,124
abo'getit right from the beginning'
ethnic group affiliatio n 6 5 -6
ethnography 1334,199g
'European school' 155
executive modvation 64
experimental studies I 38, 1 42-3,
Contrastive Analysis Hypothesis (CAH)
explicit correction 126, 154
extroversion 53,60-l
conversational language 188-9
co-operative goals 65
corrective feedback 67, 125-30, 1 51,
168-70, 184, 190_t, 197 g
negative 154,162
felelaE4
also recast(s)
67-74, r98g
58-7 0,
I41J,
deafchildren 17-19
deaf parents 22
declarative knowledge 39-40, I 98g
descriptive studies 129, 152, 158, 19Bg
determiners, possessive 88-90, 148,
170
developmental errors 81, 1989
developmental features I 60, f98g
developmental sequences l_9, 46, 82-93,
inhii
field.independent/field dependent 59,
firstlanguage 199g
l99g
influence on second language Iearning
93-6,169-70, 186-7
6t
foreign language learning I 99g
form-focused insrruction 1 43, 1 65-7 5,
177-80,1999
Initi
inna
Set
inpu
tet
j"P"
TP"
lnPu
instn
instrr
formulaic language 7, 81, 2009
see
imn
i-p
f eedb ack s e e correetivb feedback
198g
ider
info
info
foreigner talk 32-3, l99g
Foreign Language Classroom Anxiery Scale
cross-cultural rcsearch 20-2
cross-sectional studies 3,
1
acquisition 1-27
correlation 54-7, 197 -8g
Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH) 12-19,
hab
hyp
imir
imn
1999
34,78-82,93, t97g
control grorp 142,1979
see
grat
glor
integ
also chsnl<s
fossilization 80, 200g
frequency 98-9
fronting, question formation 6, 86-7,
161
intell
intell
inren
interi
cla
function words 2009
eaI
95,1989
disorders and delay s 24-5
see also rate of development
dialects 9
disorders and delays 24-5
display questions 1 16, ll9_20, 124, 125,
130-1,1989
diversiry distinction from disabiliry 25
dominant-dominant, dominant-passive
interactions 1 53
drills sesaudiolingual pattern drills;
partern practice drills; substitution
drills
educational settings 56
elicitation 127
also
enhanced
eff
171-2
genuine questions 1 16, 119,120,124,
130-1,2009
disabiliry distinction from diversiry 25
see
gender
prompts
input 148, 199g
gestural (sign) language lB-19
'get it right from rhe beginnin g'
13843,
176
'get it right in the end' 165-75,177-8
'good language learners', characteristics
54-76
grammar-based instruction 67, 177
grammar rranslation
1
12, 138,
13940,
143,2009
grammatical co mplexity 7 2
grammadcaliry judgements 36, 70-2, 20W
grammatical markers I 8- 19, 42
grammatical morphemes 2, 34, 7 2, 8T5,
141-2,200g
mc
intera
Stera
nterfi
lterl:
.:rrerlz
:lrerk
i€€
t
:t\€rs.
:n\-estt
lQ(in
TI
(I
Index 231
grammatical rules 189
group work 150*5,191-2
judgements of grammaticality 36, 7 0-2
'just listen ... and read' 143-50,176
habits 10, 34, 35
hypotheses 2009
kinaesthetic Iearners 59
'knowledge how','knowled ge thar' 39-40
ldentity 65-6
imitation l0-15,1834
laboratory studies 49, 155
language acquisition 20I-29
immersion programmes 57, 125-7, 155-8,
language acquisition order
160,170-3,193,2009
immigrants, second language acquisition
65,58,69-72,145-6
language
implicit corrective feedback 155
information processing 39- 40, 200-I
g
information questions (genuine questions)
i 16, 1 19, r20, r24, 130-1
inhibition 51
Initiation/Response/Evaluation
(IR-E')
exchanges 112
innatism 15-19, 35-6, 49, 20 1 g
see a/so commlnicative language teaching
input 1 19-21, 124-5, 201g
see also comprehensible input; enhanced
input; modified input
input fiood 147-8
input hypothesis 37
input processing 45-6
instructional settings 109-14
instrumental motivation 64, 20 I g
integrative motivation 64, 20 1 g
see
developmental sequences
language disorders and delays 24-5
learning2}2g
theories 10-24,29-51
language proficiency tests 54-6
'ianguage socializatio i 2l -2
learner
learner
learner
iearner
learner
beliefs 66-7
characteristics
30-1, 53-76
inreracrion
interaction
sae
langua ge 77
-108
preferences 59
learning by talking 47 -8
learning conditions 31, 324
learning sryles 59-60
'lett talk 150-5,176-7
linguistic com petence 142
listening 143-50, t76
longitudinal studies 3, 133-4, 1.41-2, 153,
202g
intelligibility 106
intensive ESL 147-8, 161-2, 168-70, 20lg
markers 18-19,42
meaning J/r communicative language
teaching; meaning-based
instruction; negotiation of meaning
interaction
meaning-based instruction
intelligence 56, 57, 185
classroom observation I I 5-25
early childhood 22-3
effects on second language learning
150-5, 191-2
modifred434,190
interactionist/developmental perspective,
childhood lear ning 19 -24, 49
interaction(ist) hypothesis, second language
lear ning 434, I 50, | 5 l, 1 5 3,
154-5,20tg
interference see transfer
interlanguage 80, 9 4, 95, 20 1 g
interlanguage pragmatics 1 00-4
interlocutors 22, 32, 43, 44, 47, 20lg
see aho modifred input
inversion 7, 86,87,169
investment 66
IQ (intelligence quotient) 56, 57, 185
IRE (Initiation/Response/Evaluation)
exchanges
1
12
I43, I77,
202g
memorization 34-5
memory 58
metalinguistic awareness 8-9, 26, 30-1,
202g
metalinguistic comments and feedback
1t6, 120, 124, 125, t26-7
mimicry 34-5
see alsoimitation
mitigation 102,2029
Modern Language Aptitude Test (MLAT)
58
modified input 30, 32-3,114,2029
modified interaction 434, 190,2029
modified output 154
Monitor Model 36-8
morphemes, grammatical 2,
34,
t4r-2
mother tongue srefirst language
motivation 54, 56, 63-5, 185
7
2, 83-5,
232
Index
motivation retrospection 64
multiple intelligences 57
processing instruction
1
48-9, 2039
proficiency tesr.s 54-5
prompts 772-3
pronouns, refexive 16
si
native language saefirst language
nativelike ability 68, 2029
native speakers 19, 2029
pronunciation 61, 72, 104-7, 188
psychological theories serbehaviourism;
'natural experiments' 1 7-1 8
natural order hypothesis 37
nformarion processing
puberry Critical Period Hypothesis 69-70,
see
natural settings 109-14
negation 4-5,85-6
negative feedback 1 54, 162
negotiation of form 202g
see
also Form-focused
si
i
also developmental sequences
71
instrucrion
168-70
834, 2039
sequences
sc
so
sti
sti
childrenk 507
st(
teachers'130-2
stl
rate of development 17, 18,2049
see abo cognitle development
rate oflearntngT2-3
su
tolearn7624
su
readiness
reading 8-9, 100, 143-50, 176, 188
recast(s) 21, 125-9, 155, 162-3, 172-3,
192-3,2049
pairwork 150-5,191-2
paraphrase 44
parent-child interaction 21-2, 26, 184
77-8,91-2
tas
tea
registers 9,2049
relative clauses 90-1
personal characteristics of learners
risk-taking61,94
53-76
restructuring 40
routines (formulaic language) 7, 81
scaffolding 131,2049
school years 8-9
second language2049
second language learning
explanations/theories 29-5
factors affectin g 53-7 6, I
in classroom 109_-82
tea
tea
repetitlon 44, 127 , 139
s e e a ls o imitation; promprs
requests 102-3
personality 53, 60-3
phonology 704-7
Pimsleur Language Apritude Battery
(PLAB) 58
pofuglor sauant60
positive reinforcement 1 0
possessive determiners 88-90, 148,
170
power relationships 65-6
practice 1 0-1 5
see also communicative practice; pattern
practice drills
pragmatics 100-4
pre-school years 7-8
private speech 47, 128, 2039
procedural knowledge 39-40, 203 g
tal
tat
1-2
9
pattern pracrice drills 1 39, 141, 2039
perceptually-based learning sryles 59
performance 36,2039
J0-1,
su
referential questions (genuine questions)
116, 1 19, 120, 124, 130-l
reflexive pronouns 16
reference to past
overgeneralization errors 15, Bl , 2039
processabiliry 46, 1 65
sc
sf.r
classroom 109-36
schemes 114-32
open questions 132
order of acquisition sra developmental
past tense
s(
SC
st(
questions
noticing44-5, 173-4
obligatory con texts
observation
sl
sc
qualitative research 62, 137 -8, 2039
quantitative research 62, 137 ,2039
question formation 86-8, 160-2, 163,
negotiation of meaning 44,116, 120,124,
125, 150-1,2039
neurological research 49
si
si
85
1
-9 4
individual diff er ences 5 3-7 6
learner langua ge 77 -108
segmentals 104,204g
self-repetition 44
sensitive period (Critical Period
Hypothesis) 17 -19, 67 -7 4
sequential bilinguals 25
settings 109-14
short-term memory 58
Index
'telegraphic' sentences, babies' 2
significant difG rences 2A4g
sign language 1 8-1 9
TPR (total physical response) 145-7
simpiification 81,204g
transfer 35,93,2059
s e e ah o Contrustive Analysis Hypothesis
transfer appropriate processing 40
transfer errors 82,94
'two for one' 155-60,176
simultaneous bilinguals 25
skill learning 39-40
social dynamics 65-6
social factors 65-6
social settings 56
sociocultural theory 34, 47 -8, 49, 131,
2049
sociolinguistic forms 171
sociopolitical change
13
Universal Grammar (UG) 15, 35-6,
205g
uptake 125, 127, 128-9, 2059
4-5
developmental sequences
standard variery 9, 2049
story compreh ensionT2
storltelling 72
structural grading 1 46, 2049
variables
stages sea
structure-based instruction
1
09-l
54-7,2059
personaliry 62
variational features 46, 160, 205 g
0,
rt2-13, t16-t7, t41, r43
substitution driils 205g
subtractive bilingualism 26, 186, 205g
suprasegmentals 104, 1 05, 205 g
talking, learning by 47-8
target languag es 34, 9 3, 205 g
task-based instruction 1 10, 147-8, 150,
174-5,205g
teacher taik (foreigner talk) 32-3
teaching, classroom 109-36
'teach what is teachable' 160-5. 177
vartery 9,205-69
verb forms 77-8,91-2, 172
'visual'learners 59
vocabulary 8, 9, 7 2, 9 6-100,
1
45-6, 188
wait time 132
' wlt-words' 5-6, 7, 86, 87, 93
wild'children 17-18
willingness to communicate 62
wordorder 42-3,94-5
working memory 58, 2069
'wug test'4
zone of proximal development (ZPD) 20,
47
, 48,2069
233
OXJ.ORD
UNIVERSITY PRESS
www.oup.com/elt
OXFORD ENGLISH
tsBN 978-0- 1 9-442224-6
l|[illlililllil[ffill