Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
This report evolved from contributions to the Society of Existential Analysis conference in London " Being and Doing " November, 2015. I briefly describe opening the conference with a guided Focusing session since this practice is fundamental to my report on the other contribution, a Panel Discussion entitled " Being at the Heart of Activism ". I was honoured to be joined on this panel by Alison Playford, Mark Weaver and Luke Flegg, all involved in social/political activism and developments in participatory democracy. What follows is an account of my own interests in this area as an existential psychologist and Focusing therapist. I present Focusing as a source of democratic process that forms an experiential continuity from 'within' each person 'outwards' to interpersonal and community situations. For this publication I have incorporated some references to Eugene Gendlin's (1987) A philosophical critique of the concept of narcissism: the significance of the awareness movement. Keeping with the ethos of the approach described below, this report is presented in first-person language to make it as direct and accessible as possible. Focusing To open the conference I was invited to offer a brief 10-minute Focusing (Gendlin, 1981) session. Focusing is a phenomenological practice of embodied self-awareness whereby a person can access and follow the unfolding of feelings that step-by-step help to clarify our experience of anything we are living through. At professional conferences we typically interact only from the eye-brain-thinking level of human experience. So I wanted to offer a different starting point, an intention to include all the implicit responses that are simultaneously happening at the body level but that we typically don't " drop down inside " and attend to. At that level we can sense more than is easily said and more than is usually included in thoughtful dialogues between colleagues. This is an excerpt from the last minute of the Focusing experience: " So as we begin our day together, just know that this whole responsive world is down there, available to you. Your body will generate all sorts of responses as you listen to people during the day, pause and notice the feelings that come in here-even the subtle feelings-there is creative thinking in the body. If an idea is making you feel a bit constricted inside, or a presentation brings an expansive feeling in your body, consider dropping down again and checking " what is it about this idea or presentation that makes me feel just like this? " Let the answer come from your body. If you can understand something from the feeling, you will usually feel a bit of release inside. Then consider sharing what came to you at some point during the conference, it might also resonate for others. If you speak, you might let words come from the body as you continue to attend to the feeling, sensing with each word " am I saying this right " ? And correcting yourself as you speak, so that what you say resonates bodily. This may be one way of inviting unformed 1
This report evolved from contributions to the 2015 Society of Existential Analysis conference in London. I briefly describe opening the conference with a guided Focusing session since this practice is fundamental to my report on the other contribution, a Panel Discussion entitled " Being at the Heart of Activism ". What follows is an account of my own interests in this area as an existential psychologist and Focusing therapist. I present Focusing as a source of democratic process that forms an experiential continuity from 'within' each person 'outwards' to interpersonal and community situations. For this publication I have incorporated some references to Eugene Gendlin's (1987) A philosophical critique of the concept of narcissism. Keeping with the ethos of the approach described below, this report is presented in first-person language to make it as direct and accessible as possible.
Over the last 15 years or so, my major interest has been in trying to spell out in some detail, what life is like for us from within the “interactive moment,” what it is like for us to be acting dialogically, to be reacting in a spontaneous and bodily way to the expressive-responsive activities of the others and othernesses around us. Today, however, I want to focus on just one aspect of our lives within such dialogical involvements: what is life like for us as speaker-actors within them? What is going on inside us as speaker-actors within the moment of speaking and acting? How might we chart or map the ‘inner movements’ occurring within us as we give ‘shape’ to the contributions we make to the activities in which we are participating?
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 2020
The analyst’s embodied attunement and participation arises within an embodied analytic relationship. Understanding this “deep structure” of the interaction and attention to this level of interaction opens up new modes of engagement and therapeutic action. The importance of embodied attunement is supported by recent research and theories that the developing mind is shared and dialogical through bodily communication, by rhythms of cadence, tone, intensity, and movement. The analyst’s embodied awareness of two bodies together and their interpersonal rhythm is the “tool” used to gauge the pulse, vitality of connection, and particular rhythmic qualities of a uniquely shared world. This provides a read on the most elemental way the dyad shares emotional experience (or fails to). The analyst’s embodied participation is interpretation in another mode. Clinical examples illustrate how embodied attunement and intentional participation work in the session, and their therapeutic effect. Failure...
The paper traces many mental phenomena to vocal aspects of sensorimotor empathy (Chemero, 2016). It is argued that synergies not only contribute to action but also to presence and, more strangely, how a sense of presence serves in dealing with others. In support of the view, we offer three contrasting cases. First, we discuss imagining a voice on receiving a text. Not only are such reports common but, as described, it marks a change in attitude or, precisely, the trajectory of the texting. Next we defend the claim that presence is grounded in sensorimotor empathy. Accordingly, we appeal to concerted movement in our second example. We sketch how staff use a sense of presence to reposition themselves around a hospital bed. Since no imagining is required, this is likely to be synergetic. Pursuing the argument, the third case pursues details in family conversation. Using acoustic evidence from 750 ms, we trace the particular sense of a wording (" ah bene ") to the energetics of intermeshing voices. Finally, we claim that the evidence supports, not extended mind, but, rather, what we call a systemic view of cognition. When people participate in extended cognitive systems, they do not act/perceive as organisms (or predictive brains). Rather, as human agents, they also draw on dispositions that result from a history of using both things (material engagement) and synergies that grant a sense of presence, and sensorimotor empathy. Skills in using social and solo presence enable persons to individuate and display as living human subjects. 1.0 Introducing presence Turning from neuro-centric views and theories based on a concept of mind, we pursue how voices and voicing contribute to human agency. In so doing, we adopt Chemero's (2009) methodological principle that all human powers derive from body-world interactions. The radical embodied perspective opens up how voices influence actions as people draw on presence and the sense of presence. As the epigraph suggests, when people exude presence, they feel open and vulnerable (Merleau-Ponty, 1973). Consequently, what happens is influenced by sensorimotor experience (e.g. in conversing). Cultural products evoke a 'system of relations' (ibid.) that affect what those concerned are likely to do, think and feel. In experience of coordination, they link 'what is present and what is absent' (Deacon, 2012: 506). Voices enable human agents to manage presence by bringing social coordination together with material engagement (Malafouris, 2013). The experienced world connects bodies, brains and things through a meshwork of linguistic practices. In linking the material with the routine, human agency becomes spatially and temporally multi-scalar. A person's cognitive powers derive from a history of social coordination. Given sensitivity to voices, hands and faces, experience ensures that events are shaped by a sense of presence. As people coordinate, they are constrained by affect, articulation and, above all, changing perceptions of other persons. Through presence, acting-in-the world is affected by relationships, language-activity and, in slow scales, what persons and groups become. In presenting the view around case studies, we begin with a media-enriched setting where texting re-evokes experience. We then sketch what Goffman (1963) calls 'co-presence' to trace the powers to talk in interaction. Next, we turn up the microscope to show how unthought judgements shape lived relationships. By implication, powers often ascribed to cognition use what Chemero (2016) calls sensorimotor empathy. People use bodily and vocal interplay to re-enact a collective order. We conclude by linking our observations to work on cognitive systems, extended mind, and a systemic view of cognition.
Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy (ANZJFT)
Critique d'art, 2016
In our talk of meanings, we are used to thinking of them as working in terms of mental representations, and to thinking of such representations as passive objects of thought requiring interpretation in terms of shared rules, conventions or principles if their meaning is to be understood. Here, however, I argue that the meaningfulness of our language does not initially depend on its systematicity, but on our spontaneous , living, bodily responsiveness to the others and othernesses around us. Hence, I want to explore the realm of expressive-responsive bodily activity that 'pre-dates', so to speak, the 'calculational' processes we currently think of as underlying our linguistic understandings, the realm within which direct and immediate, non-interpretational physiognomic or gestural forms of understanding can occur. Central to activities occurring between us in this sphere is the emergence of dynamically unfolding structures of activity in which we all participate in 'shaping', but to which we all must also be responsive in giving shape to our own actions. It is the agentic influence of these invisible but nonetheless felt 'real presences' (Steiner, 1989) that I want to explore. Their influence can be felt as acting upon us in a way similar to the expressions of more visible beings. Thus within this sphere of physiognomic meanings, it is as if invisible but authoritative others can directly 'call' us into action, can issue us with 'action-guiding advisories', and judge our subsequent actions accordingly with their 'facial' expressions or 'tones' of voice. Below I will explore how this—some would say, 'mystic' (Lévy-Bruhl)—form of participatory thought and understanding can help us to understand the 'inner' nature of our social lives together, and the part played by our expressive talk in their creation.
British Gestalt Journal
Academia Letters, 2021
Pipe Roll Society lecture, 2024
diacritics, 2023
M. Ölmez/T. Karaayak (eds.), Türkçe ve Moğolca Çalışmaları Semposyumu: Sir G. Clauson ve Talat Tekin’in katkıları, 2022
Management and Leadership Skills for Medical Faculty and Healthcare Executives
ЗБОРНИК НАРОДНОГ МУЗЕЈА СРБИЈЕ Recueil du Musée national de Serbie, 2023
Germinal - Revista de Estudios Libertarios, n° 17, 2024.
min - Minimally Invasive Neurosurgery, 2005
Pattern Analysis & Applications, 2002
Brazilian Journal of Health Review, 2021
Nature Genetics, 2008
International Journal of Precision Farming
Solid State Communications, 1969