Competition or Cooperation:
Cultural Perspectives on N-Effect and
Proximity-to-a-Standard
M.A. Y.D. Madurapperuma, University o f Colombo, Sri Lanka
Kim Kvung-min, Silla University, Korea
Pradeep Dharmadasa, University o f Colombo, Sri Lanka
With the escalating globalization of business
activities, the issue of how business organizations
adapt to cultural differences in host countries
and deal with cross-cultural management prac
tices becomes increasingly important for human
resource and marketing managers. The failure
to take cultural differences into account has
been :he cause of many businesses’ lack of suc
cess across national borders (Miroshnik, 2002;
Ricks, 2006). Management practices, strate
gies, structures, as well as technologies that are
suitable in one culture may lead to undesirable
consequences in another (Miroshnik, 2002). For
example, task-driven managerial practices may
deliver expected outcomes in countries where
the culture promotes competitiveness, whereas
it would not be the case in countries where a
culture encourages cooperativeness. This study
uses the theories of N-effect and proximity-to-astandard to capture the degree of competitiveness
and cooperation among individuals of different
cultural settings of the chosen countries. The Neffect is the discovery that increasing the number
of competitors (N) can decrease competitive
motivation (Garcia and Tor, 2009) meaning that
individuals behave more competitively when the
number of competitors is small. Proximity-to-astandard specifies that individuals behave more
competitively when they perceive themselves to
be close to a reference standard.
An organization’s culture cannot be separated
from the national culture of its country. Often,
firms involved in international business (multina
tional and transnational firms) attempt to adapt
their operations in foreign lands to the local
culture (Miroshnik, 2002). Most economic, man
agement, and social theories provide insights
into the ways in which firms sustain their com
petitive advantage in culturally diverse business
environments. They provide ways of promoting
a competitive environment among businesses
and departments as well as between co-workers
of a same organization. Hofstede (1980) identi
fied four variants to work and national cultures,
namely individualism-collectivism(I-C), power
distance, uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity/femininity. Fie also pointed out that national
cultures demonstrate more work-related values
and attitudes, so that work practices are differ
ent. However, as economic and organizational
practices evolve, the validity of cultural theories
may no longer apply across time and places
(Miroshnik, 2002). Social changes linked to
economic development and urbanization, for
example, bring about cultural changes that allow
individuals not only to be free from traditional
sources of social influence, but also to be more
autonomous in their decision-making. This pro
cess of modernization causes firms to confront
many operational issues, even within their own
country. It emphasizes the importance of vigi
lance and staying abreast of cultural changes.
As mentioned, the consequences of cultural
relativity on management practices should be
considered seriously by firms operating across
cultures. Individuals from individualist or col
lectivist work culture have different attitudes
towards task-driven assumptions of competition.
For instance, marketers may assume that the
most effective way of increasing sales volume
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
47
is to encourage competition among co-workers
rather than through cooperative efforts. This
task-driven managerial assumption is more likely
to work in individualistic work cultures, where
competitiveness is highly regarded, than in col
lectivist work cultures, where cooperation is
valued.
We conducted Study 1 to test the theories
that explain competitive behavior of individuals
(i.e., N-effect and proximity-to-a-standard) in
individualistic and collectivist cultural settings.
We obtained samples from the U.S., Korea,
and China, three countries with diverse cultural
orientations and stages of economic advance
ment. Broadly speaking, much prior research
(Hui, 1988; Kitayama et al., 2009; Triandis et
al., 1986; Triandis and Gelfand, 1998; Uleman
and Lee, 1996) suggests that the U.S. is char
acterized by an individualistic culture whereas
Korea and China are more collectivist. Based
on cultural modernization theory (Hamamura,
2012), it could also be said that the collectiv
ist orientations of Korean and Chinese cultures
differ from each other. To extend the cultural
and economic diversity of this study, we added
Sri Lanka. However, without reference to prior
country classification of cultural orientation, we
investigated the cultural orientation of four se
lected countries to better understand the cultural
bases of business decision-making.
This study had three objectives. First, we
wished to reinvestigate the cultural orientations
of the U.S., Korea, and China while unveiling
Sri Lanka’s cultural orientation. Second, we
were interested in investigating the relationship
between individualism-collectivism and
competitiveness across the sample at a national
level. Finally, we sought to ascertain how
individuals from individualist and collectivist
cultures respond to competitive events when
rivals are influenced by proximity-to-a-standard
(Garcia, Tor, and Richard, 2006; Garcia and Tor,
2007) and the number of competitors is small or
large (N-effect) (Garcia and Tor. 2009).
In the remainder of this paper we first
review and discuss the literature pertaining
to cross-cultural management, individualismcollectivism, and individual competitive efforts
and cooperative efforts. We then develop
hypotheses. Next, we present two studies
designed to test our hypotheses before reporting
on their results and findings. Finally, the
paper concludes with a theoretical discussion
about contribution, practical implications, and
directions for further research.
48
Literature Review and Theoretical Back
ground
Cultural diversity and management practices
Businesses that operate across national
boundaries may be confronted with more legal,
economic, and political issues linked to cultural
diversity than those operating solely in one
country. In spite of high local responsiveness,
the analysis of problems and failures of
multinationals’ operations abroad reveals that
culture is a critical factor in failure (Miroshnik,
2002). This phenomenon emphasizes the need
for a close and continuous look at local cultural
practices irrespective of businesses’ levels of
operations (international, global, multinational
or transnational).
Broadly speaking, culture can be defined as
common patterns of beliefs, assumptions, values,
and norms of behavior of groups represented by
societies, institutions, and organizations (Aycan
et al., 2000). Much prior research has showed
how the culture of a country influences business
operations and management practices. Accord
ing to Aycan et al.’s (1999) model of culture fit,
socio-cultural environments affect internal work
cultures, including both employee-related and
task-driven assumptions. Specifically, human
resource management (HRM) practices are
affected by socio-cultural factors (e.g., paternal
ism, power distance, masculinity/femininity,
uncertainty avoidance, loyalty toward commu
nity, and self-reliance) and are associated with
employee-related assumptions. Nevertheless, the
influence of workplace cultures on HRM prac
tices that derived from competitive orientation
(task-driven assumption) cannot be discounted.
Vadi and Vereshagin (2006) concluded that
Russian business organizations had not been
able to reap the expected benefits from Western
HRM strategies because their organizational
culture was influenced by the collectivist Rus
sian national culture. Hence, national culture, to
a greater extent, influences the human behavior
of business firms, which, in turn, challenges the
way employees are managed.
With regard to marketing management, Miro
shnik (2002) reported that problems arose when
marketing staff implement campaigns devel
oped in another country without adopting it to
the local culture. For instance, Arabic people
are unlikely to purchase food packaged with an
image of a naked woman or an animal even if
it is a reproduction of renowned art. Also, Japa
nese people tend to buy shampoo advertised by
young beautiful Japanese women rather than
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
brands with ads using young beautiful Euro
pean wcmen. Indeed, cultural dimensions, such
as individualism-collectivism, power distance,
uncertainty avoidance, and masculinity-feminin
ity, affect international relationship marketing
management. Samaha et al. (2014) found that
the magnitude of the individualism effect was
75% greater on relationship marketing than were
other dimensions, and masculinity had no ef
fect on relationship marketing. Kale and Barnes
(1992) emphasized the need to take national and
organization cultures and personality factors into
consideration, proposing a general framework
for cross-national personal selling. In a similar
fashion, Steenkamp (2001) stated that failing to
take cultural relativity between countries into
account was the reason for many business fail
ures, and that marketing research needed to pay
better attention to understanding cross-cultural
environments.
Much cross-cultural research has attempted
to identify management issues and problems
pertaining to national and work cultures and to
determine generic practices to be followed or
avoided by expatriates in performing their tasks.
This body of research found no single criterion
for managerial effectiveness. Expatriates work
ing in a competitive business environment tend
to apply competitive oriented HRM and market
ing practices wherever they are. For example,
top sales agents are provided with more train
ing and more back-office resources (Farrell and
Hakstian, 2001) and are allowed to use forced
ranking (i.e., ranking to scale employee per
formance relative to their peers) (Garcia and
Tor, 2007). As expected, positive outcomes are
achieved if managerial and task-related assump
tions comply with employee-related assump
tions. Conversely, if managerial and task-related
assumptions do not fit employee-related as
sumptions, work-related outcomes will not be
optimum. For instance, increasing sales volume
by promoting competition between sales repre
sentatives will be challenging in a work culture
where employees value cooperative efforts.
In that context, employees’ attitudes towards
competitiveness at work is a decisive factor in
determining the success of human resources and
marketing practices. There is, however, a paucity
of research examining the competitive behavior
of individuals across cultures. The outcomes of
such research would help address managerial
issues pertaining to individual performance. The
study presented here sought to bridge this gap by
investigating the effects of individualist and col
lectivist work cultures on employees’ responses
to competition or cooperation in four different
countries.
Cross-cultural research in social sciences,
business, and management has identified several
key dimensions that capture the complexity and
diversity of cultures. Steenkamp (2001) reported
that the four cultural dimensions of autonomy
vs. collectivism, egalitarians vs. hierarchy,
mastery vs. nurturance, and uncertainty
avoidance adequately captured the complexity
of cultures. In the same vein, Hofstede (1980)
looked to national culture to explain workrelated values and attitudes, arguing that
employees’ attitudes varied according to the
following factors: individualism-collectivism,
power distance, uncertainty avoidance,
and masculinity. A closer look at different
measurements of cultural dimensions reveals
that measurements are more convergent than
divergent. Samaha et al.’s (2014) four cultural
dimensions for meta-analysis indicates that
most prior research has used at least one of
those dimensions to evaluate how ingrained
cultural differences influence the effectiveness
of relationship marketing strategies. Their work
as well as that of Triandis et al. (1986) also
highlighted that a substantial body of research
has frequently used individualism-collectivism
(IC) as a variable in identifying cultural
differences and measuring and comparing
different cultural groups.
Based on measurements developed in
various disciplines (Chen and West, 2008;
Singelis, et al., 1995; Triandis and Gelfand,
1998), countries — and the individuals of
that country — can be classified, in a broader
sense, as either individualistic or collectivistic
societies. Kitayama et al. (2009) measured
IC against five elements (dispositional bias,
focused vs. holistic attention, experience of
disengaging vs. engaging emotions, personal
vs. social happiness, and relative self-size),
identifying cross-cultural differences among
four countries (the U.S., the U.K., Germany, and
Japan). Their results revealed that Americans
are mostly individualistic whereas Japanese are
mostly collectivistic. British and Germans are
also individualistic, but to a lesser extent than
Americans. Triandis et al. (1986) concluded
that European countries and the U.S. were
highly individualistic —with the Netherlands
scoring the highest of all nine countries
studied— whereas Asian countries, like India
and Indonesia, were the least individualistic.
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
49
In addition, India was classified as a vertical
collectivist culture with high power distance
(Hofstede, 1980; Triandis, 1995). Numerous
other cross-cultural studies have also found the
U.S. to be an individualistic country whereas
Korea and China were found to be collectivist
(Hofstede, 1980; Hui, 1988; Triandis and
Gelfand, 1998; Uleman and Lee, 1996).
At an individual level, individualist and col
lectivist attitudes are not mutually exclusive
(Triandis et al., 1986). Some of the studies
suggest that individuals may hold a combination
of IC attitudes. Further, individualist and col
lectivist attitudes can be activated as a function
of social context and social relations (Hui 1988).
Hui and Triandis (2011) and others (Moor
man and Blakely 1995) argue that IC measures
compare individual differences and not cultural
differences.
Variation of individualism and collectivism
There are different kinds of IC, and variation
among individualist and collectivist (Green
et al., 2005; Singelis et al., 1995), which can
be typified as horizontal or vertical depending
on the level of inequality or equality among
members of cultural groups (Singelis et al.,
1995) or selves - “horizontal patterns assuming
that one self is more or less like every other
self. By contrast, vertical patterns consist of
hierarchies, and one self is different from other
selves.” (Triandis and Gelfand, 1998). These two
relative patterns combine with IC to create four
types of patterns or individual characteristics of
a given society, defined by Triandis and Gelfand
(1998) as:
Horizontal individualism (HI) refers to
people who want to be unique and distinct from
groups and are highly self-reliant. However,
such people are not interested in becoming
‘distinguished’ or acquiring a high social sta
tus. Vertical individualism (VI) refers to people
who often want to become ‘distinguished’ and
acquire high status. For this purpose, they en
gage in individual competition with others.
Horizontal collectivism (HC) refers to people
who tend not to exercise authority, because
they perceive themselves as equal to others and
emphasize common goals. In addition, they are
sociable and dependent. Vertical collectivism
(VC) refers to people who are willing to sacri
fice their personal goals for the sake of in-group
goals, emphasizing the integrity of the in-group.
They support the competition of their in-group
with out-groups and submit to the will of the
50
in-group authorities for the betterment of their
group.
Having said that, although cultures consist of
static elements (i.e., cultural heritage theory), they
are mostly dynamic (i.e., modernization theory)
(Hamamura, 2012). According to modernization
theory, individualism is a trend that can be ex
pected to occur across societies, as it is seen as a
consequence of economic growth. With growth,
increasing cross-cultural contacts, through mass
and social media (e.g., flow of events, images, and
other information from one culture to other), tour
ism, traveling, and immigration, provide evidence
of an emerging global culture characterized by
modernity, technology, freedom, and individual
choice. In that context, the remarkable economic
growth recorded by the Korean and Chinese econ
omies during the past three decades or so suggests
that we can expect those countries to start exhibit
ing individualistic characteristics. This is a slow
process however, as evidenced by Cha’s (as cited
in Uleman and Lee, 1996) work showing that
even though individualism is growing in Korea,
it remains a collectivist country. This finding is
consistent with the cultural heritage theory that
emphasizes the role of cultural heritage in shaping
the course of a society’s cross-temporal change.
On that basis, we can expect China and Korea to
have dominant collectivist characteristics, com
pared with the U.S.
Sri Lanka is a South Asian country that has
been under the Indian cultural influence for
several centuries due to their geographical prox
imity. Because of this, and based on the argu
ment of modernization theory, we can expect
Sri Lankan culture to demonstrate high col
lectivist features—even higher than Korean and
Chinese—placing Sri Lanka at one end of the
spectrum, the U.S. at the other, and Korean and
Chinese cultures in the middle.
In this context, we draw attention first to the
nature of the U.S., China, and Korea’s cultural
orientations and unveil that of Sri Lanka. We
postulate that Americans demonstrate relatively
higher vertical and horizontal individualistic
characteristics than individuals in Korea
and China. We also argue that Sri Lankans
demonstrate more vertical and horizontal
collectivist characteristics than Koreans and
Chinese. Accordingly, we hypothesized that:
HI a: Americans demonstrate more
individualistic (vertical and horizontal)
characteristics than Koreans, Chinese and Sri
Lankans.
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
Sri Lankans demonstrate more
collectivist characteristics (vertical and
horizontal) than Americans, Koreans and
Chinese.
H lb :
Individual characteristic and competition
T h o u g h c o m p e titiv e n e s s is a n im p o r ta n t
p e r s o n a li ty tr a i t th a t in flu e n c e s a r a n g e o f s o c ia l
b e h a v io r , n o t e v e ry s o c ie ty o r n a tiv e o f a g iv e n
c u ltu r e p o s s e s s e s th e s a m e le v e l o f a ttitu d e
to w a r d s c o m p e titiv e n e s s (H o u s t o n e t a l., 2 0 1 2 ).
A n in d i v id u a l’s d e g r e e o f c o m p e tit iv e n e s s
is s h a p e d b y p e r s o n a li ty a n d s o c io - c u ltu r a l
f a c to r s . T h a t is, h o w p e o p le w a n t to fe e l ( “ id e a l
e f f e c t” ) is in f lu e n c e d b y c u ltu r e th r o u g h c h ild
r e a r in g , c h i ld r e n ’s lite ra tu re , in te r p e r s o n a l
c o m m u n ic a tio n , a n d re li g io n (T a s i, 2 0 0 7 ).
C u lt u r e a n d s o c ia l p s y c h o lo g y lite r a tu r e re v e a l
th a t s o m e c u ltu r e s a re m o r e c o m p e titiv e th a n
o th e rs . F o r in s ta n c e , A m e r ic a n s tu d e n ts a re m o re
c o m p e titiv e th a n B a lin e s e , J a p a n e s e , o r C h in e s e
s tu d e n ts (H o u s t o n e t a l., 2 0 1 2 ) . In g e n e r a l, m e n
a re m o r e c o m p e titiv e th a n w o m e n in A m e r ic a n ,
J a p a n e s e , a n d C h in e s e c u ltu r e s (H o u s to n e t
a l., 2 0 0 5 ), e s p e c ia ll y in a to u r n a m e n t o r w h e n
in p r o x im ity - to - a - s ta n d a r d (V a n d e g rift a n d
H o la d a v , 2 0 1 2 ). I n a c o lle c tiv is t- o r ie n te d c u ltu r e ,
te a m p e r f o r m a n c e is e m p h a s iz e d o v e r in d iv id u a l
p e r f o r m a n c e . In o th e r w o rd s , in d iv id u a l is m
is e x p e c te d to r e d u c e p ro d u c ti v it y m o r e th a n
c o lle c tiv is m ( K ir k m a n a n d S h a p iro , 2 0 0 1 ).
C o n s id e r in g IC a s s e p a r a t e v a ria b le s , H o u s to n
e t al. ( 2 0 1 2 ) f o u n d th a t w h e r e a s in d i v id u a l is m
w a s p o s itiv e ly c o r r e la te d w ith c o m p e titio n
(g e n e ra l, h y p e r a n d h e a lth y ) , c o lle c tiv is m
w a s n e g a tiv e ly a n d p a r ti a ll y c o r r e la te d w ith
c o m p e titio n . D e s p ite th is e v id e n c e s u p p o r tin g
th e c o n s tr u c tio n o f th re e s e p a r a te c o m p o n e n ts
o f in d iv id u a lis m a n d c o lle c tiv is m , C h e n a n d
W e s t c o n c lu d e d th a t C h in e s e p e o p le w e r e o n
th e w h o le le s s u n iq u e b u t m o r e in d e p e n d e n t a n d
c o m p e titiv e th a n A m e r ic a n s . S u c h d iv e rg e n t
fin d in g s s tre s s th e n e e d to f u r th e r in v e s tig a te
c u ltu r a l re la tiv ity a n d c o m p e titiv e n e s s .
W e e x p e c t in d iv id u a ls f r o m r e la tiv e ly h ig h
in d i v id u a l is tic c o u n tr ie s to d e m o n s t r a te m o r e
c o m p e titiv e b e h a v io r th a n th o s e fr o m c o lle c t iv is t
c o u n t rie s . M o r e o v e r, r e g a r d le s s o f th e d e g r e e
o f c u ltu r a l o r ie n ta ti o n o f in d iv id u a l c o u n tr ie s
(th e U .S ., K o re a , C h in a , a n d S ri L a n k a ) , v e rtic a l
a n d h o r iz o n ta l in d iv id u a l is m c o r r e la te s m o re
p o s itiv e ly w ith c o m p e titiv e b e h a v io r th a n d o e s
v e r tic a l a n d h o riz o n ta l c o lle c tiv is m . T h e r e fo re ,
w e h y p o th e s iz e d th a t:
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
Vertical and horizontal individualism
correlates more positively with competitive
behavior than does vertical and horizontal
collectivism, irrespective o f country.
H 2:
O v e r tim e , c o m p e titiv e b e h a v io r h a s b e e n an
a ttra c tiv e s u b je c t f o r e c o n o m ic s s c h o la rs . E v e n
th o u g h th e ir m a jo r c o n c e rn h a s b e e n to in v e s tig a te
th e c o m p e titiv e b e h a v io r o f c o n s u m e rs , firm s,
in d u s trie s , a n d c o u n trie s , n o rm a tiv e th e o rie s (e.g .,
g a m e th e o ry , c o n te s t th e o ry , a n d to u rn a m e n t
th e o ry ) h a v e fo c u s e d o n in v e stig a tin g th e c o m
p e titiv e b e h a v io r o f e m p lo y e e s . C o n c o m ita n tly ,
in th e d o m a in o f s o c ia l re s e a rc h , th e c o m p e titiv e
b e h a v io r o f in d iv id u a ls h a s b e e n e x p la in e d b y
s o c ia l c o m p a ra tiv e th e o ry . M o re o v e r, s o c ia l c o m
p a ra tiv e th e o ris ts c la im th a t p e o p l e w h o c o m p a r e
th e m s e lv e s to o th e rs in d ic a te s w h e th e r s o m e o n e
b e h a v e s c o m p e titiv e ly to w a rd s o th e rs (G a rc ia a n d
T or, 2 0 0 7 ). T h is n o tio n p a v e s th e w a y to o th e r
d e s c rip tiv e th e o rie s , lik e p ro x im ity -to - a -s ta n d a rd
a n d N -e ffe c t, th a t f o c u s n a rro w ly o n in d iv id u a l
c o m p e titiv e n e s s . In th is s tu d y , c o m p e titio n is
u n d e rs to o d as th e b e h a v io r o f in d iv id u a ls a n d
e m p lo y e e s v y in g f o r lim ite d s u p p o rt, re s o u rc e s ,
o r p ro m o tio n (R id lo n a n d S h in , 2 0 1 3 ) w ith o u t
c o o p e ra tio n (G a rc ia e t al., 2 0 0 6 ).
Proximity-to-a-standard and completion
R a n k in g in d iv id u a ls a g a in s t c e r ta in s ta n d a r d s
is o n e w a y o f m e a s u r in g a n d c o m p a r in g
c o m p e titiv e n e s s . A c c o r d in g to G a r c ia e t al.
(2 0 0 6 ), r a n k in g s th a t a l ig n w ith s ta n d a r d s (e .g .,
u b iq u ito u s n u m b e r 1 ra n k in g o r q u a lita tiv e
th r e s h o ld ) in te n s ifie s o f s o c ia l c o m p a r is o n s to a
g r e a te r e x te n t th a n r a n k in g s th a t d o n o t, r e s u ltin g
in g r e a te r c o m p e titio n a m o n g riv a ls. T h e y
a rg u e th a t th e re is a g r e a te r te n d e n c y to w a rd
c o m p e titio n n o t o n ly a m o n g th e to p fe w b u t a ls o
a m o n g th e b o tt o m fe w m e a n in g f u l s ta n d a r d s
( G a r c ia a n d T or, 2 0 0 7 ). I n e s s e n c e , c o m p e titiv e
b e h a v io r o c c u r s m o re fr e q u e n tly w h e n riv a ls a re
in p r o x im ity - to - a - s t a n d a r d . T h is w a s c o n f irm e d
in a r e c e n t s tu d y b y V a n d e g rift a n d H o la d a y
(2 0 1 2 ). H o w e v e r, th e y e m p h a s i z e th a t h ig h
c o m p e titiv e b e h a v io r in d u c e d b y p r o x im ity - to a -s ta n d a rd is m e re ly b e c a u s e o f c h a n g e in th e
b e h a v io r o f m e n . L ite r a tu r e h a s a s s e r te d th a t
p r o x im ity - to - a - s ta n d a r d h a s n o in flu e n c e o n
c o m p e tit iv e b e h a v io r o f w o m e n .
N-effect and competition
N - e f f e c t s h o w s th e r e la tio n s h ip b e tw e e n th e
n u m b e r o f c o m p e tito r s a n d th e ir m o tiv a tio n to
c o m p e te . In a n u ts h e ll, G a r c ia a n d T o r (2 0 0 9 )
argue that when the num ber of competitors
increases, motivation to compete can decrease.
W hen competitors are aware of how many
people will partake in a competition, they will
assume that the fewer the competitors, the higher
the probability of winning. In other words, fewer
numbers lead to high social comparison, which
results in high competition, while higher num
bers lead to the reverse (Garcia and Tor, 2009).
On the other hand, Vandegrift and Holaday
(2012) argue that the number of competitors has
no impact on competition, while individuals’
competitive behavior affects each other or in the
absence of fair rule condition. Based on their
findings, Vandegrift and Holaday (2012) gener
alized the application of the N-effect to market
situations. Again, these divergent results justify
testing the robustness of the theories in different
cultural contexts, where people have different
attitudes toward competition.
Therefore, we propose that individualists
demonstrate high motivation to compete (Hous
ton et al., 2012) through social comparison when
they are in proxim ity-to-a-standard and there
are few competitors, compared with collectiv
ists. This is because, individualists generally
seek to be unique and distinct from others or
groups whereas collectivists mostly pursue com
mon rather than individual goals. From this, we
hypothesized that:
uals from the U.S., Korea, China and Sri Lanka.
The initial questionnaire was developed in
English and translated into Korean, Chinese, and
Sinhalese by bilingual translators. The methods
of double translation and pre-testing were used
to ensure consistency and practical usage (Brislin, 1980). The questionnaire had three parts (A,
B, and C). Part A was designed to gather general
information about respondents. Part B captured
respondents’ cultural orientation, and attitude of
individuals toward competitiveness was cap
tured in part C with a series of scenario-based
questions. The questions were designed to test
whether competitive attitudes differed according
to the number of competitors and their com peti
tive position (top or low rank). For this, different
questions were asked from the same scenario
and questionnaires were administered separately
for both few-number competition samples and
large-number competition samples. The majority
of respondents were university students of sim i
lar cohorts. Approximately half of the Chinese
respondents resided in Korea.
Measurements
This cross-sectional study applied convenience
sampling technique to collect data from individ
Because IC was an attractive field of study for
a few decades, many IC measurements can
be found in literature with their own pros and
cons. Triandis and Gelfand (1998) argue that a
distinction between VI and HI is apparent not
only in individualist cultures, like the U.S., but
also in collective cultures, like Korea. They
came up with a 16-item scale that can be used
to identify four types of cultural patterns, on the
basis of a 32-item scale introduced by Singelis et al. (1995) for the same cultural patterns.
Later, Cozm a (2011) critically analysed those
widely used two measurements, taking psychometrical strengths and weakness into account,
and, ultimately, concluded that neither one was
superior to the other. For our study, we used the
reliable 16-item scale that covers VI (4-items),
HI (4-items), VC (4-items) and HC (4-items).
All items were measured using a seven-scale
anchoring ranging from 1= strongly disagree to
7 = strongly agree.
The scenario explained the selection process
candidates had to go through— an exam ination
as well as an interview on the same day. Based
on the marks obtained in the exam ination, each
candidate was then provided a rank (with rank
#1 being the highest mark) before the interview
(see A ppendix 1). The scenario was followed
by three questions. In a betw een-subject design,
the first question sought to m easure the degree
52
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
H3a: Vertical and horizontal individualists
(vs. collectivist) demonstrate high motivation
to compete when there are few competitors
(vs. relatively large).
H3b: Vertical and horizontal individualists
(vs. collectivist) demonstrate high motivation
to compete when they are in proximity-to-astandard.
S tu d y 1
With Study 1, we first sought to revisit and
identify cultural orientations of the U.S., Korea,
China, and Sri Lanka (Hypothesis 1). Second,
we measured the level of competitiveness
associated with individualistic and collectivistic
cultural traits (Hypothesis 2). Finally, we tested
the robustness o f proxim ity-to-a-standard
and N-effect in different cultural settings
(Hypothesis 3a and 3b).
Method
Table 1. Results of Factor Analysis
Factors and items
VI
I w ould rath er depend on m y self than others
I rely on m y self m ost o f the tim e, I rarely rely on others
I often do m y ow n th ing
HI
VC
HC
.680
.791
.765
.611
.826
.752
W inning is everything
C o m p etitio n is the law o f nature
W hen an o th er person does better than I do, I get tense and
and irritated
.786
.571
.685
.585
If a co-v/orker gets a prize, I w ould feel proud
T he w ell-being o f m y co-w orkers is im portant to m e
To m e, pleasu re is spending tim e w ith others
I feel g ood w hen I cooperate w ith others
Parents and children m ust stay to g eth er as m uch as possible
It is m y duty to take care o f m y fam ily, even w hen I have
to sacrifice w hat I w ant
F am ily m em bers should stick together, no m atter w hat
sacrifices are required
It is im p o rtan t to m e th at I respect the decision, m ade by
groups o f w hich I am a m em ber
.725
.782
.775
.545
Eigenvalues
2.198
1.409
1.221
3.611
C um ulative variance explain (%)
32.579
46.858
60.277
17.623
AVE
.557
.540
.439
.509
C ro n b ach ’s A lp ha
.678
.637
.669
.731
Note: V I= vertical individualism , H I= horizontal individualism , V C = vertical collectivism , H C = horizontal
co llectiv ism
K aiser-M eyer-O lkin (K M O ) m easure o f sam pling adequacy is 0.723
o f co m p etitiv en ess tow ard o th e r p articip an ts:
“I f a to tal o f 100 [for sm all n u m b er sam ple:
10] can d id ates h ad been invited to th e ex am in a
tion, to w h at d eg ree w o uld you h o ld co m p eti
tive feelin g s tow ard th e o th er ex am in ee s?” In
a w ith in -su b ject d esig n , the n ex t tw o questio n s
fo cu sed on in v estig atin g w h eth er co m petitiv e
feelin g s d iffered acco rd in g to th e ran k held: “If
y o u r ran k is #1 (#50), and a total o f 100 c an d i
d ates [for sm all n u m b e r sam ple: If y o u r ran k is
#1 (#5) and a total o f 10 can d id ates] h ad been
in v ited to atten d an interview , to w h at degree
w o u ld you have co m p etitiv e feelin g s tow ard
th e o th e r in terv iew ees?” A ll th e q u estio n s w ere
m easu red usin g a seven -scales an ch o rin g ran g
ing fro m 1 = n o t at all to 7 = very m uch.
A total o f 263 responses (m ode age group
= 20-24; 51% m ale) w as collected, distributed
am ong the U .S. (N = 58; m ode age group = 2529; 61% m ale), K orea (N = 71; m ode age group
= 20-24; 58% m ale), C hina (N = 74; m ode age
group = 20-24; 39% m ale) and Sri L anka (N =
60; m ode age group = 20-24; 47% m ale). The
study targeted at least 30 respondents from tw o
sam ple groups (i.e., large num ber com pletion
and few num ber com petition sam ple) from each
country. However, w e only obtained 28 responses
in the large num ber sam ple group from the U.S.
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
53
Analysis and results
Before testing our three hypotheses, factor analy
sis w as conducted to reconfirm the latent vari-
Table 2. Significant Univariate Effect for Country (at P < .001) and Descriptive Statistics
df error ------F-------
Dependent variables
HI
3
VI
3
HC
3
VC
Note:
3
259
259
259
259
3 4 .9 3 0
1 2 .98 3
2 4 .8 6 0
1 6 .3 1 6
Country -------- N
6 .0 9
0 .7 8 9
71
5.01
0 .8 5 6
74
4 .2 5
1 .29 6
SL
60
4 .9 7
1 .023
US
58
K o re a
C h in a
US
58
5 .1 5
1 .4 8 0
K o re a
71
5 .1 3
0 .9 2 2
C h in a
74
4 .7 8
0 .8 5 9
SL
60
4 .0 1
1 .3 5 2
US
58
5 .7 3
0 .8 3 2
K o re a
71
5 .2 9
0 .7 8 6
C h in a
74
4 .8 3
0 .6 4 4
SL
60
5 .8 6
0 .8 0 9
US
58
5 .6 6
0 .9 4 7
K o re a
71
5 .5 7
0 .8 9 5
C h in a
74
5 .1 7
0 .9 1 1
SL
60
6 .2 3
0 .7 1 0
V I= v e rtic a l in d iv id u a lis m , H I= h o riz o n ta l in d iv id u a lism , V C = v e r tic a l c o lle c tiv is m , H C = h o riz o n ta l
c o lle c tiv is m
U S = U n ite d S ta te s, S L = S ri L a n k a
A o n e -w a y b e tw e e n - g ro u p m u ltiv a ria te a n a ly
sis o f v a ria n c e (M A N O V A ) w a s p e r f o r m e d to
in v e stig a te th e c u ltu ra l d iff e re n c e o f c o u n trie s
(th e U .S ., K o re a , C h in a , a n d S ri L a n k a ), in te rm s
o f f o u r c u ltu ra l p a tte rn s (V I, H I, V C , a n d H C ).
T h e f o u r c u ltu ra l p a tte rn s w e r e c o n s id e re d as
d e p e n d e n t v a ria b le s , w h e r e a s c o u n trie s (n a tio n a l
c u ltu re s ) w e re tre a te d as in d e p e n d e n t v a ria b le s .
T h e r e w a s a s ta tis tic a lly sig n ific a n t d if fe re n c e
b e tw e e n fo u r c o u n trie s o n th e c o m b in e d d e p e n
d e n t v a ria b le s : F (1 2 , 7 7 4 ) = 1 7 .2 2 , p < 0 .0 0 1 ;
P illa i’s T ra c e = 0 .6 3 ; p a r tia l e ta s q u a re d = 0 .2 1 1 .
T h e lin e a r c o m b in a tio n o f th e d e p e n d e n t v a ri
a b le s is re p o r te d u s in g P illa i’s T ra c e te c h n iq u e
d u e to u n e q u a l N v a lu e s in th e s a m p le (P a lla n t,
2 0 0 7 ). W h e n re s u lts f o r d e p e n d e n t v a ria b le s
w e r e c o n s id e re d se p a ra te ly , u s in g B o n fe r ro n i a d
ju s te d a lp h a o f .0 1 2 5 , fo u r c u ltu ra l p a tte rn s w e r e
f o u n d to b e s ta tis tic a lly s ig n ific a n t (T ab le 2).
P o s t- h o c c o m p a ris o n w a s e m p lo y e d u s in g
T u k e y ’s h o n e s t sig n ific a n t d if fe r e n c e s (H S D )
te st. F irst, w e c o m p a re d in d iv id u a lis tic c u ltu ra l
p a tte rn s a c ro ss c o u n trie s a n d th e n c o lle c tiv is t
c u ltu ra l p a tte rn s.
W ith re g a rd to th e H I p a tte rn , m e a n v a lu e s fo r
th e U .S . (M = 6 .0 9 , S D = 0 .7 8 ) a n d C h in a (M
= 4 .2 5 , SD = 1 .2 9 ) w e r e n o t o n ly s ig n ific a n tly
d if fe r e n t fr o m e a c h o th e r, b u t a lso d if fe re n t fr o m
K o re a (M = 5 .0 1 , SD = 0 .8 5 ) a n d S ri L a n k a
(A / = 4 .9 7 , SD = 1 .02 ) a t P < 0 .0 0 1 . O n ly o n e
in s ig n if ic a n t m e a n v a lu e w a s o b s e rv e d b e tw e e n
K o re a a n d S ri L a n k a (se e T a b le 2 ). T h e m e a n
v a lu e fo r th e U .S . w a s sig n ific a n tly h ig h e r
c o m p a re d to K o re a , C h in a a n d S ri L a n k a . F ro m
th e p e r s p e c tiv e o f H I c u ltu ra l p a tte rn , h y p o th e sis
H I a w a s str o n g ly su p p o r te d . M e a n v a lu e s o f
V I p a tte rn s f o r th e U .S . (M = 5 .1 5 , SD = 1 .48 ),
54
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
a b le s (i.e., v e rtic a l a n d h o riz o n ta l C -I). T w o ite m s
w e re d isc a rd e d fro m th e in itia l 1 6 -item sca le
o w in g to w e a k lo a d in g a n d lo a d in g u n d e r th e
d iffe re n t c la ssific atio n s. T h e re m a in in g 1 4 -item s
w e re u s e d to b u ild fo u r in d ic e s. A s sh o w n in
T a b le 1, th e fa c to r lo a d in g v a lu e s ra n g e d fro m
0 .5 5 to 0 .8 3 . T h e re la tiv e ly h ig h a v e ra g e v a ria n ce
e x tra c te d (A Y E ) in d ic a te s a d e q u a te c o n v e rg e n t
v alid ity o f e a c h v aria b le. F u rth e r, th e re lia b ility o f
th e c o n s tru c ts w a s te ste d c o m p u tin g C ro n b a c h ’s
A lp h a . C ro n b a c h ’s A lp h a ’s v a lu es ra n g e d fr o m
0 .6 4 to 0 .7 3 , d e m o n str a tin g a sa tisfa c to ry lev el o f
fa c to r reliab ility .
Cultural orientation
K orea (M = 5.13, SD - 0.92) and C hin a (M =
4.78, SD - 0.85) w ere significantly different
only from Sri L anka (M = 4.01, SD = 1.35)
at P < 0.001, but not significant fo r the U .S .,
K orea, and C hin a respectively. E ven though,
the m ean value for the U .S. w as relatively high
com pared w ith that o f K orea, China, and Sri
L anka, it w as only significant w ith Sri Lanka.
So H la is slightly supported in relation to V I
cultu ral patterns. W e can, therefore, conclude
that the hypothesis th at A m ericans dem onstrate
m ore individualistic (vertical and horizontal)
characteristics over K oreans, C hinese, and Sri
L ankans is partly supported.
W ith respect to the H C pattern, the m ean
value fo r C hin a (M = 4.83, SD = 0.64) w as
significantly different from th at o f the U .S. (M =
5.73, S D =0.83), K orea (M = 5.29, SD = 0.78, p
< .05), and Sri L anka (M = 5.86, SD = 0.80) at P
< 0.001. Further, significant differences o f m ean
values w ere observed betw een K orea and Sri
L anka, as w ell as K orea and the U .S. Though,
the m ean value o f Sri L anka lies above the other
three countries, it w as not significant fo r the U.S.
H ence, from a H C cultu ral p attern perspective,
H 1b w as supported. Finally, the m ean value o f
the V C pattern for Sri L anka (M = 6.23, SD =
0.71) differed significantly from K orea (M =
5.57, SD = 0.895), C hina (M = 5.17, SD = 0.91),
and the U .S. (M = 5.66, SD = 0.94) p < 0.05.
In addition, the m ean value fo r C hin a w as also
significantly different from that o f the U .S. and
K orea at P < 0.05. T he w ell-above-significant
Sri L anka m ean value over oth er three countries
supported H lb . Therefore, w e conclude that
Sri L ankans dem onstrate m ore collectivist
characteristics (vertical and horizontal) th an
A m ericans, K oreans, and C hinese.
In this section, w e first investigate the average
com petitiveness o f individuals w ith respect to
four cultural patterns, under three conditions:
the average com petitiveness in response to
both few and large num ber o f com petitors; the
average com petitiveness o f top rankings and low
rankings W e next investigated the relationship
betw een fo u r cultural patterns and proxim ity-toa-standard and N -effect.
T he relationships betw een cultural patterns
and com petitiveness w ere investigated using
P earson product-m om ent correlation coefficient
(H 2). A m ong four cultu ral patterns only HI
pattern w as significant and positively correlated
(r = 0.15, n = 263, p < 0.05) w ith com petitive
feelings tow ard others in the presence o f eith er
few or large num ber o f exam inees w ith regard to
ranking. W hereas, the com petitive feeling o f top
rankers tow ards other interview ees w as p o si
tively correlated w ith both H I (r = 0.13, n = 263,
p < 0.05) and V I (r = 0.15, n = 263, p < 0.05),
the com petitive feeling o f low rankers tow ard
oth er interview ees w as only positively correlated
(r = 0.13, n = 263, p < 0.05) w ith HI. C orrela
tions betw een horizontal-vertical collectivism
and com petitive feeling tow ard others w ere not
significant. T herefore, hypoth esis 2 is accepted
and w e can conclude th at vertical-horizontal
individualism is m ore positively correlated w ith
com petitive behavior, com pared w ith vertical
and horizontal collectivism , irrespective o f coun
try differences.
T he relationship betw een four cultu ral p at
terns and the m otiv atio n to com pete w hen the
nu m b er o f com petitors is few (vs. relatively
large) w as investigated usin g P earson productm om ent correlation coefficient (H 3a). B efore
perform ing the correlation test, the data file was
split into tw o— few nu m b er o f com petitors and
large num ber o f com petitors— based on the fo l
low ing question: “If a to tal o f 100 [few num ber
o f com petitors; 10] candidates had been invited
to the exam in atio n, to w hat degree w ould you
hold com petitiv e feelin gs tow ard the other ex
am inees?” In both cases participants had a 20%
chance o f getting selected fo r the job. W hen the
num ber o f com petitors w as sm all, neith er one
o f four cultu ral patterns w as significantly cor
related w ith the m otivation to com pete (H I, r =
0.09; V I, r = 0.00; H C, r = 0.12; V C, r = 0.10,
p > 0.15, n=131). Ironically, in the p resence o f
a large num ber o f com petitors, H I pattern and
m otiv atio n to com pete w ere significantly co r
related (r = 0.20, n = 132, p < 0.05). H ow ever,
the rem aining cultu ral patterns w ere not signifi
cant. A ll in all, hypoth esis 3a, w hich postu lated
that vertical and horizontal individ ualists (vs.
collectiv ist) dem onstrate high m otivation to
com pete w hen the nu m b er o f com petitors is
few (vs. relatively large), w as rejected. In ad
ditio n, a one-w ay betw een-group A NO V A w as
p erform ed to fu rth er explore w hether the p res
ence o f a few or large num ber o f com petitors
influenced com petitiv e behavior. Subjects w ere
placed in sm all or large groups o f co m petitors.
T here w as no significant difference betw een a
few and a large num ber o f com petitors (F( 1,
261) = 0.043, p = 0.83). Further, w e observed
sim ilar results at country level. O verall, results
indicate that the presence o f different num bers
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
55
Competitiveness
Table 3. Hierarchical Regression Analysis for High- and Low-Rank Completion
L o w ra n k co m p e titio n
T op ra n k co m p e titio n
P re d ic to r
AR2
S tep 1
.013
P
F
AR2
1.666
.009
P
1.232
HC
.02 9
.0 5 0
VC
.0 96
.063
S tep 2
.0 1 2 t
2.156*
.013*
1.955
HC
.00 2
.022
VC
.0 8 4
.0 50
HI
.114*
.119*
S tep 3
.0 15*
2 .6 7 6 *
1.478
.0 0 0
HC
.005
.0 22
VC
.083
.0 50
HI
.07 4
.1 24
VI
.130*
-.0 1 7
T o tal R 2
.04 0
.022
N
263
2 63
F
Note: V I = v ertica l in d iv id u alis m , H I = h o riz o n ta l in d iv id u alis m , V C = v ertica l co llec tiv ism ,
H C = h o riz o n ta l co lle c tiv is m
* P < .1 0 , * p < . 0 5
o f c o m p e tito rs d o e s n o t in flu e n c e c a n d id a te s ’
c o m p e titiv e b eh av io r.
In H 3 b , w e p ro p o s e d th a t w h en in d iv id u als
are in a co m p e titiv e s itu a tio n , c o m p e titiv e fe e l
in g s w o u ld d e p e n d o n th e ir rela tiv e c o m p etitiv e
p o sitio n (ran k in g s ) an d th e e x te n t to w h ic h th ey
p o sse ss in d iv id u a lis tic an d co lle c tiv is tic c h a ra c
te ris tic s. To m e a su re th e in d iv id u a l’s co m p etitiv e
fe e lin g w ith re s p e c t to th e ir co m p etitiv e p o sitio n
(i.e., ra n k o b ta in e d fro m th e ex a m in a tio n ), w e
a sk e d re sp o n d e n ts : “I f y o u r ra n k is #1 (# 5 0 ),
an d a to ta l o f 100 ca n d id a te s [fo r fe w -n u m b e r
g ro u p : I f y o u r ra n k is #1 (# 5 ), a n d a to ta l o f 10
ca n d id ate s] h a d b e e n in v ited to an in terv iew , to
w h a t d e g ree w o u ld y o u h o ld c o m p e titiv e fe e l
in g s to w ard s th e o th e r in te rv ie w e e s ? ” R a n k 1
w as c o n s id e re d as to p ran k , w h ile ra n k s 5 an d 50
w e re re g a rd e d as low ran k s. H ie ra rc h ic a l m u l
tip le re g re ssio n te c h n iq u e w as a p p lie d to te st h y
p o th e sis 3b. F o u r ty p e s o f c u ltu ra l p atte rn s w e re
seq u en tially ad d ed to th e m o d e l in th ree steps,
as in d e p e n d e n t v a ria b les , to te st th e ir im p a c t on
in d iv id u a ls ’ c o m p e titiv e fe e lin g (d e p e n d e n t v a ri
ab le) a n d o b serv e th e ex p la n a to ry p ow er. T h e
se q u e n c e o f ad d in g ty p e s o f c u ltu ra l p a tte rn s to
th e m o d el b eg an w ith co lle c tiv is m , b e c a u se it
w as p o s tu la te d th a t reg ard le ss o f th e ir ra n k s c o l
le ctiv is ts m ig h t h o ld less m o tiv atio n to co m p ete.
A fte r c o n tro llin g fo r H C an d V C , in d iv id u alis tic
p atte rn s H I an d V I w ere e n te re d in th e sec o n d
an d th ird step s, resp ectiv ely . T h e se q u e n c e o f
e n te rin g p re d ic to rs in to re g re s s io n w as id e n tic a l
in b o th h ig h -ra n k an d lo w -ran k c o m p e titio n c o n
d itio n s. T h e m u ltic o llin e a rity m e a su re d b y th e
v aria n ce in flatio n fa c to r (V IF ) fo r tw o co n d itio n s
w ere w ith in th e a c c e p te d lim its an d in d ic a te d n o
m u ltic o llin e a rity a m o n g in d e p e n d e n t v aria b le s.
W ith reg ard to h ig h -ra n k c o m p etitio n , H C an d
V C w e re en tered at first, e x p lain in g ju s t o n ly
1.3% v arian ce in co m p etitiv e n e ss. A fte r en te rin g
H I in th e sec o n d step an d V I in th e th ird step, th e
total v arian ce ex p la in ed b y th e m o d el as a w h o le
in c re as ed to 4 % (F (4 ,2 5 8 ) = 2 .6 7 6 , P < 0 .0 5 ).
H I (R 2 ch an g e = 0 .0 1 2 , F ch an g e = 3 .1 0 8 , P =
0 .0 7 9 ) an d V I (R 2 ch an g e = 0 .0 1 5 , F c h an g e =
4 .1 5 8 , P < 0 .0 5 ), ex p la in e d an ad d itio n al 2 .7 % o f
v arian ce in c o m p etitiv e n e ss. In th e fin al m o d el,
o n ly V I w as sta tistic ally sig n ifican t, /? = 0 .1 3 0 , t
< 0 .0 5. In co n tra st, in lo w -ran k c o m p etitio n c o n
d itio n , alth o u g h th e m o d e l as a w h o le ex p la in ed
56
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
Table 4. Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Few and Large Number of Competitors
large number of competitors (100)
few competitors (10)
Predictor
AR2
Step 1
.026
P
F
AR2
1.693
.018
P
1.155
HC
.118
-.095
VC
.069
.148
Step 2
1.634
.011
1.484
.016
HC
.088
-.123
VC
.078
.108
HI
.110
.140
Step 3
.000
1.216
3.722**
.071**
HC
.088
.107
VC
.078
.102
HI
.110
.063
VI
.000
.277**
Total R2
.037
.105
N
131
132
F
Note: VI = Vertical individualism, HI = Horizontal individualism, VC = Vertical collectivism,
HC = horizontal collectivism
*p < .05, **P < .01
the total variance of 2.2%, none of the predictors
explained the variance in competitiveness signifi
cantly (Table 3). HI and VI together explained an
additional 1.3% of variance in competitiveness.
However, it was less than half than the variance
explained by the two variables in the high-rank
competition condition. This indicates that vertical
and horizontal individualists who are in proximity-to-a-s:andard (top rankers: rank 1) demon
strate high motivation to compete, compared with
those who are away from the standard (low rank
ers: rank 5 or 50), whereas vertical and horizontal
collectivist maintained low and same degree of
competitive feeling irrespective of their ranks.
Therefore, we can accept H3b while concluding
that proximity-to-a-standard is more pronounced
among individualists than collectivists.
Further, we analysed data to investigate wheth
er competitive feelings in response to a proximi
ty-to-a-standard was influenced by the number of
competitors at an event. After splitting data files
into groups of fewer (N = 131) and larger (N =
132) numbers of competitors, hierarchical regres
sion was executed entering predictors similar to
the above hierarchical model. Our analysis was,
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
however, restricted to high-rank completion,
because the result of the previous hierarchical
analysis was not significant for low-rank com
petition. None of the predictors explained the
variance in competitiveness significantly when
the number of candidates being interviewed was
10 (i.e., fewer competitors) (see Table 4). In con
trast, in the large number of competitors’ group,
after controlling HC and VC, the total variance
explained by the model as a whole increased to
10.5 % (F(4,127) = 3.722, P = 0.007). HI (R2
change = 0.016, F change = 2.212, P = 0 .148)
and VI (R2 change = 0.071, F change = 10.121, P
= 0.002) explained an additional 8.7% of vari
ance in competitiveness. In the final model, only
VI was statistically significant (J3 = 0.277, P =
0.002). It reveals that individualistic individuals
are highly competitive when they are ranked 1
(vs. 5) and the number of candidate presence in
the interview is 100 (vs. 10).
Additional analysis
We conducted two additional tests to investigate
the composition of four cultural patterns at
the country level and to explore the impact of
c o u n tr y ( c u ltu r a l o rie n ta tio n ) a n d th e n u m b e r o f
a m o r e c o lle c tiv is t- o r ie n te d c o u n try a n d , m o r e
p a r tic ip a n t/N - e f f e c t (fe w a n d la rg e n u m b e r) o n
s p e c ific a lly , a V C c o u n try .
c o m p e titiv e n e s s .
F in a lly , th e d iffe re n c e b e tw e e n th e fo u r
I n th e f ir s t in s t a n c e , to e x p lo r e th e c u lt u r a l
c u ltu ra l p a tte rn s w a s s ig n ific a n t (F (3 , 5 7 ) =
p < .0 0 1 ;
o r ie n ta tio n o f e a c h c o u n try , a o n e - w a y re p e a te d
4 1 .4 2 3 ,
A N O V A w a s c o n d u c te d s e p a r a te ly f o r e a c h
S r i L a n k a . A ll d if f e r e n c e s w e r e s ig n if ic a n t
c o u n tr y to c o m p a r e th e v a lu e o f th e f o u r c u ltu ra l
a s p e r th e p a irw is e c o m p a ris o n . T h e m e a n
W ilk s ’ L a m b d a = 0 .3 1 ) fo r
SD = 0 . 8 0 , p
SD = 0 . 7 1 , p <
p a tte r n s ( H I , V I , H C , a n d V C ) . I n th e U .S ., th e r e
v a lu e s f o r b o th H C (M = 5 .8 6 ,
w a s a s ta tis tic a lly s ig n ific a n t d if fe re n c e b e tw e e n
< 0 .0 0 1 ) a n d V C (M = 6 .2 3 ,
th e f o u r c u ltu r a l p a t te r n s ( F ( 3 , 5 5 ) = 1 0 .7 3 6 ,
p
< 0 .0 0 1 W i l k s ’ L a m b d a = 0 .6 3 ) . P a ir w is e c o m
p a r is o n in d ic a te s th a t th e m e a n v a lu e o f V I
= 5 .1 5 ,
SD = 1 . 4 8 )
(M
w a s s ig n ific a n tly d if fe re n t
SD = 0 . 7 8 , p < 0 . 0 0 1 ) , H C
0 .8 3 , p < 0 .0 5 ) a n d V C ( M =
p < 0 .0 5 ) . F u r th e r , th e r e w a s a
f r o m H I ( M = 6 .0 9 ,
( M = 5 .7 3 ,
SD =
SD =
0 .9 4 ,
5 .6 6 ,
.0 0 1 ) ( c o lle c tiv is t p a tte r n s ) w e r e s ig n if ic a n tly
h ig h e r th a n b o th H I (M = 4 .9 7 ,
.0 0 1 ) a n d V I (M = 4 .0 1 ,
SD
SD
= 1 .3 5 ,
p <
=
1 .0 2 ,
p
< 0 .0 0 1 )
(in d iv id u a lis tic p a tte rn s ). T h e re fo re , w e c a n
c o n c lu d e th a t S ri L a n k a n s a re m o re c o lle c
tiv is t-o rie n te d a n d p o s s e s s a h ig h e r V C
{p -
.0 3 4 ) p a tte r n th a n H C .
s ig n ific a n t m e a n v a lu e d iff e r e n c e b e tw e e n H I
T o s u m m a r i z e , t h e f o u r c o u n t r i e s d if f e r e d in
a n d V C (s e e T a b le 5 ). D e s p ite th e h ig h m e a n o f
c u ltu r a l o r ie n ta tio n . It is f a ir e n o u g h to c o n
H I c o m p a r e d w ith H C a n d V C , it is h a r d to s ta t e
c l u d e th a t th e U .S . is a m o r e h o r iz o n t a l in d i-
c le a r ly w h e th e r A m e r ic a n s w e r e m o r e in d iv id u
v id u a lis tic - o r ie n te d c o u n try , th o u g h in d iv id u a ls
a lis tic d u e to th e f a c t th a t V I w a s b e lo w H C a n d
p re s e n t a m ix o f in d iv id u a lis tic a n d c o lle c tiv is t
V C a n d b e c a u s e th e d iff e r e n c e b e tw e e n H C a n d
c h a r a c te r is tic s o n a v e r a g e ( s e e F ig u r e 1 ). K o
V C w a s in s ig n if ic a n t. S u b s e q u e n tly , n e ith e r
r e a is a m o d e r a te l y lo w c o l l e c t iv is t- o r i e n te d
in d iv id u a lis tic p a tte r n s n o r c o lle c tiv is tic p a tte rn s
c o u n tr y , s h a r in g in d iv id u a lis tic c h a r a c te r is tic s ,
o v e r d id e a c h o th e r o n a v e ra g e .
w h e r e a s C h in a is a m o d e r a te ly h ig h c o lle c tiv
F o r K o re a , th e d if f e r e n c e o f f o u r c u ltu r a l
p a tte r n s w a s c o m p a r a tiv e ly le s s s ig n if ic a n t ( F ( 3 ,
6 8 ) = 5 .7 0 1 ,
p
< 0 .0 5 W i l k s ’ L a m b d a = 0 .7 9 ) .
is t- o r ie n te d c o u n tr y . S ri L a n k a is a c o lle c tiv is to r ie n te d c o u n tr y w ith h ig h v e r tic a l c o lle c tiv is t
c h a ra c te ris tic s .
A c c o r d in g to th e p a ir w is e c o m p a r is o n o n ly V C
(M
= 5 .5 7 ,
SD
= 0 .8 9 ) w a s s ig n if ic a n tly d if f e r
e n t f r o m H I ( M = 5 .0 1 ,
a n d V I ( M = 5 .1 3 ,
SD =
SD
= 0 .8 5 ,
0 .9 2 ,
p
p < 0 .0 0 1 )
< 0 .0 5 ) . E v e n
F o r th e s e c o n d s ta g e o f th is a d d itio n a l
a n a ly s is , w e p e r f o r m e d a tw o -w a y b e tw e e n g r o u p A N O V A to e x p lo r e th e im p a c t o f c u l tu r
a l o rie n ta tio n a n d th e n u m b e r o f p a rtic ip a n ts
th o u g h th e m e a n v a lu e s o f b o th c o lle c tiv is t
o n c o m p e titiv e n e s s . F o r th is , w e u s e d o n ly a
p a tte r n s w e r e h ig h e r th a n th e in d iv id u a lis tic
h ig h -ra n k c o m p le tio n m e a s u re m e n t a s a d e
p a tte r n s , th e m e a n v a lu e s b e tw e e n H I, V I, a n d
p e n d e n t v a ria b le , b e c a u s e in d iv id u a l c u ltu ra l
H C w e re in s ig n if ic a n t. F u r th e r , th e m e a n v a lu e
p a tte rn s s ig n ific a n tly e x p la in e d th e v a ria tio n
o f H C w a s n o t s ig n if ic a n tly d if f e r e n t f r o m V C .
in c o m p e titiv e n e s s in th e f ir s t s ta g e o f th e a d
C o n s e q u e n tly , it is n o t p o s s ib le to id e n tif y K o r e
d itio n a l a n a ly s is . In d e p e n d e n t v a ria b le s w e re
a n s a s h ig h ly c o lle c tiv is t. I n s te a d w e m ig h t b e
c u ltu r a l o r ie n ta tio n ( th e U .S ., K o re a , C h in a ,
a b le to s ta te th a t th e y a re m ix e d in d iv id u a lis tic
a n d S ri L a n k a ) a n d th e n u m b e r o f p a r tic ip a n ts
a n d c o lle c tiv is tic .
W ith r e g a r d to C h in a , th e d iff e r e n c e b e tw e e n
th e f o u r c u ltu r a l p a tte r n s w a s s ig n ific a n t (F ( 3 ,
7 1 ) = 1 1 .6 7 8 ,
p
< .0 0 1 ; W i lk s ’ L a m b d a = 0 .6 7 )
a n d th e p a irw is e c o m p a r is o n re v e a le d th a t,
(fe w a n d la rg e n u m b e rs ). T h e in te r a c tio n e f
fe c t b e tw e e n c o u n try a n d n u m b e r o f p a rtic i
p a n ts w a s s ta tis tic a lly in s ig n ific a n t ( F ( 3 , 2 5 5 )
= 0 .5 8 1 ,
P
= 0 .6 2 ). H o w e v e r, th e m a in e ffe c t
fo r b o th c u ltu ra l o rie n ta tio n (F (3 , 2 5 5 ) =
P-
e x c e p t f o r th e m e a n v a lu e d iff e r e n c e b e tw e e n
3 .6 3 5 ,
V I a n d H C , a ll o th e r m e a n d if f e r e n c e s w e re
th e n u m b e r o f p a rtic ip a n ts
s ig n if ic a n t. T h e m e a n v a lu e f o r V C ( M = 5 .1 7 ,
P
SD =
tic a lly s ig n ific a n t. P o s t-h o c c o m p a ris o n s u s in g
0 .9 1 ) w a s s ig n if ic a n tly h ig h c o m p a r e d w ith
0 .0 1 p a r t i a l e t a s q u a r e d = 0 .0 4 ) a n d
(F( 1 ,
2 5 5 ) = 5 .7 6 8 ,
= 0 .0 1 p a r tia l e ta s q u a r e d = 0 .0 2 ) w a s s ta tis
(M = 4 . 8 3 , SD = 0 . 6 4 4 , p < 0 . 0 5 ) ,
H I ( M = 4 . 2 5 , SD = 1 . 2 9 , p < . 0 0 1 ) a n d V I ( M =
4 . 7 8 , SD = 0 . 8 5 , p < 0 . 0 5 ) . M o r e o v e r , a l t h o u g h
4 .9 5 ,
SD
H C ’s m e a n v a l u e w a s a b o v e t w o i n d i v i d u a l i s t i c
fro m
K o re a
p a tte r n s , th e d iff e r e n c e w a s s ig n ific a n t o n ly f o r
a n d C h in a (A / = 5 .6 4 ,
H I. A s a r e s u lt, w e c a n c o n c lu d e th a t C h in a is
A lth o u g h , th e m e a n v a lu e o f c o m p e titiv e n e s s
58
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
th a t o f H C
T u k e y ’s H S D t e s t i n d i c a t e d t h a t t h e m e a n
v a lu e o f c o m p e titiv e n e s s f o r S ri L a n k a (M =
= 1 .7 0 ) w a s s ig n i f i c a n t l y d i f f e r e n t
(M =
5 .8 ,
SD = 1 . 1 9 , p
SD = 1 . 6 6 , p
= 0 .0 0 8 )
= 0 .0 4 ),
Table 5. Significant Multivariate Effect for Country (at P < .001) and Descriptive Statistics
Wilks’
Lambda
df
d f error
F
Dependent
variables
N
M
SD
US
0.63
3
55
10.736
HI
VI
HC
VC
58
58
58
58
6.09
5.15
5.73
5.66
.789
1.480
.832
.947
Koreaa
0.79
3
68
5.701
HI
VI
HC
VC
71
71
71
71
5.01
5.13
5.29
5.57
.856
.922
.786
.895
China
0.67
3
71
11.678
HI
VI
HC
VC
74
74
74
74
4.25
4.78
4.83
5.17
1.296
.859
.644
.911
SL
0.31
3
57
41.423
HI
VI
HC
VC
60
60
60
60
4.97
4.01
5.86
6.23
1.023
1.352
.809
.710
Country
Note: VI = vertical individualism, HI = horizontal individualism, VC = vertical collectivism,
HC = horizontal collectivism
US = United States, SL = Sri Lanka
ap < .05
Figure 1. Cultural Orientation of Countries
Country
---- US
— Korea
—— China
— SL
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
59
Figure 2. Cultural Orientation and Competitiveness
$<D
a
>
0)
a
so
u
Country
for the U.S. (M = 5.48, 5Z) = 1.53) was higher
than Sri Lanka, it was not statistically signifi
cant. The mean value differences between the
U.S., Korea, and China were also not signifi
cant (Figure 2).
These results demonstrate that individuals’
competitiveness depends on the extent to which
their respective country shares individualistic
or collectivistic cultural dimensions. Coun
tries with higher orientation of collectivism,
like Sri Lanka, indicated lower competitive
ness, whereas Korea, being a moderately low
collectivist-oriented country sharing individu
alistic characteristics, indicated higher levels of
competitiveness. China’s levels of collectivism
and competitiveness fell between Sri Lanka
and Korea. However, though the U.S. was a
more HI country, in our sample, competitive
ness was lower than that of Korea and China.
This provides evidence that ranking match with
a standard (when they are closer to win the
competition) influences even low collectivist
countries (relatively high shared individualistic
characteristics among collectivist countries)
to stay highly competitive, while discouraging
high collectivist to compete. In other terms, we
can state that higher collectivists tend to work
collaboratively even when they are in a highly
competitive situation.
Further, pairwise comparison indicated that
the mean value for larger groups of competitors
(M = 5.7, SD = 1.46, p = 0.01) is higher than for
fewer numbers of competitors (M = 5.2, SD =
1.61). To further explore this phenomenon, we
conducted one more analysis that investigated
how proximity-to-a-standard effect differs be
tween large and small groups of competitors.
First, the data set was split into small and large
competitive groups. Then, a one-way repeated
ANOVA was conducted to compare competi
tiveness between top-ranking and low-ranking
competitors in both sets of groups. We found a
significant effect for ranking in the large compet
itive group (Wilks’ Lambda = 0.92, F(l,131) =
10.893, p = .001). The mean value for top rank
ing (M = 5.7, SD - 1.46, p = 0.001, n = 132) was
greater than that of low ranking (M - 5.2, SD =
1.7). In contrast, for competitors in the smaller
group, the effect of ranking on competitive
ness was insignificant (Wilks’ Lambda = 0.98,
F(l,130) = 2.262, p = 0.13), though it was near
the cut-off mark. The mean value for top ranking
(M = 5.25, SD = 1. 61, p = 0.13, n = 131) was
lower than that of low ranking (M = 5.47, SD =
1.35). This additional analysis confirmed that
proximity-to-a-standard is pronounced in the
large competitive group, but less pronounced for
the group with few competitors.
6C
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
Discussion
Despite weak loading and loading under the
different classifications of two items, the HI,
VI, HC, and VC factors emerged in all four
countries. This provides further confidence in the
use of the 16-item scale of four cultural patterns
for cross-cultural studies.
Cultwal orientation: Americans demonstrated
more individualistic characteristics than Kore
ans, Chinese, and Sri Lankans. However, the
HI pattern among Americans was predominant,
whereas the VI pattern was not significantly
different for Korea and China. Sri Lankans were
more collectivistic, over and above the U.S.,
Korea, and China with respect with both HC
and VC. Nevertheless, Americans showed high
collectivist characteristics compared with Ko
reans and Chinese. We can therefore argue that
Americans were more collectivist than Koreans
and Chinese. To address this issue, we need to
explore how the four cultural characteristics are
distributed within each country. Among Ameri
cans, HI dominated over the collectivist pattern,
while collectivist patterns dominated among Ko
reans and Chinese to varying degrees. To a lesser
extent, Koreans and Chinese were collectivistic
compared to Sri Lankans. Further, Koreans were
moderately low collectivistic, sharing relatively
higher individualistic characteristics than Chi
nese.
Disregarding country differences,
individualism was positively associated with
higher degrees of competition. However, the
positive correlation between horizontal-vertical
individualism and degree of competitiveness
varied depending on the competitive context. For
instance, in the presence of different numbers
of participants only horizontal individualism
was positively correlated with competitiveness,
whereas with top-ranking participants,
irrespective of the number of competitors,
both patterns of individualism were positively
correlated. As for country levels, individuals’
competitiveness depended on the extent to
which their country shared individualistic and
collectivistic cultural dimensions. Nevertheless,
Americans showed relatively low degrees of
competitiveness. Prior research has suggested
that the U.S. was an example of VI and selfreliance in terms of competition (Triandis, 1995;
Triandis et al., 1988). In our sample, this shift
in competitiveness can partially be understood
as due to the high self-reported HI. Singelis
et al. (1995) reported that horizontal-vertical
individualism is varied in the U.S. and depends
on social structures. In support of this, Snibbe
and Markus (2005) reported that self-expression
and the pursuit of uniqueness are more apparent
among Americans with a high socioeconomic
status relative to a low socioeconomic status.
Our results showed diverging evidence from
the notion of N-effect, which holds that fewer
participants leads to higher competition. With
our study, we could not find a significant differ
ence of competitiveness between few and large
competition groups. Furthermore, vertical-hori
zontal individualists were not positively associ
ated with competitiveness when the number of
competitors was small or large. Similarly, Vandegrift, and Holaday (2012) found that changes
in the number of competitors had no impact on
competitive behavior under the condition where
competitive behavior affects each other or there
is no fair rule condition. Our study’s diverging
results may reflect various reasons, including
multiple contest situations where lower weight
had been allocated to the examination (i.e., 20%
for the first context) as opposed to higher weight
for the interview (i.e., 80% for the second con
test) in the overall evaluation of the selection
process. This reduced emphasis on the exami
nation might have induced competitors in the
smaller group to scale-down their motivation
(ratchet down effect) through social comparison
process to compete with examinees. In contrast,
despite this low importance, competitors of the
large group appeared to maintain the same level
of competitive attitude toward examinees, ow
ing to the lack of social comparability. Also,
multistage competition might have drawn par
ticipants’ attention to the process of competition
away from the number of competitors, attenuat
ing the N-effect.
With regard to proximity-to-a-standard,
horizontal-vertical individualists, who obtained
top ranking in the first selection stage,
demonstrated high levels of motivation to
compete in the next stage compared with those
who were further away from the standard. In
contrast, competitiveness of horizontal-vertical
collectivists remained unchanged, irrespective
of their ranks. Thus, proximity-to-a-standard
is well supported among individualistic rather
than collectivists. Moreover, we found that
proximity-to-a-standard was evident only in
large competitive groups. This is, contradictory
to the findings of Vandegrift and Holaday
(2012). This inconsistent result of N-effect may
be due to the allocation of different weights,
which might have caused proximity-to-a-
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
61
standard to disappear in the group of fewer
competitors. Lower levels of motivation to
compete when competitors in the small group
received top ranking induces a tendency to
maintain the same level of competition or less
because of over-estimating their superiority
or moral hazard (Ridlon and Shin, 2013).
However, obtaining a low rank led participants
to oe more competitive than before owing to the
feeling that they had not been competitive in
the first contest and were still not far from the
standard (higher comparability). That is why
the mean value for low ranking was greater than
that of top ranking participants in our study. In
contrast, those who received top-rankings in the
large competition group may have maintained
the same or even a higher level of motivation,
whereas a low rank seemed to discourage
participants to compete. This is because, though
low-ranked participants extend considerable
efforts to compete in the first contest, they
found they were too far from the standard (i.e.,
low comparability makes them feel they are
distant from the standard). Having said this,
further testing is required, which is beyond the
scope of this study.
Overall, our findings reveal that individualists
tend to put extensive effort in competing once
they progress toward a competitive goal or their
ranking coincides with a standard. This suggests
that it is hard to expect individualists to work
collectively as they come closer to a desired
end. We can, however, expect such a cooperative
effort from collectivists. We shed light on this
notion in Study 2 while testing the robustness of
our findings in Study 1.
Proximity-to-a-standard and cooperative ef
fort
Study one illustrated that individualistic
individuals were more competitive in the •
advanced stage of competition than in the
middle stage, whereas collectivists remained
less competitive or uncompetitive across
different rankings. Szu-Chi et al. (1996)
explored how individualists and collectivists
respond to cooperative efforts. They showed
that even in a zero-sum game situation,
individuals are likely to regard shared-pursuit
peers as opponents and like to surpass them at
the advanced stage of goal pursuit more than
at the initial stage. In the same vein, Garcia
et al., (2006) reported that ranking coincides
with the standard influence of social behavior
of individuals, which, in turn, motivates
62
high competition. Moreover, proximity-toa-standard stresses that ranking discourages
cooperative efforts and negatively affects
socializing with others. In other terms, ranking
can affect the social behavior of individuals by
preventing beneficial effects of cooperation.
In contrast, Yang (1986) reported that people
with different types of self-construal pursue
different goals. Individuals from collectivist
cultural backgrounds (e.g., China) tend
to possess predominantly interdependent
self-construal (i.e., stability and social
relations), which motivates them to pursue
the maintenance of goals. On the other hand,
those from individualist cultural backgrounds
(e.g., the U.S.) tend to possess independent
self-construal (i.e., unique achievement
and advancement-related activities), which
motivates them to pursue the attainment of
goals. Together with study 1 results, which
showed that higher collectivism is likely to
extend cooperative efforts even if individuals
are in a highly competitive situation, we can
argue that although individualism discourages
cooperative efforts, collectivism encourages
cooperative efforts, even when ranking agreed
with standards in a competitive situation. In
line with this notion, we propose that cultural
differences moderate cooperative effort and
result in individuals either focusing their effort
on collaboration or on competition.
H4: Although proximity-to-a-standard
discourages cooperative efforts among
individuals from individualist cultures,
collectivism encourages cooperative efforts
regardless o f such standard.
Study 2
The primary objective of this study was to
investigate whether cultural differences between
the U.S., Korea, China, and Sri Lanka influenced
cooperative efforts (hypothesis 4). The secondary
objective was to test the robustness of the
relationship between cultural orientation and
levels of competitiveness.
Method
The tools used in Study 2 were the same as
those in Study 1. However, the design of the
questionnaire differed in having two parts (A
and B). Part A was designed to gather general
information from respondents and part B
was designed to capture collaborative and
competitive efforts. In part B, scenario-based
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
questions were directed to respondents to test
whether cooperative efforts differed according to
cultural orientation or competitive position (top
or low rank) or both.
Measurements
The scenario presented situations to participants
where sales representatives were assigned ranks
each year and explained why such ranks were
important to them. Fifty sales representatives
were ranked from 1 (top) to 50 (bottom) based
on their sales’ performance, such as volume,
efficiency and service. This personal ranking was
important to them to promote sales and increase
their earnings. Further, sales representatives
were chosen to compete individually or work
collaboratively. However, earning capacity
increased from 5% to 7% as a result of shifting
from competitive to collaborative work, while
collaborators increased their sales by 15%
(see Appendix 2). The scenario was followed
by two narratives and two sub-questions. In a
within-subject design, two narratives provided a
background about different ranking and sales: “In
this scenario, imagine that your rank is #1. While
you are searching for ways and means to further
increase sales, another sales representative
ranked #2 (second narrative; ranked #11)
proposes that you work collaboratively.”
Participants were first asked to select their
preferred option (collaborative or competitive)
to increase their sales performance and then
asked to indicate the extent to which they held
competitive feelings toward the other sales
representative (if they were supposed to compete
on a seven-scale rating from 1 = not at all to 7
= very much). These same questions were also
used with the second narrative.
The total sample of 131 (highest mode age
group = 20-24; 40% male) was distributed in
the U.S. {N = 27; highest mode age group =
20-24; 70% male), Korea (N = 33; highest mode
age group = 20-24; 48% male), China (N = 35;
highest mode age group = 25-29; 39% male),
and Sri Lanka (N = 36; highest mode age group
= 25-29; 12% male). This study was designed
to cover 30 respondents from each country, but
it was only possible to obtain 27 responses from
the U.S.
Analysis and results
A four-country (U.S., Korea, China, and
Sri Lanka) X 2 (competitive option and
collaborative option) chi-square test of
independence was performed to examine the
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
relationship between the cultural orientation
of countries and cooperative effort (vs.
competitive effort) when the ranking coincided
with a standard (proximity-to-a-standard). We
found a significant relationship between the
cultural orientation of a country—as identified
in study 1—and cooperative efforts when
both sales representatives were ranked 1 and
2 respectively (A2 (3, n - 131) = 14.69, p =
0.002, Cramer’s v = 0.33). Collectivism was
highly associated with cooperative efforts while
individualism was associated with competitive
efforts (see Figure 3). Thus, H4 was verified,
and we can conclude that although proximityto-a-standard discourages cooperative efforts
among individuals from individualistic cultures,
collectivist cultures encourage cooperative
efforts. Put simply, Americans are unlikely
to accept an invitation from an immediate
competitor to work collaboratively, whereas Sri
Lankans, Chinese, and Koreans are likely to do
so to various degrees. However, the relationship
between a country’s cultural orientation and
cooperative efforts (vs. competitive effort) was
not significant (A2 (3, n = 130) = 5.35, p = .15)
when ranking did not agree with a standard.
In other terms, the identified relationship (i.e.,
invitation from an immediate competitor)
disappeared once a distant competitor proposed
to work (from rank 11 to 1) collaboratively.
Individuals in all countries studied were willing
to work cooperatively with distant competitors,
as long as they felt that their position was
squared. Even though this willingness was not
statistically significant, the total percentage of
individuals (34.6%) who chose the competitive
option was analogue to the total percentage
of individuals who selected to compete with
an immediate competitor. This study provides
further evidence to support H3b.
Additionally, we performed a one-way
between-group ANOVA to explore the impact
of cultural orientation in the U.S., Korea, China,
and Sri Lanka on competitiveness. We used the
degree of competitive feelings toward immediate
competitors (top rankings) as a dependent
variable because, in Study 1, individual cultural
patterns significantly explained the variation
in competitiveness. There was a statistically
significant difference at the p < .05 level in the
degree of competitive feeling across the four
countries (F(3, 127) = 5.308, P = .002, partial
beta squared = .11). Post-hoc comparisons using
Tukey’s HSD test indicated that the mean value
of competitiveness for the U.S. {M = 4.07, SD
63
Figure 3. Relationship between Cultural Orientation and Competitiveness/Collaborative Effort
Response
I Competitive
I Collaborative
US
Korea
China
SL
Country
= 2.48) was significantly different from China
(M = 2.23 ,SD = 2.32, p = 0.01) and Sri Lanka
(M = 1.89, SD = 1.93, p = 0.001). Nevertheless,
the mean value for Korea (Af = 2.73, SD = 2.36)
was not statistically different from the other
three countries (see Figure 4). These results also
follow the same pattern illustrated in Figure 2,
except for the U.S.
Discussion
In Study 2, we demonstrated that although
proximity-to-a-standard discourages cooperative
efforts in individualist cultures, collectivist
cuhures encourage cooperative efforts. Working
cooperatively results in a marginal improvement
of position while improving a competitor’s
position more than two fold in terms of sales
performance. In that context, collaborating
with an immediate competitor would adversely
affect future gain associated with ranking,
more so than collaborating with a distant
competitor. Hence, it seems that individualists
were attracted to the competitive option to
secure their position. To further support this
point, competitors also chose to collaborate with
distant competitors. This finding is consistent
with the notion that ranking stimulates
individuals to compete on scale rather than on
task comparison, undermining rational choice
strategy (Garcia and Tor, 2007). In contrast,
collectivists accepted to work collectively not
only with distant competitors, but also with
immediate competitors, perhaps because they
valued communal sharing more than competitive
(market) sharing (Singelis, et al., 1995). Study 2
also provided evidence to support the robustness
of proximity-to-a-standard only among
individualists rather than collectivists.
This study also highlighted a similar pattern,
which emerged in Study 1, namely that there is a
positive relationship between a country’s cultural
orientation and competitiveness, though with
a slight difference. The difference was that the
competitiveness of Americans was recorded as
highest out of the four countries in accordance
with prior research (e.g., Houston et al., 2012).
This convergence is, perhaps, due to relatively
high representation of VI, more than HI, subjects
in the Study 2 sample, despite the absence of
individualism-collectivism measurement data.
This issue does not appear in collectivism
measurements because horizontal-vertical
collectivism is not infinitely distinguishable
(Singelis et al., 1995). In essence, individuals
from individualist cultures like the U.S., and
cultures in transition like Korea, were highly
compeiitive compared with collectivist cultures
like Sri Lanka.
Results from Study 1 indicated that the
U.S., Korea, and China were different in
cultural orientation. To a greater extent Sri
Lankans were collectivistic (horizontal and
vertical) compared to Koreans and Chinese.
Koreans were moderately low collectivistic,
64
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
Figure 4. Cultural Orientation and Competitiveness
whereas Chinese were moderately high collectivistic. Americans were individualistic,
although, in our sample they were poor in
VI. This provides convergent results to those
exposed in modernization theory as well as
cultural heritage theory. The World Economic
Outlook (International Monetary Fund, 2015)
has classified the U.S. and Korea as advanced
economies, while classifying China and Sri
Lanka as emerging and developing economies,
respectively. Consistent with modernization
theory, Sri Lanka, being a developing country,
is associated with high levels of collectivism,
while China, being an emerging country, is
associating with moderately high collectiv
ism. As an advanced country, the U.S. is
aligned with individualism whereas Korea is
moderately low in collectivism. This cultural
asymmetry between Korea and the U.S. is
congruent with cultural heritage theory, in the
sense that in Korea, being an advanced coun
try, collectivism is still apparent because of its
resistance to change and strong connections to
traditions and heritage. However, parallel to
the rapid economic growth, the generation gap
is not sufficient to confirm the role played by
the cultural heritage. Meanwhile, our results
indicate that the gap between individualistic
and collectivistic dimensions was minimal in
Korea compared with the other two collectiv
ist countries. Congruent with this idea, VI
appeared to emerge among individuals from
China and Korea, highlighting the possibility
that the next generation might be competitive.
Yang (1986) also mentioned that, over the
years, the personality profile of Chinese had
been shifting toward individual orientation
away from social orientation.
Consistent with prior research findings
(Houston et al., 2012) across the four countries,
individualism was positively correlated with
competition. At the country level, the competi
tiveness of individuals depended on the extent
to which each country shared individualistic and
collectivistic cultural dimensions. With regard to
the U.S., though Study 1 reported modest levels
of competition associated with HI, study 2 de
picted the highest competitiveness. This incon
sistency may be the result of the clear divisibility
of horizontal-vertical individualism in the U.S.
(Singelis et al., 1995) which has been reflected
in our two samples. Thus, our results are congru
ent with prior findings—American students are
more competitive than Balinese (Houston et al.,
2012). Being a low collectivist country, Korea
showed higher competitiveness, whereas China
showed modest competitiveness. However, Sri
Lanka, being a high collectivist country, present-
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
65
ed notably lower competitiveness. Moreover, the
positive correlation between horizontal-vertical
individualism and the degree of competitiveness
varied depending on the competitive context—
proximity-to-a-standard. Individualists who held
top ranks appeared to be highly competitive
compared with collectivists across both studies.
The proximity-to-a-standard was robust only
among individualists, imposing boundary condi
tion for the theory. If IC is treated as individual
differences (Cozma, 2011), the individualist and
collectivist behavior is aligned with the social
comparison model of completion (Garcia et al.,
2013) in which situational factors (i.e., proximity-to-a-standard and N-effect) indirectly shape
individual factors (i.e., individual difference; in
dividualist vs. collectivist). In our study, perhaps
the collectivist individual factor has dominated
over the indirect influence of situational factor—
proximity-to-a-standard— resulting in coopera
tive behavior. In contrast to small competition
groups, proximity-to-a-standard was evidenced
in large competition group. The N-effect was
neither found nor associated with individualism
in our study because of the use of multi-contest
competition, where insufficient weight seems to
discourage competitive behavior in groups with
fewer competitors.
condition. We suggest that multi-contest
competitions with different weights for each
stage may lead to perceived different contests
and competitions as a whole in accord with
how each stage is weighted. Response to such
contextual changes can be immediately observed
in small groups more than in large groups; in
effect, individuals are likely to ratchet their
competitive behavior upward or downward.
Such behavior may limit the N-effect. In our
study, the cooperative behavior of competitors
among top rankers results either from the
dominant effect of task (vs. scale) comparison
or individual factors (vs. situational factors)
attributed from collectivist cultures. Otherwise,
collectivist cultures may trigger task comparison
over scale comparison. However, both studies
(i.e., minimum task-oriented and high taskoriented) provide evidence that cooperative
behavior is prevalent because of individual
factors—individualist and collectivist - rather
than task comparison. Therefore, we suggest that
individual factors (personality), which are highly
influenced by cultures, attenuate the effect of
proximity-to-a-standard.
Concluding Remarks
Theoretical contribution
Though our main focus was not to investigate
cultural dynamism rigorously, this study contrib
utes to the existing body of knowledge of cross
culture in three ways. First, prior research has
only compared one type of culture with another
type, whereas this study has covered three col
lectivist cultures and one individualistic culture
wi;h vertical-horizontal and individualisticcollectivist cultural dimensions. Therefore, our
findings advance our knowledge, especially, in
relation to how the orientations of three col
lectivist countries differ from each other (e.g.,
Sri Lanka’s cultural orientation is different from
Korea’s and China’s) and differ according to the
level of economic advancement. Second, prior
researchers have stressed the importance of in
vestigating whether existing cultural dimensions
adequately describe the cultural complexity of
less developed countries. The inclusion of Sri
Lanka as a developing country in our study helps
to fill this knowledge gap to some extent (e.g.,
classification of Sri Lanka as VC culture).
Third, we contribute to proximity-to-astandard and N-effect by introducing boundary
Implications of the study
Managers who expect to work as expatriates in
Asia can regard Korea and China (perhaps even
Japan) as separate cultures from Sri Lanka with
regard to collectivism. This study demonstrated
that Western managerial assumptions about
competition cannot be applied outside of
these countries as it can be in countries or
cultures that promote in-group cooperative
efforts. Therefore, the promotion of out-group
competition rather than in-group would be
productive in VC cultures like Sri Lanka and
India—for example, between departmental,
product lines or branches. Further, though
ranking motivated competition and efficiency
in organizational contexts, Study 1 and authors
such as Garcia and Tor (2007) highlight that top
ranking precludes beneficial cooperative efforts,
boosting organizational overall performance.
On the other hand, such competitive behavior
may hinder the sharing of beneficial information
and organizational resources and not be suitable
for organizations where managers expect to
foster a learning culture. In contrast, the effects
of ranking methodology seemed to be minimal
in promoting competition in collectivist work
cultures. Collectivists seemed to accept ranking
as a relative position among colleagues rather
than as a compare their scale with others,
66
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
because pow er distance is also part o f the culture
(G raf et al., 2012). N onetheless, group ranking
m ethods (individuals are assig ned to a group on
a rational basis to prom ote com petitio n betw een
groups) w ould be m ore suitable fo r collectivist
w ork cultures to m otivate com petitiveness.
A ll in all, our findings provid e guidance for
in creasin g overall organizational perform ance by
optim izin g com petitive behavior in tw o different
natio nal w ork cultures.
A senior lecturer in the Department o f Business
Economics, Mr. Madurapperuma is working
toward a Ph.D. Dr. Kyung-min is a professor
o f marketing, and Dr. Dharmadasa heads the
Department o f International Business.
REFERENCES
A ycan, Z., R abin dra, K ., M anuel, M ., K aicheng, Y., Jurgen,
D ., G unter, S., and A nw ar, K urshid. (2000). Im pact o f
cultu re on hum an resource m anagem ent practices: A
10-C ountry com parison. Applied Psychology, 49(1),
Limitations and future directions
T he results presented here are subjected to
few lim itations. E liciting decision-m aking
m eth odolo gy from respondents based on
hypoth etical scenarios is a w idely used and
accepted m eth odolo gy in consum er behavior and
psychology research. To rule out order effect,
questions are p u t to respondents in half-reverse
or random order. In our study, how ever, w e w ere
not able to adhere to this m eth od ow ing to the
difficulty o f ad m in istering questionnaires in
four languages in fo u r countries. N evertheless,
w e believe th at the effect o f the o rder d id not
ham per the validity o f findings.
Further, although w e have discussed w hy
N -effect w as not found in our study, the
m echanism th at brought about such results has
not been explored. First, it is w orth investigating
N -effect in a m ultistage com petitio n context
w ith different w eights for each stage. Second, it
is w orth investigating proxim ity-to-a-standard
and N -effect in a context o f repeated contests
(R idlon and Shin, 2013).
O ur results show ed th at collectivists tend to
w ork collaboratively w ith im m ediate com peti
tors in the sam e group even w hen they are in
proxim ity-to-a-standard. It can be expected
that collectivists— especially V C — behave in
a com petitive fashio n w hen they co m pete w ith
outsid e groups. F utu re research is required to
investigate this phenom enon by using a group
rankin g m ethod. Finally, w e observed som e
vertical-horizontal and individ ualism -collectiv
ism cultural patterns in relation to the level o f
econom ic advancem ent. F o r instance, less devel
oped countries appeared to be dom inated by V C,
but w ith econom ic advancem ent this is likely to
shift tow ards V I. F utu re research should focus
on buildin g a m odel reflecting such cultu ral
shifts relative to econom ic advancem ent. In this
respect, cross-cultural as w ell as longitudinal
research w ould be helpful.
192-221.
A ycan, Z., R abin dra, N ., K anungo., and Jai, B. R S. (1999).
O rganizational cultu re and h u m an resource m an ag em en t
practices: T he m odel o f cultu re fit. Journal o f CrossCultural Psychology, 30(4), 501-526.
B rislin , R. W. (1980). Translation and content analysis o f
oral and written material. B oston: A lly n & B acon.
C hen, E , and S tephen G. W. (2008). M easurin g
in d iv idualism an d collectivism : T h e im p o rtan ce o f
consid erin g d ifferential co m ponents, reference groups,
and m easu rem en t invariance. Journal o f Research in
Personality, 42(2), 259-294.
C ozm a, I. (2011). H ow are in d iv idualism and collectivism
m easured? Romanian Journal o f Applied Psychology,
73(1), 11-17.
Farrell, S., an d R alp h, A . H . (2001). Im pro ving salesforce
perform ance: A m eta-analy tic in vestigation o f the
effectiveness and u tility o f perso n n el selection
procedures and trainin g interventions. Psychology and
Marketing, 18(3), 281-316.
G arcia, S. M ., and Tor, A. (2 009).T he N -E ffect: M ore
com petito rs, less com petition. Psychological Science,
20(1), 871-877.
G arcia, S. M ., and Tor, A. (2007). R ankin gs, standards, and
com petition: Task vs. scale com parisons. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(1), 95-108.
G arcia, S. M ., Tor, A ., and R ichard , G. (2006). R anks and
rivals: A theory o f co m petition. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 32(1), 970-982.
G arcia, S. M ., Tor., and Tyrone M . S. (2 013).T he
p sy cholo gy o f com petition: A social com parison
persp ective. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
8(6), 634-650.
G raf, A ., Sabin e, T., K oeszegi., an d E va-M aria , P. (2012).
C ros-cultural n egotiatio ns and p ow er distan ce:
S trategies applied b y A sian and E uro p ean b uyers and
sellers in electro n ic negotiatio ns. Nankai Business
Review International, 3(3), 242-256.
G reen, E. G . T., Je an-C la ude, D ., and D ario , P. (2005).
V ariation o f indiv idualism and co llectivism w ithin and
betw een 20 countries: A typo lo g ical analysis. Journal of
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36(3), 321-239.
H am am ura, T. (2012). A re cultu res b ecom in g
in div idualistic? A cross-te m poral co m p ariso n o f
in d iv id u alism -co llectiv ism in th e U n ited S tates and
Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1),
3-24.
H ofstede, G. (1980). M otivation, leadership, and
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
67
organization: D o A m erican theories apply abroad?
Organizational Dynamics, 9(1), 42-63.
H ouston, J. M ., H oyt, E., Libby, E. A., C okorda, B.
J. L., and Luh, K. S. (2012). C om petitiveness and
individualism -collectivism in B ab and the U .S. N orth
American Journal o f Psychology, 14(1), 163-73.
H ouston, J. M ., Paul, B. H., Robert, M ., R ebecca, B., and
H ideki, K. (2005). C om petitiveness am ong Japanese,
Chinese, and A m erican undergraduate students.
Psychological Reports, 97(1), 205-212.
H ui, C. H. (1988). M easurem ent o f individualism collectivism. Journal o f Research in Personality, 22(1),
17-36.
International M onetary Fund. (2015). World economic
outlook: Adjusting to lower commodity prices.
W ashington: International M onetary Fund.
Kale, S., and John, B. (1992). U nderstanding the dom ain
of cross-national buyer-seller interactions. Journal o f
International Business Studies, 23(1), 101-132.
K irkm an, B. L., and D ebra, L. S. (2001). The im pact of
cultural values on jo b satisfaction and organizational
com m itm ent in self-m anaging work team s: The
m ediating role o f em ployee resistance. The Academy o f
Management Journal, 44(3), 557-569.
K itayam a, S., H yekyung, P. A., Timur, S. M. K ., and Ayse,
K. U. (2009). A cultural task analysis o f im plicit inde
pendence: C om paring N orth A m erica, W estern Europe,
and E ast Asia. Journal o f Personality and Social Psy
chology, 97(2), 236-255.
M iroshnik, V. (2002). C ulture and international
m anagement: A review. Journal o f Management
Development, 21(1), 521-544.
PaLant, J. (2007). Survival manual: A step by step guide
to data analysis using SPSS fo r Windows. Berkshire:
M cGraw -H ill House.
Ricks, D. A. (2006). Blunders in international business (4th
ed.). O xford: Blackwell.
Ridlon, R., and Jiw oong, S. (2013). Favoring the w inner
or loser in repeated contests. Marketing Science, 32(5),
768-785.
S a n a h a , S. A., Joshua, T. B., and Robert, W. P. (2014). The
role o f culture in international relationship marketing.
Journal o f Marketing, 78(5), 78-98.
68
Singelis, T. M ., Harry, C. T., Dharm , R. S. B., and M ichele,
J. G. (1995). H orizontal and vertical dim ensions of
individualism and collectivism : A theoretical and
m easurem ent refinem ent. Cross-Cultural Research,
29(3), 240-275.
Snibbe, A. C., and H azel, R. M. (2005). You can ’t alw ays
get w hat you want: E ducational attainm ent, agency, and
choice. Journal o f Personality and Social Psychology,
88(4), 703-720.
Steenkam p, J. E. M. (2001). The role o f national culture
in international m arketing research. International
Marketing Review, 18(1), 30-44.
Triandis, A. C., R obert, B., Hector, B., M ichael, B.,
K wok, L., A belando, B . , ... G erm aine, D. M. (1986).
T he m easurem ent o f etic aspects o f individualism and
collecticism across cultures. Australian Journal o f
Psuchology, 38(3), 251-261.
Triandis, H. C., Robert, B., and M arcelo, J. V. (1988).
Individualism and collectivism : Cross-cultural
perspectives on self-ingroup relationships. Journal o f
Personality and Social Psychology, 54(2), 323-338.
Triandis, H. C., and M ichele, J. G. (1998). Converging
m easurem ent o f horizontal and vertical individualism
and collectivism . Journal o f Personality and Social
Psychology, 74(1), 118-128
Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism & collectivism.
Boulder: Westview.
U lem an, E. R., and Jam es, S., and H oon, K. L. (1996).
Variations in collectivism and individualism by ingroup
and culture: C onfirm atory factor analyses. Journal o f
Personality and Social Psychology, 71(5), 1037-1054.
Vadi, M., and M ichael, V. (2006). T he deposit o f
collectivism in organizational culture in R ussia: Som e
consequences o f hum an resources m anagem ent. Baltic
Journal o f Management, 7(2), 188-200.
Vandegrift, D ., and B rian, H. (2012). C om petitive behavior:
Tests o f the N -effect and proxim ity-to-a-standard.
Journal o f Neuroscience, Psychology, & Economics,
5(3), 182-192.
Yang, K. (1986). Chinese personality and its change. The
social psychology o f Chinese people. M ichael H arris
Bond, ed. H ong Kong: O xford U niversity Press.
SAM Advanced Management Journal — Autumn 2016
Copyright of SAM Advanced Management Journal (07497075) is the property of Society for
Advancement of Management and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites
or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However,
users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.