University of Massachusets - Amherst
ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst
Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series
Linguistics
1996
Prosodic Morphology 1986
John J. McCarthy
University of Massachusets, Amherst, jmccarthy@linguist.umass.edu
Alan Prince
Follow this and additional works at: htp://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs
Part of the Morphology Commons, Near Eastern Languages and Societies Commons, and the
Phonetics and Phonology Commons
McCarthy, John J. and Prince, Alan, "Prosodic Morphology 1986" (1996). Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series. Paper 13.
htp://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/13
his Book is brought to you for free and open access by the Linguistics at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in
Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please
contact scholarworks@library.umass.edu.
Prosodic Morphology 1986
John J. McCarthy
Department of Linguistics
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
MPFFDUWK\#OLQJXLVWXPDVVHGX
Alan S. Prince
Department of Linguistics & RuCCS
Rutgers University, New Brunswick
SULQFH#UXFFVUXWJHUVHGX
Final Revision: October 25, 1996
Acknowledgments (1986)
This document is excerpted from a longer work now in progress. The research reported here was first presented
at the 3rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics held at UCLA on March 29, 1985. Subsequent
presentations were made to audiences at the University of Texas, Austin, Brown University, Harvard University,
AT&T Bell Laboratories, the Second Seoul International Conference on Linguistics, the Max Planck Institute,
and the authors’ home institutions. We note with gratitude the contributions of Junko Itô and Armin Mester at
various stages, and we thank the audience at the 3rd WCCFL (especially Bruce Hayes and Nick Clements) and
our colleagues Lisa Selkirk and Moira Yip for their challenging questions.
Introduction (1996)
This work has circulated in manuscript form since October, 1986. Its basic contents were first
presented at WCCFL 3 in spring, 1986 to an audience that was not devoid of convinced believers
in the C and the V. It has been cited variously as McCarthy & Prince 1986, M&P forthcoming, and
even (optimistically) M&P in press.
Many of the proposals made here have been revised, generalized, or superseded in subsequent
work (see the bibliography below, p. 84), including a book ms. of nearly the same title by exactly
the same authors. Junko Itô and Armin Mester have suggested to us that it might still be useful
to make the 1986 manuscript available through an official venue. Pursuing their suggestion, we
are putting the ms. out in final form as a technical report, with a proper and indeed augmented
bibliography, with outright errors noted and corrected, and with some added commentary.
We have kept the text very much the same as in the original version. A few trivial mistakes have
been corrected silently. More serious missteps are discussed in remarks like this one, interpolated
among the paragraphs of the original text. We have also inserted a number of brief comments
and pointers to later developments, particularly when they have extended (or put shut to) a theme
raised in these pages. For convenience, these scholia are included in the table of contents, in the
same distinctive typeface.
Preparation of this document was supported by the National Science Foundation (grant SBR9420424) and Rutgers University. We owe a large debt to John Alderete and Jill Beckman, whose
assistance was indispensable.
John McCarthy
Alan Prince
i
Table of Contents
1.
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
1.1
Templatic Morphology within Prosodic Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Counting Allomorphy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
The XXX Template . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
1.2
Outline of the Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
The Prosodic Categories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
Minimal Word . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6
More on the Minimal Word . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Foot Typology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Foot Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
Mapping Principles . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
2.
Elaboration and Exemplification . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10
2.1
Prefixation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10
The Simple Syllable as Prefix Target . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10
Ilokano Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 10
Orokaiva Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12
ONSET, Templates, & Alignment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
The Core Syllable as Prefix Target
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
The Core Syllable . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
Sanskrit Vowel-Initial Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
The Heavy Syllable as Prefix Target . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
Mokilese . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
Quantitative Transfer . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
Ponapean: Heavy and Light ) as Prefixes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
More on Ponapean . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
2.2
Ponapean I: Consonant-initial Stems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Ponapean II: Vowel-Initial Stems . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Ponapean III: Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Ponapean IV: Proleptic and Historical Note . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Minimal Word/Foot as Prefix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
19
23
24
24
24
Diyari Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Lardil Phonology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Reduplication in Yidig and Lardil . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Makassarese Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Prosodic Reparsing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
25
25
29
29
30
Suffixation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
The Minimal Word or Foot as Suffix Target . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30
Manam Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
The Syllable as Suffix Target . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
Alignment in Suffixing Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32
Kamaiurá Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34
2.3
Infixation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34
Infixation via Extraprosodicity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Prefix as Infix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35
Infixing Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
VC Infixation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Uradhi and Timugon Murut Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Washo Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
38
39
40
43
Suffix as Infix . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
Korean Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
Infixation via Affixation to a Prosodic Constituent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44
Infixation by Affixation to Prosodic Constituent . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
ii
2.4
Templatic Morphology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
Truncation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
More on Truncation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
Zuuja-go . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48
Templatic Well-formedness Requirements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
True Templatic Morphology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51
Semitic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51
More on Semitic Morphology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52
Cupeño . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54
Other Templatic Morphology . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
3.
Other Issues in Templatic Structure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
3.1
Syllable-internal Structure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56
Moraic Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Intrasyllabic Structure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Tübatulabal Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Language Games . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
3.2
56
57
58
60
Prespecification? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 61
Tone . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Fixed i in Yoruba Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
The Fixed High Vowel in Akan Reduplication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
Melodic Overwriting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .
63
63
65
68
3.3
Contour Segments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
3.4
Phonological Consequences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
Affricates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 69
Compensatory Lengthening in Moraic Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
4.
The Nature of the Mapping Relation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
4.1
Melody Copying on the Same Tier . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
Edge-in Association & Alignment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
Base/Reduplicant Identity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
4.2
4.3
Melody Copying on a Different Tier . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
Synchronous Skeleta . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
4.4
Line Crossing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
Overapplication, Transfer . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
Line-Crossing Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
Dorsey’s Law . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
Bibliography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
iii
1.
Introduction
1.1
Templatic Morphology within Prosodic Theory
A central strategy for deriving words requires that a base accommodate to a target frame: the invariant that
identifies a morpheme lies in its overall shape rather than in its phonemic composition. The reduplicative perfect
prefix of Indo-European is always (C)V; its content is borrowed from the root. The causative/factitive ‘measure
II’ of the Classical Arabic verb conforms to the pattern CVCCVC. Such descriptive observations lie at the heart
of recent studies of reduplicative and root-and-pattern morphologies. Here we will inquire into the nature of the
targets used in such systems: we will show that they must be defined in terms of the categories and rules of
prosody, as provided by the theory of syllabification, stress, and accent. Our immediate goal is to provide a basis
for nonconcatenative morphology; our broader goal is to circumscribe the modes of reference to structural
information in phonology; and to characterize the class of structures that are authentically essential to
phonological representation.
Basic findings in prosody place strong conditions of adequacy on template theory. It is worth examining
the chief interactions, since we can immediately rule out many plausible-seeming approaches while establishing
the general constraints within which template theory must work.
Consider first the role of counting in grammar. How long may a count run? General considerations of
locality, now the common currency in all areas of linguistic thought, suggest that the answer is probably ‘up to
two’: a rule may fix on one specified element and examine a structurally adjacent element and no other. For
example, the ‘End Rule’ of Prince (1983) focuses on one edge of a domain and selects the element adjacent to
that edge for some specified operation; similar cases can easily be multiplied.
What elements may be counted? It is a commonplace of phonology that rules count moras (µ), syllables
()), or feet (F) but never segments. Many languages place a two-mora bound on the minimum size of majorcategory words; this follows from the prosodic hierarchy, if prosodic words must contain feet and feet can be no
smaller than 2 µ. Exactly this state of affairs is demonstrated for Estonian in Prince (1980); an interesting sideeffect is that the rule of apocope apparent in the nominative singular is blocked when its output violates this
condition:1
(1) Estonian Word Minimality
a. /kana/
b. /konna/
c. /tänava/
kana
kon:n
tänav
*kan
‘chicken, nom.sg.’
‘pig, nom.sg.’
‘street, nom.sg.’
Final consonants are provably extrametrical, so that no form like *kan is admissible as a noun. In Kyoto (Kansai)
Japanese, where the one allowed final consonant (N) is fully moraic, content words shaped CV are excluded: all
historically monomoraic items have been lengthened (CV > CV:) to conform to the 2 µ limit. A typical variation
is reported for Caughnawaga Mohawk in Michelson (1981): verbs must be disyllabic, and undersized collocations
of morphemes are expanded by epenthesis.
(2) Mohawk Word Minimality
a. /k + tats + s/
b. /hs + yaks + s/
iktats
ihsyaks
‘I offer’
‘you are cutting’
Crucially, Mohawk prosody is insensitive to the light/heavy distinction, so that F is minimally [))].2
1
A similar point is made about apocope in Lardil by Wilkinson (1986).
2
We might say µ=) in such cases.
2
McCarthy & Prince
Counting restrictions often determine nonphonological allomorphy as well. In Dyirbal (Dixon 1972), by
a kind of compensation, the ergative suffix is bimoraic -×gu with minimal (disyllabic) bases but monomoraic -gu
with longer ones.
(3) Dyirbal Size-based Allomorphy
a. /yaqa/
yaqa-×gu
b. /yamani/
yamani-gu
‘man’
‘rainbow’
A short word contains a single F; a long word, something more.3 . In English, comparative -er and superlative -est
are pretty much restricted to minimal (monopod) words:
(4) English Size-based Allomorphy
a. redder
stupider
b. yellower
*obtuser
noblest
*augustest
Outside the realm of morphology proper, we find that the to-dative alternation in English is essentially limited
to one-foot verbs (Grimshaw 1985): thus, ‘give/offer the men the ball’, but * ‘donate the men the ball’.
Counting Allomorphy
An analysis of syllable/mora-counting allomorphy in terms of prosodic circumscription is offered
in McCarthy &Prince 1990a. A different account, based on prosodic subcategorization, is put
forward in M&P 1993a: Chapt. 7. Other recent work includes Mester 1995 and Kager 1996ab.
No language process, however, is known to depend on the raw number of segments in a form: a robust
finding, given the frequency and pervasiveness of counting restrictions. It should come as no suprise that
templatic morphology can’t count segments either. If a reduplicative prefix target could be XXX — three
segments, unadorned with prosodic structure — the following impossible type of system should be common:
(5) Hypothetical XXX Reduplication
a. badupi BAD badupi
b. bladupi BLA bladupi
c. adupi ADU adupi
What’s prosodically incoherent here is the segmental equation of monomoraic BLA with bimoraic BAD and
ADU.
It is striking, then, that current theories of template form are essentially segmental, allowing prosodic
annotation as an option or alternative to be called on when necessary. The CV-theory of McCarthy (1981), taken
up in Marantz (1982), has been generalized to the syllable-point theory of Lowenstamm & Kaye (1986) and the
X-theory of Levin (1983), most extensively explored in Levin (1985). In the syllable-point theory,
uncharacterized segmental skeletal nodes are seen as dependents of syllables. In the X-theory, in its various
instantiations, a level of segmental structure, unmarked for the C/V distinction, is distinguished by higher-level
prosodic structure. Although studies conducted within these theories have vastly increased our knowledge and
understanding of templatic systems, their basic representational assumptions cannot stand. Templates by their
nature count elements: CV- or X-theories must count segments, and must count many of them. Consider the
template-of-templates that generates the various forms of the Classical Arabic verb:
3
Word-length distinctions are made on similar grounds in Biblical Hebrew (Dresher 1983), Japanese (M. Liberman, reported in Poser
(1984b)), and Ponapean (see section 2.1). Other cases of counting allomorphy abound — Spanish (Harris 1979), Pukapukan (Chung 1978),
Maaori (Biggs 1961) — all referring to syllables or moras.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
3
(6) Segmental Skeleton for the Arabic Verb
a. (C) C V (X) C V C
N
N
b. (X) X X (X) X X X
By this, 7 segments must be counted. Our proposal (in section 2.4) will be that the template is [) )], the familiar
count to 2, with extrametricality allowing for the extra initial position. Within X-theory, the simplest and
therefore most highly valued templates are purely segmental: indeed Levin (1985) proposes that the (impossible)
template XXX is attested in Mokilese: below, we show that the actual template is )µµ , a heavy syllable. The
descriptive success of XXX is an artifact of the restricted syllable structure of this language.
The XXX Template
In fact, what Levin 1985 proposes is that the Mokilese template is [XXX]), not bare XXX. (The
erroneous attribution in the text is a result of consulting a low-quality photocopy of Levin 1985.)
The point still holds, though. In segmentalism, XXX is the simplest and therefore most highly
valued template, yet it is factually impossible. Moreover, even with a syllabic appurtenance, as in
[XXX]), it still characterizes a factually impossible situation of segment counting, in which there
is an equivalence among the prosodically disjoint set CCV, CVV, CVC, and VCC.
Alone among students of the template, Hyman (1985) has rejected a segmental level of representation
in favor of a weight structure that is essentially moraic. Our results, although largely complementary to his, bear
significant resemblances to his work.
The fundamental goal of a template representation system must be to characterize the shape-invariant
that unites the various allomorphs. Here prosody diverges notably from segmentalism. If we say that the template
is [)], then all segmental sequences comprising a licit syllable of the language are in the equivalence class: {V,
CV, CVC, CCVC}, for example, would be a typical set of realizations. Since no single segmental string is
conserved, segmentalism must supplement the representational theory with principles that serve to equate strings
in the set. Following Marantz (1982), segmental theories spell out the template as the longest observed realization
(or even the union of the observed realizations, if distinct from the longest); when an insufficiency of melody
leaves template slots empty, they are discarded. The distinction between the two approaches can be made clear
with an example. In the Philippine language Ilokano, the progressive is formed by reduplicative prefixation, as
shown below:
(7) Reduplication in Ilokano
a. /basa/
b. /adal/
c. /takder/
d. /trabaho/
ag - BAS - basa
ag - AD - adal
ag - TAK - takder
ag - TRAB- trabaho
‘be reading’
‘be studying’
‘be standing’
‘be working’
4
McCarthy & Prince
Segmentalism must analyze the prefix as CCVC, explicitly counting out the maximal monosyllable.4 We propose
that the target is simply ); given a copy of the bare melody, it satisfies itself to the fullest extent allowed by the
usual rules of the language.
(8)
)
+ )))
trabaho trabaho
Notice that stem syllabification is inhibited by the usual onset priority considerations, hence [tra][ba]...; the prefix
) faces no such competition, hence [trab].
As example (7) illustrates, segmentalism is typically faced with an excess of underlying slots. There are
well-known ways in which unfilled slots influence phonology and morphology (Selkirk 1981, Clements & Keyser
1983, Marlett & Stemberger 1983, Lowenstamm & Kaye 1986). It is a remarkable fact that empty templatic slots
have never been convincingly detected outside their endo-theoretic role in melody association.5 We conclude that
they do not exist.
In essence, segmentalism must hold that all template elements are optional until proven otherwise. It is
thus in principle incapable of specifying, in the representation, that certain elements are obligatory, a common
situation. We show below that the reduplicative prefix in Ponapean is a heavy syllable: segmentally, this means
CVX, with the X required and the C optional. The additional conditions follow immediately from the syllabic
characterization, since onsets are optional initially in the language and heavy syllables must of course have a
postnuclear element. Nothing in the segmental theory guarantees this result.
One final observation seals the case against excess elements in templates. It is a stable empirical finding
that templates imitate — up to extrametricality — the prosodic structure of the language at hand. There is no
Arabic template CVCCCVC; correlatively, the syllabification of the language disallows triconsonantal clusters.
Segmental theory, however, cannot derive this result. Since excess or stray elements are erased, they are free to
occur, and indeed must occur in other circumstances. Were they present, even fleetingly, they could perturb
melody association in easily discoverable ways. For example, the Arabic template CVCCVC, with which
*CVCCCVC would be neutralized, requires special conditions to override left-to-right association; these could
be stated to make a phony distinction between CC and *CCC, introducing an otherwise inexpressible contrast
into the language. In this way a pseudo-contrast in the CV-domain, protected from surfacing by stray erasure, can
be projected into the melodic domain, where it would survive to visibility. Section 2.4 contains further discussion
of the Arabic case.
Within prosodic theory, where the actual shape-invariant can be identified, it is possible to assume a
natural condition on template interpretation:
4
Notice that syllable theory proper doesn’t even do this kind of extensive counting: most syllable length restrictions follow from pairwise
sonority transition requirements; the rest from hierarchy. Discussion appears in section 3.1.
G.N. Clements has suggested to us (April, 1985, p.c.) that a descriptor C* could be used to refer to consonant sequences of unspecified
length. The development of prosodic theory has eliminated such devices from phonological rules proper; it seems worthwhile to us to extend
the result generally.
5
The one argument in the literature which crucially relies on unfilled template slots is Everett & Seki (1985); we deal with it below in section
2.2.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
5
(9) Satisfaction Condition
All elements in a template are obligatorily satisfied.6
All three of the problems stemming from segmental shape specification are resolved:
i. Under the Satisfaction Condition no excess material is ever present in the representation,
giving us the easiest and least stipulative explanation for its unresponsiveness to phonological
probing: nonexistence.
ii. Patterns of obligatoriness and optionality will follow in general from independent
characterization of the prosodic units, both universally and language-specifically. (This is
merely a somewhat tardy extension of reasoning well-established in phonology, where such
optionality-stipulating notations as ‘(...)’ and zero-subscript have faded in the face of accurate
representation of prosody.)
iii. The fact that the templates are bounded by a language’s prosody follows from their being
literally built from that prosody.
The actual shape-invariant defining a templatic morpheme must be prosodic, then, rather than segmental.
Even at this descriptive level it becomes clear when languages with moderately complex prosody are examined
that prosodic categories must be admitted into template theory. ‘CVC’ seems a plausible enough prefix; but when
the next language over (e.g. Ilokano instead of Agta), shows ‘CCVC’, correlated with the appearance of 2consonant onsets, it becomes harder to avoid the correct generalization. The Classical Arabic templates appear
relatively simple (though, as noted above, spelled segmentally they violate counting norms); turn to Modern
Hebrew, with a rich range of syllable-initial clusters to include, and the stipulative character of segmental spellout becomes apparent (McCarthy 1984c; section 2.4 below). Nash (1980, p.139) identifies the Warlpiri verbal
reduplicative element as a foot, indeed as the ordinary stress-foot of that language, because it equates a single
long-vowelled syllable to two short-vowelled syllables. In fact, the literature demonstrating the need for reference
to prosodic structure in characterizing morphological structure is quite substantial; in addition to the works just
cited it includes Archangeli (1983, 1984), Lowenstamm & Kaye (1986), Yip (1982, 1983), Steriade (1985),
Levin (1983, 1985), and Marantz (1982). What these works share is a concern with showing the necessity for
prosody in the template; but they also share the recognition of a segmental level of skeletal representation.
Template theory therefore includes prosody; considerations reviewed here from counting theory and from
the expression of shape invariance show that it must include nothing else. The rest of this document constitutes
a demonstratio (in the sense that brought Galileo before the Inquisition) of this result.
1.2
Outline of the Theory
Here we sketch the system of available categories and the principles of mapping that accommodate a base to a
prosodically specified template.
6
Condition (9) is probably a special case of the general principle for interpreting structural descriptions throughout phonology. As the notion
of strict adjacency at the appropriate level (tier, grid stratum) replaces string specification devices, it becomes likely that no language-specific
stipulation of optionality is allowed in rules.
6
McCarthy & Prince
THE PROSODIC CATEGORIES
The following units of structure will be called on:
(10) The Prosodic Categories
Wd
‘prosodic word’
F
‘foot’
)
‘syllable’
)µ
‘light (monomoraic) syllable’
)µµ
‘heavy (bimoraic) syllable’
)c
‘core syllable’
These elements are well-established outside of morphology. The theory of phonology uncontroversially
recognizes the categories ‘prosodic word’ (Wd) and ‘syllable’ ()). Stress theory provides the categories ‘foot’
(F), ‘light syllable’, and ‘heavy syllable’. We adopt the traditional moraic terminology: light syllables ()µ ) contain
one mora, heavy syllables ()µµ ) two (v. Hyman (1985), Prince (1983) for recent discussion). Studies of
syllabification proper have long recognized the centrality of the syllable CV, the ‘core syllable’ ()c). We interpret
)c to include ) = V in languages which allow optionality of onsets.7 The prosodic units are arranged hierarchically
(v. Selkirk (1980ab) for the most explicit discussion of this point).
Special status is often accorded to the minimal version of a category; we therefore recognize as part of
morphology a minimizing predicate ‘min’. In general, if X n is a level-n prosodic category expanding into several
categories X n 1 , then min(X n) [X n 1]X n . For example, a prosodic word is typically a sequence of feet; so
min(Wd) = [F]Wd. Appropriate technical development, which we postpone, would simplify the descriptive
vocabulary in favor of a more restricted set of categories interacting with the ‘min’ operator: )c can be identified
as min ), and perhaps )µµ as min F.
Minimal Word
Further formal development is found in M&P (1990a, 1991ab). One refinement that emerges in
this work is the “loose” minimal word, which contains a foot plus an unfootable syllable, in a loose
interpretation of the prosodic hierarchy (cf. M&P 1993a:Appendix, 1995a; Itô & Mester 1992;
Hewitt 1992).
The category min Wd is particularly central; indeed, the many appearances of the category word in
reduplicative and templatic systems all have the minimality requirement attached. There’s an interesting variation
in interpretation of min Wd which shows that the min-operator can be composed with itself at least once: in 2.4.1
below, we show that min Wd may be any licit foot of the language, as in the Yup’ik proximal vocative, or it may
be the minimal Foot which can yet be a word, that is to say a single syllable, as in the English hypocoristic: min
min Wd.
7
There are various ways to guarantee this interpretation. In essentially the terms of Steriade (1982), one can say that the primary act of
syllabification is to adjoin at most one C to a following V — the Onset Rule. This is Hyman’s (1985) conception of the creation of the
elementary mora. This is distinct from, say, Kahn’s conception in which the primary move is to associate ) to V. Then )c indicates an
application of this primary adjunction, which may fail to create CV if there’s no C to take.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
7
More on the Minimal Word
The notion “minimal word” builds on earlier work by Prince 1980 and Broselow 1982. Subsequent
literature on the minimal word in phonology includes: Akinlabi 1995; Buller, Buller, & Everett 1993;
Dunlap 1991; Golston 1991; Hayes 1991/1995; Itô & Hankamer 1989; Kager 1993b, 1996a;
Kiparsky 1992; McDonough 1990; Mester 1993; Myers 1987; Orgun & Inkelas 1992; Piggott 1992;
Prince & Smolensky 1993; Wilkinson 1986. Work on the minimal word in morphology includes:
Cho 1992; J. F. Cole 1990; Crowhurst 1991ab; Itô 1991; Itô, Kitagawa, & Mester 1992; Itô &
Mester 1992, 1995; McCarthy & Prince 1991ab; Mester 1990, 1995; Ola 1995; Spring 1990ab;
Tateishi 1989; Weeda 1992; Yip 1991. In M&P 1994ab, we argue that the minimal word has no
actual status as a primitive template — instead, it is just the most harmonic form of PrWd under
the metrical constraints PARSE-SYLL and ALL-FEET-RIGHT/LEFT
A final point. Nothing in our proposals hinges on any conception of the mora as a unit of intrasyllabic
constituency beyond its essential role in measuring weight. Thus, although we adopt the notational expedient of
adjoining, for example, the onset to the first mora of each syllable, this in no way bears on our results. Instead,
the issue of appropriate intrasyllabic constituency is addressed on its own terms in section 3.1.
FOOT TYPOLOGY
The repertory of feet that we require will differ somewhat from that made familiar in the work of HalleVergnaud (1978) and Hayes (1980); in particular, we will need to recognize the foot [µ µ]. We will therefore
propose and justify a modified universal typology, which is closer to the practice of McCarthy (1979) and Prince
(1980), and which reflects the findings of Hayes (1985).
Our first assumption is that feet are maximally binary; ‘unbounded feet’ are nonprimitive, as
demonstrated in Prince (1985). We distinguish between two fundamental foot-types on the basis of the
quantitative relation between the two members: the balanced foot [u u] and the asymmetrical foot [v w] where
v<w (in practice, [)µ )µµ ]). In his (1985) study of alternating patterns, Hayes found that in quantity sensitive
(QS) systems heavy syllables are always foot-final; he points to the psychology of grouping temporal sequences
as the cause. The asymmetrical foot must therefore be quantitatively iambic.
A second important finding of Hayes’s is that quantity-insensitive (QI) feet are overwhelmingly trochaic
in labeling. Here again he points to the psychology of grouping: a sequence of objectively even pulses is typically
parsed as trochaic (cf. 2/4 and 4/4 time). We therefore assume two prominence principles responsive to
quantitative relations.
(A)
Quantity/Prominence Homology. for a,b F, if a>b quantitatively then a>b stresswise.
(B)
Trochaic Default. for a,b F, if a=b quantitatively then F = [s w].
In languages which do not recognize distinctions of quantity, only rule (B) applies; feet being of the
balanced variety, they are necessarily trochaic. In languages distinguishing heavy and light syllables, the
bracketing is as we predict: feet are [)µ )µµ], [µ µ], and when permitted [µ]. The assignment of prominence shows
some interesting variations. If (A) and (B) were the only principles involved, we would expect that quantitysensitive systems would have both iambic and trochaic feet in them: iambic on the asymmetrical feet, trochaic
on the balanced feet. Such systems are in fact attested: Cairene Arabic (McCarthy 1979) has exactly this pattern.
But the most commonly encountered system has [w s] prominence on all feet, regardless of their quantitative
make-up. We propose that this is due to a requirement of uniformity which has more to do with the integrity of
the system than with its phonetic bases. If a quantity-sensitive language is to have a single labeling rule, it must
be [w s], since Quantity/Prominence homology cannot be systematically denied. A third type of system enforces
uniformity of labeling only within individual words: the example is Yidig (Hayes 1985). Words are bracketed
into bisyllabic feet from left to right; then quantity sensitivity is invoked: long vowels are shortened in foot-initial
8
McCarthy & Prince
position, leaving only legitimate balanced or asymmetrical feet. Any word containing an asymmetrical foot has
iambic rhythm throughout; words with only balanced feet are trochaic. We can express this typology of quantity
sensitivity in this way:
(C) Uniformity Parameter
A language may require that all feet have the same labeling (i) everywhere (ii) within the word.
The three principles (A), (B), and (C) have somewhat different status. Principle (A) ‘Quantity/Prominence Homology’ is dominant: the familiar QS systems all observe it. Principle (C) ‘Uniformity’ is
parasitic upon (A), and as a parameter of description, it may be turned off, as in Cairene. Principle (B) ‘Trochaic
Default’ is typically a true default rule, subject to overrule by ‘Uniformity’.
The range of possible prosodic systems is generated by the various possible combinations of foot-types
and prominence rules. There’s only one QI system, with the balanced foot [) )], necessarily trochaic. Three major
QS systems emerge:
[I] using both feet [)µ )µµ ] and [µ µ],
[II] using only [)µ )µµ ],
[III] using only [µ µ].
System [I] is of course the usual QS alternating pattern, with a (possibly dominant) iambic component. System
[II] is the ‘unbounded foot’ type and may be supplemented by the placement of a (balanced) foot at word-edge
(Prince 1985). System [III] is found in Japanese (see Poser (1985 [1990]) and below) and may also be attested
in Southern Paiute (Sapir 1930) and Weri (Boxwell & Boxwell 1966; Hayes 1980).
The Southern Paiute case deserves some discussion. The language is remarkable in having a stress rule
that can evidently divide long vowels between feet. According to Sapir’s description, the stress pattern is
generated by applying feet [µ µ] left-to-right, where µ=V and long vowels are VV. In such word-shapes as CVCVV-CV..., this results in syllable-splitting, giving [CV-CV][V-CV]. Although this is unusual, the truly odd
thing from the present point of view is that the feet are iambic.
A further datum bears on the matter: R. Harms (1966) and K. Hale (p.c.) report that there is a surface
difference between true long vowels and underlyingly heterosyllabic VV sequences (<*VGV): whereas the
sequences may surface iambically stressed, the true long vowels always have phonetic stress on their initial mora.
We take this to be the result of a rule erasing syllable-internal foot structure and assigning prosodic status to ),
which allows the normal prominence structure of the syllable to assert itself. In certain environments, such a rule
will derive feet [)µ )µµ ]. Consider the crucial example [CV-CV][V-CV]: adjusting )-internal F-structure gives
[CV-CVV][CV]. If this is right, the Southern Paiute system does indeed contain the crucially iambic foot [)µ
)µµ ]. At prominence assignment, uniformity may be invoked to guarantee iambic labeling.
An important consequence of this system is that iambic rhythm is crucially dependent upon the
appearance of heavy syllables in a language. (Curiously, this does not follow from previous theories even if
iambicity is directly linked to QS. For QS, as a property of rules rather than representations, can be defined in
such a way that a given language has no candidates for heavy syllables: for example, suppose the quantity
distinction is set at V/VV in a language without long vowels.) Iambic rhythm is phonetically proper only to
asymmetrical feet; uniformity spreads it to balanced feet.
The revised typology argued for here provides exactly the feet we shall encounter in templatic systems:
QI [) )] and QS [)µ )µµ ] and [µ µ].
Foot Theory
A similar foot theory was independently proposed by Hayes 1987. Subsequent developments of
this theory include Prince 1991, Hayes 1991, 1995, Hewitt 1992, Kager 1992a, 1993ac, and
Mester 1995.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
9
MAPPING PRINCIPLES
Accommodation to a template is essentially the prosodic reparsing of a copy of the base. Under this
rubric there are many variations consistent with the general prosodic hypothesis that we wish to establish: here
we sketch one approach, deferring detailed discussion of alternatives (in part until section 4) on the grounds that
choice between them, though of great empirical interest, is largely orthogonal to the main issue. In order to
highlight the main line of argument — the prosodic character of template structures — we will for now refrain
from radical revision of the mapping process.
We’ll assume with Marantz (1982) that the entire segmental melody of the reduplication domain is
copied; with Broselow & McCarthy (1983-4), that it is copied onto a new plane, although we will not carefully
represent this where non-crucial.
We also assume that mapping of the segmental material into the template is directional: LR for prefixes,
RL for suffixes, free choice for root-and-pattern systems. For reduplicative affixation, this presumably boils down
to the fact that the affix occurs at an edge: prefixes reprosodize at the beginning, suffixes at the end of the domain
(-copy). Call this edge-in reprosodization; we return to it below.
Current views require emendation, under any conception of template form, in their handling of
template/melody mismatch. Free loss of melodic material under phoneme-driving leads to false predictions.
Consider the prefix ), commonly treated as CVC in languages where that is the maximal syllable. What happens
when it attaches to a word of the form CV.VC? The Ilokano progressive provides an example:
(11) “No-Skipping’‘ in Ilokano Reduplication
/dait/
ag-DA-da.it
‘sew/ be sewing’
When /i/ fails to map, persistence in the LR sweep should extend the search to the final /t/, predicting *ag-DATda.it, a pattern of loss that appears to be impossible. The same effect is met with in the other direction — see the
discussion of Manam in 4. The doctrine of persistence is motivated by actual losses observed in CV (that is, )c)
reduplication. For example, Sanskrit druv- > DU-druv- ‘run’ shows that failure of a C (here, r) to map does not
prevent association from continuing until the target is satisfied. The persistence doctrine vastly overgeneralizes
from this one pattern. Aside from mapping to )c, there are no other cases where nonadjacent melody elements
are rendered adjacent by directional mapping to template: loss occurs freely only when the mapping process is
finished and the continuous substring left over disappears, as in ag-TAK(der)-takder, ag-DA(it)-dait, etc. Stemtemplate systems are similar, when they allow any loss from the root, e.g. Arabic quinqueliterals (McCarthy
1981).
A plausible account of this finding is that mapping must always be continous, except that under
compulsion the head of a constituent such as onset can be taken for the whole thing. We will put off explicit
technical development, however, since competing theories offer no advantage in dealing with the problem, and
simply assume that skipping of melody elements is impossible outside accomodation to )c. It is not implausible
that the mapping operation actually defines )c: if the core syllable is removed from the vocabulary of prosodic
constituents, it can be derived from the light syllable ()µ ) by this idiosyncratic mode of mapping. As before, we
postpone technical development of this possible simplification.8
A related issue also emerges from our results. The otherwise reduplication-specific principle of phonemedriven association (Marantz 1982) turns out to be superfluous. With a prosodic theory of the skeleton, association
is effectively skeletally-driven9 — it is edge-in reprosodization of the copied melody by the affixal skeleton. We
develop this consequence of our theory explicitly in section 4.
8
It is possible, for example, that the mapping observed in )c is related to the notion “minimal affix”.
9
Davis (1986) proposes skeletally-driven association only for infixes; we regard it as universal.
10
McCarthy & Prince
Finally, we will follow Broselow & McCarthy (1983–4) in assuming that the domain of affixation may
be delimited prosodically as well as morphologically. In particular, the notion min Wd may be called on to pick
out a subsequence of the stem which can serve as a kind of pseudo-stem for purposes of affixation and associated
processes. This notion of domain is important not only in certain types of infixing reduplication but in peripheral
reduplication as well.
2.
Elaboration and Exemplification
In this section, we look at three types of reduplication, prefixation, suffixation, and infixation, and then turn to
nonreduplicative templatic morphology.
2.1
Prefixation
THE SIMPLE SYLLABLE AS PREFIX TARGET
A common form of reduplication prefixes to the base as much of its initial substring as can be put into
a syllable of the language. The Ilokano progressive, cited above, provides a clear example (Bernabe et. al. 1971):
(12) Ilokano Progressive Reduplication
ag + ) + BASE
BASE
a. /basa/
ag - BAS - basa
b. /dait/
ag - DA - da.it
c. /adal/
ag - AD - adal
d. /takder/
ag - TAK - takder
e. /trabaho/
ag - TRAB- trabaho
‘be reading’
‘be studying’
‘be studying’
‘be standing’
‘be working’
It has been emphasized in the literature that reduplication does not in general copy a prosodic constituent
of the base (Moravcsik 1978); forms such as ag.BAS.ba.sa confirm the observation. What’s copied is the base’s
segmental melody, as in Marantz (1982);10 the prefix ) then draws its content from that melody according to the
syllabification rules of the language.
(13)
)
))
basa basa
The difference between the prefix syllable bas and the stem-initial syllable ba is explained by the different
prosodic requirements placed on the two domains. Since the stem must be through-syllabified, its syllable ba is
limited by competition from the following syllable, which maximizes its own onset; the prefix syllable bas, being
alone in its domain, is free to develop to the greatest extent allowed.
Ilokano Reduplication
The form of reduplication in Ilokano is in fact a heavy syllable, not a simple syllable. Hence, /dait/
reduplicates as da:-da.it or, in some dialects, dad-da.it. See Hayes & Abad 1989.
10
Refinements on melody copying are explored in section 4.3.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
11
In Orokaiva (Healey, Isoroembo, & Chittleborough 1969), repetitive prefixation interacts in an
interesting and typical way with syllabification constraints. These are the relevant data:
(14) Orokaiva Verbal Reduplication
a. waeke
WA-waeke
b. hirike
HI-hirike
c. tiuke
TI-tiuke
d. uhuke
U.H-uhuke
‘shut’
‘open’
‘cut’
‘blow’
Healey et al. describe reduplication as copying the “first CV or VC of stems’‘. The language only allows syllables
V, CV, and CVN; the N is homorganic to a following C and disappears word-finally in favor of vowel nasality.
Codas, then, must be nasal and can only share place-of-articulation specification — they must be ‘linked’ in the
sense of Steriade (1982); word-finally a nasal may be extrasyllabic. We interpret the coda requirement in the
manner of Itô (1986) as a filter on syllable-final elements:
(15)
Coda Condition
* )
c
Place
Condition (15) asserts that syllable-final consonants may not have a place of articulation (whence the fact that
hypothetical word-final nasals are expressed only by vowel nasality); since the Geminate Constraint (Schein &
Steriade 1986, Hayes 1986) will prevent it from analyzing a doubly linked place-matrix, blocking its application,
it follows that an admissible syllable-final consonant will be place-linked to a following consonant.
The Coda Condition will rule out ever taking the prefix ) to include CVN, since the N will never be
linked to the stem-initial consonant, which follows it syllabically.
The behavior of the )-prefix establishes that the principles of lexical syllabification hold in the
prefix+stem domain. An immediate consequence is the special treatment accorded to vowel-initial stems: in the
LR sweep mapping phonemes into prosody, the first C of the copied melody finds a place as the onset of the
stem-initial syllable.
(16)
)
)) )
uhuke uhuke
As is perhaps universal in lexical syllabification, a syllable will take an onset whenever it can.
The Orokaiva CV/VC pattern might suggest to the unwary that the prefix is XX, the long-sought-for
example of segmental reduplication. As with the XXX affix discussed above, any descriptive success of XX is
no more than a freak of the limited prosody at hand. The putative bisegmental affix can hardly be expected to
make its appearance in a language where #CC and #VV are found. The )-affix, on the other hand, is entirely free
to occur, with its realizational variants determined by independent considerations. This particular realization —
in which an application of the Onset Rule has resulted in apparent ‘extra’ copying — we will see to be of
fundamental importance as we look at other languages. It characterizes not only the distribution of XX and its
congeners, but also the otherwise inexpressible notion ‘maximal intersyllabic cluster’ and the typology of
reduplicative suffixes.
12
McCarthy & Prince
Orokaiva Reduplication
The analysis of Orokaiva reduplication given in the text is unlikely to be correct. The theoretical
point — that the template needn’t be co-extensive with the observed reduplicant — is made
equally well by the Oykangand example, immediately below, and the Mokilese and-andip pattern,
on p. 16). But in examples like uhuhuke, it is almost certainly the case that the reduplicant is
infixed, after an initial onsetless syllable: u-HU-huke. This pattern of infixation is also seen in
Uradhi, Timugon Murut, as on p. 36, as well as Sanskrit (p. 15) and many other languages. (The
question then arises as to how an Oykangand-type analysis of Orokaiva is to be ruled out on
principled grounds!) Optimality Theory sheds some light on this infixing pattern; see M&P 1993ab,
1994ab, and the remark on p. 40. On phenomena in Arrernte [ “Aranda”] that are similar to those
of Oykangand, involving an apparent “VC” unit, see Breen 1990, Henderson (to appear), Turner
& Breen 1984, and Wilkins 1984, 1989.
Strikingly similar facts, which bear on the realization issue, are reported for Oykangand in Sommer
11
(1981):
(17) Reduplication in Oykangand
a. /eder/
ED-eder
b. /algal/
ALG-algal
c. /igu-/
IG-igun
‘rain’
‘straight’
‘go’
Here the )-affix creates a somewhat richer array of patterns because final consonants are allowed more freely:
(18)
)
) )
algal algal
Oykangand words may not begin with consonants. Sommer has taken this to mean that Oykangand syllables
must — contra naturam — be similarly restricted, at least underlyingly; he cites reduplication as presumptive
evidence for the claim, proposing that it copies a ‘syllable’, i.e. VC*. Since such an operation is in all likelihood
impossible, rather than merely unusual (as Sommer himself suggests), the reduplication evidence cannot support
the syllabic claim. The present theory resolves the issue, providing an analysis which depends only on the
universally-expected (and phonetically observable) syllabification of the language.
The evidence reviewed here shows that the ultimate shape of a reduplicated sequence is sensitive in subtle
ways to the character of syllabification in a language. When syllabification across the prefix-stem boundary is
permitted, as in Orokaiva and Oykangand, an extra consonant will be taken to fill an empty onset position. In
Ilokano, by contrast, stem and prefixes form separate syllabification domains, and empty onsets are filled with
epenthetic glottal stop: from /ag+)+adal/, we get [a.gad.adal]. Consequently no )C-pattern is found. A like
pattern, pervading all prefixes, reduplicative and nonreduplicative, is observed in Sundanese (Robins 1959).
11
Sommer cites a pattern of internal reduplication for some polysyllabic stems:
a. /iyalme-/
iy-ALM-almey
‘play’
b. /ana×umi-/
ana×-UM-umin
‘peek’
Without a more extensive account of Oykangand phonology and morphology, it’s not entirely clear what to make of these examples. They
do suggest, however, that prefixation might actually be to the minimal word rather than to the word proper: this would cover all the cases.
See below, section 2.3. Sommer also cites the pair /oyelm/ ‘back again’ and OYEL-oyelm ‘straight back again’, which he notes is limited to
this one word.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
13
ONSET, Templates, & Alignment
The constraint ONSET is fundamental to OT syllable typology (Prince & Smolensky 1993). Through
domination of constraints on the form of the reduplicant (that is, templatic constraints), it leads to
the kind of “extra’‘ copying seen in Oykangand and other examples below (Mokilese, p. 16, Tzeltal,
p. 32, and Chamorro, p. 44). The templatic constraints involved here are those that pertain to the
alignment of its edges — for instance, in Oykangand, the right edge of the reduplicant is misaligned with a syllable-edge, in AL.G-algal. See M&P (1993a: Chapt. 7, 1994ab, 1995a).
THE CORE SYLLABLE AS PREFIX TARGET
The most familiar and well-studied instances of core syllable ()c) reduplication are provided by Sanskrit
and Greek (Steriade 1982, 1985), but the phenomenon is by no means restricted to Indo-European. Tagalog uses
it to mark several morphological categories (Bowen 1969), for example the Recent Perfective:
(19) Core Syllable Reduplication in Tagalog
a. ka-TA-trabaho
‘just finished working’
b. ka-I-ipon
‘just saved’
c. ka-GA-galit
‘just got mad’
d. ka-BO-bloaut
‘just gave a special treat’
Characteristic is the reduction of initial clusters to one element, a result that comes through mapping to )c, which
tolerates no more than one onset consonant. The normal selection of the first consonant in a cluster, as we have
noted, engages the notion of phoneme-driving and the theory of what elements may be skipped.
(20)
)c +
)))
trabaho trabaho
The Core Syllable
Steriade 1988, expanding on the claims of the present work, proposes that the simple onset of
the core-syllable template is to be related to a syllable-markedness parameter, and she
implements this idea with a truncation rule applying to the copied material. In M&P 1993a, 1994ab,
1995a, universal constraints on syllable structure interact, under Optimality Theory, with
constraints on exactness of copying, to produce the core-syllable phenomenon as well as other
possible unmarked properties of the reduplicant, exactly paralleling the way syllable restrictions
are imposed in ordinary (nonreduplicative) phonology. On this ‘Generalized Template’ theory of
prosodic morphology, see also Gafos 1995, Rosenthall 1995, Urbanczyk 1995, 1996ab.
The Sanskrit verb reduplicates in 5 of its forms, 4 according to the pattern )c: the present, the aorist, the
perfect, and the desiderative. In every case the simplification of initial cluster proceeds in the same way: the least
sonorous member is preserved. Steriade (1982) is able to derive this with LR mapping on the assumption that
onsets are of strictly rising sonority; if copying takes only syllabified material, then extrasyllabic elements such
as initial /s/ in s-obstruent clusters will not appear on the prefix: thus tsar > TA-tsar, but sthaa > TA-sthaa.12.
Choice of vocalism in the prefix varies from category to category, and involves considerable phonological
12
Alternative accounts are made possible by the present theory. Suppose for example that mapping is not really LR but rather head-to-head
on prosodic constituents. Then the head —least sonorous member — of the onset cluster would be chosen, regardless of the extrasyllabicity
situation. An account along these lines must assume that the representation analyzed by the prefix contains prosodic information.
14
McCarthy & Prince
complexity irrelevant to present concerns (v. Steriade (1985) for discussion). However, the behavior of vowelinitial roots is of some interest. They appear to be poorly represented in all categories except the perfect, where
the following rules hold (Whitney 1889, section 783):
(21) Perfect Reduplication in Sanskrit V-initial Roots
a. )c+ aC
a+aC > a:C
b. )c+ iC
i+iC > i:C
(weak grade)
i+aiC > iyaiC < iye:C
(strong grade)
c. )c+ uC
u+uC > u:C
(weak grade)
u+auC > uvauC < uvo:C
(strong grade)
d. VCC, VVC do not usually form reduplicated perfects.
Since the postvocalic C is not taken as an onset, as it is in Orokaiva and Oykangand, we must conclude that
syllabification is not allowed across the prefix-root boundary, at least at the relevant level of the lexical
phonology. Evidence that this is true comes from the behavior of the high-vowel roots (21b,c). Steriade (1985)
points out that ‘the general rule of Glide Formation fails to apply to such forms: intermediate u-áuca does not
become váuca, surface *voca.’ She offers an account in terms of rule ordering and the Strict Cycle. But if Glide
Formation is a process of filling an empty onset, then it cannot apply across a boundary that is a barrier to onset
formation. The surface forms are derived as in Steriade’s analysis, by the application of postcyclic Glide Insertion
and vowel fusion (Whitney, section 126).
Most of the relatively small number of vowel initial forms attested from outside the categories (21a,b,c)
show a remarkable variation on the normal pattern:
(22) Other V-Initial Forms in Sanskrit
a. iir
iir-IR-é
b. am
aam-AM-at13
c. aap
aap-IP-an
d. arp
arp-IP-am
e. edh
ed-IDH-isa
f. aç
aç-Iç-isa
(pf.)
(aor.)
(aor.)
(aor.)
(des.)
(des.)
Although the form aamamat (a+am-am+at) suggests syllabification into the prefix, the other forms are
inconsistent with this. The vocalism in (22c,d,f), with /a/ reduplicating as /i/, is normal for aorist and desiderative;
cf. aor. a+ti-tras+am, des. bi-badh+isa (Whitney, sections 858, 1029). Furthermore, it is always the second
instance of the root which is segmentally reduced, as in arp-IP+am. What additional data is available from
grammatical sources follows this pattern without exception:
13
Lengthening of the initial vowel is due to the ‘temporal augment’ /a+/.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
15
(23) V-initial Aorist and Desiderative Forms
a. Aorists:
arc
aarc-IC+am
ubj
aaubj-IJ+am14
arh
aarj-IH+am
rdh
aard-IDH+am
iiks
aaic-IKS+am.
b. Desideratives:
arh
arj-IH+isa
und
und-ID+isa
rdh
ard-IDH+isa
We conclude that vowel-initial roots do indeed use suffixation. In addition, the pattern of losses indicates that
the suffix can only be )c, with cross-boundary syllabifications.
Sanskrit Vowel-Initial Reduplication
As noted in Kiparsky 1986, the pattern in the aorist is infixation, not suffixation, as incorrecty
claimed here. E.g. ar-PI-pam, not arp-IP-am. In M&P 1993a, the various reduplicative peculiarities
of Sanskrit vowel-initial roots result from the force of the constraint ONSET. This relates aspects
of Sanskrit reduplication to Orokaiva above (p. 11) and Timugon Murut etc. below (p. 36).
Melody copying and association are incoherent with a )c suffix; see sections 2.2 and 4 for discussion.
The suffix is therefore satisfied by spreading, as in Steriade’s (1982) analysis of Greek. Consider the linking
process that creates arpipam:
(24)
If copying were involved rather than spreading, the vowel could be copied as a and turned into i by a rule
associated with reduplication of aorist, desiderative, and (sometimes) present. The desiderative undidisa from
und permits no such account, since u nevers reduplicates as i; such forms are explicable only if the suffix’s
vowel has been fixed at i. The aorist aaubjIJam (/a+ubj+)c+am/) from ubj is a similar case.15 The vowel i is
therefore inserted, evidently by default specification, and the filling of then open syllabic positions comes about
through spreading rather than copying, in much the same way as Steriade (1982) has proposed for Greek
prefixing reduplication.
Roots with final clusters of falling sonority behave in the same way:
(25)
) + )c + ) )
und
isa
The one case where infixing occurs (aaikIKSam from iiks) follows smoothly, given Steriade’s theory of Sanskrit
syllabification by which the rising sonority of ks- suffices to license it as an onset:
14
The long initial diphthong aau is a regular consequence of adding the augment a- to vowel-initial roots (Whitney, 136a) and has nothing
to do with the peculiarities of the reduplicating subclass of aorists.
15
The aorist evidence is less clearcut since the syllabic incorporation of the augment a- into the erswhile root syllable provides a source for
i, if it can be copied (this being a rather familiar way that syllabification muddles morpheme boundaries).
16
(26)
McCarthy & Prince
) + )c + )
aiks
am
The proposed analysis depends crucially on the assumption that the final suffix (the one after the reduplicant )c)
begins with a vowel, opening up an onset position. This is in fact the case for both aorist and desiderative.
The reduplicating aorist shows an interesting divergence from the simple )c form: it ‘aims always at
establishing a diversity between the reduplicating and radical syllables, making the one heavy and the other light
(Whitney, section 858b)’. There are three general rules:
(27) Prefix/Stem Complementarity
a. If the root is light (CVC, since all aorist suffixes are vowel initial), the prefix vowel is lengthened; thus
ris gives a+RII-ri.s+am.
b. If the root begins with a cluster, the prefix is already heavy and nothing happens: krudh gives a+KUk.ru.dh+am.
c. If the root is heavy, the prefix remains light: diks gives a+DI-dik.s+am.
Evidently there is a rule making the prefix syllable heavy — inserting a mora —before a light stem syllable.16
The rule cannot apply if the stem syllable already has the full complement of 2 moras. By familiar processes
(Ingria 1980), the prefix vowel spreads automatically to fill the empty mora position. Notice that the designation
)c governs only the initial mapping process, creating a ) which is as liable to phonological manipulation as any
other.17
THE HEAVY SYLLABLE AS PREFIX TARGET
Mokilese
Mokilese reduplication has been insightfully analyzed by Levin (1983, 1985) within the segmental
framework. We argue here that the affix shape invariant must be construed prosodically.
The progressive aspect of the Mokilese verb is formed by prefixation of a heavy syllable target, as can
be seen from the following data provided by Harrison & Albert (1976), who explicitly note the generalization:
(28) Reduplication in Mokilese, /CVC.../ Stems
p@dok
p@d-p@dok
mwi×e
mwi×-mwi×e
kas@
kas-kas@
wadek
wad-wadek
pil@d
pil-pil@d
d@pw@
d@pw-d@pw@
poki
pok-poki
‘plant’
‘eat’
‘throw’
‘read’
‘pick breadfruit’
‘pull’
‘beat’
16
The same rule applies in the intensive (section 1002.III.f) in the older language. Discussion of the poorly attested intensive, which involves
a number of interesting descriptive problems (v. Steriade 1985), will be postponed to a later version of this work.
17
Quantitative complementarity should probably be understood in terms of higher-order prosodic structuring. Suppose that the actual rule
is to impose an asymmetrical (iambic) foot LR. This is always possible, because the aorist prefix a- necessarily forms a light syllable. If the
root syllable is heavy, a clash would result: this typically blocks the process. Interestingly, there are cases where the root syllable is lightened
(section 861) to accomodate the iambic pattern; thus raadh gives a-RAA-radham, krand gives a-CIk.rad.am. This equivocation is familiar
from other clash-driven processes: when there are several solutions, variability is likely. Quantitative complementarity also shows up in
Ponapean (v. inf.) and in most cases of counting allomorphy rules (section 1), with strong indications of a similar but not identical prosodic
basis.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
17
(29) Reduplication in Mokilese, /CV/ and /CV.V.../ Stems
pa
paa-pa
‘weave’
wi.a
wii-wi.a
‘do’
di.ar
dii-di.ar
‘find’
(30) Reduplication in Mokilese, /CV:.../ Stems
kook@
koo-kook@
s@@r@k
s@@-s@@r@k
gaak
gaa-gaak
‘grind coconut’
‘tear’
‘bend’
The examples in (28) show the target )µµ being satisfied by an initial substring of melodic elements.
In (29) association from the copy must fail to satisfy the target. Examples like /pa/ simply lack the stuff
to fill out a heavy syllable by 1:1 mapping; and since the vowel sequences of the other two examples are always
heterosyllabic, a form like di.ar can only link di, never dia, to a syllabic prefix. (As noted above, the mapping
must stop with the failure of a to link, so that dir-di.ar is not a possible outcome.) The single successfully linked
vowel must therefore be spread to fill the 2nd mora position.
The examples of (30) show that a long vowel is copied as long — a phenomenon dubbed ‘transfer’ in
Clements (1985a), which obviously requires some refinement of the mapping procedure. We consider techniques
for dealing with it in section 4.3.
Quantitative Transfer
This phenomenon and its significance were first noted by Levin 1983. An account of it is given in
M&P 1988. Other relevant work includes Hammond 1988, Steriade 1988, and Selkirk 1988. For
recent work on Mokilese reduplication, see Blevins 1996.
A search of the dictionary reveals that superheavy CVVC syllables are found only at word-end,
presumably because of the availability of consonant extrametricality there. The two mora requirement imposed
by the prefix is therefore satisfied maximally by CVV: whence s@@-s@@rok, never *s@@r-s@@rok.
A third pattern generating long vowels is observed in (31), where CVG-initial stems give a CVV prefix.
(31) Reduplication in Mokilese, Diphthongal Stems
pou.ce
poo-pouce
dau.li
daa-dauli
au.do
aa-audo
‘connect’
‘pass by’
‘fill’
Mokilese has a general rule of diphthong formation, summarized as follows by Harrison & Albert: ‘a high vowel
becomes a glide after a lower vowel and before a consonant or at the end of a word.’ (This is not fully accurate—
their own example is ka+onopda k@wnopda ‘cause+prepared= to prepare’.) All instances of [y] are derivable;
[w] is arguable phonemic in only a few words (p. 32). This suggests an analysis in which the basic syllabification
is po.u.ce, da.u.li, a.u.do, paralleling wi.a, di.ar in (29), giving rise to exactly the same pattern of association.
Interestingly, some speakers allow CVG reduplication, with audo > au-audo, showing that for them diphthongal
nuclei are at least possible in the basic syllabification.
The behavior of the CV.V-initial stems in (29) and (31) demonstrates clearly that the prefix cannot be
XXX, pure segmentalia, but must include the information that the result in these cases must be a licit single
syllable of the language.
18
McCarthy & Prince
Of particular interest— as usual — is the behavior of vowel-initial stems.
(32) Reduplication in Mokilese, V-initial Stems
a. ir
ir-r-ir
b. onop
on-n-op
c. idip
id-d-idip
d. alu
al-l-alu
e. uruur
ur-r-uruur
f. andip
an-d-andip
‘string’
‘prepare’
‘walk’
‘laugh’
‘spit’
Form (32f) shows the syllabification effect we’ve seen before: both the prefix )µµ and the stem-initial onset are
satisfied from the copied melody:18
)µµ
(33)
andip
+) )
andip
In cases where there is no separate consonant for the stem onset, the moraic consonant spreads to fill it. This can
be best understood in relation to a process Harrison & Albert call ‘Boundary Lengthening’. Before ‘looselybound’ suffixes the following changes take place:
(34) Boundary Lengthening
a. Before a V-initial suffix, a single final consonant is geminated:
did # e didde
‘this wall’
puk # @r pukk@r
‘only books’
b. Before a C-initial suffix, final vowels are lengthened.
indi # la indiila
‘go-down and away’
si # pas siipas
‘a bone’
pina # ki # di pinaakiidi
‘cover with’
This class of suffixes adds a mora to a preceding syllable, as is clear from (34b). Associated with the process is
a rule spreading the stem-final C (construed melodically) into the empty onset position of V-initial (ditto)
suffixes, which we display in (35):
(35) Onset Filling
)
)
c
v
This is, of course, exactly the rule evidenced in the reduplicated prefixes (32a-e).
A further generalization is possible. If the reduplicative prefix is included in the class of ‘loosely-bound’
affixes, it is no longer necessary to specify that the prefix is bimoraic: this follows from the general pattern of
boundary lengthening. The prefix, then, is plausibly interpreted as a kind of clitic, prefixation as a kind of (near-)
compounding. The actual progressive prefix then reduces to the familiar ), with its prosodic properties following
from its morphological affinities.
18
Harrison & Albert claim that this effect is only observed with NC clusters, but they cite no evidence to support the restriction. The
alternative we would conceivably find in other clusters is appapta from hypothetical apta.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
19
Ponapean: Heavy and Light ) as Prefixes
Ponapean, a Micronesian language closely related to Mokilese, uses a richly varied pattern of
reduplications to mark the durative in verbs. It has been analyzed in Rehg & Sohl (1981), the source of all data,
in Levin (1985) from the segmentalist point of view, and in McCarthy (1984a), where fundamental elements of
prosodic conditioning in the system are recognized and treated. We examine it afresh not only because it provides
instances of )µµ and )µ as prefix targets, but also because it illustrates how higher order categories, F in
particular, can determine affixal prosody.
More on Ponapean
More recent work on Ponapean phonology and morphology includes Goodman 1995, Itô 1989,
M&P 1991ab.
As elsewhere, the empty onset provokes special treatment; we will focus first on the more perspicuous
behavior of C-initial stems.
Ponapean I: Consonant-initial Stems
With monosyllables, the reduplicative prefix takes the form )µ or )µµ in quantitative complementarity
with the base:
(36)
Light base - )µµ prefix
a. pa
PAA-pa
b. mi
MII-mi
c. pu
PUU-pu
d. lo
LOO-lo
d. lal
LAL-lal
e. rer
RER-rer
f. mem
MEM-mem
g. ka×
KA×-ka×
h. pap
PAM-pap
i. dod
DON-dod
j. dil
DIN-dil
k. kik
Ki×-kik
l. pwil
PwIL-i-pwil
m. par
PAR-a-par
n. tep
TEP-e-tep
o. tep
TEP-i-tep
‘weave’
‘exist’
‘bent, crooked’
‘be caught’
‘make a sound’
‘tremble’
‘sweet’
‘eat’
‘swim’
‘frequent’
‘penetrate’
‘kick’
‘flow’
‘to cut’
‘kick’
‘begin’
Some phonology is visible here. At the surface, nonfinal syllables can close only on geminates or assimilated
nasals. A variety of assimilation rules at various strata of the grammar respond to this restriction; see Rehg &
Sohl (1981, pp. 56-64), McCarthy (1984a), Itô (1986) for discussion. When there is no assimilation,
impermissible clusters such as those in (l-o) are broken up with vowels. It is important to note that the process
20
McCarthy & Prince
of cluster break-up is not limited (as some assimilations are) to reduplicated structures.19 We therefore abstract
away from the Epenthesis process in identifying the prefix as syllabic.
(37)
Heavy base - )µ prefix.
a. duup
DU-duup
b. miik
MI-miik
w
c. m aaw
MwA-mwaaw
d. laud
LA-laud
e. reid
RE-reid
f. pou
PO-pou
g. pei
PE-pei
h. mand
MA-mand
i. le×k
LE-le×k
j. kens
KE-kens
‘dive’
‘suck’
‘good’
‘big, old’
‘stain’
‘cold’
‘fight’
‘tame’
‘acrophobic’
‘ulcerate’
The light base is CVC; the heavy base CVV, CVCC, or CVVC. This contrast is reconciled with the
familiar light-heavy distinction (CV vs. CVX) if final consonants are extrametrical. McCarthy (1984a) adduces
independent evidence in support of this claim. Nouns are subject to a min Wd requirement demanding two moras
at the surface. When they are underlyingly CV or CVC, they are vowel-lengthened to CVV and CVVC
(respectively). Forms CVV, CVCC, and CVVC are untouched. Extrametricality puts the CVC forms in the
monomoraic class and further predicts that only the (intrametrical) vowel is available for satisfying the obligatory
second mora.
Jane Grimshaw suggests that quantitative complementarity be viewed as the consequence of a
requirement on higher-order structure to the effect that a reduplicated monosyllable contain no more than a single
foot. Although the stress system of Ponapean is not discussed in Rehg & Sohl (1981), it is fair to assume that
any quantity-sensitive prosody will tolerate at most one heavy syllable per foot. We further assume that F is
minimally bimoraic: this allows us to interpret the Noun minWd requirement in the usual way as minWd = F. We
can take the prefix to be ), unspecified for weight, which will satisfy itself maximally up to the foot limitation,
recorded here:
19
Rehg & Sohl distinguish 4 types of vowel insertion.
<1> Prothesis of i/u, providing a vocalic nucleus to supplant a syllabic nasal. Thus, m.pe ‘beside it’ may be pronounced
im.pe. Choice between i and u is phonetically predictable. (p. 55-6)
<2> Copying of the following vowel, whereby ak+dei ak-e-dei, ak+tantat ak-a-tantat. Variousconditionsobtain
(p.92-94).
<3> General Epenthesis of i/u, with choice determined as in <1>, to break up any impermissible cluster not otherwise
dealt with. “In slow, careful speech they are less likely to be employed than in rapid, less careful speech.” (p. 94)
<4> Appearance of ‘base vowel’ (p.87-91). Of great interest to the present argument is the preservation of some
etymological final vowels in suffixation and in reduplication of monosyllabic roots. With the verbalizing suffix -niki ‘to
have the thing characterized by the base’, we find, for example:
i. kiil
‘skin’
kil-i-niki
×il-e-niki
ii. ×iil
‘voice’
iii. diip
‘sin’
dip-a-niki
According to Rehg & Sohl, it is the base vowel that breaks up the cluster in (36l-o), accounting for the phonologically inexplicable difference
between tep-e-tep ‘kick’ and tep-i-tep ‘begin’ — cf. transitives tepek ‘kick’ and tapi ‘begin’. Given the plethora of insertions in the language,
it’s not at all clear that the cited data unambiguously establishes the claim. Supposing however that it is correct, are we driven to the
etymologizing conclusion that reduplication is bisyllabic (= F), with various subsequent reductions? We think not. With McCarthy (1984a)
we propose that the ‘base vowel’ is a kind of floating affixal melodeme associated with certain roots, which is allowed to surface when a
free vowel slot presents itself. In support of this, we note that there are only 3 types of base vowel /i a e/, out of a 7 vowel system, a typical
grammaticalization of opaque phonology. The reader is referred to Harris (1985) for discussion of a rather similar phenomenon in Spanish,
and to Itô (1986) for analysis of the assimilation/epenthesis system in Ponapean.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
21
(38) Monosyllable/Monostress Rule (Grimshaw).
Reduplicated monosyllables contain one and only one F.
If feet are strictly iambic then a form like mem meets (38) by containing one F and one loose syllable:
[mem]me(m). If asymmetrical trochaic feet can be derived, then it ends up as a single tidy foot. Stems like mand,
of course, reduplicate as perfect iambs: maman(d).
Polysyllables also choose between )µ and ) µµ, but the grounds for choice are, on the face of things,
remarkable. First, the mundane: when the initial syllable is light, the prefix is )µµ :
(39)
Polysyllabic Stem - light initial syllable
a. rere
RER-rere
b. dune
DUN-dune
c. deyed
DEY-deyed
d. dilip
DIN-dilip
e. pepe
PEM-pepe
f. sarek
SAN-sarek
g. siped
SIP-i-siped
h. taman
TAM-a-taman
i. tepek
TEP-e-tepek
j. l@×e
L@×-(i)-l@×e
k. katoore
KAT-(i)-katoore
l. li.aan
LII-li.aan
m. ri.aala
RII-ri.aala
n. lu.ak
LUU-lu.ak
o. lu.et
LUU-lu.et
‘to skin or peel’
‘attach in a sequence’
‘eat breakfast’
‘mend thatch’
‘swim to’
‘uproot’
‘shake out’
‘remember’
‘kick’
‘pass across’
‘subtract’
‘outgoing’
‘be cursed’
‘jealous’
‘weak’
Phonological notes: Forms (d-f) show the typical assimilations. Forms (g-k) show insertion into unassimilable
clusters: those in (j,k) are optional and characteristic of casual speech; those in (g-i) are obligatory and copy the
following vowel, as can be seen by application of the rule elsewhere.20. As above, we abstract away from
predictable epentheses to reveal the uniformly syllabic character of the prefix. Forms (l-o) show automatic
spreading of the vowel to fill the required second mora. Since vowel sequences like i.a and u.a are necessarily
heterosyllabic, they can’t be mapped into the prefix ) and the mapping must stop with the stem’s first vowel
melodeme. (As we have seen before, it is universally impossible to skip over the unassociated vowel to seize on
a following consonant: *lin-liaan.)
Complementarity makes its appearance when the first syllable is heavy,21 but surprisingly does not
respond to the first syllable at all: the second syllable’s weight determines the weight of the prefix.
20
Rehg & Sohl cite the following paradigm:
i.
/ak+dei/
akedei
ii.
/ak+pwu×/
akupwu×
iii.
/ak+tantat/
akatantat
For discussion of conditions under which a copy vowel appears, see Rehg & Sohl (1981, p.92-94).
21
Rehg & Sohl say more particularly that the first syllable must contain a long vowel, but they cite no evidence to support a distinction
between CVV and CVC.
22
(40)
McCarthy & Prince
)µ with Heavy 2nd Syllable
a. luumwuumw
b. maasaas
c. tooroor
d. waantuuke
(41)
LU-luumwuumw
MA-maasaas
TO-tooroor
WA-waantuuke
‘be sick’
‘cleared of vegetation’
‘be independent’
‘count’
)µµ with Light 2nd syllable22
a. duupek
b. meelel
c. nOOrok
d. peese
DUU-duupek
MEE-meelel
NOO-nOOrok
PEE-peese
‘starved’
‘true’
‘greedy’
‘be acquainted’
Viewed in terms of the sequential structure of the syllable string, the rule of complementarity is mysterious
indeed: not only is it nonlocal, but it skips over an entity ()µµ ) of exactly the same type that it’s looking for.
The prosodic effect is however uniform and simple: the output contains exactly two feet. We therefore
adopt for polysyllables a foot-condition analogous to that imposed on monosyllables:
(42) Polysyllable/Two Stress Rule.
Reduplicated polysyllables must have exactly 2 feet.
If there are two heavy syllables in the stem, as in (40), the prefix must shrink to )µ to avoid running over
the 2F limit: thus [maa][saas] [mamaa][saas]. With only one heavy syllable in the stem, as in (41), the prefix
) is free to expand maximally, indeed must do so: whence [duupek] [duu][duupek].
Notice that the Polysyllable Rule applies equally to well to the forms of (39); all are monopod bases, so
that the FF target can only be achieved via )µµ -prefixation: thus from [dune] we get [dun][dune].
What we have here is a kind of templatic morphology superimposed on the reduplication process. The
prefix is always ), but monosyllable stems satisfy a template F, polysyllables a template FF. The templates
impose weight requirements on the prefix and mediate the transmission of just that kind of nonlocal information
which affects the foot structure of the word.
There is one final class of C-initial polysyllable to consider: those beginning with a syllabic nasal. They
reduplicate exactly as expected, given the associated phonology. (Syllabic nasals capitalized in the examples.)
(43)
Syllabic Nasals
a. M.med
b. ×.×ar
c. Mw.mwus
d. M.pek
e. N.da
f. Mw.pwul
M.m-i-m.med
N.×-i-×.×ar
Mw.mw-u-mw.mwus
M.p-i-m.pek
N.d-i-n.da
Mw.pw-u-mw.pwul
‘full’
‘see’
‘vomit’
‘search for lice’
‘say’
‘to flame’
Syllabic nasals are only allowed word-initially, preceding a consonant to which they are homorganic. Even there
they are liable to degemination in forms like (a-c) (p.36) and in forms like (d-f) they show optional prothesis of
/i/ or /u/, the latter appearing with rounded initial consonants or when the vowel of the first syllable is round
(p.56). Rendered word-internal by morphology, the nasals must desyllabify: for example, ka+mmed ‘cause to
be full’ emerges as bisyllabic kam.med. Epenthesis of i/u is therefore internally obligatory.
Note here the transfer of vowel length. See below, section 4 for discussion. The )µµ prefix cannot extend itself all the way to CVVC
(*duup-duupek) because superheavy syllables are allowed only word-finally. Compounds, of course, consist of two words: e.g. (40d)
waantuuke ‘count’.
22
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
23
Putting all this together, we see that a form like M.pek reduplicates like e.g. dune (39b). As a
polysyllable, it must satisfy a template FF; therefore ) expands as )µµ , Mp. The derived form Mp-M.pek
undergoes the general rule of epenthesis, emerging as M.pim.pek.23
Ponapean II: Vowel-Initial Stems
The single essential peculiarity of vowel-initial stems is that a vocalic element of one sort or another
appears between the expected )-satisfying prefix and the stem:
(44) Reduplication of Vowel-initial Stems
a. el
el-e-el
b. uk
uk-u-uk
c. it
it-i-it
d. aan
a-y-aan
e. oon
o-y-oon
f. eed
e-y-eed
g. iik
i-y-iik
h. uuk
u-y-uuk
i. uutoor
uu-y-uutoor
j. alu
al-i-alu
k. inen
in-i-inen
l. urak
ur-u-urak
‘rub, massage’
‘fast’
‘stuffed’
‘be accustomed to’
‘hung over’
‘strip off’
‘inhale’
‘lead’
‘independent’
‘walk’
‘straight’
‘wade’
Except for (44j) uu-y-uutoor, which should parallel (40c) to-tooroor, the general rules for choosing between )µ
and )µµ are clearly in effect: the heavy monosyllables of (d-h) get the )µ prefix; the others, )µµ .
Mokilese resolves the empty onset problem by spreading the syllable-final C: Mok. alu ‘walk’ becomes
al-l-alu. Ponapean responds to the structural pressure in a different way: the empty stem-initial onset is filled,
we propose, with a glide /y/. Intervocalically, as in (d-i), that’s exactly what we see. But glides are disallowed
post-consonantally. Therefore they vocalize, presumbly via a rule which adjoins the preceding consonant,
whereupon they will assimilate in quality to any following high vowel and to any vowel in monosyllables.24 . By
this account, Ponapean alu goes to al-y-alu, surfacing as a.li.a.lu, while el ‘rub’ goes to el-y-el, surfacing as
e.leel.
There is some variation in the high-vowel class of heavy monosyllables: iik may reduplicate as ik-iik,
uuk as uk-uuk. Rehg & Sohl note a possible source in analogy with uk (b) and it (c). Formally, we can get this
by exceptionally blocking glide insertion and allowing the stem-initial onset to pick the copy’s final consonant,
as in Orokaiva and others above. Another reported variant is uwuuk for uyuuk, presumably due to allowing the
language’s general glide insertion process to operate in the reduplication environment.
One small class remains, which lies outside the general pattern discussed here: glide-initial light
monosyllables, which reduplicate with a fixed prefix Ge-.
(45) Reduplication of Glide-initial Stems
a. wa
we-wa
b. was
we-was
c. yang
ye-yang
‘carry’
‘obnoxious’
‘accompany’
23
Another possible line of analysis would be to hold that syllabic nasals arise by optional dropping of initial i/u. Then impek exactly resembles
Mokilese andip.
24
Rehg & Sohl report some optionality in what appears to be the nonhigh vowel class: thus amas reduplicates as either amiamas or
amaamas, ewetik as either ewiewetik or eweewetik.
24
McCarthy & Prince
It’s possible to imagine a story assimilating these to the vowel-initial class, whereby the inserted y vocalizes and
dissimilates to e — so that ua > u-y-ua > u-e-ua. But it hardly seems worth it,25 especially since the pattern is
being lost from the language in favor of treating the forms as consonant-initial.
Ponapean III: Conclusion
Ponapean demonstrates quite unambiguously the fully prosodic character of reduplication. The categories
foot, syllable, and mora interact to characterize the reduplicating prefix in a quite general way. Even the (typically
quirky) onsetless stems fit into the core system with a minimum of special handling.
Ponapean IV: Proleptic and Historical Note
The survival of the ‘base vowel’ in the reduplication of a small class of (currently) monosyllablic stems
indicates that the process was originally one of foot reduplication, where F = µ µ, subsequently subject to a
variety of reductions particular to the reduplication structure. (A similar remark can be made for Mokilese.) It
is clearly possible to mount a synchronic description based on this premise. We resist the temptation on the
grounds that the bulk of the reductions follow immediately from specifying the prefix target as ) ()µ , )µµ ).
Independent rules of the language defining syllable and mora tell us what to take and when to amplify by
insertion. Furthermore, the principles (38) and (42) assigning foot templates to mono- and polysyllabic bases
determine the form of the prefix ) in an entirely straightforward way, resolving the otherwise inscrutable issue
of quantitative complementarity — if we take the prefix to be ). These considerations suggest strongly that the
system has in fact been reanalyzed along the lines suggested here.
MINIMAL W ORD/FOOT AS PREFIX
A particularly clear illustration of the interaction between reduplication and prosodic constituency is
provided by the Australian language Diyari, described by Austin (1981). We will discuss below another
Australian system, which involves interesting variations on the same pattern.
Diyari has both CV and CVC syllables, with no vowel length contrast. Consonants are prohibited at the
end of a phonological word. Within a root, stress is assigned to each odd-numbered nonfinal syllable, counting
from the left (this is a typical pattern in Australian languages);26 thus, the foot is of the trochaic, non-quantitysensitive type which must branch (whence the absence of stress on final syllables). All phonological words of
Diyari contain at least two syllables.27 It follows, then, that the minimal phonological word of Diyari is just a
single foot, which we know independently to be disyllabic. Diyari prosody is indifferent to the subsyllabic moraic
structure (that is, ) is a µ).
Diyari reduplication is of the type most commonly found in Australia, a prefixed copy of CV(C)CV:
(46)
Diyari Reduplication
wi,la
kanku
ku`ku×a
tjilparku
×ankan, ,ti
wi,la-wi,la
kanku-kanku
ku`ku-ku`ku×a
tjilpa-tjilparku
×anka-na×kan, ,ti
‘woman’
‘boy’
‘to jump’
‘bird sp.’
‘catfish’
Consider what we must explain about this pattern of reduplication. The reduplicated sequence is exactly two
syllables, of which the first may be CV or CVC, while the second is CV. From our observations about Diyari
prosody, we conclude that the reduplicative affix in this language is just the minimal phonological word, Wmin.
25
Notice that among other peculiarities ua would have to be treated as heavy monosyllable to get the )µ prefix.
26
For discussion of some irrelevant complications in Diyari stress assignment, see Poser (1986).
27
All grammatical words of Diyari also contain at least two syllables, except for the particle ya ‘and’.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
25
Everything follows from this. We must reduplicate two syllables, because the minimal phonological word is a
trochaic foot. The second syllable of the reduplication must be open, because it immediately precedes a
phonological word juncture.
In effect what we are saying is that reduplicated forms in Diyari are word-level compounds of an F (=
Wdmin) template with a normal word. This is confirmed by Austin’s (1981) careful arguments demonstrating that
the reduplicated string forms a separate phonological word from the base. Each portion of a reduplicated string
takes a separate main word stress (dúnkadúnka ‘to emerge’), and the vocalic allophony in the stressed syllable
of the reduplication as well as the prestopping of intervocalic nasals after the stress confirm this. It follows, then,
that the reduplicative affix in Diyari is the minimal free base for word-level compounding.
Diyari Reduplication
The original proposal that Diyari reduplication is foot-based is due to Poser 1982. The analysis
was subsequently revised and published as Poser 1989. In the text, we argue that the Diyari
reduplicative template is to be identified as the minimal word of the language. In M&P 1994a we
show that the special status of the minimal word as template follows, under Optimality Theory,
from its being the most harmonic prosodic word possible with respect to constraints on metrical
parsing. Background for this claim includes Spring 1990a, where the general PrWd is argued to
play a templatic function, and Itô & Mester 1992, where differing notions of minimality are derived
by placing branching constraints on PrWd; see Itô, Kitagawa, & Mester 1996 for development of
these in term of Alignment.
In M&P 1994b the argument is taken one step further: we argue that PrWd itself enters
as the canonical prosodic realization of the morphological category stem. Diyari reduplication is
therefore stem-compounding, and the behavior of the Diyari reduplicant follows from the
identification of its lexical status as stem. No prosodic template is required. This then leads to the
Generalized Prosodic Morphology Hypothesis: that templatic conditions are the reflection of
canonical prosodic restrictions on the morphological category that an item (such as a reduplicative
morpheme) belongs to, categories like stem and affix. These restrictions are imposed by the
standard constraints on prosodic structure and morphology-prosody alignment, universal under
OT, which dominate the faithfulness constraints relevant to the item’s realization. There are no
reduplication-specific structural constraints — `templates’. This seems like the minimal theory one
could hold, since morphemes must be assigned to some category (affix, stem,...).
A similar example, but one in which the evidence for minimum word size is of an even more striking
character, is provided by the Australian language Lardil. All information on Lardil comes from Hale (1973),
Klokeid (1976), and Hale’s unpublished field notes and dictionary. Our discussion of Lardil closely follows the
insightful treatment of Lardil phonology in Wilkinson (1986).
Lardil Phonology
Wilkinson 1986 was published, in revised and truncated form, as Wilkinson 1988. Prince &
Smolensky 1993, chapter 7, show how (the bulk of) Lardil nominal phonology follows from the
interaction of constraints, almost all of which are clearly universal in character.
26
McCarthy & Prince
Lardil syllables are of the form CV(V)(C), but there are quite rigid restrictions on final consonants. Only
apicals and palatoalveolars are licensed syllable finally.28 As is characteristic of Australian languages, only
vowels count as morae, and stress is assigned by a trochaic foot.
Lardil actively enforces a minimum word size requirement of two morae (that is, one foot). Words
containing only one mora must be augmented by the suffixation of a morphologically empty a:
(47) Augmentation in Lardil
Underlying
a.
/peer/
/maan/
b.
/par×a/
/kela/
c.
/wik/
/wun/
Uninflected
peer
maan
par×a
kela
wika
wunta
Accusative
peerin
maanin
par×an
kelan
wikin
wunin
‘ti-tree sp.’
‘spear gen.’
‘stone’
‘beach’
‘shade’
‘rain’
The nouns in (47a) are monosyllabic but bimoriac, while those in (47b) are disyllabic and bimoraic. In these two
types, no augmentation occurs in the uninflected form. The nouns in (47c) are underlyingly monomoraic, since
they contain only a single short vowel. In the uninflected form, they must undergo augmentation to meet the
minimum word-size requirement of two morae.
This requirement functions in another way as well. Words that are three or more morae long undergo
truncation of any final vowel:
(48)
Truncation in Lardil
/yiliyili/
/yukarpa/
yiliyil
yukar
yiliyilin
yukarpan
‘oyster’
‘husband’
The fact that shorter nouns like those in (47a) do not undergo this truncation follows directly from the minimum
word-size requirement — no truncation is possible without reducing such nouns below the minimum size. (V.
the Estonian parallel in section 1.)
Lardil has at least two types of reduplication, nominal and verbal. Nominal reduplication, as in many
cognate languages, is frozen but nevertheless clearly discernible as reduplication. It generally copies two morae:
/mu×kumu×ku/ ‘wooden axe’, /karikari/ ‘butterfish’. Verbal reduplication, a phenomenon whose existence and
properties were first noted by Wilkinson, is a more productive morphological process with a discernible iterative
meaning:
(49)
Reduplication in Lardil
/keleth/
/kelith/
/parelith/
/lath/
/neth/
/×aalith/
Simple
kele
keli
pareli
latha
netha
×aali
Reduplicated
kelekele
kelikeli
parelpareli
laala
neene
×aal×aali
‘cut’
‘jump’
‘gather’
‘spear’
‘strike’
‘thirst’
The underlying forms of all verb roots end in the verb marker -th. This marker protects the final vowel of long
verbs from truncation (as in pareli) and appears overtly in the simple form of short verbs (like petha) preceding
the augment a.
28
As Itô (1986) shows, the interaction of syllabic well- formedness conditions with constraints on the analyzability of geminates in Lardil
and other languages permits the homorganic nasal-stop clusters as well.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
27
It is apparent that verbal reduplication is foot or minimal word sized as well, and it exhibits several
properties that we have met with and will again as we treat reduplication phenomena from other languages. In
cases like kelekele, the consequences of foot reduplication are obvious. Less apparent, but equally straightforward, is the fact that a long vowel, although a single syllable, contains two moras and thus satisfies the
foot/minimal word template, as in ×aal×aali.29 Furthermore, the copying of a final consonant in this case and in
parelpareli is explained by the independent syllabic well-formedness conditions of the language — only apicals
and palatoalveolars are copied finally, because only they are permitted in syllable-final position.30
Examples like laala exhibit the strict shape invariance called for by the Satisfaction Condition. Here we
find a short vowel copied as long, thereby satisfying the requirement that the reduplicative affix contain two
morae. Such requirements are immutable — since there is no erasure of unassociated skeletal positions, all
prosodically characterized positions in the affix must be filled in accordance with the requirements of the
language. Since no way to fill a mora other than a vowel is permitted in Lardil, what we find is spreading of the
vowel to this slot, as in the diagram in (50):
(50)
Wmin
F
)
µµ
l a th + lath
The final th is not associated because it is not a licit syllable- (and therefore foot-) final consonant of the
language.31
The situation in yet another Australian language, Yidig (Dixon 1977; Nash 1979-1980), is somewhat
different. The relevant data are as follows:
(51)
Reduplication in Yidig
mulari
mulamulari
kintalpa
kintalkintalpa
kalamparaa
kalakalamparra
‘initiated man’
‘lizard sp.’
‘March fly’
In Yidig, the foot is disyllabic, QI (at the point when stress is assigned — cf. section 1), and so stress
is assigned to every odd-numbered syllable from the left. It is clear from Dixon’s discussion that the phonology
systematically treats Yidig reduplicated words as compounds, just as Diyari does.
We analyze Yidig reduplication as F (= Wmin), the disyllabic foot, as do Nash (1979-1980) and Hayes
(1985). The problem is obviously one of accounting for whether or not the reduplicative affix contains a final
consonant. In Diyari, this determination is made solely by word-level prosody — no word-final consonants are
licensed, so none appear in the compound reduplicative affix. In Lardil, the same determination is made by
29
This example also exhibits the phenomenon of transfer, which we take up below in section 4.3.
30
In Wilkinson’s analysis, even derived palatoalveolars count for this purpose. Lardil has a rule converting all consonants to the
corresponding palatoalveolar before a labial: /pit+puri/ pitypuri ‘smell (source)’, /wik+puri/ witypuri ‘shade (source)’. This rule applies to
the verb marker th in reduplications of labial-initial roots, as in the example patitypati already cited, or in petha/peetype ‘bite’. Given the
extensive alternations among apicals and nonapicals in the language, it is likely that this analysis is reconstructible without the notion “derived
palatoalveolar”.
31
There is an additional complication in Lardil reduplication. We find reduplications like thaltii/thaltiithaltii ‘stand up’, where base-final
length appears in original and copy. Against this are forms like patii/patitypati ‘sit with legs outstretched’ where length is lost and noun-to-verb
reduplications like ×arnda/×arnda×arndaa ‘crotch/spread the legs apart’ in which length appears only in the original and not the copy. It
appears that these length alternations have a great deal to do with a poorly- understood rule of final vowel lengthening marking intransitives.
28
McCarthy & Prince
syllable-level prosody — only apicals and palatoalveolars are permitted syllable finally, so only they associate
with the right edge of the copy. Yidig exhibits a different situation.
Nash (1979-1980) proposes that only the phonemic melody elements of the first foot in the base are
available for reduplicative association. This crucially distinguishes the r of mulari, which is not in the foot, from
the l of kintalpa, which is. In our terms, this is stated somewhat differently: reduplication is prefixation of the
minimal word to the minimal word. Thus, Yidig reduplication comes under the parameter of our theory (and of
Broselow and McCarthy’s (1983–4)) in which a designated prosodic constituent is the base for reduplicative
affixation, rather than a morphological constituent. This situation is obvious and essential in the analysis of
certain types of infixing reduplication in section 2.3. In the Yidig case, the prosodic constituent base is initial and
it receives a prefix, so positionally the affix appears to be simply prefixed. But the behavior of final consonants
reveals the true nature of the process.
Affixation to a prosodic constituent has essentially the same formal properties as affixation to a
morphological one. The affix is placed relative to the designated constituent, and only the phonemic melody
elements associated with that constituent are copied and available for association. We can now turn to the details
of Yidig.
Nash (1979-1980) argues that clusters like lp and mp are systematically distinguished in Yidig
phonology; the latter are prenasalized stops (therefore tautosyllabic), while the former are heterosyllabic. Several
factors support this view. First, the only major type of triconsonantal cluster in the language is CNC, where NC
is a putative prenasalized stop. Second, various phonological alternations support this interpretation. Third, slow
speech pronunciations show the expected loss of prenasalization (which does not occur word-initially), but do
not affect true clusters. We will therefore write the prenasalized stops as a single melodic element, B.32
The examples in (51) would then be prosodized as follows:
(52)
a.
b.
F
c.
F
F
F
)))
)))
))))
mulari
kiTalpa
kalaBara
Under our account, affixation to the minimal word (F) means that only the phonemic melody elements associated
with this constituent are copied and available for association. This produces exactly the desired result: the only
consonant that can associate with the end of the reduplicative skeleton is one that itself is part are the
prosodically-characterized base of reduplication — the initial foot of the word. There is no notion of ‘footcopying’ here; rather, the Yidig paradigm follows from the interaction of the minimal-word base and the minimalword reduplicative affix.
It is strong evidence in support of this approach that combining independently needed properties of the
theory in this way yields results that are otherwise inexplicable. Moreover, we now have a closely parametrized
account of the minimal distinction between Lardil parel-pareli and Yidig mula-mulari: the former characterizes
the base of reduplicative affixation in purely morphological terms (the stem), while the latter opts for a
phonological characterization (the minimal word). The distinction is not capturable in segmental skeleton terms
32
There is an alternative account of clusters like mp that is equally compatible with our theory. If they are regarded as true heterosyllabic
clusters, rather than single melodic units, then all facts about their surface distribution will equally well follow from Itô’s (1986) licensing
theory, presented above in connection with the analysis of Orokaiva. Only homorganic nasal-stop clusters will have the crucial branching
structure at the articulator tier, thereby exempting them from various aspects of syllabic well-formedness. The pattern of reduplication in kalakalamparaa (*kalam-kalamparaa) then follows from the Geminate Constraint constraint as well: the putative minimal word kalam would
contain only half of a structure that branches at the articulator tier — therefore only kala is copied and available for association.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
29
without invoking mechanisms that have the same effect as those we need in any case; it does no good to say that
Yidig reduplicates CVCCV because of kintal-kintalpa.
Reduplication in Yidig and Lardil
This contrast is analyzed in terms of prosodic circumscription in M&P 1990a.
This account of Yidig suggests an analysis of certain recalcitrant cases in Austronesian languages. We
focus on Makassarese. According to Aronoff (1985),33 reduplication in Makassarese displays a peculiar kind of
phonological and morphological sensitivity:
(53)
Reduplication in Makassarese
a. Disyllabic Words
ballak
ballak-ballak
golla
golla-golla
tau
tau-tau
tau×
taun-tau×
b. Longer Words
kaluarak
kaluk-kaluarak
manara
manak-manara
balao
balak-balao
baine
baik-baine
‘house’
‘sugar’
‘person’
‘year’
‘ant’
‘tower’
‘rat’
‘woman’
The consonant k is evidently in complementary distribution with ; we may therefore regard the k appearing at
the right edge of the copy in (53b) as an inserted .
The reduplicative affix in Makassarese is obviously disyllabic (the vowel sequences are all
heterosyllabic); since the language has penultimate stress and appears to lack monosyllables, this too is an
instance of Wmin = F reduplication. Aronoff states the generalization underlying these two patterns of
reduplication — one without and one with final — as follows. If the boundary of the second syllable copied does
not coincide with a morphological boundary, then insert at the end of the second syllable. Otherwise copy the
syllable-final consonant if there is one.
Our interpretation of this regularity is the following. In Yidig, the minimal word that is the base of
reduplication actually coincides with a foot already present in the form, since stress is assigned from the left. In
Makassarese, though, stress is assigned from the right, so the minimal word base must result from a prosodic
reparsing of the original from the left. This reparsing simply selects the first two syllables as a minimal word.
Copying of the phonemes of this minimal word and association with the affixal minimal word yield the pattern
in (53a). The pattern in (53b) (and its near parallel in Tagalog) involves a situation where the minimal word
derived by reparsing — the phonologically characterized base — does not exhaust a morpheme. It is precisely
in this conflict between morphology and phonology that Makassarese develops the intrusive k.
Makassarese Reduplication
The analysis given in the text is incorrect; it cannot account for additional data that were not
available at the time. For the evidence, and a different analysis, see Aronoff, Arsyad, Basri, &
Broselow 1987. M&P 1990a offers an account of Makassarese based on prosodic circumscription.
M & P 1994a develops a complete treatment within Optimality Theory.
33
Reporting on joint work with Ellen Broselow.
30
McCarthy & Prince
Yidig and similar Australian languages, with stress assigned from the left, have no such reparsing,
deriving the minimal word base from simple inspection of the foot structure; consequently they can have no such
rule. Makassarese and like-minded languages must reparse; they may then compensate for the difference between
phonological and morphological edges. The formal characterization of this process in Makassarese remains
obscure; but the context is not. Our theory independently requires a minimal word base, derived by reparsing,
to achieve the surface pattern of copying in this language. The rule of insertion records the success of this
reparsing as a measure of morphological integrity. The otherwise inexplicable condition on insertion follows
from this conception of the phonological base.
Prosodic Reparsing
The notion of prosodic reparsing is developed within circumscription theory in M&P 1995a.
2.2
Suffixation
An adequate theory of suffixing reduplication should derive all differences between prefixes and suffixes from
the positional differences between them alone — since an analysis must in any case stipulate the position. As we
will see, there are certain important distinctions between prefixing and suffixing types, and we will show how
these distinctions are reconstructible in our theory.
THE MINIMAL W ORD OR FOOT AS SUFFIX TARGET
Manam (Lichtenberk 1983) displays foot reduplication in the suffixing mode, and it is clear that the foot
and minimal word are identical in this language. Underlyingly, Manam syllables are of the form (C)V(N), where
N is a nasal homorganic to a following consonant or one of × or m in free variation word-finally. This regularity
is supported phonologically by regular i-epenthesis after any nonnasal consonants in preconsonantal or word-final
position. Vowel-initial syllables are freely distributed, and tautosyllabic vowel clusters or long vowels do not
occur.
In general, stress is assigned to the syllable containing the penultimate mora, where a mora is reckoned
as either a vowel or a syllable-final consonant:34
(54)
Stress in Manam
si×ába
soái
lúnta
malabó×
‘bush’
‘tobacco’
‘moss’
‘flying fox’
The foot is therefore bimoraic in our terms. Two other rules, which have no effect on the morphological function
of the Manam foot, retract stress to the antepenult if either the penult is vowel-initial or the antepenult is closed
and the penult is not. Both these retraction rules are prohibited from applying in morphologically complex forms.
34
The vowel sequence oa is, however, monomoraic. Both final reduplication and initial (monomoraic syllable) reduplication copy oa
sequences as a single mora: raboaaboaa ‘plumeria’, goazagoaza ‘clean (sg.)’; moamoatubu ‘heavy (pl.)’, goagoaza ‘clean (pl.)’. There
are also several phonological indications that oa sequences are monomoraic. First, oa becomes wa when preceded by a vowel, a wordboundary, or a medial labial consonant. Second, oa sequences not subject to these rules are reduced to oë when stressed if followed by a
consonant. These reductions are peculiar to this vowel sequence, and they obviously correlate closely with the behavior of the two types of
reduplication.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
31
Manam Stress
The literature on Manam stress has grown in the intervening years. See Chaski 1986, Halle and
Kenstowicz 1991, Kager 1993b, and Buckley 1994.
Reduplication in Manam suffixes F, the bimoraic foot:
(55)
Reduplication in Manam
salaga
salagalaga
moita
moitaita
arai
arairai
lao
laolao
malabo×
malabombo×
ulan
ulanla×
‘long’
‘knife’
‘ginger sp.’
‘go’
‘flying fox’
‘desire’
These forms are entirely straightforward. A bimoraic foot is suffixed, and right-edge-in reprosodization applies
without regard to syllable structure.35
There are two interesting complications. First, forms ending with two identical syllables reduplicate only
a single syllable:
(56)
ragogo
oo
ragogogo
ooo
‘be warm’
‘be plentiful’
This is clearly a case of haplology applied to the sequence of four identical syllables derived by reduplication.
Second, Manam has five monomoraic verb roots, all of which reduplicate only a single mora:
(57)
ra
pi
rara
pipi
‘talk to’
‘be forceful’
The roots of this type have a number of other peculiarities as well which account for their failure to show the
invariant bimoraic reduplicative pattern. All of them irregularly take final stress when unsuffixed: i-rá ‘it is bad’,
i-pí ‘he is forceful’. All but pi have disyllabic variants like raya. Thus, one option is simply to regard these forms
as underlyingly bimoraic, as Lichtenberk (1983) does, taking raya as the base. This has an additional advantage
— it eliminates the only apparent counterexamples to Fµµ = Wdmin.
An alternative is to invoke exceptionality. The normal interpretation of this pattern of exceptionality to
stress assignment (Hayes 1980, 1983) is a lexical foot — here obviously monomoraic. This lexical foot is
sufficient to fulfill the requirements imposed by the foot-sized reduplicative suffix and the Satisfaction Condition.
The treatment of monomoraic bases in reduplication is unambiguous in the parallel case of the Bolivian
language Siriono (Priest 1980). With few exceptions, the language has penultimate stress (that is, the foot is
trochaic, QI). Reduplication suffixes a copy of the last two syllables (58a), but there is an additional lengthening
when monosyllabic bases are copied (58b):36
(58)
Repetitive Reduplication in Siriono
a.
ñimbuchao
ñimbuchaochao
achisia
achisiasia
embui
embuimbui
esiquio
esiquioquio
eochi
eochiochi
‘separarse’
‘yo corto’
‘divide’
‘quebrar’
‘está sin nada’
35
Unfortunately, Lichtenberk reports no examples of reduplication with words of the lunta type.
36
These examples are reported in the orthography, not a phonemic transcription.
32
McCarthy & Prince
b.
ño
ja
ñooño
jaaja
‘no más’
‘todo’
The obvious comparison here is with Lardil. In Lardil, reduplication of monomoraic bases yields a bimoraic copy
by spreading of the copy vowel. In Siriono, the mirror image occurs: the original vowel spreads onto the affixal
template. In both cases, what we see is enforcement of a universal principle: the prosodic requirements imposed
by the affix must be fulfilled. There is no erasure of unassociated melodic elements because there are no
unassociated melodic elements.
THE SYLLABLE AS SUFFIX TARGET
Major apparent differences between prefixing and suffixing reduplication arise when we consider
syllable-sized reduplicative affixes. The apparent VC suffix of Tzeltal and the apparent CCVC suffix of
Kaingang illustrate this difference.
(59)
Suffixing ) Reduplication
a. Tzeltal (Berlin 1963, Kaufman 1971)
nit
nititan
net’
net’et’an
haš
hašašan
gol
gololan
p’uy
p’uyuyan
b. Kaingang (Wiesemann 1972; Poser 1982)
Root
Plural stem
vâ
vâvâ
jêmî
jêmîmî
kry
krykry
vâsân
vâsânsân
‘push’
‘press’
‘feel with palm’
‘make rows’
‘grind in fingers’
‘to throw away’
‘to grasp’
‘to itch’
‘to exert, fatigue’
These are both fairly typical patterns of suffixing reduplication. While Kaingang adds a full syllable, including
onset, as Poser (1982) has argued, Tzeltal appears to add only a rhyme, deriving its onset from phonological
material already present in the base.
Considered segmentally, while the apparent CCVC suffix of Kaingang is paralleled prefixally, the
apparent VC suffix of Tzeltal is not. There are no cases of prefixing reduplication of this type.37 The goal of
deriving all differences between prefixing and suffixing reduplication from position alone is obviously not
achieved under this conception.
Our approach does derive the difference in a straightforward way. Recall that prefixing reduplication
rules (like rules of prefixation in general) differ in whether or not the juncture between affix and base is
transparent to the Onset Rule. Where the juncture is transparent, we get the pattern of Mokilese, Orokaiva, or
Ponapean, but where it is not we find systems like Sundanese or Ilokano. The same difference is at work in
suffixation. The transparent juncture produces the Tzeltal pattern; the opaque one derives Kaingang. Thus, the
VC suffix that is apparently restricted to suffixes has a straightforward place in our account.
Alignment in Suffixing Reduplication
See the discussion and references in the first box on p. 13.
37
Tübatulabal, which has been considered to be of this sort (Levin 1983), is discussed in section 3.2.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
33
Exercising the typological resources of our theory on suffixing reduplication yields some other
conclusions as well. We have found no cases of bimoraic syllable suffixation. This gap does not appear to be a
principled one; the relative rarity of suffixing reduplication, joined with the relative rarity of bimoraic
reduplicative affixes, is sufficient to account for it. The absence of stipulated monomoraic or core syllable
suffixes is more deep. What data would force such an analysis? A word like hypothetical badag would copy only
da, yielding badagda. But this violates a quite general condition on the locality of reduplication — the identical
strings must be stricly contiguous. This condition is expressed in Marantz (1982) via phoneme-driven
association, and in our approach it follows from the rigid restrictions we impose on skipping melody elements
(v. section 4).
A final case of suffixing reduplication uses the mechanisms of our theory to solve an outstanding
problem in skeletal theory. The contribution of Everett and Seki (1985) on Kamaiurá represents the sole argument
for the overt phonological status of unassociated skeletal elements. Recall that one of the tenets and purposes of
the prosodic theory of skeleta is to eliminate the phonologically impotent unassociated skeletal elements by
requiring that the entire skeleton, conceived prosodically, be filled. A major problem with segmental conceptions
of the skeleton, represented by C/V or X units, is that unassociated units must be erased immediately, else they
would do harm to the phonological and morphological derivation.
Everett and Seki’s claim is that, in Kamaiurá reduplication, unassociated skeletal units not only do no
harm, but in fact do positive good. Their view is that Kamaiurá has a CVCVC reduplicative suffix, and a surface
constraint prohibiting consonant clusters except in word-final position. This constraint is expressed by a cluster
simplification rule of the skeletal tier, C 0 / ___C, which is evidently independently motivated.
(60)
Reduplication in Kamaiurá (Everett and Seki 1985)
ohuka
ohukahuka
ojenupâ
ojenupânupâ
ereo
ereoreo
omotumu×
omotumutumu×
omokon
omokomokon
apot
apoapot
‘he laughed’
‘he hit himself’
‘you go’
‘he shook it’
‘he swallowed it’
‘I jump’
The relevant cases in Everett and Seki’s analysis are those like apoapot, where the root consonant is apparently
lost before the reduplicative suffix. The derivation proceeds as follows:
(61)
a.
b.
c.
d.
VCVC
VCVC+CVCVC
VCV +CVCVC
VCV+VCVC
apot
apot
apot
apo
apot
apot
apot
Affixation and association proceed normally, as in (61b), but the unassociated initial C of the suffix is not erased.
Now, even though the t does not precede a consonant, its associated C does precede another C, albeit an empty
one. This is sufficient to trigger the CC simplification rule, yielding (61c). Erasure of unassociated skeletal and
melodic elements yields (61d). The thrust of this argument is that application of the cluster simplification rule
intervenes between reduplicative association and the erasure of unassociated skeletal elements.
There are two difficulties with this account. First, since unassociated skeletal C or X slots are otherwise
invisible to the phonology in all other languages, it is surprising that just in this one case they should exert such
influence. The second problem is related to the first, but is far more important. As Itô (1986) demonstrates,
phonological theory must countenance a general process of erasure of unsyllabified elements, Stray Erasure. Stray
Erasure, which is needed in any case, is sufficient to account for the Kamaiurá cluster-simplification phenomenon.
Syllable-final consonants are licensed only word-finally, and so the rule of cluster simplification is apparently
34
McCarthy & Prince
redundant with respect to a universal phonological condition — Stray Erasure — and the independently-needed
syllabic well-formedness conditions of this language.
Our alternative account is as follows. Kamaiurá syllable structure is simply (C)V, with the option (C)VC
possible only in word-final position (conceivably by moraic extrametricality). The phenomenon of cluster
simplification is a consequence of this reduced syllabic structure and Stray Erasure. The reduplicative suffix is
)), and we must stipulate that the base-suffix juncture is opaque to the Onset Rule in any case, simply to derive
omokomokon rather than *omokonokon (which would correspond to the transparent Tzeltal pattern). These
requirements are sufficient to derive omokomokon and apoapot as follows:
(62)
a.
Underlying
b.
)) )
))
omokon
apot
)) )+
Reduplication
) )
) ) +) )
omokon omokon
apot apot
)) )+))
) ) +) )
Stray Erasure
omoko mokon
apo apot
In the second step of the derivation, the syllable-final consonants of the bases, now no longer word-final, are in
unlicensed positions and so must desyllabify. The Onset Rule is simply unavailable, and so the unsyllabified
consonants are erased. Kamaiurá has no rule of consonant-cluster simplification — rather, it has quite rigid
constraints on syllabic well-formedness and the universal principle of Stray Erasure.38
Kamaiurá Reduplication
M&P 1993a argue that Kamaiurá shows pre-final-coda-C infixing reduplication, a symmetric
counterpart to post-initial-onsetless-syllable infixation, as in Uradhi and Timugon Murut
(immediately below), Sanskrit and Orokaiva (misanalyzed above p.11,15). Chamorro is another
similar case, identical to Kamaiurá except for the size of the reduplicant — see below, p. 44.
2.3
Infixation
Partial reduplication comes not only in the prefixing and suffixing varieties, but also infixing. In our account of
infixing reduplication, we adopt some of the typological distinctions made by Broselow and McCarthy (1983–4),
but in our formal system a somewhat different interpretation is given to these phenomena.
Broselow and McCarthy identify two major types of infixing reduplication. One, exemplified by the
Samoan pattern of section 2.3.2, involves precisely the same mechanisms exhibited by prefixing and suffixing
38
There is an alternative account of Kamaiurá within our theory as an instance of infixing reduplication. Rendering the final consonant
extraprosodic and suffixing )) will produce the same results. This analysis is vulnerable to one of the criticisms that Everett and Seki raise
against a quite different sort of infixing analysis — it fails to derive the apparent loss of a consonant from independently motivated rules of
the language. The analysis presented in the text is obviously not susceptible to this criticism.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
35
reduplication, but where the base of the affixing operation is not a morphological constituent but rather a
phonological one. The other type, which is known through the Arabic, Temiar, and Zuni patterns, inserts a copy
of a peripheral segment at some relatively remote position inside the root. We differ from Broselow and
McCarthy in that we see this sort of behavior as unique to root-and-pattern or templatic morphological systems
and involving the exercise of the rather special morphological properties of such systems.
Our greatest point of difference with earlier work is the recognition of a third type of infixing
reduplication, one which can be detected by a conjunction of two phenomena: copying of some string at a
morphological periphery, but where the copied string does not contain some peripheral segment(s) and appears
‘inside’ that segment(s). This, the most common pattern of infixing reduplication, is attributed to peripheral
extraprosodicity on the melodic tier and involves considerable interesting exercise of the resyllabification
mechanisms we have developed.
It is argued, then, that infixing reduplication is not a single phenomenon with a unitary formal
explanation, but rather is the result of the intersection of various formal parameters of the theory.39
INFIXATION VIA EXTRAPROSODICITY
Infixing reduplication of this type can be diagnosed by the following characteristic: the placement of the
copied string is relative to some element(s) that is not itself copied. This element is invariably peripheral (or a
peripheral string). Our theoretical interpretation is that this element exhibits melodic extrametricality only. The
effect of this extrametricality is solely that the designated melodic element is detached from the skeleton of the
base; it is still available for copying and association with the reduplicative affix. Melodic extraprosodicity is
therefore to be distinguished from templatic extraprosodicity. The latter is associated with a particular
constellation of properties in prosody (Hayes 1982b, Prince 1984) and melody copying (Steriade 1982). We
invoke melodic extraprosodicity as well to account for peripheral incompletely-formed prosodic constituents —
see the discussion of Arabic in 2.4.
Prefix as Infix
In the following examples, the extrametrical elements and the skeletal affix are indicated after the name
of the language:
(63)
39
Infixing Reduplication
Pangasinan I (Benton 1971:99): #V EM, prefix )c
Singular
Plural
amigo
amimigo
kanayon
kakanayon
libro
lilibro
niog
niniog
plato
paplato
balbas
babalbas
‘friend’
‘relative’
‘book’
‘coconut’
‘plate’
‘beard’
Broselow and McCarthy (1983–4) discuss three putative examples of infixing reduplication which we disregard here on the grounds that
they are almost certainly misanalyzed. Quileute, a Chimakuan language of the Northwest Coast (Andrade 1933), is reported to have a
frequentative reduplication that takes qa:le to qaqle ‘he failed’ and a plural that takes qa:wats to qa:qe:wats ‘potato’. It is simply impossible
to rule out a phonological account of these phenomena — in particular, deletion of a reduced vowel in /qaqale/ would make a great deal of
sense in the overall context of the phonology of these languages. The Takelma example, in which the root hemg ‘take out’ has aorist hemeg
and frequentative hememg, has been convincingly demonstrated by Goodman (1983) to be a case of root-and-pattern morphology complete
with vowel and consonant segregation (see also Lee (forthcoming [1991])). The apparent reduplication, then, is nothing more than spreading
of vocalic and consonantal elements to empty skeletal slots. Finally, Nakanai, a language claimed to exhibit several infixing reduplication
processes under complex phonological control, on closer inspection turns out to have foot-suffixing reduplication with a number of reduction
processes applied to the now-destressed original.
36
McCarthy & Prince
Pangasinan II (Benton 1971: 151): #V EM, prefix )
Numeral
‘only’
sakey
saksakey
talo
taltalora
apat
apatpatira
anem
anemnemira
siam
siasiamira
Uradhi (Crowley 1983): #(C)V EM, prefix ).
Verb
Continuous
wili
wilili
ipihi
ipipihi
wampa
wampampa
unytya
unytyanytya
uhya
uhihya
ikya
ikikya
Mangarayi (Merlan 1982): #C EM, prefix ).
Singular
Plural
gabuji
gababuji
yirag
yirirag
jimgan
jimgimgan
wa×gij
wa×ga×gij
muygji
muygjuygji
Timugon Murut (Prentice 1971): V-initial ) EM /#___, prefix ).
Diminutive/Instrumental
bulud
bubulud
tulu
tutulu
dondo
dodondo
ulampoy
ulalampoy
indimo
indidimo
ompod
ompopod
‘one’
‘three’
‘four’
‘five’
‘eight’
‘run’
‘swim’
‘float’
‘sleep’
‘eat’
‘speak’
‘old person’
‘father’
‘knowledgeable one’
‘child’
‘having a dog’
‘hill’
‘point at’
‘one’
not glossed
‘five times’
‘flatter’
Consider first of all the Mangarayi example. Induction over the cited examples provides an assessment
of Mangarayi syllable structure that fairly well accords with the facts; syllables are CV(C), with CVCC syllables
occasionally occurring. Since this language has no vowel-initial syllables, it is actually sufficient to say that any
initial melodic element is rendered extraprosodic and a syllable is prefixed. The derivation, then, proceeds as
follows:
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
37
(64)
) )
a.
jimgan
)+
b.
(j) imgan
)+
c.
))
jimgan
) )
(j) imgan
In (64b), we see the immediate result of the reduplication rule. An empty ) is prefixed, and the initial melody of
the base is marked as extrametrical (indicated here by parenthesization). The effect of this is exclusively to detach
j from the base skeleton. With phonemic melody copying in (64c), we see the result of association. Left-to-right
association of the copied melody with the empty ) yields the syllable jim, as expected, but a further thing
happens. As in all cases of infixing reduplication, the Onset Rule applies, linking the next phonemic element g
with the empty onset of the initial ) of the root.
Consider what the generalization underlying the Mangarayi facts is. The rule could be paraphrased as
‘after the initial consonant, insert a copy of the first VC* sequence, where C* is the maximal licit intersyllabic
consonant cluster of the language.’ Obviously C* represents a finite number of consonants, so there is a
paraphrase in terms of a CV skeleton or X skeleton which will work, but the underlying generalization that this
precisely replicates the intersyllabic clustering possibilities — whatever they are — is not captured. The approach
we offer, in which the ‘copied’ melody shares skeletal associations with prefix and base, captures exactly that
generalization by exploiting the syllabic well-formedness conditions of the language and a minimal stipulation
of extraprosodicity, just sufficient to indicate that this underlying prefix appears on the surface to be an infix.
Moreover, we relate the C* pattern of infixing reduplication to the universal Onset Rule, to the prefixing
reduplication of Mokilese or Orokaiva, and to typological differences in suffixing reduplication.
This approach also provides for a straightforward learning theory even for systems as complex as
Mangarayi. Learning the Mangarayi rule requires only the following steps: (1) recognize that reduplication is
involved; (2) subtract the canonical pattern of the original (say, ))) from the canonical pattern of the derived form
(say, )))) to determine the form of the affix; (3) establish the location and extent of extraprosodicity. The crucial
step is the second; only under a prosodic conception of the skeleton is the determination of the shape-invariant
in Mangarayi reduplication so trivial.
38
McCarthy & Prince
Infixing Reduplication
The basic line of attack here, which sees this form of infixing reduplication as a variant of
straightforward prefixation/suffixation, is surely on the right track. The use of extraprosodicity to
deal with peripheral material is also a reasonable first cut at the problem of getting the infix inside
the base; on this, see also Kiparsky 1986. But the view of extraprosodicity as desyllabification is
clearly incorrect — it cannot explain why the “extraprosodic” segment of the base is available for
copying, yet not for association. A superior account of extraprosodicity emerges from the theory
of prosodic circumscription in M&P 1990a, in which the extraprosodic or ‘negatively circumscribed’
material is available for neither copying nor association. All extraprosodicity-based accounts,
however, fail to relate the shape of the affix to the conditions of its insertion. This relationship is
obtained when infixation is occasioned by domination of Alignment constraints, as in M&P 1993a:
Chapt. 7 and Prince & Smolensky 1991, 1993.
If infixing reduplication is to be accounted for by peripheral extraprosodicity together with the
mechanisms of affixation and association, it is to be expected that other infixes — those that appear to be fully
specified, will exhibit similar properties. This is in fact the case. Consider, for example, the characteristics of the
plural ar infix of Sundanese (Robins 1959), evidently the only productive infix in this language
(65)
Infixation in Sundanese
Singular
niis
naho
Plural
nariis
naraho
‘to cool oneself’
‘to know’
This infix lodges immediately after the first consonant of the base. Under our proposal, this infix will
appear as in (66a), and the derived representation of nariis will appear as in (66b):
(66)
a.
b.
ar
)
)) )
ar
(n) i i s
= nariis
Here, the Onset Rule has applied twice. One application links the floating n with the affixal ), and the other
resyllabifies r of the infix with the initial ) of the base. The representation of the affixal melody on a separate
tier is independently required by Sundanese nasal harmony (Hart 1981) and by observations about infixes in
numerous languages (McCarthy 1981).
Virtually identical forms of infixation are met with in other Austronesian languages as well. A
particularly interesting case, in which moraic specifications are also exercised, are met with in the Philippine
language Balangao (Shetler 1976). Balangao has quite a few infixes, including a contrast between distinct
morphemes in and inn, both appearing after the initial consonant:
(67)
Infixation in Balangao
a. in Infixation
Singular
bubae
madan
Plural
binubae
minadan
‘girl’
‘old woman’
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
b. inn Infixation
Root
bayu
balon
basol
39
Derived Form
binnayu
binnalon
binnasol
‘pound’
‘lunch’
‘sin’
The skeletal representation of the in infix is identical to that of Sundanese ar, but the inn infix is represented
lexically with a bimoraic syllable, as in (68a). The Onset Rule, then, does not effect complete reassociation
(because the bimoraic syllable blocks that), but rather simply adds an association line, to yield the result in (68b):
(68)
a.
)
b. in
µµ
)
ar
µ µ
) )
(b)alon
= binnalon
The contrast between geminating and nongeminating applications of the Onset Rule in the two Balangao infixes
is also seen in our discussion of prefixing reduplication systems like Mokilese/Ponapean and Orokaiva: when the
affix is a heavy syllable, we get gemination; when the affix is a syllable of unspecified weight, we get simple
resyllabification. Although the bimoraic pattern is not attested among our cases of infixing reduplication, it does
arise here in a case of fully specified infixation.
There is another interesting point about infixation that our proposals explain. Examples like Sundanese
and Balangao, as well as the broader evidence from language games described in section 3.1, demonstrate that
the locus classicus of infixation — after the first consonant — is reserved for infixes of an apparent VC canonical
pattern. This follows elementarily from the need to integrate the infix into the syllabic structure of the base; an
apparent CV infix could not in general do this. Thus, the usual character of nonreduplicative infixes (vowel-initial
syllables) follows from the requirements of the syllable — there must be an empty onset to which the initial
extraprosodic consonant of the base associates.
VC Infixation
The observation that infixation of VC prefixes is related to syllable structure was originally made
by Anderson 1972 (also see Cohn 1992). But as Prince & Smolensky 1991b, 1993 observe, none
of the proposed analyses are able to make a formal connection between infix placement and
syllabic well-formedness/unmarkedness. Their account, by contrast,calls directly on the syllablestructure constraint, NO-CODA, which dominates and forces minimal violation of the constraint
demanding peripheral placement of the affix. On moraic infixation (gemination), see SamekLodovici 1992, 1993.
The identification of infixation with extraprosodicity naturally leads to the question of what sorts of
elements can be extraprosodic. Obviously a single initial consonant (Mangarayi) or vowel (Pangasinan) can be
disassociated in this way, but can sequences be extraprosodic? Inspection of the other cases in (63) is somewhat
misleading, since in both of them certain peculiarities of the syllable structure make the situation less than clear.
The Paman language Uradhi is in the unusual situation of having relatively few words that do not begin
with a vowel. The statement in (63), which renders a (C)V sequence extraprosodic, is intensionally equivalent
to the description in Crowley (1983), but in fact, all the cited examples begin with a vocoid of some sort, and the
materials available to us provide no instances of reduplication applied to a word with an initial true consonant.
40
McCarthy & Prince
Certainly all examples are consistent with an account in which #[-cons] is rendered extrametrical, in which
circumstance we have derived representations like those in (69):
(69)
a.
) + ))
b.
)
+
)
)
c.
) + ) )
wili (w)ili
wampa (w)ampa
uñta (u)ñta
=wilili
=wampampa
=uñtañta
In the final example, not only the Onset Rule but also the Nucleus Rule have applied from the copied phonemic
melody to the base skeleton, again as required by the well-formedness conditions of the language (that is,
syllables must have [-cons] nuclei).
We have, then, insufficient information to determine whether Uradhi is applying extraprosodicity to a
sequence. Timugon Murut, on the other hand, seems far more clearly to detach an entire vowel-initial syllable.
This in itself is unsurprising — in stress systems, syllabic extrametricality is the norm, and even nonconstituent
sequences are sometimes extrametrical (witness the treatment of ga(laxy) in Hayes 1982a).
Uradhi and Timugon Murut Reduplication
These two languages, and others cited in (63) (except Mangarayi), place a reduplicative infix after
an initial onsetless syllable. Remarkably, only reduplicative infixes have this distribution — an
observation that cannot be made to follow from extrametricality or its generalized and sharpened
successor, prosodic circumscription (as developed in M&P 1990a, 1991b). An explanation
emerges under the assumption that such infixation results from domination of an Alignment
constraint by syllable structure constraints. The core idea is that infixation of a reduplicative prefix
past initial V avoids duplicating a violation of the constraint ONSET, whereas infixation of fixedsegmentism affixes has no such effect. See M&P 1993ab and cf. Downing 1994,1995ab,1996.
Murut also has some special restrictions on syllabic well-formedness, although these are not unusual.
In Timugon Murut, the only licit intersyllabic clusters are sequences of homorganic nasal and stop. The conditions
on syllabic structure, and their interaction with the Geminate Constraint in Itô’s (1986) proposal, are identical
to those of Orokaiva in section 2.1. As in Orokaiva, it follows that that no syllable-final consonant can be copied.
Witness the derived representations:
(70)
a.
)+
) ) )
b.
)
) ) )
c.
)
) )
indimo (in)dimo
ulampoy (u)lampoy
dondo dondo
=indidimo
=ulalampoy
=dodondo
Even when a homorganic consonant follows, as in (70c), the nasal cannot be associated because the structural
requirements of Itô’s explanation are not met. The first nd sequence in *dondondo does not have the required
shared feature structure, since it is composed partly of copied melody and partly of original. We could, of course,
stipulate this result, but it is obviously far more attractive to derive it from independently needed properties of
this language and of phonological theory.
What has to this point remained the most recalcitrant system of infixing reduplication — that of the
Hokan language Washo — also submits to analysis under our assumptions. The fundamental logic of Washo
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
41
reduplication is that the reduplicated form is longer than the unreduplicated one by exactly one monomoraic or
core syllable:
(71)
Washo (Jacobsen 1964, Winter 1970, Broselow & McCarthy 1983–4)
Singular
Plural
daa
daaa
‘mother’s brother’
damal
damamal
‘to hear’
maagu
magoogo
‘sister’s child’
c’iige
c’igeege
‘to scratch’
duwe
duwewe
‘to try to’
bali
balali
‘to shoot’
šemug
šemumug
‘brother’s child (of woman)’
ic’iš
c’ic’iš
‘black’
ac’im
c’ac’im
‘green, yellow’
emc’i
c’imc’i
‘to wake up’
ipc’ib
c’ipc’ib
‘perfect’
saksag
sasaksag
‘father’s father’s brother’
nent’uš
net’unt’uš
‘to be an old woman’
mokgo
mogokgo
‘shoe’
The puzzle presented by Washo is the precise nature of the reduplicative affix. Essentially following the
analysis of Winter (1970), Broselow and McCarthy (1983–4) identify the reduplicative affix as VCV, inserted
after the initial consonant. Association of melody with affix is contrived so that the first V is associated with the
first vocalic melody of the stem, the second V with the second vowel, and the C with the intervocalic consonant
or the second consonant of any medial cluster. A resulting form like c’-ige-iige then undergoes coalescence of
the medial vowel sequence, yielding a derived representation with the length of the second vowel and and quality
of the first or second, depending on their exact featural makeup. (Compare c’igeege in which the first vowel of
the derived sequence wins with balali in which the second prevails.) Finally, initial unstressed vowels are
deleted, yielding forms like c’ac’im from intermediate ac’i-ac’im.
The reduplicative affix VCV is obviously not straightforwardly discoverable from the alternations —
as we noted, the difference between singular and plural is simply that the latter is longer by one monomoraic
syllable (modulo initial vowel deletion) than the former. Instead, we assert that the immediate result of
reduplication is as follows:
(72)
)+
) )
µ
µµ µ
c’ige (c’)i ge
=c’igeege
In other words, the affixal skeleton is responsible only for the copying of the first CV sequence of the base. The
medial consonant is copied via the Onset Rule, and the second vowel of the copy is linked to the base syllable
as well. It is this latter move that is responsible for the array of Washo vowel coalescence phenomena.
There are two arguments for this approach to vowel coalescence. First, it accounts for the otherwise
unexplained preservation of the length of the vowel under coalescence. This is an otherwise abnormal result of
coalescence cross-linguistically, which typically yields vowels that are long or short uniformly in any given
language. Second, it accounts as no other analysis can for the identical facts of coalescence elsewhere in the
42
McCarthy & Prince
language. Washo has two types of morpheme-final vowels. The majority show up unchanged in all contexts, only
inducing an intrusive y before following vowels (Jacobsen 1964:260):
(73)
/aadu/
aadu
aaduya
aadulu
‘hand’
‘hand’
‘in hand’ (cf. a×ala ‘in house’)
‘with hand’
But some apparent morpheme-final vowels have a quite different distribution, impossible in VC___CV or ___#,
coalescing with a following vowel, and emerging overtly only when in the middle of an unsyllabifiable cluster
(Jacobsen 1964:286, 296):
(74)
/ge/
gasaw
gelšim
gešim
geege
gebeyu
‘Imperative’
‘laugh!’
‘sleep!’
‘sing!’
‘grind!’
‘pay him!’
<
<
<
<
<
asaw
elšim
išm)
iige40
beyu
The coalescence effects seen with the morpheme-final vowels of this type are identical to those in internal
reduplication — length of the second vowel is preserved, and likewise the quality of one or the other prevails
depending on the relationship between them (cf. huubii from /hu+iibi+i/, where /hu/ also contains this type of
morpheme-final vowel). The emergence of a full independent vowel seen in the final example is obviously
conditioned by syllable structure; it respects Washo’s avoidance of tautosyllabic clusters.
Our intepretation of this phenomenon is that a morpheme like /ge/ is represented lexically with no
prosodic structure.41 It therefore parasitizes its environment by linking both its consonant and vowel to any
following syllable if possible — that is, just in case the following syllable is vowel-initial. This circumstance, in
which two different vowels are linked to a single syllable, will uniquely trigger the Washo coalescence rule. In
word-final or preconsonantal position, no such move is possible. Cases like gebeyu, where the prosodically empty
morpheme shows up as an independent syllable, will be the result of an epenthesis (that is, syllable-creation) rule
that the language needs in any case.
The advantages of this approach are several. First, it provides a natural account within the overall theory
of the distinction between coalescing and noncoalescing morpheme-final vowels, since only the former are not
provided lexically with prosodic structure. Second, it explains why the first vowel in coalescence is irrelevant to
determining the length of the resulting vowel, since coalescing vowels have no mora to contribute. Third, it
accounts for the otherwise inexplicable absence of a length distinction in coalescing vowels, despite the fact that
all true vowels of the language have both long and short counterparts. Fourth, it accounts for the defective surface
distribution of coalescing vowels — they can only occur independently where epenthesis would apply anyway,
and they can only occur covertly where a vowel-initial syllable permits them to coalesce. Fifth, it accounts for the
defective underlying distribution of coalescing vowels as well — they are restricted to morpheme-final position,
even in longer morphemes, presumably because, in conjunction with the usual constraints on extraprosodicity,
unsyllabified elements must be peripheral.
All properties of the medial vowels in reduplicated forms then fall out from this independently-motivated
conception of the coalescence phenomenon. Only a single issue in Washo remains. Since we have identified this
system of reduplication as prefixation of a monomoraic or core syllable, the direction of association must be left
40
41
Constructed on the basis of the discussion in Jacobsen (1964: 294).
A morpheme like c’ilu ‘descriptive of hips’, which ends in a vowel that undergoes coalesence, will have lexical prosodic structure only
for its first vowel.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
43
to right. This accounts correctly for the initial CV sequence, which exhausts the monomoraic affix, and the
independently needed coalescence rule accounts for the linking of the second vowel melody to the former initial
syllable. But there is no obvious account for the behavior of the Onset Rule, which normally associates the second
consonant of a medial cluster (as in ešiwši, *ewiwši). We could consider this to be the normal behavior of
such applications of the Onset Rule, distinguishing it from the left-to-right association of melody with affix. As
it happens, none of our other cases of Onset Rule application provide the critical conjunction of circumstances
that Washo has, so this solution is necessarily somewhat speculative.
Washo Reduplication
De Haas 1988 presents a strong argument against an analysis of Washo based on vowel
coalescence: in all other known cases, coalescence yields a long vowel (or a short one, in
languages where long vowels are prohibited); it never preserves a length contrast the way Washo
does. (Sanskrit, for example. is typical in this respect.) Urbanczyk 1992 develops an account of
Washo based on negative circumscription of a mora that does not require coalescence, and so
escapes this criticism. For discussion of coalescence within Optimality Theory, see Lamontagne
& Rice 1995, McCarthy 1996.
Suffix as Infix
In view of the general scarcity of suffixing reduplications, it is not surprising that suffixing reduplication
in the infixing mode is also poorly attested. We know of two examples, both exhibiting exactly the same pattern:
(75)
Suffixing Infixing Reduplication
Chumash (Applegate 1976: 275)
Lexical
walalaq’
oxoxon
halala
hamama
oxyoyon
muc’uc’u
mixixin
Korean (Kim 1984)
culuk
cululuk
asak
asasak
t’ali×
t’alili×
holok
hololok
allok
allolok
‘lichen’
‘cough’
‘to quarrel’
‘so much!’
‘to be crazy’
‘small bead’ (cf. muc’u ‘small’)
‘to be hungry’ (cf. mixin ‘id.’)
‘dribbling’
‘with a crunch’
‘ting-ting’
‘sipping’
‘mottled’
With the final consonant melody rendered extrasyllabic in our sense, suffixation of a ) yields the desired pattern.
Korean Reduplication
For further discussion of Korean reduplication, see Jun 1994 and Lee & Davis 1993.
44
McCarthy & Prince
INFIXATION VIA AFFIXATION TO A PROSODIC CONSTITUENT
The vocabulary of prosodic constituents — word, foot, and syllable — provides not only a theory of
possible reduplicative affixes but also a theory of possible bases of reduplication. It is, of course, most normal
for reduplicative or other affixes to adjoin to words, characterized either phonologically or morphologically at
some stage of the derivation. There are, however, some cases where one of the lower-order prosodic constituents
functions as the base for reduplicative affixation (Broselow and McCarthy 1983–4). We can detect such
phenomena most directly only when the conjunction of affix position (prefix vs. suffix) and location of the base
constituent is such that the reduplication appears to take place root internally. Such is in fact the case in the
following examples:
(76)
Infixation via Affixation to Prosodic Constituent
a. Prefixation of core/monomoraic syllable to (main-stress) foot
Chamorro Continuative (B&M 56)
saga
sasaga
‘stay’
egga
eegga
‘watch’
hugando
hugagando
‘play’
Samoan Plural (B&M 30)
taa
tataa
‘strike’
nofo
nonofo
‘sit’
alofa
alolofa
‘love’
b. Prefixation of bimoraic syllable to final syllable
Afar Intensive (Bliese 1981:127)
usuul
usussuul
‘laugh’
biyaak
biyayyaak
‘hurt’
idigil
idigiggil
‘break’
amm
amamm
‘throw’
ess
essess
‘take out’
c. Prefixation of core/monomoraic syllable to final syllable
Chamorro Intensifying (B&M 56)42
dankolo
dankololo
‘big’
bunita
bunitata
‘pretty’
metgot
metgogot
‘strong’
ñala×
ñalala×
‘hungry’
The typological possibilities afforded by the proposals we have made are obviously not exhausted by
this list, but the coverage is nevertheless respectable for a domain involving an indisputably rare phenomenon.
We exhibit cases in which both foot and syllable are the base (examples of word as base are provided elsewhere),
and we show various modifications of the simple syllable appearing as the affix. The only conspicuous lacuna
is the absence of languages with suffixation to a prosodic constituent. This is not astonishing; affixation to a foot
— the prevalent pattern — must arise historically through a reanalysis in which affixation to short words (those
one foot long) is extended to less common long words. Thus, the relative scarcity of suffixing reduplication in
general produces this apparent skewing.
The fundamental premises of affixation to a prosodic constituent are no different from those of prefixing
and suffixing reduplication. The specified base of the reduplication — characterized prosodically rather than
42
There is an alternative analysis of Chamorro in our terms — final C EM and suffix ).
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
45
grammatically — contributes the phonemic melody material available for association and also is the unit with
respect to which the linear order relations of the affix are defined.
Infixation by Affixation to Prosodic Constituent
This is analyzed as positive prosodic circumscription in M&P 1990a, and additional cases are
presented. An account in terms of subcategorizational Alignment constraints, much closer to the
analysis here and in Broselow & McCarthy 1983-4, is given in M&P 1993ab. On Samoan, see
Levelt 1990. On the role of circumscription within OT, from Maori, see de Lacy 1996.
2.4
Templatic Morphology
The first arguments for morphological skeleta came from consideration of a purely templatic system, the
derivational categories of the Arabic verb. Such systems are characterized by nearly complete independence of
melody from skeleton throughout the morphology. In addition to this pure templatic morphology, there are at least
two other types of templates which typically occur in languages without pervasive reliance on this mode of word
formation. Templatic truncation phenomena involve massive reductions in word size under specific morphological
requirements. In another class of cases, morphemes of a particular type are required to meet a templatic shape,
even though no direct support from alternations supports active enforcement of this requirement.
TRUNCATION
What we are calling here truncation is not that; it is specification of a template to which the melody
(possibly enhanced with some prosodic structure along the lines developed in 4) is directly associated. Words are
not being chopped to fit by leaving off prosodic units. Instead, starting at some designated point, the melodic
elements of a word are associated with a template, providing a nearly universal analogue to the morphological
resources of Semitic.
Most commonly in systems of vocative or nickname formation, but occasionally elsewhere, languages
enforce a foot/minimal word template, resulting in systematic patterns of shortening input words:
(77)
Truncation
Yapese (Jensen 1977: 101, 114) Wmin
Full Noun
Vocative
luag
lu
bayaad
bay
ma×fl
ma×
46
McCarthy & Prince
Central Alaskan Yup’ik Eskimo (Woodbury 1985) F (= Wmin ?)
Full Noun
Proximal Vocative
A×ukaSnaq
A× ~ A×uk
NupiSak
Nup ~ Nupix/Nupik
CupFl:aq
Cup ~ CupFl
A×ivSan
A×if
Kalixtuq
Kal ~ Kalik
qFtunSaq
QFt ~ QFtun
MaSwluq
MaXw
ASnaSayaq
ASFn
NF×qFXalSia
NF×Fq
QakfaSalSia
Qak ~ QakFf
AkiuSalSia
Akiuk
Afar (Bliese 1981: 97, 267) )+am (= Wmin?)
Frequentative
Gloss
tokam tokmeeni
‘you (pl.) ate’
yuam yurufeh
‘he rested’
aram argauk
‘he cut’
tifam tifi
‘it dripped’
tubam tubleeni
‘you (pl.) saw’
yamam yamaateeni
‘they come’
Japanese (Poser 1984a, 1984b:42ff, Itô p.c.) F(F)
a. Hypocoristics
Name
midori
Hypocoristic
mii+tyaN
mit+tyaN
mido+tyaN
siNzaburoo
siN+tyaN
siNzabu+tyaN
wasaburoo
waa+tyaN
wasa+tyaN
sabu+tyaN
wasaburo+tyaN
b. ICU Student Argot
iN kuri
zyene edo
iN liN
fure maN
iN toro
‘Introduction to Christianity’
‘General Education’
‘Introduction to Linguistics’
‘freshman’
‘introduction’
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
c. Abbreviations
paasonaru koNpyuutaa
waado purosessaa
imeezyi tyenzyi
paNtii sutokkiNgu
konekusyon
sutoraiki
zeminaaru
irasutoreesyon
47
paso koN
waa puro
ime tyeN
paN suto
kone
suto
zemi
irasuto
We have presented a rich variety of examples because this type of templatic morphology has been
insufficiently recognized in the literature. Let us take each case in turn.
More on Truncation
The literature on truncation is much more extensive now than it was in 1986. It includes a
comprehensive survey by Weeda 1992, new theoretical developments in Benua 1996, and
treatments of particular languages, including (and this is only a sample) Arabic (McCarthy and
Prince 1990b), Swedish (Morris 1989), French (Plénat 1984, Steriade 1988, Scullen 1993),
Spanish (de Reuse n.d., Crowhurst 1992a), Nootka (Stonham 1990), Choctaw (Martin 1989,
Lombardi & McCarthy 1991), Papago (Hill & Zepeda 1992), Catalan (Cabré & Kenstowicz 1995),
Rotuman (McCarthy 1995), and Japanese (Tateishi 1989, Mester 1990, Poser 1990, Itô 1991,
Itô and Mester 1992, 1996, Perlmutter 1992, Itô, Kitagawa, & Mester 1996).
In Yapese, the smallest licit independent word is a CVC syllable, and this clearly corresponds to the
output of vocative truncation. The monosyllabism requirement is not surprising, but the demand that the word
be consonant-final is. Since the language tolerates long vowels, it is somewhat surprising that CVV
monosyllables are not permitted. We have already seen — for instance, in the case of Kamaiurá — that languages
may place special requirements on word-final consonants. Such requirements, in our theory, are expressed purely
melodically, via the mechanisms of association. That is, a nonvocalic melody element must be linked to the final
mora in the minimal (bimoraic syllable) word. We show in section 2.4.3.1 that similar rules of association are
needed even in a theory that has C/V skeleta and that ours is in fact a superior theory of their properties. Yapese
extracts the entire melody, then associates with the ) template from left to right.
The Yup’ik case, which is closely analyzed in prosodic terms by Woodbury (1985), is a clear (and
unique) example in which the morphology must make reference to a quantity-sensitive iambic foot. The patterns
assumed by proximal vocatives correspond exactly to the complex requirements that the Yup’ik stress system
must in any case place on this foot type — it is monosyllabic or disyllabic, it contains at least two morae, it must
end in a consonant, and bimoraic syllables are permissible only on the right. In contrast with the complete
generality of the template, the mode of association is somewhat idiosyncratic. Normally, association begins at
the left edge, but occasionally it starts inside the word. Pairs like Cup/CupFl show that both types of iambic
quantity-sensitive feet can be associated with a single melody. The form Akiuk exhibits a compressed (that is,
monomoraic) diphthong. This compression also fulfills the requirements of the language — full diphthongs are
impossible in closed syllables.
In Afar, a minimal word (monosyllabic like Yapese) template is associated with the base melody from
the left. The gerundial suffix am is attached to this truncated form, and the result is used as an independent word
in a paronomastic construction. This is not reduplication of the usual sort, since it is evidently postsyntactic, but
is rather a mechanism for creating cognate gerunds. (Afar also has the option of using an untruncated form in the
same way.)
48
McCarthy & Prince
Finally, the case of Japanese is the richest we have yet seen. Poser (1982) carefully demonstrates that
the bases of hypocoristics with suffixed tyaN are composed of one or two bimoraic units. As in Eskimo, the mode
of association is somewhat idiosyncratic, with most speakers confining their choices to one of the options
reported. Nevertheless, it is clear that the requirement of bimoraicity can be fulfilled in several different ways
without regard to syllabic structure.
Poser reports impressionistic evidence of a bimoraic rhythmic unit in Japanese which he calls the foot.
The other data from this language, coming from two sorts of abbreviations, confirm this result. With very few
exceptions, such abbreviations are also constructed from one or two bimoraic units. Even more compelling
evidence for this conclusion emerges in a secret language of entertainers described by Tateishi (1985 [1989]).
This secret language performs the following permutations:
(78)
Japanese Secret Language
Base Form
maneezyaa
koohii
ippatu
oNna
mesi
hi
Secret Form
zyaamane
hiikoo
patuiti
naaoN
siimee
iihii
‘manager’
‘coffee’
‘a shot’
‘woman’
‘meal’
‘fire’
As Tateishi observes, the secret language forms are all composed of exactly two bimoraic feet, regardless of the
mora count of the original.43 The skeleton of the disguised form, then, is simply FF, while the mode of association
is obviously one of great complexity.
Zuuja-go
A comprehensive analysis of this secret language, with significant theoretical development, is
presented in Itô, Kitagawa & Mester 1996.
The English truncated words are a type of templatic morphology based on the composition of minimality:
min(min(Wd)). The minimal phonological word of English is the foot; it functions in the formation of echo words,
as we show below. In truncated words, the template is the minimal foot: min(F) = min(min(Wd)) = ). English
and Yup’ik truncations provide a nice contrast: the former is min(min(Wd)), therefore ); the latter is min(Wd),
therefore F.
English has a large number of truncated words, which Jespersen (1928–) calls ‘stump-words’, that may
appear alone or with an affix like -er(s), -ie, -y, or -o. A few examples of the many hundreds of these appear in
(79):
(79)
English “stump-words’‘
rugby
pregnant
Bolshevik
Jonathan
rugger
preggers
Bolshy
Jono
Similar data, although without a regular morphological relationship, are found in English affective verbs in -er,
like patter, quaver, flicker, glister.
43
There is one exception to this: trimoraic trisyllabic words like piyano become yanopi. Although there is no reason to doubt the accuracy
of this observation, it is interesting that Osamu Fujimura, a native speaker of Japanese but not of this secret language, strongly felt that this
must be a mistake.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
49
The base of the truncated form, minus any affix, is invariably a single stressed syllable (=
min(min(Wd))).44. In no case does the truncated form contain more than one syllable (modulo the affixes), nor
does it ever retain any consonants which could not be assigned to a minimal word template. Thus, rugby or
pregnant cannot form the phonotactically permissible but morphologically ungrammatical affectives *rugber
or *pregners. Furthermore, the assignment of melodic material to the ) (= Wmin) template is indifferent to the
syllabification of the base, so the affective is Bolshy in spite of the fact that Bolsh does not constitute a syllable
of Bolshevik.45 In other words, the formation of stump-words is not an operation of truncation, but rather of
assignment of melody to template. This is true cross-linguistically: truncation the phenomenon is not truncation
the operation, but rather is association with a specified template. Finally, we note that the affixes appearing on
these affective words all are stress-neutral, as we would expect of a morphological pattern that requires a form
in the shape of a phonological word.
The mere volume of the forms and the ease with which they are coined suggests that this process is quite
productive in English. Nevertheless, there are a few idiosyncrasies. Nicknames, but not other affective words, are
subject to various neotenous segmental changes, like Bobbie from Robert. Association of the melody is normally
from the left edge in, as we would expect from unmarked left-to-right association, but in a very few forms
association begins at the stressed syllable: tater (<potato), tec (<detective).
A few instances of truncation invoke a prosodic constituent demonstrably different from the foot or
minimal word. Both known cases involve taking reduction of the weakly-stressed member of a compound to its
limiting case: truncation.
Truncation in Zuni (Newman 1965) applies to the left-branches of all compounds (80a) and to stems
before certain suffixes (80b). The result of truncation retains only the initial (that is, stressed) consonant and
vowel of the original:
(80)
Zuni Truncation
a.
tukni
melika
melika
pagu
b.
kw’alasi
suski
kuku
tu-mokwkw’anne
me-kw’iššo
me-oše
pa-lokk’a-akwe
Familiar
kw’a-mme
su-mme
ku-mme
‘toe-shoe = stocking’
‘Non-Indian-negro = black man’
‘Non-Indian-be:hungry = hobo’
‘Navajo-be:gray=Ramah Navajo’
‘Crow’
‘coyote’
‘father’s sister’
It is evident that the truncation process is based on a monomoraic syllable template with LR association
of the entire melody.46 Can this template be derived from some higher-level unit of the language? Since words
invariably have initial stress, there is no appeal to foot here. Nonfunction words appear to always be at least
bimoraic, so the minimal word is also not in play. The Zuni monomoraic or core syllable does, however,
correspond to the minimal root, and this seems a plausible constraint to place on the members of compounds or
suffixed words.
Like Zuni, Madurese (Stevens 1968; Weeda 1986 [1987]) also displays truncation in connection with
compounding, and also like Zuni Madurese truncation is anchored on the stressed syllable, in this case final.
44
The syllabic character of stump-words has been independently noted by David Nash and Jane Simpson in unpublished work.
45
Those with strong intuitions of ambisyllabicity may wish to contemplate examples with a different stress pattern. For example, Altoona
would necessarily truncate as Altie, *Allie.
46
Newman (1965) has initial clusters in his analysis of Zuni, but these are restricted to C. This idea has little merit and causes many
problems, so I have reinterpreted these as glottalized consonants.
50
McCarthy & Prince
Truncation applies to the left branch of certain compounds (81a), to the left branch of one type of root
reduplication (81b), and spontaneously in certain words (81c):
(81)
Madurese Truncation
a.
usap
uri×
tuzhu
pasar
b.
bit
buwa-an
maen-an
×astan-e
estre
chapphluk-an
c.
setto×
duwa
enghi
uri×
sap-lati
ri×-tua
zhu-Fnpul
sar-suri
bit-abit
wa-buwa-an
en-maen-an
tan-×astan-e
tre-estre
phluk-chapphluk-an
to×
wa
ghi
ri×
‘handkerchief’ (‘wipe’+’lip’)
‘parents’ (‘person’ + ‘old’)
‘pinky’ (‘finger’ + ‘pinky’)
‘afternoon market’ (‘market’+ ‘afternoon’)
‘finally’
‘fruits’
‘toys’
‘to hold’
‘wives’
‘a noise’
‘one’
‘two’
‘yes’
‘person’
The template here is a simple syllable ). As Weeda (1986 [1987]) notes, the introduction of tautosyllabic
clusters into Madurese (‘wives’ and ‘a noise’ in 81b) is paralleled by a comparable development in the truncated
forms. The ) template explains this; a segmental skeleton cannot. As in the other cases, the operation is not true
truncation. Rather, association to the ) template begins with the left edge of the root-final syllable and proceeds
until the phonemic melody is filled or the independently characterized positions in ) are exhausted.
The only roots of Madurese shorter than two syllables are function words (Stevens 1968:51), so neither
minimal root nor a fortiori minimal word can be appealed to. The template here, then, is independent of higherlevel prosodic units, and so rests directly on the syllable.
TEMPLATIC W ELL-FORMEDNESS REQUIREMENTS
Languages frequently place templatic requirements on morphemes or derived forms of a particular class.
These requirements are not actively enforced in the sense of generating alternations the way truncation does, but
they exist passively in that all members of the class must conform to them. The by now familiar minimal word
or root is a clear instance of this type, but in most cases the template is unmodified by the minimality predicate.
For example, Indo-European root monosyllabism is expressed by a ) template which demands that all roots be
exactly one syllable long, neither more nor less. A substantial subset of the English affective vocabulary involves
a similar rule.
English echo words involve total reduplication of a (typically) nonoccurring base with some unsystematic
changes in vocalism or consonantism: jingle-jangle, helter-skelter. Although this is not a regular or orderly
domain of English morphology, it is nevertheless a very common and possibly productive one; Jespersen (1928–)
observes that it is quite frequent, and Thun (1963) has collected over 4000 different examples from throughout
the history of the language.
Inspection of these extensive data yields the following observations, which hold virtually without
exception. An English echo word may be a word-level compound of two stressed syllables (hob-nob), of two
disyllabic words of the form stressed-unstressed (fiddle-faddle), or of two trisyllabic words stressed-unstressedunstressed (higgledy-piggledy). Occasionally the two halves of the compound differ in canonical pattern, but
always within this range of possibilities: plug-ugly, pitter-pat, kitty-cat.
In sum, an echo word must be a compound of exactly two metrical feet, each of which constitutes a
separate phonological word. This is equivalent to regarding them as compounds of two minimal words Wmin.
English metrical feet are composed of a stressed syllable followed by zero, one, or two unstressed syllables (the
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
51
last derived by syllabic extrametricality at both junctural positions in the compound), which corresponds to the
distribution of reduplicated echo words.
This formal requirement not only generates the occurring types but, naturally enough, excludes many
nonoccurring ones. Echo words are impossible with an unstressed syllable preceding the foot (*banana-cabana)
or with more than one foot (*phalarope-kalatrope).
Another example of this sort is provided by a class of Jamaican Creole affective words called iteratives.
According to DeCamp (1974; see also McCarthy 1983b), iteratives are reduplicated words built on a
monosyllabic template, to which is optionally added an unspecified (and therefore harmonizing) core syllable to
indicate greater intensity or a specified i to mark jocosity:
(82)
Jamaican Iteratives
mak-mak
gra×-gra×
maka-maka
priti-priti
‘muddy’
‘firewood’
‘muddy (intensive)’
‘very pretty’
Potential iteratives not conforming to the monosyllabic pattern are excluded (e.g., kyerfl-kyerfl). DeCamp not
only collected a very large set of actual iteratives, but also went so far as to experimentally confirm the boundaries
of the phenomenon by testing hypothetical iteratives with five informants. Thus, the generalization about
canonical pattern is confirmed by quite careful scrutiny.
TRUE TEMPLATIC MORPHOLOGY
The final case is represented by the classic templatic systems, those in which the template directly
expresses the morphological possibilities of the language. We discuss here Semitic and the templatic formation
of the habilitative in Cupeño.
Semitic
Proposals to reduce the Classical Arabic templatic system to prosodic structure have been pursued before
(Levin 1983, Lowenstamm and Kaye 1986, Yip 1983), but always with a skeletal tier of segment-sized units. We
will show that the segmental skeleton is entirely superfluous, and we will develop several novel arguments for
a prosodic characterization of this system, considering fully the consequences of our moves.
The basic data, presented in the CV notation, are contained in (83):
(83)
Arabic Templates
a. Verbal Templates
CVCVC
CVVCVC
CCVCCVC
b. Nominal Templates
CCVC
CVCVC
CCVVC
CVCCVC
CVCCVVC
CVVCVVC
CCVVCVVC
CVCCVC
CCVCVC
CCVVCVC
CVCC
CVVCVC
CVCVVC
CVVCVC
CCVCCVVC
CCVCVVC
These skeletal patterns exhaust those assumed by canonical, unaffixed nouns and verbs. There also exist
noncanonical nouns — these generally fail to participate in Arabic root-and-pattern morphology or behave
somewhat idiosyncratically when they do.
52
McCarthy & Prince
More on Semitic Morphology
An account of Arabic nominal and verbal template-forms that aims at thoroughness is presented
in M&P 1990b. It is further refined in Prince 1991, McCarthy 1993, and M&P 1991b. (The account
of Arabic plural templates in M&P 1990a should also be mentioned.) Other relevant literature on
the morphology of various Afro-Asiatic languages includes Bat-El 1989, 1992; Dell & Elmedlaoui
1992; Gafos 1996; Guerssel & Lowenstamm 1994; Hayward 1988; Hoberman 1988; Inkelas 1990;
Lowenstamm & Kaye 1986; Moore 1989; Prince 1991; Sharvit 1994; Yip 1988.
All indications in Arabic point toward a split between monomoraic CV syllables and bimoraic CVC and
CVV syllables. The stress system divides up light and heavy syllables along these lines, and a corresponding
distinction is made in the otherwise unrelated system for scanning verse.
We must also recognize a place for peripheral unsyllabifiable consonants. Such consonants are a
consequence of peripheral extraprosodic syllables in the skeleton. These syllables, we stipulate, have exactly one
consonant melody associated with them; they will develop vocalism only when they cease to be peripheral and
therefore extraprosodic (or, ultimately, in the postlexical phonology). This vocalism is supplied either by affixes
or ultimately by epenthesis. We will say a bit more about these syllables below.
A syllabic characterization of the Arabic skeleta, using the moraic distinction between light and heavy
syllables and extraprosodicity defined in this way, is as follows:
(84)
Arabic Skeleta Prosodically
Syllabic Skeleton
Corresponding CV-skeleta
) () )
CVCC
() ) )
CCVC
() ) ) ( ) )
CCVVC
)µ )
CVCVC
)µµ )
CVVCVC, CVCCVC
) µ ) () )
CVCVVC
)µµ ) ())
CVVCVVC, CVCCVVC
() ) ) µ )
CCVCVC
()) )µµ )
CCVCCVC, CCVVCVC
()) )µµ ) ())
CCVCCVVC, CCVVCVVC
() ) ) µ ) ( ) )
CCVCVVC
It should be noted that the weight (mora count) of the last metrical syllable in each skeleton is unmarked because
these syllables are invariably heavy. We will bring this up again below.
There are many results that follow from this reanalysis of the Arabic skeletal system.
1. This approach permits us to require that Arabic skeleta in general are subject to a minimum
size of one metrical syllable and a maximum size of two metrical syllables. The former is not
somehow indendently necessary — there are a few noncanonical nouns with no syllables, like
bn. The latter follows from general conditions of locality imposed on counting rules, as
described in section 1. Verbs are subject to more stringent constraints: they are minimally and
maximally disyllabic.
2. We can extract out the generalization, already noted, that the last metrical syllable is always
bimoraic. That is, ) µµ /___].
3. Unsyllabifiable sequences are limited to peripheral position, a consequence of the usual
constraints on the distribution of extraprosodicity. The corollary to this is that, modulo the
extraprosodic elements, Arabic skeleta are well-formed sequences of syllables. This is not a
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
53
trivial result; since Classical Arabic and most dialects have epenthesis rules that would
regularize even template-internal unsyllabifiable strings, the underlying well-formedness of the
template demands an explanation. Moreover, as we noted in section 1, medial unsyllabifiable
segments could be used to cook the results of left-to-right association.
4. Subject to these other three requirements, the occurring skeleta are a result of the free
concatenation of the representational vocabulary (heavy and light syllables, extraprosodicity).
There are no gaps: all and only the elementary patterns generated by free concatenation function
in the morphological system.
5. There is no true skeletal contrast between CVC and CVV syllables; the apparent differences
are accounted for by melody-to-skeleton association rules of great generality. To prove this, we
distinguish two cases. The last metrical syllable, always heavy, is also always CVC if it is truly
the last syllable. If it is followed by an extraprosodic syllable, it is CVV except in when it is also
initial (that is, in the template [) ())]). The language simply displays no contrast in these cases;
the nouns that would require such a contrast (gaaz ‘gas’, usquff ‘bishop’) are transparent
loans, extraordinarily rare, and thoroughly noncanonical. Thus, it is a simple matter to describe
the contexts in which a CVC syllable occurs; these contexts trigger a rule of root-to-skeleton
association, linking the rightmost root consonant with some unit of the skeleton.
Likewise, there is no CVC/CVV contrast in other syllables (those marked as bimoraic
in (84)). The distinction between CVV and CVC syllables internally is accounted for by rules
of somewhat lesser generality. In particular, the verbal skeleton CVCCVC is not freely available
— it is the result of an association rule deriving medial gemination (kattab) that any analysis
requires, or it arises under the force of the requirement that all root consonants be associated
with some prosodic element (as in the quadriliteral verb daHraj).47
6. As we noted in section 1, the apparent restrictions on Arabic skeleta that would follow from
segment counting actually follow from syllable counting and the relatively simple independently
motivated syllable structure of this language.The proof of this claim is the evidence adduced in
McCarthy (1984c) from Modern Hebrew. The richer syllable structure of Modern Hebrew is
reflected in the richer possibilities for disyllabic templatic verbs: išknez ‘make Ashkenazic’,
šlimper ‘make sloppy’, stingref ‘take shorthand’. There is no difference between the cognate
templates in Classical Arabic and Modern Hebrew; the sole difference lies in the licensing of
tautosyllabic consonant clusters in the latter.
7. Arabic consonants linked to extraprosodic syllables do not have the same properties as truly
unsyllabified consonants. In Cairene Arabic, triconsonantal clusters are normally resolved by
epenthesis after the second consonant: VCCiCV. As expected, initial clusters in loans are split
in the equivalent way, so that the epenthetic vowel also lodges in an open syllable: sibirtu
‘spirit’, bilastik ‘plastic’. But initial clusters of words formed on templates (that is, all words
except loans) undergo epenthesis in a way otherwise unprecedented in the language: ijtama9
‘he met’. The posited linking to an initial extraprosodic syllable distinguishes this sort of
epenthesis from the more general phenomenon observed in loans and other clusters.
These arguments lead to a single conclusion: all vestiges of segmental structure, cast as C/V, X, or
whatever, must be removed from the Arabic skeleton.The maximum generality and regularity of skeletal form
is achieved by limiting the descriptive vocabulary to syllables, moraic annotations, and extraprosodicity.
47
A comparable point has been made by Levin (1983) and Broselow (1984).
54
McCarthy & Prince
Cupeño
The Uto-Aztecan language Cupeño requires a foot template to characterize a morphological category
called the habilitative (McCarthy 1984c). Habilitatives are confined to one of the forms shown in (85), where the
shape of the habilitative is determined in part by the shape of the base:
(85)
Cupeño Habilitative
Verb Stem
gál
a.
t Fw
hFlyFp
kFláw
b.
págik
gá×nFw
c.
pínFwFx
xálFyFw
gí
d.
hú
áyu
Habilitative
gáaal
tFFFw
hFlyFFFp
kFláaaw
págiik
gá×nFFw
pínFwFx
xálFyFw
gí
hú
áyu
‘husk’
‘see’
‘hiccup’
‘gather wood’
‘leach acorns’
‘be angry’
‘sing enemy songs’
‘fall’
‘gather’
‘fart’
‘want’
In (85a, b, c), the stressed syllable of the habilitative is followed by two unstressed syllables. In (85a), both of
these posttonic syllables contain V sequences, where V is a copy of the tonic vowel. In (85b), the final syllable
contains a single such V sequence, and V copies the vowel of the immediately posttonic syllable. And in (85c),
there is no copying at all. These three types correspond directly to the position of stress in the original: oxytone,
paroxytone, and proparoxytone respectively. Vowel-final roots like those in (85d) differ from the others — they
have no copying and remain unchanged (modulo phonological insertion of after final stressed vowels) in the
habilitative.
The shape-invariant in the Cupeño habilitative is a trisyllabic, consonant-final foot at the end of the word.
Material preceding the stressed syllable is irrelevant; one may say that it is extraprosodic for the purposes of
habilitative formation. Although trisyllabic feet are not part of the inventory of primitive foot types, since this
trisyllabic foot is necessarily word-final it can be derived from a disyllabic foot plus final syllable
extrametricality. This derived foot constitutes the Cupeño habilitative template:
(86)
F
) ) )
The analysis also requires one rule of melody-to-skeleton association, a rule linking the rightmost consonantal
melody unit to the rightmost syllable of the template. This rules has three partly independent functions in Cupeño.
(1) It is indispensible to the association of the rest of the melody, as we will see. (2) It expresses the
generalization that all templatic habilitatives of the language are consonant-final. (3) It blocks templatic
habilitative formation in vowel final roots like those of (85d).
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
55
With this template and this consonant association rule, we derive some relevant examples as follows:
(87)
a.
Consonant Association
LR 1-1 Association
Spreading
b.
c.
F
F
F
)))
)))
gal
pagik
)) )
pinFwFx
F
F
F
)))
)))
gal
pagik
)) )
pinFwFx
F
F
)))
)))
gaaal
pagiik
F
))
)
pinFwFx
Consonant association first establishes a rightward bound for spreading — it is therefore needed independently
of its role in describing the canonical pattern of Cupeño habilitatives. LR association of the other elements of the
melody establishes a leftward bound of spreading, and then the vowel spreads to satisfy the trisyllabic
requirement of the F skeleton. Cupeño, like many languages, independently requires insertion of to resolve
vowel-initial syllables; application of that rule in (87a, b) derives the correct results.
Other Templatic Morphology
This type of morphology is also found in several Penutian languages — Miwok (Freeland 1951;
Broadbent 1964; Bullock 1990; Crowhurst 1991a, 1992b; Goldsmith 1990; Lamontagne 1989;
Sloan 1991; Smith and Hermans 1982; Smith 1985, 1986), Yokuts (Newman 1944; Archangeli
1983, 1984, 1991; Steriade 1986; Prince 1987, 1991), and Takelma (Sapir 1922; Goodman 1988;
Lee 1991).
3.
Other Issues in Templatic Structure
The consequences of a skeletal theory are not confined to shape-invariant morphology; they reverberate down
through the phonology. We address here four major topics of relevance to the skeleton: the status of syllableinternal structure, prespecification, the analysis of contour segments like affricates, and the relation between a
prosodic skeleton and phonological rules.
56
3.1
McCarthy & Prince
Syllable-internal Structure
The conception of syllable-internal structure that we have assumed so far in our discussion is nearly minimalist
beyond the recognition of the light/heavy distinction encoded as mora count; nothing in our results has hinged
on this aspect of the representations. We can imagine, in complete conformity with the rest of our argument,
several different views of intrasyllabic representation. Consider the following different illustrations of the English
bimoraic syllable plane, that is [plen]:
(88)
a.
b.
c.
)
)
)
µµ
µµ
O µµ C
plen
plen
ple n
The theory in (88a) represents one traditional view of moraic structure. The melodic elements of a syllable are
exhaustively parsed into morae. But since morae are invariably parts of syllables in our conception (that is, there
are no languages without syllables, as a consequence of the prosodic hiearchy), our proposals are equally
compatible with (88b), in which each mora is associated with exactly one melodic element. Here, the moraic
structure designates only weight; it is syllabic structure that provides a locus for melodic units that do not
contribute to weight. In theory (88c), the nonmoraic melodies of the syllable are gathered up into labeled
categories, Onset and Coda, providing a somewhat greater degree of expressive freedom than (88b). Adoption
of theories (88b) or (88c) provides another option: we might choose to assume that they are subject to a general
constraint of Uniformity of Linking, in which no melodic element may bear simultaneous associations to two
distinct levels of prosodic structure (thus ruling out, for example, a consonant which is simultaneously a µ and
a Coda).
Moraic Theory
The literature on this topic that has appeared since 1986 is much too large to cite here, but a few
studies should be particularly mentioned: Hayes 1989, Itô 1989, Sherer 1994, Steriade 1993, Zec
1988, For our own proposals about morafication, see M&P 1988.
The representations in (88a, b, c) are all natural conceptions of intrasyllabic structure in a theory which
gives primacy to the mora. A far less natural one would include a Rhyme constituent as well, conceived as
follows:
(89)
)
R
O µµ C
pl e n
In this case, the fact that reference to the internal structure of syllables in prosodic morphology is restricted to
mora count is completely surprising, since moras are not represented on a tier directly adjacent to ). In all the
other conceptions of intrasyllabic structure, the mora is the most immediately available modification of the
syllable; here it is not. (89), then, is in no way a natural option in our theory.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
57
We are, then, in agreement with Clements and Keyser (1983) in rejection of the Rhyme as the primary
constituent of the syllable and in fact as having any role in syllabic organization. We shall evaluate the evidence
for intrasyllabic structure as it bears on this question.
Intrasyllabic Structure
Some of the points made below can also be found in Davis 1985b, which the author brought to
our attention after this document was first circulated.
The primary argument for a rhyme/onset division comes, of course, from considerations of syllable
weight, particularly as they apply in stress systems. The branching of the rhyme or of a subordinate subsyllabic
constituent, called the nucleus, has been taken in most accounts of stress to record the weight of syllables. From
our perspective, this task is better left to the mora. In particular, as Prince (1984) has demonstrated, the notion
of ‘branching’ that is crucial to such accounts is empirically inadequate. (V. section 3.4 for further discusssion.)
It is profitable to explore the systematic cross-linguistic variation in what causes a syllable to count as
heavy. In Lardil, only long vowels figure in the computation (see section 2.1). In Kwakiutl, as Bach (1975) shows,
long vowels and VR sequences, where R is a sonorant, count as heavy.48 And in Arabic, heavy syllables are just
the class VV and VC (C a consonant or glide). In our interpretation, this brief paradigm explores the range of
what constitutes a single mora; it obviously corresponds to the familiar sonority hierarchy (cf. Prince (1983)).
We note two other consequences. Since monomelodic long vowels must branch from melody to mora level
(simply to be represented, if we impose Uniformity of Linking), long vowels must be heavy. Languages which
make no distinctions of syllable weight are determined at the syllable-mora interface: ) is a µ.
Consider, too, the usual failure of onsets to participate in computations of syllable weight. In onset/rhyme
theories, this result is normally stipulated by some combination of a theory of the onset/rhyme division and an
assumption about projection of rhymes or nuclei in any syllable-weight computation. It falls out trivially from
any of the theories in (88). We must in any case say that onsets are nonmoraic in order to enforce the requirement
that syllables have onsets (cf. Hyman (1985)); all the representations in (88) respect that requirement. It follows,
then, that onsets have no role in determining the weight of syllables.49
The sole other major source of evidence for the rhyme/onset distinction has been the statement of
distributional regularities within the syllable. It is claimed that subsyllabic constituency governs the domain of
intrasyllabic cooccurrence restrictions. Such restrictions fall into two types. On the one hand, they are quite clearly
interpretable as limits on syllable weight, as is the case with the familiar prohibition of CVVC syllables (that is,
all postnuclear elements must be moraic, and syllables contain no more than two morae) or the more baroque
instantiations of this effect in English. Restrictions of this sort fall clearly within the purview of the melody/mora
or the mora/syllable mapping. More general cooccurrence restrictions within the syllable are to be interpreted as
effects of the distribution of sonority within the syllable, a distribution that shows no particular discontinuity at
the putative onset/rhyme division.
There is also a small literature invoking the rhyme as the domain of some phonological process. Effects
of this sort are inherently more compelling as evidence than the statement of static distributional regularities, but
here the argument stumbles for a different reason. Rhyme-domain phonological rules are typically trivially
reformulable without reference to rhymes; we know of no examples where the absence of a rhyme would
substantially complicate the statement of several well-supported regularities within a language which are not
48
49
With Bach (1975), we take Kwakiutl glottalized resonants to be nonsonorant.
Pirãhá (Everett and Everett 1984) is claimed to display onset sensitivity in stress assignment of a very different type: a voiceless onset
attracts the stress. Interestingly, the more obviously structural case of onset sensitivity in Western Aranda (Davis 1985a) comes from a
language in which the requirement that syllables have onsets seems to be nearly suspended.
58
McCarthy & Prince
simply a matter of moraic syllable weight. This is not an unreasonable requirement; it is in fact precisely the test
to which syntactic constituency has been put for more than thirty years.
The only case known to us in which a bare rhyme has been proposed as a reduplicative affix is
Tübatulabal (Voegelin 1935). Levin (1983) has argued that this language marks the telic form of verbs by
prefixation of a rhyme constituent. A fraction of the facts as reported by Voegelin follow:
(90)
Tübatulabal Reduplication
Atelic
tIk
ela
Telic
ItIk
eela
That is, a copy of the vowel in the first syllable is prefixed to the base, for which rhyme reduplication makes
eminent sense, since it eschews copying of the initial consonant. Tübatulabal reduplication also has a number of
complications the analysis of which we postpone: vowel length in the copy is subject to complex permutations,
tautosyllabic diphthongs copy only their first vowel, and nasals copy before voiced stops even though the
language licenses a wide variety of clusters (preventing the sort of analysis given to Southern Paiute in Marantz
(1982)). These factors ultimately require a heavy syllable )µµ prefix, but our concern at this point is with the
failure of the onset to copy.
A superior analysis of this phenomenon is possible which makes no reference to the rhyme. The
transcriptions in (90) are at odds with the description in Voegelin (1935) and Swadesh and Voegelin (1939) in
one major respect (cf. Heath (1981), McCawley (1969), Kenstowicz (1977)): there is an initial glottal stop in
words written as vowel-initial. Thus, these forms should be recast as follows:
(91)
Atelic
tIk
ela
Telic
ItIk
eela
In other words, the minimal reduplication in Tübatulabal is plus a copy of the first vowel.
Tübatulabal Reduplication
In M&P (1994ab), we propose that Tübatulabal reduplication is an instance of emergence of the
unmarked (M&P 1994a) in Optimality Theory. The idea is that structural complexity — that is,
markedness — can be avoided by inexact copying, and is in Tübatulabal, where the initial
consonant is not copied. Nonetheless, ONSET must be obeyed, so the least marked onset, the
glottal stop, emerges in the reduplicant. On the general characteristics of fixed segmentism in
reduplication, see Alderete et al. 1996. For another view of Tübatulabal reduplication, see
Crowhurst 1991ab.
Depending on details of the theory of prespecification, we can suggest two possible accounts of
Tübatulabal that are compatible with our approach and that do not involve reference to the rhyme. On the one
hand, we can treat the affix as a heavy syllable with prespecified :
(92)
)
µµ
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
59
Only the vowel melody of the base links with this to complete the bimoraic syllable; it is unambiguously
linearized as VV.50
Alternatively, in light of the pattern of total reduplication minus an initial consonant evidenced in (102c),
we could appeal to a detachment rule of the sort described in section 3.2 to delink the base melody from the
affixal )c. Then a general default rule of the language, reflecting the requirement that syllables must have onsets,
inserts . In either analysis, the mechanism we appeal to is a conventional syllabic template with prespecification
or its equivalent.
The advantage of prespecification over rhyme affixation is that the former makes a great deal more sense
of the historical basis of this affix. The other Uto-Aztecan languages clearly have some form of initial syllable
(core or bimoraic) reduplication; Tübatulabal with rhyme reduplication seems a strange aberration in this context.
No mechanism known to us would cause a syllable to be reinterpreted as a rhyme.
In the prespecification analysis, contemporary Tübatulabal emerges as a straightforward case of analogic
leveling. The copied initial glottal stop of verbs beginning with this segment metastasized to all verbs of the
language: ela : eela :: tIk : Itik. This sort of analogy is undoubtedly a common if not exclusive source of
apparent prespecified elements. It may have been helped along by some dissimilatory effect, although we can find
no independent evidence for this.
Other literature invoking the onset or rhyme for the statement of morphological regularities is limited
to discussions of language games. There is a class of language games in English and German which seem to
demand these subsyllabic constituents:
(93)
50
Language Games
a. Chicken Language
English
Secret languages are fun
sihilFfi krFthFtlFfFt læ×hæ×lFfæ× gwIjhIjlFfIj FzhFzlFfFz arharlFfar fFnhFnlFfFn
German
Ein gutes Wort findet einen guten Ort
Einheinlefein guhulefu testheslefes worthortlefort finhinlefin dethetlefet eiheilefei
nenhenlefen guhulefu tenhenlefen orhortlefort
b. Goose language
Ich lieb’ dich aus Herzensgrund
Ichicherfich liebieberfieb dichicherfich ausauserfaus Herzerzerfers
grundunderfund
c. Bern Matteänglish
und
inde
‘and’
Bänne
innebe
‘small cart’
Benu
inube
‘Bernard’
Blofer
iferble
‘pencil’
bschummle
immlebsche
‘deceive’
Chrampf
impfchre
‘work’
Gschtöös
isgschte
‘socks’
The long vowel then shortens by an independently motivated rule in the cited examples. This approach involves treating the glottal stop
as underlying, at odds with Voegelin’s (1935) theory of organic and inorganic glottal stop. The organic glottal stops alone are moraic. Heath
(1981) argues vigorously against this idea, it is in any case incoherent, and it is clearly inconsistent with Voegelin’s later analysis in Swadesh
and Voegelin (1939).
60
McCarthy & Prince
d. Pig Latin
dog
trip
street
og-day
ip-tray
eet-stray
language
street
labang guabage
strabeet
e. Abi-dabi
A naive conception of these language games is as follows. In the Chicken and Goose languages, the rhyme is
copied with constant monoconsonantal onsets. In Matteänglisch and Pig Latin, the onset is postposed and a
constant vowel is added. In Abi-dabi and similar languages, a constant VC sequence intervenes between onset
and rhyme.
Language Games
There is a very substantial recent literature on language games. See: Bagemihl 1988, 1989ab,
Bao 1990, Chiang 1992, Duryea 1991, Hammond 1990, Itô, Kitagawa, and Mester 1992, Plénat
1985, Tateishi 1989, Turner & Breen 1984, Vago 1985, Yin 1989, Yip 1982, and the contributions
to Plénat 1991. Bagemihl 1989b and Plénat 1991 also include comprehensive bibliographies.
Cross-linguistically, there is a systematic difference between language game prosody and normal
prosody. In games of the Goose, Chicken, and Abi-dabi types, each syllable of the original form becomes a
separate phonological word of the derived form, with the characteristic prosody of a word. Moreover, in the
Goose and Chicken varieties, each syllable of the derived form is a separate phonological word: cf. gameshames-le-fames, with a succession of syllables that are impossible word internally. (The syllable lF ultimately
cliticizes.) No remnant of the prosody of a single word survives in the language game forms of a polysyllable.
It follows, then, that the language game is operating separately on virtual words like se and cret, the individual
syllables of the real word secret.
Under this conception, Chicken language (or, mutatis mutandis, Goose language) is essentially word
reduplication with a fixed initial consonant, h in one copy and f in the other. The fact that the ‘word’ coincides
with a syllable of the original is something that must be said in any case. There is no rhyme reduplication; the
mechanism is formally indistinguishable from the treatment of echo words in section 3.2. Abi-dabi is a different
story. Paralleling games like Abi-dabi or Alfalfa (stralfeet) in which a fixed VC1 sequence is inserted before the
vowel of each virtual word, there are games in which a fixed C1V sequence is prefixed (or suffixed) to each
virtual word: Texas Spanish rosa kutiro kutisa (Sherzer 1982). The explanation for this distribution is
obviously syllabic: the infixes lodge in the only place where they are licensed syllabically, so only vowel-initial
infixes are possible in post-onset position. This generalization is observed with perfect regularity in all of the
more than 30 relevant language games known to us. It would obviously be a mistake to stipulate the placement
of the infix by making direct reference to the onset. Moreover, this parallels the behavior of real infixes noted in
section 2.3. Reference to subsyllabic constituency in the rule of infixation would simply fail to capture this
regularity, which derives in a straightforward way from independent syllabic well-formedness constraints.
The remaining examples, Pig Latin and Matteänglish, are apparent instances of transposition language
games. We propose that in fact they involve reduplication with extensive prespecification. In all cases known to
us where transposition can be distinguished from reduplication empirically, reduplication is clearly the mode by
which apparent transposition is effected. Yip (1982) demonstrates this compellingly for Chinese secret languages:
prenuclear glides appear in both original and copy, although the standard analysis treats this as an instance of
rhyme transposition. The Japanese secret language in (78) is shown by Tateishi (1985 [1989]) to necessarily
involve copying rather than transposition; the behavior of oNna and hi is otherwise inexplicable. And in Cuna
(Sherzer 1970, 1975), a classic case of a transposing language game, the game form bansab from underlying
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
61
/sappan/ (surface sapan) cannot be accounted for in terms of transposition without giving up the monomelodic
analysis of geminates in this language that is required independently.51 Monomelodic geminates are, however,
entirely compatible with apparent transposition via reduplication. Quite apart from these empirical considerations,
the identification of apparent transposing language games with reduplication yields theoretical fruit: transposition
is unprecedented in real morphology, while reduplication is common. All other operations performed by language
games are paralleled in morphology. By treating transposition as reduplication, we remove the one remaining bar
to full concert between the two domains, with language games clearly parasitizing the morphological resources
provided by linguistic theory.
Thus, although we have no direct evidence that Pig Latin is reduplicative, we are compelled by the logic
of the theory to treat it that way nevertheless. Close inspection of the data reveals its character. The game form
is composed of two phonological words. The prosody clearly reflects this, and the treatment of words like eat,
in Pig Latin phonetically iyt ey or iyD ey, with t allophony characteristic of word junctures, confirms the
observation.52 If the result were a single phonological word, we would expect eat and tea to merge in Pig Latin
as iythey.
It follows, then, that Pig Latin is simply word reduplication, with a result composed of two phonological
words, the second of them minimal (that is, a monosyllabic foot). The characteristic transformations of these two
phonological words — the first lacking initial consonant, the second monosyllabic with fixed ey — are paralleled
closely in the other cases of total reduplication, particularly echo words. The fixing of the initial consonant(s) as
0 is identical to the union of the phenomena in (102a) (table-shmable) and (102c) (amne-samne from samne).
The fixing of the vocalism in the second copy is comparable to Tzeltal total root reduplication (Berlin 1963):
b’ah, b’ahbu ‘strike with hammer’; t’os, t’ostu ‘snap fingers’. The mechanisms for apparent prespecification
in these and comparable cases will be identical to those developed for echo words.
What we have seen, then, is that the need for reference to rhymes and onsets as skeletal units is
nonexistent, given an analysis of the language game data in the light of independently motivated properties of
authentic morphology.
3.2
Prespecification?
Prespecification is the mechanism in McCarthy (1981) and Marantz (1982) by which templatic or reduplicative
morphemes have invariant melodic material. Prespecification may be complete or partial. In the former case, the
prespecified melody blocks association of or, nearly equivalently, takes precedence over any melodic element
contributed by the base. In the latter, one or more features, constituting less than a complete segment, take
precedence over only the same features of the base melody, yielding, for example, a reduplicated vowel that is
invariantly high but whose other properties are determined by the quality of the original.
Prespecification was developed in the context of a skeletal theory with segment-sized units; it therefore
has considerable potential that no language exploits. Consider, for example, the familiar CCVC reduplicative
prefix with prespecification of the second consonant as r:
51
Independent evidence for monomelodic geminates in Cuna comes from several sources. First, a rule changing k to y preconsonantally is
blocked just in case k is geminate. Second, many Cuna geminates result from rules of assimilation, properly formulable only as autosegmental
spreading rules. Third, Cuna simplifies triconsonantal clusters preferentially by degemination. Fourth, Cuna has a number of rules applying
only to geminates. Fifth, Cuna apparently restricts the distribution of syllable-final obstruents except when they are geminate. This conjunction
of properties is inexplicable unless the language has monomelodic geminates.
52
Some Pig Latinists introduce a glide for vowel-initial words only — it wey — exhibiting the kind of context dependence we also see in
echo words. Cf. section 3.2
62
(94)
McCarthy & Prince
r
CCVC
This yields an unattested pattern of reduplication like BRAD-badupi, RAD-adupi, BRAD-bladupi. Similar cases
involving other consonant positions or vowels can be easily multiplied.
Two other problems lurk behind prespecification, although they are not intimately tied to segmental
skeleta. First, there is preunderspecification — the fact, to be demonstrated below, that some cases of melodic
invariants in reduplicative affixes are the result of a lack of copying rather than the blocking of association by
a prespecified element. Second, there is a large class of phenomena — also described below — in which total
reduplication exhibits melodic invariants. Since total (root, stem, word) reduplication cannot in general stipulate
a segmental skeleton even in a theory with such a skeleton, there are no positions with which to link the
prespecified melody.
Consider first the case of Yoruba reduplication. Yoruba prefixes a Ci copy of a verb to derive a gerund:
(95) Reduplication in Yoruba
lo
lilo
du
didu
In Marantz (1982), the reduplicative prefix is analyzed as CV, with V prelinked i. This analysis appears
unexceptionable.
But there are several other interesting points about Yoruba reduplication. First, since verbs are invariably
of the pattern CV (Pulleyblank 1983:120),53 CV reduplication is equivalent to total reduplication. In fact, a CV
reduplicative skeleton merely recapitulates the Yoruba verb canon, a canon that the grammar must state in any
case. Since, as we show below, prespecification is impossible with total reduplication, the sole reason for the
redundant specification of a CV affix in this case is to provide a V slot to which i is linked.
The second point of relevance is that i is the default vowel of Yoruba, as demonstrated by Pulleyblank
1984 [1988]. From Pulleyblank’s results, it appears that there is no evidence for i anywhere in the lexical
phonology — it must be unspecified, to be introduced by a default rule at the end of the lexicon. Prespecification
of a default vowel is plainly impossible: default vowels have no features to preempt those of the copied melody.
A third point involves tone. The prefix in the Yoruba reduplicated verb has invariant high tone — a case
of classic prespecification (but of the whole root, not of a single vowel), since the Yoruba default tone is mid. But
total reduplication54 of nouns, which has a distributive meaning, displays a situation in which the first tone of the
base spreads leftward onto the prefixed copy (high and low tone are marked in the familiar way, mid tone is
unmarked):
(96)
Total Reduplication Yoruba
àgbà
àgbààgbà
òru
òròòru
ègbé
ègbèègbé
osù
osoosù
ogba
ogboogba
alé
alaalé
‘elder’
‘evening’
‘side’
‘month’
‘equal’
‘evening’
53
The second example in (95) is cited in the Yoruba orthography as dun by Marantz (1982). The final n is not a segment (since it cannot bear
tone), but rather the orthographic sign of vowel nasalization. Yoruba does have syllable-final, tone-bearing nasals, but they are not possible
in verb roots.
54
Although this is sometimes described as VCV reduplication, the canonical Yoruba noun is itself VCV, so total reduplication is clearly
what’s involved. The canon is quite forceful; noncanonical nouns reduplicate according to the canon too.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
63
These examples, from Akinlabi (1984: 198), exhaust the tonal patterns of canonical Yoruba nouns. The tone of
the copy is derived by leftward spreading of any tone on the initial syllable of the base, or, if none (in the case
of mid tone), by application of the default rule. In noun reduplication, the tone of the base cannot be copied.
This situation is rather common in reduplication in tonal systems — sometimes the tones copy,
sometimes they are preempted by a specified tone, but often the tones do not copy, and we find instead either
spreading or the application of a default rule.55 The behavior of the vocalism in Yoruba root reduplication shows
that this failure to copy is not limited to tone. Prespecification cannot stop something from copying — it only
overrides things that do. The corollary: you cannot prespecify something as nothing.
The only solution to the Yoruba noun paradox is the one provided by Akinlabi (1984): the tonal tier does
not copy, and therefore it is unavailable for association. We propose that each reduplicative affix may stipulate
that certain tiers cannot copy (making reduplication of all tiers the unmarked case). This has the smell of brute
force; the fragrance improves when we look at nontonal cases.
Tone
On tone in reduplication, see Walsh 1992, Pulleyblank 1994, and Carleton & Myers 1994.
We turn back to the problem of i in Yoruba verb reduplication. If vowel and consonant melodies are
represented on separate tiers, and if this reduplicative affix stipulates against copying the vocalic tier, we obtain
the desired result: the vocalism does not copy, but rather awaits the default rule. This is not the morphologically
governed segregation of vowels and consonants of Semitic. Rather, it is the conception of segregation in Prince
(forthcoming) — just as in traditional phonetics, distinctions in vowels and consonants are expressed by disjoint
predicates. The tongue height and backness of vowels is primitively separate from the articulator and point of
articulation of consonants. It is not so much vowels and consonants that are on separate tiers as the features
distinguishing among vowels and among consonants that are segregated.
Considerations of the fine structure of featural geometry naturally arise in connection with the discussion
of contour segments in section 3.3, but they also are important in the treatment of the partial prespecification
phenomenon.
Fixed i in Yoruba Reduplication
In more recent work, we pursue a somewhat different account of the fixed i in Yoruba that still
preserves the idea that it is a default segment. Falling under the same general analytical rubric
as the fixed in Tübatulabal (see comment box on p. 58), Yoruba i is an emergent unmarked
property of the reduplicant. See M&P 1994ab, 1995b for discussion. On the general characterization of unmarked segmentism in reduplicative affixes, see Alderete et al. 1996.
In a similar way we account for the pattern of apparent partial prespecification in Akan. Although we
have consulted the major source on Akan reduplication, Schachter and Fromkin (1968), our understanding of this
language has considerably benefited from the treatment in Lee (1984).
In subsequent discussion, we shall abstract away from certain irrelevant properties of Akan phonology.
Since Akan vowels harmonize in [tense], and since the distribution of this feature has no bearing on the facts of
reduplication, we shall not indicate the tenseness distinction.
With Lee and Clements & Keyser (1983), we assume that the mora in Akan is the minimal unit that can
exhibit a tonal contrast. Akan reduplication appears to bifurcate into two quite different types depending on the
55
We are grateful to Scott Myers for discussion of the behavior of tone in reduplication.
64
McCarthy & Prince
mora count of the base. The reduplication rule is applied to verbs to make iteratives, and may freely apply to its
own output:
(97) Reduplication in Akan
a. Monomoraic Base
H tone
ba
da×
pam
ka×
do×
se
so
haw
sow
fer [feri]
pir [piri]
sor [sori]
bar [bari]
tun [tunu]
b. Bimoraic Base
LH tone
pai
kasa
nantew
feri
piri
sori
da×
pam
do×
sinsan
pimpam
LH tone
biba
dinna×
pimpam
ki×ka×
dunno×
sise
suso
hihaw
susow
fifer [fiferi]
pipir [piperi]
susor [susori]
bibar [bibari]
tuntun [tuntunu]
‘come’
‘apply to’
‘sew’
‘count’
‘soak’
‘say’
‘light’
‘trouble’
‘catch’
‘swing’
‘go along’
‘pray’
‘cover’
‘forge’
LHLL tone
paipai
kasakasa
nantenantew
feriferi
piripiri
sorisori
danna×
pampam
donno×
sinsansinsan
pimpampimpam
‘break’
‘speak’
‘walk’
‘shun’
‘defend’
‘rise’
‘turn’
‘drive away’
‘walk affectedly’
< san ‘return’
< pam ‘sew’
At first glance, it appears that monomoraic bases generally copy CV, where V is prespecified as [+high] (Marantz
1982), while bimoraic bases copy the entire root. The [+high] prespecification, in Marantz’s conception, overrides
the inherent value of [high] in the melodic copy, yielding essentially a high vowel agreeing in rounding with the
original. We note also that both monomoraic and bimoraic bases exhibit a similar pattern of retention of a final
consonant — it is copied only if it is a nasal.56
56
Verb roots exceed two morae only very rarely, and then only in loans or ideophones. Lee (1984) reports data about one such root patiriw
‘slide’, which copies as patipatiriw and patiripatiriw in different sources. No inferences can be drawn from this fact.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
65
The Fixed High Vowel in Akan Reduplication
The account given in the text now seems to us incorrect. More promising is an approach along
the general lines of fn. 60 (also see Schachter & Fromkin 1968, Hyman 1970, 1972), combined
with the same conception of unmarked structure in the reduplicant as in the boxed notes on
Tübatulabal (p. 58) and Yoruba (p.63). See M&P 1994b,1995b and Padgett & Ní Chiosáin 1995.
Whether a base is monomoraic or bimoraic is not determinable solely by its segmental content. CV and
CV{ ,w} bases are invariably monomoraic, and CV{n,r} bases are monomoraic underlyingly but receive an
epenthetic final vowel (described below) at some post-reduplicative stage of the derivation. Roots containing two
vowels in underlying representation are necessarily bimoraic. Roots containing a single vowel and ending in m
or × are either monomoraic or bimoraic, with clear minimal pairs with respect to this distinction. The distinction
is, however, in no sense opaque, since bimoraic roots invariably have LH tone, while monomoraic ones have H
tone only.57
Let us first consider the pattern of retention or loss of the final consonant in the copy, an effect that spans
the two patterns of reduplication. Other than in word-final position, the only consonants Akan licenses syllablefinally are nasals. This is obviously the explanation for what occurs in reduplication — a nasal consonant is
copied because it is the only licit final consonant in a word-internal syllable.58
This gives a straightforward intepretation of the bimoraic pattern: there is total root reduplication, subject
to the independently motivated syllabic well-formedness condition permitting only nasal codas in word-internal
syllables. An additional advantage of total root reduplication is the obvious indifference of reduplication to the
canonical pattern of the base: the distribution of consonants and the number of syllables are of no import.
The monomoraic pattern is reducible to total root reduplication as well, given the proviso about licit
codas, if we can address the problem of apparent prespecification.
Disregarding the features [tense] and [nasal], which are accounted for by processes entirely independent
of reduplication, we have the following vowel system in Akan:
(98)
Schematic Akan Vowel System
i
u
e
o
a
We will consider the feature [back] to be redundant, and will indicate backing distinctions by use of the feature
[round]. The copy vowel then agrees in rounding with the original, yielding the corresponding copy vowel system:
(99)
Schematic Akan Copy-Vowel System
i
u
i
u
i
Removal of the feature [back] from lexical representations, prespecification of the reduplicative vowel
as [+high], and some assumptions about [+high] overriding [+low] and [-high] will yield the desired pattern of
57
Lee (1984) provides a natural interpretation of these tonal facts: the tonal melody is LHL, and the H is linked to the second mora if there
is one, else the first mora. Spreading of L derives the surface tonal distribution. Reduplicated forms have tone derived by applying this LHL
melody after reduplication.
58
Root-internal syllables show there is considerable dialectal variation in the disposition of the copied nasal; it may assimilate or not, and
it may delete or not, in some cases depending on whether it is moraic. Details are provided by Schachter and Fromkin (1968:167-177).
66
McCarthy & Prince
alternations. This is essentially the analysis of Marantz (1982). But it obscures important relations between the
behavior of reduplication and other rounding harmony phenomena in Akan.
Akan has some quite clear instances of rounding harmony elsewhere. First, in all dialects the vowels of
disyllabic verb roots must meet this requirement: If both root vowels are [-high], they must be identical. The sole
exception to this regularity reported by Schachter and Fromkin is a root that also is disharmonic with respect to
[tense] — a very unusual situation in Akan. The interpretation in terms of feature geometry, along the lines
developed by Mester (1986), is the following. The features [low] and [back], which distinguish the nonhigh
vowels, are dependent on (that is, further from the skeleton than) the feature [high]. Suppose [high] is expressed
lexically only in its negative value; the value [+high] is contributed by a default rule. Thus, the vowels of Akan
are reducible to the following:
(100)
µ
i=
µ
[-rnd]
u=
[+rnd]
µ
e=
µ
[–high]
o=
–rnd
–low
[-high]
+rnd
–low
µ
[–high]
a=
–rnd
+low
The positions of [high] and [round] are crucial to the root rounding harmony process. If two root vowels agree
in the specified value of [high] (that is, [-high]), they must agree in all other features. This follows from
abstracting these vowel features out of the consonants, and applying the OCP over the specified value of [high].
The representations in (101) show this. (101a) shows a permissible root with e in both syllables; (101b) shows
a permissible root with the vowel sequence i o; (101c) shows an impermissible root e o, blocked by its failure
to observe the OCP over [high]:59
(101)
a. µ
µ
b. µ
µ
[–high]
–rnd
–low
[+rnd]
–rnd
–low
c.
µ
µ
[–high]
[–high]
–rnd
–low
+rnd
–low
This is not merely a somewhat baroque reformulation of the Akan root rounding harmony constraint. Rather, by
positing two fundamental properties of the Akan featural system — unspecified [+high] and the hierarchical
relationship between [high] and [round] — it makes strong claims about what other sorts of harmony
relationships we should find in this language. These claims are extensively borne out by the harmonic properties
of nonreduplicative affixes in the language.
59
It is not relevant here, but we could easily make the further move of treating [-rnd] as a default value as well.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
67
We can now use this knowledge to understand what is happening in the reduplicated monomoraic roots.
If we suppose that the [high] tier, isolated as in (100), fails to copy, then default introduction of [+high], along
with the copied value of [rnd], yields the correct result. The vowel of the Akan reduplicative prefix is a default
with respect to height, but does copy the rounding of the base. Thus, in combination with the independently
motivated featural tier structure of the language, Akan differs minimally from Yoruba in this respect. The
difference in behavior between monomoraic and bimoraic roots within Akan is also minimal: the latter copy
everything, the former fail to copy vowel height.60
We now turn to another kind of apparent prespecification phenomenon, this one in connection with total
reduplication of forms of unbounded size.
Echo words are a class of total word reduplication in which some systematic change is effected in one
copy. This phenomenon seems to be nearly universal; the systematic changes known to occur affect peripheral
element(s) or vowels. Echo words may have either the pejorative meaning typical of English or a loose kind of
plurality (‘X and such’) characteristic of the languages of the Indian subcontinent:
(102) Echo Words
a. English
table-shmable
book-shmook
fantastic-shmantastic
apple-shmapple
strike-shmike
b. Marathi Type I (Apte 1968)
kholi-bili
‘room’
aras-biras
‘decoration’
kal-bil
‘yesterday’
c. Marathi Type II (Apte 1968)
Fwti-bhFwti
‘around’
amne-samne
‘in front of’
d. Kamrupi (Goswami 1955-6)
ghar-sar
‘house’
gharaa-saraa
‘horse’
khori-sori
‘fuel’
e. Kolami (Emeneau 1955)
pal-gil
‘tooth’
kota-gita
‘bring it!’
iir-giir
‘water’
maasur-giisur
‘men’
saa-gii
‘go (cont. ger.)’
In the English, Marathi I, and Kamrupi types, the initial consonants, if any, of the suffixed copy are replaced by
a specified consonant or cluster. In Marathi II, the prefixed copy simply lacks the initial consonant. In Kolami,
60
Another analysis of Akan is possible: copy no vowel features, and allow the quality of the reduplicated vowel to be determined by the
independently motivated cross-consonantal harmony rules of the language. The advantage of this approach is that it allows the quality of the
intervening consonant to determine the reduplicative vowel just in case the consonant bears some vocalic properties (as it does when labialized
or palatalized). This effect is needed in the Akuapem dialect of Akan and also in all other cases of partial prespecification known to us: Nupe,
Fe-fe Bamileke, and Grebo. Like Akan, these other languages impose the partial prespecification phenomenon on a system of total root
reduplication.
68
McCarthy & Prince
and in other Dravidian languages as well, the initial CV is replaced by fixed material, gi in this case, preserving
the underlying vowel length distinction.
Phenomena of this sort are incompatible with templatic prespecification in the style of Marantz (1982)
for three reasons. First, the reduplicative template, in this case simply the phonological word W, does not indicate
the skeletal positions with which the melodic elements are preassociated. Prespecification, under this conception,
is at the level of individual skeletal C’s and V’s. The reduplicative template cannot specify individual C’s and
V’s without being infinitely long (since words are of unbounded length). Second, melodic prespecification is
incompatible with a system like Marathi II, since there the grammar must stipulate the absence of a segment in
the copy rather than its presence. Third, melodic prespecification is unable to derive the vowel length preservation
of Kolami and related languages; since prespecification indicates both melodic and skeletal information, it cannot
indicate that the vowel is melodically i but preserves the skeleton (i.e., length) of the original.61
The first of these problems is the one most strongly analogous to the issue that prespecification raises
for prosodic morphology. The reduplication in echo words must be characterized by a very high level constituent,
W, but prespecification is done at the level of individual segments. Similarly, reduplicative skeleta characterized
by morae, syllables, or feet at best lead to problems in the interpretation of prespecification. The problem with
prespecification is the same; the difference is that echo word reduplication simply cannot be translated into CV
or equivalent notation, since it is not bounded.
The rationalization of this conundrum comes from closer consideration of the reduplicative aspect of echo
words. Any of the three truly nonlinear models of reduplication described in section 4 provides a configuration
of melody-to-skeleton association which is unique for reduplicated melodic elements, either because they are on
their own designated melody copy tier or because the involve multiple linking. In any of these models, prior to
linearization, we can detect reduplicated melodic elements by inspection of the structure. Echo words differ from
reduplicated words in that they have rules sensitive to the reduplicated configuration, detaching melodic elements
from the affixal skeleton and replacing them by invariant shm or whatever.62
Melodic Overwriting
Melodic overwriting is refined and applied to the Arabic plural vocalism in M&P 1990a. Also see
Steriade 1988, Bao 1990, and Yip 1992 for further applications of this idea and Alderete et al.
1996 for articulation of this idea within Optimality Theory.
There are three arguments for this approach, apart from the fact that it accounts for an array of facts that
find no other place in reduplicative theory. First, it provides a formal mechanism to account for an otherwise
unexplained fact about echo words: they cannot appear to be reduplicated. For example, even English speakers
with little experience of the phenomenon in (102a) report that words already beginning with the cluster shm
cannot enter into this pattern: *shmaltz-shmaltz (with the intended reading). An English speaker with
considerable experience of the same phenomenon reports that words of this type systematically have initial shp
instead: shmaltz-shpaltz. Goswami (1955-6) reports a similar fact for Kamrupi: lexical items in initial s form
echo words with t: saati-taati ‘lamp’. That is, echo formation is necessarily dissimilatory; this sort of contextsensitivity is straightforwardly expressible with rules, but is simply incompatible with blind prespecification.
Second, a rule of echo word formation predicts a relatively restricted array of contexts in which melodic elements
will be dissociated. We expect the observed pattern of peripheral changes simply because there are only a very
few rule contexts — peripheral, all vowels, all consonants — that will be met by all words, as echo word
formation requires. Third, having rules sensitive to reduplicative structures applying in echo words predicts other
61
Vowels are involved in echo words in other ways; witness Gta in McCarthy (1983a).
62
This view of echo words is developed in considerable detail in Uhrbach ( forthcoming [1987]).
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
69
sorts of sensitivity to reduplication. This is in fact the case: Uhrbach (forthcoming [1987]) reports a rule restricted
to reduplicated forms that is not reconstructible as prespecification.
Once we countenance this approach to a class of phenomena that cannot be accounted for with
prespecification, similar mechanism are available for the description of reduplication in general. This appears to
be necessary to express certain kinds of context sensitivity that are incompatible with prespecification. For
example, Yip (1982) reports that a Chinese reduplicative secret language involves a systematic polarity relation
between copy and original. Since this polarity relation is not a more general property of the language, it must be
specific to reduplicated forms. An even more complex case of context sensitivity of a rule limited to reduplication
is evidenced by Grebo (Innes 1966, Beck 1983).
We have seen, then, that the phenomenon of prespecification does not have a single interpretation when
we consider more completely worked out analyses and a broader range of data. What consequences does this have
for skeletal structure? At a minimum, there are no known cases that are an impediment to our conception of the
skeleton, and there should not be any if the rules exhibited by echo words are more generally available. Our
approach is compatible with a limited amount of prespecification of the familiar sort: prespecifying a mora will
have a reasonable interpretation, and it also makes sense to prespecify an entire syllable or more. Closer study
of the relevant cases is obviously indicated.
3.3
Contour Segments
Contour segments are those with linearly-ordered internal structure, like affricates or prenasalized stops (but not
segments with multiple points of articulation). At least two theories of contour segments in the literature require
a skeleton with segment-sized units, and so we must address them here.
Consider the case of a language that opposes an alveolar affricate c to a tautosyllabic cluster ts,
distinguished by various phonological tests (to be discussed below) and by greater duration of the cluster.
Clements and Keyser (1983) propose that this distinction is represented by identical melodies linked to different
skeletal configurations:
(103)
Linking Theory
a. Affricate c
b. Cluster ts
C
CC
ts
ts
The shorter duration of the affricate in comparison with the cluster follows from the ‘timing unit’ interpretation
of the CV skeleton: duration rules must figure some count of skeletal elements into their calculations. The
affricate (or other contour segment) is recognizable by its multiple linking of melody with skeleton.
Affricates
Research on affricates since 1986 has consistently supported their uni-segmental character,
though there are important differences of detail in the representation of affricates in featural terms.
See Hualde 1988, Lombardi 1990, Steriade 1993, and Rubach 1994.
Halle and Clements (1983) suggest a different account of contour segments, one which involves biplanar
representation. The same contrast is marked by abstracting [continuant] from the melodic representation:
70
McCarthy & Prince
(104)
Biplanar Theory
a. Affricate c
b. Cluster ts
[–cont] [+cont]
[–cont] [+cont]
C
C
C
T
T
T
The archisegment T represents all the features of c or ts except for [continuant].
The differences between these two approaches are at best subtle and, in any case, are not our concern
here. Rather, what we note is that both theories require a skeleton composed of segment-sized units. In both, the
contour segment and tautosyllabic cluster are melodically indistinguishable — they differ only in their
associations with the skeletal units. With direct association of melodic elements to morae or syllables, this
representational contrast simply cannot be made.
There is, however, another coherent view of the affricate/cluster distinction. Let us suppose, in light of
results in the investigation of feature geometry by Clements (1985b), Sagey (1986), Mester (1986), Archangeli
and Pulleyblank (1986), and McCarthy (1985), that there is a general separation of features into three major types
on three associated tiers. A root tier contains the major class features [cons] and [son], a manner tier features like
[cont] and [nas], and an articulator tier contains marks major articulator (with dependent finer distinctions, as in
the description of Akan vowels in section 3.2). These separate tiers are coplanar, linked in this order with the
skeleton. Combined with a prosodic representation of skeleta, this yields the following distinction:
(105)
Tier Theory
a.
b.
)
Skeletal Tier
Root Tier
Manner Tier
Articulator Tier
)
[+cons]
[+cons]
[–cont] [+cont]
[–cont] [+cont]
T
T
The root tier differs from the CV skeleton in one important respect: it does not indicate quantitative distinctions,
and thus in no sense corresponds to segments. Rather, a single unit on the root tier corresponds to the notion
‘single melodic element’.
Empirical differences between this approach and the other two are not hard to come by. Consider first
the possibilities for constructing contour segments. The linking theory permits any arbitrary pair of melodic
elements to constitute a contour segment, provided the elements match in C/V status. The other two
representations require that they agree in some features. In fact, the Tier Theory makes a very strong claim: since
the cited literature on feature geometry demonstrates a hierarchical relationship between manner features and
articulator features, it predicts that contour segments are possible only if they differ in some manner feature(s)
and agree in articulator features. Since we know of no demonstrable cases of contour segments that differ in major
articulator, this prediction seems to be correct.
In the case of simple autosegmental spreading, which demonstrably observes no transfer effects (since
a melody may freely spread between vowels or consonants of differing length), only the Tier Theory derives the
correct results. In Modern Hebrew and in Arabic, the respective affricates c and j spread intact to multiple
positions in the skeleton: kicec ‘he cut’ (Bolozky 1980), hajjaj ‘he made the pilgrimage to Mecca’. To derive
these results it is crucial that affricates constitute a single melodic element that spreads, a notion that is available
only in the Tier Theory.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
71
With the cited literature on feature geometry, then, we conclude that there are hierarchical relationships
among features of different major types. From this it follows that contour segments have a kind of melodic
integrity that the other two theories do not provide, and the behavior and distribution of contour segments confirm
this. Only a single question remains: how do we account for the durational differences between tautosyllabic
clusters and affricates? In a sense no theory has provided a satisfactory answer to this question. Although a timing
unit interpretation of the C/V or X units of a segmental skeleton predicts that clusters should be longer than
contour segments, it also makes another, unintended, but even stronger prediction: all languages should have
segmental isochrony. This is plainly false, since by using the term ‘timing unit’ it imputes to the segmental
skeleton sole responsibility for the durational interpretation of the phonological representation. In fact, an
articulated theory of duration that went beyond the contour segment/cluster distinction would necessarily invoke
melodic factors as part of the mechanism for determining physical duration.
Consider a remarkably clear case of this sort. All analysts agree that a Mandarin syllable with surface
third (MLH) tone is longer than any otherwise identical syllable with a simpler tonal gesture. This is not taken
to mean that such syllables have more timing units than others, but rather that the determination of duration
respects the need to perform a series of melodic adjustments in these syllables that are not paralleled in others.
Since linguistic theory must in any case countenance the idea that the durational interpretation of timing units is
modulo the complexity of the associated melodic sequence, we can naturally appeal to the same idea in
distinguishing contour segments from clusters. The more complex melodic structure of clusters leads naturally
to greater duration under this account, while the true domain of relative isochrony — the mora, syllable, and foot
— is left to a skeleton unadorned with segment-sized units.
3.4
Phonological Consequences
It is not our purpose here to provide an exhaustive account of the phonological consequences of prosodic skeleta,
and, in any case, many points concerning the phonology of morae have already been explored in Hyman’s
important work (1985). Nevertheless, we shall at least offer a brief impression of how phonology comes down
on the matter of skeletal make-up.
At the outset, we deploy a general typology of phonological rules for expository purposes. First, there
are rules that simply manipulate features in a bald way. Such rules include dissimilations, nonassimilatory
changes (like the Sanskrit ruki rule), and the like. Second, there are assimilation rules, all arguably reducible to
autosegmental spreading. Third, there are rules of insertion or deletion, crucially involving operations of some
sort on the skeleton or melodic elements directly associated with it. Fourth, there are rules erecting prosodic
structure in, for example, stress systems.
Feature-manipulating rules will in general be indifferent to the constitution of the skeleton. Such rules
may exhibit some effects of the internal geometry of the features as described in sections 3.2 and 3.3, but they
will not in general refer to the skeleton in any crucial way. This is not surprising — the formal apparatus for this
phonological miscellany has remained largely untouched by advances in nonlinear phonology.
Assimilations too are dependent on the internal geometry of the features, but total assimilations alone
do involve reference to the skeleton. Since, if we adopt the Uniformity of Linking principle of section 3.1,
geminate vowels and consonants can only be represented heteromoraically, it follows that total assimilation will
yield a geminate only under these circumstances as well. Where one might expect to see total assimilation of
tautomoraic elements, we predict apparent deletion of the assimilated melodic element.
Insertion, deletion, and metathesis rules involve far more direct reference to the skeleton. Consider first
the case of vowel epenthesis. Broselow (1982) presents a valuable typology of epenthesis rules which is useful
to our discussion. First, there are epenthesis rules required by constraints on words (rather than syllables). These
constraints are in fact reducible to minimal word requirements of the sort that are by now familiar; we discuss
several such cases in section 1, and they are all obviously compatible with our view of the skeleton. Second, there
are epenthesis rules that respect melodic sequencing constraints. Dorsey’s law in Winnebago is a rule of this type,
72
McCarthy & Prince
enforcing a prohibition on obstruent-sonorant sequences without regard for their syllabification. Such rules are,
by any account, indifferent to skeletal architecture. Third, the largest class of vowel insertion rules is represented
by epenthesis solely to allow syllables to be constructed out of otherwise unsyllabifiable (that is, stray) elements.
The close study of the last type of epenthesis has been a major area of research in skeletal theory, as the
insightful contributions by Levin (1985) and Itô (1986) demonstrate. But a segmental skeletal theory works under
an insuperable impediment in such matters: it predicts the existence of a fourth type of epenthesis, skeletally
conditioned epenthesis that does not deal with stray elements. This follows from the nature of the segmental
skeleton — epenthesis involves insertion of a V or nuclear X slot, and it is difficult to restrict this in the intended
way. To achieve the desired result, we would somehow have to prohibit rules that insert the most primitive
element of skeletal representation. In a prosodic theory, on the other hand, the result falls out essentially from the
nature of the representational system: there are no V or X slots to insert.
A comparable typology and comparable results hold for consonant epenthesis. The syllabic conditioning
of consonant insertion involves, in all cases known to us, observance of the general requirement that syllables
have onsets, as in the case of insertion of before initial vowels in Arabic. Here, too, we rely on the absence of
C slots to account for the absence of rules of consonant insertion not motivated in one of the three ways described
above.
Deletion phenomena also crucially bear on skeletal representation. Since Prince’s (1975) and Ingria’s
(1980) demonstration that compensatory lengthening follows from melodic deletion plus reassociation to the
vacated skeletal slot, it has been known that this phenomenon is a strong source of evidence for skeletal theory.
What has never been satisfactorily explained is why deletion of the initial consonant in a CV syllable does not
yield compensatory lengthening of the following vowel, or why deletion of the first consonant in a tautosyllabic
VCC sequence does not produce lengthening of either the adjacent vowel or consonant (if the two consonants are
also tautomoraic). The explanation for this is straightforward in a purely prosodic theory of the skeleton:
compensatory lengthening, like the formally similar process of total assimilation, is possible only between
heteromoraic elements.
Compensatory Lengthening in Moraic Theory
The argument made here was later developed at length by Hayes 1989.
The last class of phonological phenomena to which we turn are the rules erecting prosodic structure.
Although stress theory has gained considerable ground with a conception of nucleus, rhyme, and syllable
projections, it has never explained why this entire class of rules must always operate on projections at all, instead
of the segmental skeleton, the most basic level of prosodic structure. The same question, of course, can be asked
about tone. The problem is rendered more urgent by the disappearance of the notion ‘projection’ from the domain
that originally motivated it, harmony, where it has been essentially supplanted by an articulated feature geometry.
One could, perhaps, shift the burden of this conundrum to some theory of phonological substance — ‘it only
makes sense to stress syllables’ — but this is unsatisfactory. Phonological substance may constrain the grammars
generated by an overly free formal theory, but it never induces the formal theory to assert a mechanism like
projection. Moreover, one could imagine quite sensible test cases that respect the substantive theory: build binary
feet on the skeleton directly, interpreting this as stress on any syllable containing a segment in strong position.
In any language containing some (C)V syllables, this makes straightforward predictions about the pattern of
accentuation. The absence of such cases constitutes a serious liability of the segmental skeleton.
The prosodic skeleton responds to this objection in the best way possible: all levels of prosody are
available for the construction of superordinate levels of prosody. Projections have no role in such a system; the
sensitivity of stress only to moraic, syllabic, or higher structure follows from the nature of the prosodic hierarchy.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
4.
73
The Nature of the Mapping Relation
Until now, we have largely eschewed discussion of reduplicative association for two reasons. One is purely
expository: we wished to focus on the independent question of the form of the skeleton. The other is principled:
the effects of choice of skeleton are not limited to reduplication, with its peculiar demands on association, but
instead extend to purely templatic morphology, to phonology, and to the theory of counting as well. It is now
appropriate for us to turn to this question.
The independence of choice of skeleton from the mechanism of reduplicative association is not perfect.
We will therefore briefly examine several different views of the association mechanism, evaluating their
consequences for our theory.
4.1
Melody Copying on the Same Tier
Up to this point in the discussion we have adhered rather closely to the claims of Marantz (1982) about the
mechanism of association in reduplication. Let us review the tenets of that analysis:
(106) Reduplicative Association Principles (after Marantz 1982)
a. The phonemic melody of the base is copied ‘over’ the empty skeleton of the reduplicative
affix on the same tier as the original phonemic melody.
b. The association of the phonemic melody with the affix is one-to-one, left-to-right in the case
of prefixes, right-to-left in the case of suffixes. Marked exceptions to the directionality of
association are permitted.
c. Association is phoneme-driven in the sense that elements are taken from the phonemic melody
in the stipulated direction, the skeleton is scanned for compatible, accessible slots in the same
direction, and association is accomplished if possible. [If a phonemic melody element finds no
accessible, compatible skeletal slots, go on to the next phonemic melody element.]
d. Erase unassociated elements of the phonemic melody and of the skeleton.
The notions of compatibility and accessibility are independently characterized in such a theory; a slot and melodic
element are compatible if they meet any requirements imposed by the C/V distinction or, equivalently, by
prosodic structure. A slot is accessible to a phonemic melody element if it can be reached without crossing
association lines. The addition to (106c) is not explicitly made by Marantz, but is clearly a necessary concomitant
of the analysis, since it is the sole means by which phonemic melody elements may be skipped in the course of
association.
We noted already in section 1 that this skipping of phonemic melody elements is in fact (but in no theory)
rigidly constrained. Association is normally continuous; it terminates when it encounters an unassociable melody
element. For example, in Ilokano progressive formation (v. (7)) the ) prefix applied to da.it, in which the vowel
sequence is necessarily heterosyllabic, does not go on to link the t when it fails to link the i: *dat-da.it, da-da.it.
Likewise, foot suffixation in Manam reduplication (section 2.2) does not pass over vowels in search of an initial
consonant once it has its two morae: mo.ita mo.ita-ita, *mo.ita-mita. A final case: earlier (section 2.2), we
raised the question of why there is no monomoraic suffixing reduplication, yielding, for example, badagDA. With
phoneme-driving, this is impossible. Even without phoneme-driving, it involves an impossible pattern of skipping
a melodic element.
The skipping phenomenon is limited to )c, as in Sanskrit du-druv; we have no suggestion except to
stipulate it.63 We take cold comfort in the fact that there is no known theory that does any better.
63
Other apparent cases of skipping are all associated with the phenomenon of prespecification, and are therefore irrelevant to this issue. See
section 3.2.
74
McCarthy & Prince
A related issue involves the notion of phoneme-driven association. In a theory with segmental skeleta
and without the Satisfaction Condition, phoneme-driven association is essential. In this view, the Diyari affix of
2.1 is CVCCV, and skeleton-driven association would yield such absurd results as *NAPRI-napiri. (Even worse
things happen if bases can be vowel-initial.) But this is simply an artifact of the hyperspecified skeleta required
in such theories; a true prosodic characteriation of the skeleton ()) in this case) encounters no such difficulties.
Parsing the copied phonemic melody by the affixal skeleton — essentially skeleton-driven association — yields
exactly the desired result. Eliminating phoneme-driving also eliminates one of the most apparent differences in
the mode of association employed by reduplicative versus templatic systems.
The stipulated dependence of direction of association on whether the affix is prefixed or suffixed is
clearly unprecedented in any domain outside reduplication. We take it to be related to prosodic structure;
reprosodization is always from an edge inward. (Comparably, prior to any affixation, prosodization also starts
at an edge, but a stipulated one. See Itô (1986) for discussion of the directionality of syllabification parameter.)
Edge-in Association & Alignment
There is a strong connection between the “edge-in’‘ character of reduplicative association and the
special status of constraints aligning the edges of morphological and prosodic constituents. See
M&P 1993a, 1994b. Also see Yip 1988 for further discussion of edge-in association.
In this way we relate direction of association to more general considerations of the assignment of
prosodic structure. The claimed virtue of a the purely stipulative direction of (106b) is that it represents merely
an unmarked alternative; that is, some patterns of reduplication systematically deviate from the normal order of
left-to-right in prefixes and right-to-left in suffixes. The known examples of this sort are either misanalyzed or
too sparsely described for inferences to be drawn. First, Chukchee reduplications of the nutenut type are claimed
to display left-to-right association with a CVC suffix. This is false: the language independently deletes stem-final
vowels that are also word-final (Kenstowicz (1979), Kim (1984)), and the purported base of this reduplication
nute is not a free form but rather an underlying representation. Second, the Madurese (Stevens 1968) pattern in
which a copy of the stem-final syllable is prefixed (as in wa-buwa-an ‘fruits’), is straightforwardly derived by
total stem reduplication, yielding /buwa-buwa-an/, and a subsequent rule reducing the left branch of a
compound to its stressed (that is, final) syllable (see section 2.4 for further discussion). Both of these rules, stem
reduplication and compound truncation, are extensively independently motivated in the language.64 Tzeltal,
described by Berlin (1963), is a case of total root reduplication in which material other than the initial consonant
is fixed (prespecified); it has no bearing on directionality of association. The final case, from the Northwest Coast
language Tillamook (Reichard 1959), is so poorly described and inconsistent that a number of plausible
alternatives (like cluster simplification) simply cannot be tested. In sum, with these examples out of the way, there
is an inviolate requirement of contiguity or locality between the original string and its copy (Kim 1984). In our
conception, locality emerges as a theorem of edge-in reprosodization.
These three moves — a rigid restriction on skipping, elimination of phoneme-driven association, and
edge-in reprosodization — are not strictly relevant to the overall structural possibilities of reduplication. We turn
now to examining alternatives to (106a).
64
According to Stevens’s (1968) characterization of reduplication and truncation, the two rules display different junctures. No data in support
of this juncture distinction are provided, but were they to exist they would merely show that the truncation rule is applicable at more than one
lexical level. See Weeda (1986 [1987]) for a different view.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
75
Base/Reduplicant Identity
Our recent work on this subject sees identity in terms of constraints on a relation of correspondence between the base and the reduplicant (M&P 1993a, 1994ab, 1995b), which are formally
parallel to the faithfulness constraints between input and output. Reduplicative over- and
underapplication (Wilbur 1974, Marantz 1982, Carrier-Duncan 1984, Kiparsky 1986, Mester 1986)
can then be understood in terms of the interaction of such Base-Reduplicant identity constraints
with other phonological constraints. On this, see M&P 1995b; Benua 1995, forthcoming, Burzio
1996, Cohn & McCarthy 1994, Flemming & Kenstowicz 1995, Kenstowicz 1995, Itô, Kitagawa, &
Mester 1996, Orgun 1995, McCarthy 1996, Pater 1995, Urbanczyk 1995, 1996ab, Yip 1995ab.
4.2
Melody Copying on a Different Tier
In Broselow and McCarthy (1983-4), it is proposed that the reduplicative melody is copied on a different tier
from the original. Two advantages were claimed for this approach: it conforms more closely to the one
morpheme-one tier rubric of McCarthy (1981, 1986), and it is necessary for the description of certain kinds of
infixing reduplication (to be described below).
Levergood (1983) notes an incorrect consequence of this approach to melody copying in the overall
context of Broselow and McCarthy’s theory. In that theory, the Washo pattern of section 2.3 is analyzed as VCV
inserted after the initial C, with stipulated right-to-left association (cf. ewši eši.ewši. What prevents there
from being a language in which the copy appears inside the string it copies This will occur any time the infix
is large enough to associate with some of the material relative to which it was inserted, since the entire melody
is available, in copy form, on another tier.
Our approach simply does not have that consequence for cases like Washo, even if the melody is copied
on a separate tier. Since we have, in effect, eliminated infixing reduplication in such languages by use of
extraprosodicity, the nonoccurring situation is in fact impossible.
Having eliminated the one liability of a separate tier for the melodic copy, we can now explore its
advantages relative to copying on the same tier. There are two, one quite general and the other more specific.
We demonstrated in section 1 that unassociated skeletal elements constitute a serious problem for
reduplication, since phonological theory provides ample mechanisms by which such elements might exert various
influences on the derivation. Thus, the erasure of unassociated skeletal positions in reduplicative affixes must be
stipulated to occur instantaneously, rather than in conjunction with some wholescale stray erasure later in the
phonology. We adduced a theory, involving the Satisfaction Condition, which meets this objection by allowing
no unassociated skeletal elements. The same objection, though, can be raised for copied but unassociated melodic
elements. Such floating melody units are not unknown either in the tonal or nontonal literature. They may be
expected to condition melody-level phonological processes; in particular, they could condition the application of
purely melodic assimilation rules on the tier-adjacent melody elements of the base. In fact, no copied phonemic
melody elements ever display such behavior;65 they are entirely silent in the phonological derivation, and so they
too must be erased immediately as part of the reduplicative procedure. Cases where this is particularly important
include Orokaiva in 2.1 and Murut in 2.3; in neither case is the homorganic nasal-stop structure created across
the two melodies.
Copying the melody on a different tier from the original largely meets this objection without requiring
a special melodic erasure principle. Consider the representation of hypothetical bad-badupi:
65
But see the discussion of Lardil in footnote 2.1.4.
76
McCarthy & Prince
(107)
Copied Melody
badupi
Skeleton
)+
Original Melody
) ))
badupi
The offending melody elements are the unassociated parts of the copied melody upi. These elements will be
invisible to rules referring to both skeleton and melody under either conception of the tier structure, but a
difference emerges in rules that apply on the melody level only. Suppose the language has a rule palatalizing b
after i, a rule that is purely melodic. If original and copy are on the same tier, and if no special provison for
erasure is invoked, then this rule may be expected to apply to the second b in bad-badupi, since melodic
contiguity is all that matters. If original and copy are on different tiers, however, the same rule is inapplicable;
no adjacency relation holds between the two melodic elements involved in the rule.
Since this sort of super-overapplication is unprecedented in analyses of reduplication, we take this to be
a desireable consequence of copying the melody on a separate tier. Two other questions arise. First, are there
melody-only interactions between the associated and unassociated elements within the copied melody?66 The
answer is yes; that type of overapplication is instantiated in Luiseño, as interpreted by McCarthy (1979) and
Mester (1986).67 What about melody-only interactions between the original and copied melody? These come
under the rubric of Tier Conflation in McCarthy (1986) (or some equivalent mechanism for intrepreting
structures); when the tiers are conflated, the two melodies are linearized on a single tier and unassociated elements
necessarily drop out in an independently motivated way.
Thus, we have briefly shown that melody copying on a separate autosegmental tier provides an accurate
account of intramelodic interactions without appeal to immediate erasure. It also accounts for a system of infixing
reduplication that is peculiar to templatic morphology. Templatic systems, with their total separation of melody
and skeleton, exhibit a type of infixation that is not found elsewhere. In Arabic, for example, there are infixes with
melodic but without skeletal content, whose position in the skeleton is determined not by infixation or
extraprosodicity, but by rules of association. Templatic systems have a comparable freedom to use reduplication
in a relatively unpredictable way; Arabic contrasts lexical items falfal and sammam, built on identical
independently needed skeleta from biconsonantal roots, but with reduplication in the first case and spreading in
the second. It is impossible to predict which option will be taken, and often both are in related words.
Reduplication is not simple affixation in root-and-pattern systems (fal is an impossible stem), any more than rootand-pattern morphology itself is simple affixation.
Templatic systems can compose their special brand of reduplication and infixation to yield a type of
infixing reduplication that is quite different from that described in section 2.3. In some modern dialects of Arabic,
a widely used pejorative verb form is barbad, from the root brd, in which a copy of the first root consonant
appears at the beginning of the second syllable. The [)µµ )µµ ] template occurs independently (see section 2.4);
it is not derived by a rule of infixation. Rather, the root melody is copied on a separate tier and associated in the
direction universally used in Arabic, LR, to yield the following representation (somewhat simplified):
66
67
A different conception of the role of a copy tier in overapplication is pursued in Cowper and Rice (1985).
Marantz’s (1982) analysis of Luiseño relies on claiming that the relevant rule (a g/š alternation) is cyclic. The rule is, however, nonstructure-preserving (that is, nonneutralizing) and surface true, incompatible with cyclicity. Two arguments for its cyclicity are presented. One
involves a demonstration that the rule is neutralizing; the sole example is an interjection. The second involves showing that the rule treats
certain external (Level II) affixes like word boundaries. In light of Borowsky’s (1986) demonstration that Level II phonology and postlexical
phonology are coextensive, this argument is without force.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
77
(108)
brd
)
µ
)
µ
µ
µ
brd
The comparable case in Temiar (sglog < slog) is derived in the same way. As Broselow and McCarthy (1983–4:
42) note, the difference in direction of association in Arabic and Temiar infixing reduplication (LR and RL,
respectivel) can be derived from the overall direction of mapping and spreading observed elsewhere in the two
languages.68 The reduplicatve aspect of the Arabic example is paralleled by falfal; the infixation is paralleled by
ktanbab (< ktb). Templatic infixing reduplication emerges as a natural consequence of the independently needed
properties of purely templatic systems if we have melody copying on a separate tier.
4.3
Synchronous Skeleta
Two classes of empirical problems with melody copying approaches have been addressed in the literature. The
first involves the phenomena of phonological rule over- and underapplication in reduplicated forms.
Overapplication occurs most commonly when an external affix triggers some transformation in both copies of
the base: Indonesian /mFN+pikir-pikir/ mFmikirmikir. Underapplication is most common when a rule that
would occur in one copy under the influence of the other copy is blocked. Obviously we cannot deal adequately
with this rich field here, but there is a literature (Mester 1986; Uhrbach 1985, forthcoming [1987]; Cowper and
Rice 1985; Carrier-Duncan 1984; Odden and Odden 1985) arguing for a treatment of over- and underapplication
different from that in Marantz (1982).
A different problem, noted in the first instance by Levin (1983), occurs in Mokilese. Recall from our
earlier discussion that Mokilese heavy syllable reduplication copies copies long vowels as long. We meet with
the same effect in Lardil foot reduplication. This effect, which Clements (1985a) has dubbed transfer, is
instantiated at levels of prosodic constituency higher than the syllable as well. One aspect of the general problem
is that long vowels are required to copy as long, providing there is sufficient room in the skeleton for them, and
similarly for long consonants. The theory as outlined in (106) makes no allowance for the preservation of length
distinctions. A more dramatic case of such transfer is provided by total reduplication of stems or words, in which
every prosodic characteristic of the original is precisely replicated.
Overapplication, Transfer
On overapplication and other paradoxical phonology/reduplication interactions, see above p. 75.
On transfer, see M&P 1988, 1993a: Chapt. 5 and Steriade 1988.
Two recent proposals for a reorganization of the relation between copy and original in reduplicative
structures have emerged which address these issues. Clements’s (1985a) view, which we will call the Parafix
Theory, holds that the skeleta of base and affix are simultaneous and linked to one another, and that the effect
of phonemic melody copying is achieved by transferring onto the affixal skeleton the associations of linked base
skeletal elements with the phonemic melody (109a). The Unimelodic Theory in Mester’s (1986), McCarthy
(1985), and Kitagawa (1985 [1987]), on the other hand, holds that linking is between the affixal skeleton and
68
Zuni (Broselow and McCarthy 1983–4) is also a case of this sort. The system is clearly templatic; only CVCV verbs, whose vowels are
identical in all known examples, display the alternation.
78
McCarthy & Prince
the phonemic melody directly (109b). Consider the intermediate representations generated by these two theories
for the Agta form barbari:
(109)
a. Parafix Theory
b. Unimelodic Theory
bar
CVC
CVC
CVCV
bari
CVCV
bari
These approaches have several virtues. First, both eschew copying of the entire phonemic melody, and thus
bypass the objection that copied but unassociated phonemic melody elements are phonologically impotent. The
Parafix Theory, moreover, provides a direct account of the transfer phenomenon; since the phonemic melody
elements are copied with their associations from the base, transfer is a necessary consequence of such an
approach. And the Unimelodic Theory, although it cannot account for transfer in any nonstipulative way, does
open the way for a new means of accounting for reduplication/phonology interactions. Since a single melodic
element is associated to more than one skeletal position, overapplication can be interpreted as a rule applying to
the melodic tier alone, necessarily having effects on more than one ‘copy’ at a time. Underapplication can arise
from two sources — rules whose structural descripton is not met before linearization of the synchronous skeleta,
and geminate unanalyzeability effects. After linearization, rule application is normal.
Clements (1985a) explores a comparable approach to over- and underapplication in the Parafix Theory,
but this theory predicts a wider variety of rule/structure interactions than the Unimelodic Theory. Before transfer
of the melody to the affixal skeleton, we get both overapplication and underapplication. After transfer, but before
linearization, we get underapplication but no overapplication (which relies on the single melody). After
linearization, finally, all rule application is normal. The Parafix Theory, then, has three stages of representation
to the Unimelodic Theory’s two. The need for the middle stage of the three has never been demonstrated, although
it should be noted that the critical ordering arguments are typically of great subtlety.
Obviously no property of either theory is incompatible with the exclusively prosodic conception of the
skeleton that we develop — in fact, both Clements and Mester use prosodic skeleta in some cases. But certain
differences between melody copying and these theories do emerge in light of our results.
These approaches share the characteristic that reduplication involves a skeletal affix which is
synchronous with the base. Although morphology which is synchronous in some sense is not unknown (witness
Semitic), skeletal synchronicity is unique to reduplication. In effect, then, the claim that reduplication is affixation
is discarded by these approaches. Both theories are at a serious disadvantage in comparison with the melody
copying theories in another respect. On the one hand, the two theories must stipulate some direction of association
of each affix with the base. On the other hand, though, they must somehow retrieve this information when it
comes time to linearize the representations in (109). If direction of association and direction of linearization are
entirely divorced from one another, then the requirement of locality or contiguity between the original and the
copied strings becomes unattainable. This problem is by no means a trivial one; if association and linearization
are separated by the application of other rules, as Clements and Mester carefully argue to account for over and
under-application, then some global mechanism (like features [Prefix] and [Suffix]) will be needed to transmit
information through the course of the derivation. Furthermore, this linearization operation, whatever its character,
will be another mechanism peculiar to reduplication, since any efforts to collapse it with the independently
motivated mechanism of Tier Conflation (McCarthy 1986) run afoul of precisely this linear order problem.
There are also specific empirical problems with the synchronous skeleta approaches in their relation to
the Onset Rule. Recall from our discussion of all three types of reduplication (suffixing, infixing, and especially
prefixing) that applications of the Onset Rule across the affix-base juncture are critical to accounting for patterns
of reduplication. This is especially true of the apparent XX or XXX variety: wa-waeke, uh-uhuke. At the
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
79
appropriate points we noted a large number of typological results that follow from this approach; in particular,
we saw that XX or XXX is an illusion based on onset creation, and therefore breaks down precisely on Cinitial/V-initial lines. This explanation and the other results of the Onset Rule are simply unavailable if the skeleta
are synchronous; we could never associate h to the onset of the base skeleton.
How then do we capture the not insignificant virtues of the synchronous skeleta theories without
incurring this liability? We have already spoken briefly to the issue of overapplication; if the melody is copied
on a different tier but immediate erasure of unassociated elements is not invoked, some effects of overapplication
are available and others may be obtainable as well. Underapplication is also possible in these terms; melody-only
rules will fail to apply to the melodies on distinct tiers until they have been linearized. The overall pattern of
overapplication and underapplication needs reevaluation, however, in the context of Lexical Phonology (Kiparsky
1982), in which phonological/morphological interactions of this sort are not a peculiar feature of reduplication
but instead are the norm for all aspects of the morphology.
As for transfer, we defer first to Marantz (1982: 455): ‘We must assume that the Cs and Vs of the stem
in syllabic reduplication are copied clustered in the syllabic units that they form in the stem.’ Referring here to
a pattern he analyzes as disyllabic reduplication in Yidig (foot/minimal word in our terms), he thereby explains
the transfer of syllable structure from original to copy.69 In our approach, there are no C’s or V’s, and the notion
of clustering is not independently defined, but we have an alternative interpretation in the same spirit. Rather than
copy the phonemic melody, we copy all the prosodic structure subordinate to the category stipulated by the
reduplicative affix. That is, the prosodic hierarchy defines the properties of transfer.
The explanation for perfect transfer in total reduplication is trivial in these terms; every aspect of
prosodic structure below W is copied along with the melody. If the reduplicative affix is F, we copy all syllabic
and moraic structure along with the skeleton. This accounts for vowel length transfer in Lardil. If the reduplicative
affix is ), as it is in Mokilese (since bimoraicity is predictable from membership in the ‘loosely-bound’ affix class
of the language), then we copy moraic structure only. Mokilese transfer is thereby accounted for.
This copied prosody does not survive unaltered. In Mokilese gaagaak, the final k cannot remain linked
in the copy, since CVVC syllables are not licensedy word-internally. In Lardil and in all other languages, the edge
between linked and unlinked elements is subject to maximization of association: pareli reduplicates as
parelpareli. In effect, what transfer preserves, subject to the requirements laid down by the reduplicative affix,
is the moraic structure of the original, and nothing else.
4.4
Line Crossing
All theories of the mapping operation entertained so far involve a stipulated difference between reduplication and
spreading. In reduplication but not in spreading, either the melody is copied or the skeleta are synchronous rather
than linearly ordered. Here we briefly explore a novel conception of reduplication, one in which spreading and
reduplication are instances of the same phonological principles. It does require that we give up the only hitherto
inviolate constraint of autosegmental phonology, but it replaces it by something nearly as good. Our remarks are
naturally speculative at this point, but the consequences we point to are worth considering.
In fact, we regard Yidig as prefixation of a minimal word to a minimal word, not as transfer. The contrast between Yidig and Lardil in
the treatment of CVCVCV bases is otherwise inexplicable. See section 2.1 for discussion of these two languages.
69
80
McCarthy & Prince
Our proposal is embodied by the representation of barbari in (110), and we shall develop the underlying
principles immediately below:
(110)
Crossing Theory
)+))
bari
It is obvious that this tree contravenes the prohibition on crossing association lines, the only principle of the
theory which has survived unscathed since Goldsmith (1976). The affix ) is linearly ordered on the same tier with
the base skeleton )), yet the phonemic melody has not been copied. Rather, the phonemic melody elements bar
exhibit dual linkings, one each with base and affix.
Line-Crossing Theory
See Bagemihl 1989a for an interesting development of line-crossing theory, as well as Itô et al.
1996. For our current view of reduplicant/base identity, see the references cited on p. 75.
Rather than immediately describe the principles underlying such a representation and the motivation for
it, we shall reflect for a moment on the line-crossing prohibition. There is a certain redundancy in representations
which observe the prohibition on line-crossing, first noted implictly by Halle and Vergnaud (1980), who suggest
that lines be replaced by indices. This redundancy idea is developed in Sagey (1986), who argues as follows.
Consider the autosegmental representation (111):
(111)
ABC
d e f
In (111), linear order relations are fully defined among {A, B, C} and among {d, e, f}, since the members of each
set are on the same tier. We define the association relation as one of simultaneity (in her terms, overlap in time).
Association therefore links elements occurring at the same time. Under this definition, adding a link from d to
C would necessarily lead to a contradiction (since d cannot be simultaneous with A and C and not with intervening
B).
Her conclusion is that the line-crossing prohibition is not a primitive of the theory, but rather follows
from these two premises: linear order relations are fully defined within a tier, and association expresses
simultaneity.
Melody-copying approaches to reduplication respect this view of the world; they have ordering within
tiers and association as simultaneity. The theories with synchronous skeleta do not; although they preserve
ordering within tiers, association cannot be construed as simultaneity, since two skeleta cannot occur at the same
time. (The linearization of skeleta at some later stage of the derivation reflects this interpretation; the interface
with the real world of phonetics does respect simultaneity.)
In a sense, then, synchronous skeleta give up the line-crossing prohibition since they give up one of the
premises from which it is derived. The alternative we explore here gives it up in another way: by denying the strict
ordering of elements within a melodic tier. The redundancy in the theory is eliminated either way.
There is another class of arguments, empirical this time, that suggest that an absolute prohibition on linecrossing is too strong. Dorsey’s law in Winnebago (Miner 1979, Hale and White Eagle 1980) is a typical example
of the phenomenon of vowel-copying epenthesis. Dorsey’s law accounts for the following partial derivations:
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
(112) Winnebago
Underlying
hoikwe
krepna
pras
rupri
xwu
81
Derived
hoikewe
kerepana
paras
rupiri
xuwuxuwu
This rule is impeccably justified by alternations, by the behavior of reduplication, by interactions with
phonological rules, and by the distribution of stress. The generalization is that a [-son][+son]V sequence, medial
or initial, is broken up by insertion of a copy of the vowel between the obstruent and sonorant. The problem we
are attending to is obviously the phenomenon of vowel copy. There are four possible approaches. First, we might
suggest that vowels and consonants are represented on separate planes. If this is so, then Winnebago is surely
the frailest reed on which this far-reaching claim has yet leaned, since no other facts of the language point that
way. Second, we could appeal to the notion that consonantal and vocalic articulators are disjoint (as in 3.2), but
this immediately runs into the fact that the glide w is transparent to the copying process (as it happens, no
examples with y in the requisite environment are attested). Third, we could adopt a phonological copying
operation, which unfortunately would have none of the character of the reduplicative one (since the former is
strictly local) and which would produce a massive increase in the power of phonological theory. The fourth
alternative, and the one we shall support, is that cases of this sort are instances in which association lines do in
fact cross.
Dorsey’s Law
On Dorsey’s Law and similar phenomena, see Steriade 1990 and Gafos 1996.
Let us now turn to the details of the theory and rejoin Winnebago at a later point. Our proposal is
embodied in the following principles:
(113) Principles of Association
a. Crossing Avoidance. Do not cross association lines which have not yet been crossed.
b. Maximization of Association. Associate as many different phonemic melody elements as possible.
c. Priority Clause. In case of conflict between (a) and (b), (b) has priority.
The essential character of this proposal is that the prohibition on crossing association lines is relativized under
clause (c); that is, it is subordinated to association of phonemic melody elements under certain conditions.
Overriding all aspects of these principles are certain absolute requirements: the demand by the Satisfaction
Condition that all skeletal positions be exhausted and whatever language-particular licensing conditions there are
on the melody-to-skeleton association.
Let us now consider the consequences of this proposal. For cases like (110), the association principles
as stated give just exactly the nested pattern of crossing dependencies that is required. The unadorned
representation, before any lines are drawn to the reduplicative affix, first undergoes association of b to the initial
position of the affix — no lines are crossed, so both (a) and (b) are fulfilled in their perfection. Subsequent
associations exhibit the priority of (b) over (a) — one line is newly crossed for each new melodic element
associated.
It is apparent that the relative contiguity of the original and its copy follows from the minimization of
crossing association lines demanded by (a). This means that (a) alone is responsible for the universal direction
of association in reduplication: left-to-right with prefixes, right-to-left with suffixes. In a sense, clause (a) is
82
McCarthy & Prince
independently motivated by virtually all autosegmental analyses: analyses in which lines are never cross
minimally entail a theory in which line-crossing is avoided.
Clause (b) of the association principles is also independently motivated. It is implicit in any principles
of association like phoneme-driving, and it serves other functions as well. The hundreds of Arabic quadriliteral
roots are almost never applied to a skeleton in which there are insufficient free skeletal slots for all root
consonants to be associated. Rules that would disturb the complete association of the root are also blocked —
for example, the reassociation rule in McCarthy (1981) responsible for medial gemination in kattab is suppressed
with quadriliteral verbs like tarjam because of a principle essentially identical to clause (b).
Like both the Parafix and Unimelodic theories, this has the advantage of not requiring phonemic melody
copying. It has the advantage over these two theories with respect to applications of the Onset Rule; it encounters
no difficulty in handling cases like Orokaiva and the like. It also allows an account of over- and underapplication
similar to that of these two theories.70
The representations with multiply-linked melodic-elements are submitted to a block of (lexical)
phonological rules, and rule application is governed by familiar constraints independently needed for the
treatment of geminates. At some later point — arguably at the end of each level or the end of the lexicon — Tier
Conflation applies, linearizing the representation by performing the actual melody copying operation. This
corresponds closely to the conception of Tier Conflation in McCarthy (1986): TC produces representations in
which the linear order of melodic elements is identical to the linear order of the associated skeletal elements.
Unlike in the synchronous skeleta theories, we can actually appeal to Tier Conflation, because the representations
we posit for reduplication contain all information about linear order, just as those in root-and-pattern systems do.
We have seen, then, that a set of principles of association which relativize the prohibition on line crossing
considerably reduce the amount of reduplication-specific machinery required by the theory. Can we eliminate such
machinery entirely? Perhaps not, but at the very least we can demonstrate that many of the same mechanisms are
needed independently to account for longstanding problems outside the realm of reduplication.
Recall the generalization embodied in Dorsey’s law in Winnebago, which is representative of a whole
class of vowel-copying epenthesis rules. We showed that separation of consonantal and vocalic articulators will
not account for this phenomenon, since even a high glide (melodically indistinguishable from a vowel) may
intervene between original and copy. We dismissed as absurd the suggestion that vowels and consonants are
represented on separate, morphologically-characterizeable planes in this language. Our conclusion, then, is that
either phonological theory recognizes a primitive operation of melodic copying, or that it licenses line crossing
in general, as in the derived representation of ...kewe:
(114)
)
)
k
we
Contemplation of such phenomena, in fact, yields a laudably restricted theory of apparent phonological
copying. The copy is always determined locally with respect to the set of vowels — that is, gratuitous crossing
of association lines is avoided in conformity with clause (a) of the principles. The process is nevertheless rule
governed — we must stipulate the direction from which the vowel comes in Winnebago, and in cases like
Palestinian Arabic (Kenstowicz 1981) we must restrict spreading to stem-internal contexts, since otherwise a
default vowel emerges. The general case of languages without copying epenthesis also demands a rule-based
account. But the fact that such a rule exhibits the sort of locality predicted under our approach confirms both the
need to permit line crossing and the need to restrict it as we have done.
70
For transfer, it can, like the melody-copying theories, rely on linking of higher-level prosodic elements.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
83
Having demonstrated that there is great utility in permitting lines to cross under certain circumstances,
we must also show that no results of the absolute prohibition are unavailable to us. For example, it has been
suggested (McCarthy 1979, 1981; Marantz 1982) that the line-crossing prohibition accounts for the nonexistence
of mirror-image reduplication rules — those that copy and reflect a string. In fact, Marantz’s approach has
redundant explanations for this effect — it follows not only from the prohibition but also from the nature of the
association procedure. It is ruled out by clause (a) in our case, since it would inevitably involve gratuitous line
crossing. Clause (a) also accounts for the fact that lots of phonology can be done without ever finding instances
of lines crossing. The override of (a) by (b) under clause (c) is met with chiefly in reduplicative systems because
they alone have substantial skeletal regions bearing prior asssociations and substantial skeletal regions without
prior associations.
The remaining case in which an absolute prohibition has been claimed to have utility is the phenomenon
of geminate integrity, first examined in this connection by Steriade (1982). The fact that geminate consonants
resist internal epenthesis is taken in Steriade’s work and subsequently to be a direct consequence of the inability
to associate a vocalic melody with a skeletal slot ‘inside’ a geminate without line crossing. As Levin (1985: 87)
is careful to point out, however, if epenthetic vowels are in fact default vowels, so that they are represented
through much of the derivation without associated melodic material, it follows that this sort of explanation is
simply no longer available. Even if we would later discover that the representation could not undergo the default
rule, intervening rules would have treated the epenthetic vowel as if it were there. Rather, it is likely that an
appropriate condition on the analyzability of geminates by phonological rules will derive this result instead.
Since the failure of epenthesis into geminates is the sole result of an absolute line-crossing prohibition
in phonological theory (other results following equally well in general from avoidance of line-crossing), we
conclude that there may be some merit in exploring more dramatic shifts in the character of phonological
representation than have previously been entertained.
84
McCarthy & Prince
Bibliography
Akinlabi, Akinbiyi (1984) Tonal Underspecification and Yoruba Tone. Doctoral dissertation, University of
Ibadan.
Akinlabi, Akinbiyi and Eno Urua(1993) Prosodic Targets and vocalic specification in Ibibio verbs. WCCFL 11,
1-14.
Akinlabi, Akinbiyi (1995) Prosodic Truncation and Template Satisfaction in Ibibio Verbs. In Kola Owolabi, ed.,
Language in Nigeria: a Festschrift for Ayo Bamgbose. Group Publishers: Ibadan, pp. 75-90.
Alderete, John, Jill Beckman, Laura Benua, Amalia Gnanadesikan, John McCarthy, & Suzanne Urbanczyk
(1996) Reduplication and segmental unmarkedness. Ms., UMass Amherst. ROA-134, Rutgers
Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Anderson, Stephen R. (1972) On nasalization in Sundanese. Linguistic Inquiry, 3, 253–268.
Anderson, Stephen R. (1992) A-Morphous Morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Andrade, M. (1933) Quileute. New York: Columbia University Press.
Apte, Mahadeo (1968) Reduplication, echo formation, and onomatopoeia in Marathi. Poona: Deccan College,
Postgraduate and Research Institute.
Applegate, Richard B. (1976) Reduplication in Chumash. In Margaret Langdon & Shirley Silver, eds., Hokan
Studies. The Hague: Mouton. Pp. 271–283.
Archangeli, Diana (1983) The root CV-template as a property of the affix: Evidence from Yawelmani. Natural
Language & Linguistic Theory, 1, 348–384.
Archangeli, Diana (1984) Underspecification in Yawelmani Phonology and Morphology. Doctoral dissertation,
MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Archangeli, Diana (1991) Syllabification and prosodic templates in Yawelmani. Natural Language & Linguistic
Theory, 9, 231–284.
Archangeli, Diana & Douglas Pulleyblank (1986) The Content and Structure of Phonological Representations.
Ms., University of Arizona and University of British Columbia.
Archangeli, Diana & Douglas Pulleyblank (1994) Grounded Phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aronoff, Mark (1976) Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Aronoff, Mark (1985) Talk on Makassarese reduplication presented at University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Aronoff, Mark (1988) Head operations and strata in reduplication: A linear treatment. Yearbook of Morphology,
1, 1–15.
Aronoff, Mark, Azhar Arsyad, Hassan Basri, & Ellen Broselow (1987) Tier configuration in Makassarese
reduplication. In Anna Bosch, Barbara Need and Eric Schiller (eds.), Papers from the 23rd Annual
Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Part Two: Parasession on Autosegmental and
Metrical Phonology (pp. 1–15). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Austin, Peter (1981) A Grammar of Diyari, South Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bach, Emmon (1975) Long vowels and stress in Kwakiutl. Texas Linguistic Forum.
Bagemihl, Bruce (1988) The morphology and phonology of Katajjait (Inuit throat games). Canadian Journal
of Linguistics, 33, 1–58.
Bagemihl, Bruce (1989a) The crossing constraint and `backwards languages'. Natural Language & Linguistic
Theory, 7, 481–550.
Bagemihl, Bruce (1989b) Alternate phonologies and morphologies. Doctoral dissertation, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver.
Bagemihl, Bruce (1991) Syllable structure in Bella Coola. Linguistic Inquiry, 22, 589–646.
Bao, Zhiming (1990) Fanqie languages and reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 317–350.
Bat-El, Outi (1989) Phonology and Word Structure in Modern Hebrew. Doctoral dissertation, UCLA, Los
Angeles.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
85
Bat-El, Outi (1992) Stem modification and cluster transfer in Modern Hebrew. Ms., Tel-Aviv University, TelAviv.
Bat-El, Outi (1995) Selecting the best of the worst: The grammar of Hebrew blends. Ms., Tel-Aviv University.
Bates, Dawn & Barry Carlson (1992) Simple syllables in Spokane Salish. Linguistic Inquiry, 23, 653–659.
Beck, Hanno T. (1983) Grebo verbal morphology. Term paper, University of Texas, Austin.
Bell, Sarah (1983) Internal C reduplication in Shuswap. Linguistic Inquiry, 14, 332–338.
Benton, Richard (1971) Pangasinan Reference Grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Benua, Laura (1995) Identity effects in morphological truncation. ROA-74. Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. In Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, & Suzanne Urbanczyk, eds.,
University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18: Papers in Optimality Theory.
Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association. Pp. 77-136.
Benua, Laura (forthcoming) Transderivational Identity. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts,
Amherst.
Berlin, Brent (1963) Some semantic features of reduplication in Tzeltal. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 29, 211–218.
Bernabe, Emma J. Fonacier, Virginia Lapid & Bonifacio Sibayan. (1971) Ilokano lessons. Honolulu: University
of Hawaii Press.
Biggs, Bruce (1961) The structure of New Zealand Maaori. Anthropological Linguistics, 3, 1–54.
Black, H. Andrew (1991) The optimal iambic foot and reduplication in Axininca Campa. Phonology at Santa
Cruz, 2, 1–18.
Black, H. Andrew (1994) Constraint-ranked derivation: A serial approach to optimization. Doctoral
dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz. Pp. 187.
Blevins, Juliette (1996) Mokilese reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry 27.3, 523-530.
Bliese, Loren F. (1981) A Generative Grammar of Afar. The Summer Institute of Linguistics, University of
Texas at Arlington.
Boas, Franz & Ella Deloria (1941) Dakota Grammar. Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences 23.
Washington, DC: US Govt. Printing Office.
Bolozky, Shmuel (1980) On the monophonematic interpretation of Modern Hebrew affricates. Lingustic Inquiry
11, 793–9.
Borowsky, Toni (1986). Topics in the Lexical Phonology of English. Doctoral dissertation, University of
Massachusetts, Amherst.
Bowen, J. Donald, ed. (1969). Beginning Tagalog: A course for speakers of English. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
Boxwell, H. & M. Boxwell (1966) Weri phonemes. Papers in New Guinea Linguistics 5, 77–93.
Breen, Gavan (1990) The syllable in Arrernte phonology. Ms., School of Australian Linguistics and IAD
(Institute for Aboriginal Development.
Broadbent, Sylvia (1964) The Southern Sierra Miwok Language. University of California Publications in
Linguistics. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Bromberger, Sylvain & Morris Halle (1988) Conceptual issues in morphology. Ms.. MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Broselow, Ellen (1982) On the interaction of stress and epenthesis. Glossa, 16, 115–132.
Broselow, Ellen (1983) Subjacency in morphology: Salish double reduplications. Natural Language &
Linguistic Theory, 1, 317–346.
Broselow, Ellen (1984). Default consonants in Amharic morphology. In M. Speas and R. Sproat (eds.), Papers
from the January 1984 MIT Workshop in Morphology (pp. 15– 32). Cambridge MA: MIT Dept. of
Linguistics.
Broselow, Ellen & John McCarthy (1983–4) A theory of internal reduplication. The Linguistic Review, 3, 25–88.
Buckley, Eugene (1994) Alignment in Manam stress. Ms., University of Pennsylvania. ROA-56, Rutgers
Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
86
McCarthy & Prince
Buller, Barbara, Ernest Buller, and Daniel Everett (1993) Stress Placement, Syllable Structure, and Minimality
in Banawa. IJAL 59:3, 280-293.
Bullock, Barbara (1990) V/C planar segregation and the CVC syllable in Sierra Miwok nonconcatenative
morphology. In Michael Ziolkowski et al. (eds.), Papers from the 26th Regional Meeting of the Chicago
Linguistic Society, Volume 2: The Parasession on the Syllable in Phonetics and Phonology (pp.
17–32). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Burzio, Luigi (1996) Multiple Correspondence. Paper presented at the BCN Workshop on Conflicting
Constraints, Groningen, July 5, 1996. Ms. Johns Hopkins University: Baltimore.
Cabré, Teresa and Michael Kenstowicz (195) Prosodic Trapping in Catalan. ROA-32, Rutgers Optimality
Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. Linguistic Inquiry 26.4, 694-705.
Carleton, Troi & Scott Myers (1994) On tonal transfer. ROA-16, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. To appear in Proceedings of CAL 1993.
Carleton, Troi & Scott Myers (1995)Tonal transfer in Chichewa. To appear in Phonology.
Carrier, Jill (1979) The Interaction of Phonological and Morphological Rules in Tagalog: A Study in the
Relationship between Rule Components in Grammar. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Carrier-Duncan, Jill (1984) Some problems with prosodic accounts of reduplication. In Mark Aronoff and
Richard Oehrle (eds.), Language Sound Structure (pp. 260–286). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chaski, Carole E. (1986) Linear and metrical analyses of Manam stress. Oceanic Linguistics, 25, 167-209.
Chiang, Wen-yu (1992) The Prosodic Morphology and Phonology of Affixation in Taiwanese and Other
Chinese Languages. Doctoral dissertation, University of Delaware.
Cho, Young-mee Yu (1992) A phonological constraint on the attachment of particles in Korean. To appear in
Harvard Studies in Korean Linguistics, 4.
Chung, Sandra (1978) Case marking and grammatical relation in Polynesian. Austin: University of Texas
Press.
Clements, G. N. (1985a) The problem of transfer in nonlinear morphology. Cornell Working Papers in
Linguistics, 7, 38–73.
Clements, G. N. (1985b). The geometry of phonological features. Phonology Yearbook, 2, 225– 252.
Clements, G. N. & Kevin Ford (1979) Kikuyu tone shift and its synchronic consequences. Linguistic Inquiry,
10, 179–210.
Clements, G. N. & S. J. Keyser (1983) CV Phonology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cohn, Abigail C. (1992) The consequences of dissimilation in Sundanese. Working Papers of the Cornell
Phonetics Laboratory, 7, 17–40. [Revised version appeared (1992) in Phonology, 9, 199–220.]
Cohn, Abigail & John McCarthy (1994) Alignment and parallelism in Indonesian phonology. Ms., Cornell
University and University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Pp. 73. ROA-25, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.roa
Cole, Jennifer Fitzpatrick (1990) The minimal word in Bengali. In Aaron Halpern (ed.), The Proceedings of the
Ninth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 157–170). Stanford, CA: Stanford
Linguistics Association.
Cole, Jennifer Fitzpatrick (1991) Phrasal reduplication in Bengali. To appear in Proceedings of the Third Leiden
Conference of Junior Linguists. Leiden: University of Leiden.
Cowper, Elizabeth & Keren Rice (1985) Phonology and reduplication. Ms., University of Toronto. Presented at
June, 1985 Canadian Linguistic Association meeting.
Cowper, Elizabeth & Keren Rice (1987) Are phonosyntactic rules necessary? Phonology Yearbook, 4, 185–194.
Crowhurst, Megan (1991a) Minimality and Foot Structure in Metrical Phonology and Prosodic Morphology.
Doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, Tucson.
Crowhurst, Megan (1991b) Demorafication in Tübatulabal: Evidence from initial reduplication and stress. In Tim
Sherer (ed.), Proceedings of North East Linguistic Society 21 (pp. 49–64). Amherst, MA: Graduate
Linguistic Student Association, University of Massachusetts.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
87
Crowhurst, Megan (1992a) Diminutives and augmentatives in Mexican Spanish: A prosodic analysis.
Phonology, 9, 221–253.
Crowhurst, Megan (1992b) The minimal foot and epenthesis in Sierra Miwok. Ms., University of Texas, Austin.
Crowhurst, Megan (to appear) Foot extrametricality and template mapping in Cupeño. Natural Language &
Linguistic Theory.
Crowley, Terry (1983). Uradhi. In R. M. W. Dixon & B. Blake (eds.), The Handbook of Australian Languages
vol. 3. Melbourne: OUP Australia.
Davis, Stuart (1985a). Syllable weight in some Australian languages. In M. Niepokuj, M. VanClay, V.
Nikiforidou & D. Feder (eds.), Papers from BLS 11 (pp. 398– 407). Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics
Society.
Davis, Stuart (1985b). Topics in syllable geometry. Doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, Tucson.
Davis (1986) Published as Davis (1988a).
Davis, Stuart (1988a) On the nature of internal reduplication. In Michael Hammond & Michael Noonan (eds.),
Theoretical Morphology: Approaches in Modern Linguistics (pp. 305–323). San Diego: Academic
Press.
Davis, Stuart (1988b). Syllable onsets as a factor in stress rules. Phonology, 5, 1– 19.
Davis, Stuart (1990) Evidence for the full copy analysis of reduplication. Ms., Indiana University, Bloomington.
de Haas, Wim (1988). A Formal Theory of Vowel Coalescence: A Case Study of Ancient Greek. Publications
in Language Sciences 30. Dordrecht: Foris.
de Reuse, Willem J. (n.d.) The derivation of Spanish hypocoristics in a nonconcatenative theory of morphology.
Ms., University of Texas, Austin.
de Lacy, Paul (1996) Circumscription revisited: An analysis of Maori reduplication. ROA-133, Rutgers
Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Dell, François (1985) A propos de Svantesson, J.-O., Kammu Phonology and Morphology. Cahiers de
Linguistique Asie Orientale, 14, 259–275.
Dell, François & Mohamed Elmedlaoui (1992) Quantitative transfer in the nonconcatenative morphology of
Imdlawn Tashlhiyt Berber. Journal of Afroasiatic Languages, 3, 89-125.
DeCamp, David (1974) Neutralizations, iteratives, and ideophones: The locus of language in Jamaica. In David
Decamp & Ian Hancock (eds.), Pidgins and Creoles: Current Trends and Prospects. Washington, DC:
Georgetown University Press.
Dixon, R. M. W. (1972) The Dyirbal Language of North Queensland. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, R. M. W. (1977) A Grammary of Yidiny. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, R. M. W. (1988) A Grammar of Boumaa Fijian. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Doron, Edit (1981) Spirantization melodies: A prosodic treatment of Modern Hebrew spirantization. Ms.,
University of Texas, Austin.
Downing, Laura (1994). SiSwati verbal reduplication and the theory of Generalized Alignment. In Mercè
Gonzàlez (ed.) Proceedings of the North East Linguistic Society 24, 81-95. Amherst, MA: Graduate
Student Linguistic Association.
Downing, Laura J. (1994) SiSwati verbal reduplication and the theory of Generalized Alignment. In Mercè
Gonzàlez (ed.), Proceedings of the North-East Linguistics Society 24. Pp. 81-95. Amherst, MA:
Graduate Linguistic Student Association.
Downing, Laura J. (1995a) On the prosodic misalignment of onsetless syllables. Ms., University of Pennsylvania.
Downing, Laura J. (1995b) On the extraprosodicity of onsetless syllables. Ms., University of Pennsylvania.
Downing, Laura J. (1996) Prosodic misalignment and reduplication. Ms., University of Pennsylvania. Presented
at January, 1996, LSA meeting.
Dresher, B. Elan (1983). Postlexical phonology in Tiberian Hebrew. In M. Barlow, D. Flickinger & M. Wescoat
(eds.), Proceedings of WCCFL 2 (pp. 67– 78). Stanford: Stanford Linguistics Association.
88
McCarthy & Prince
Dunlap, Elaine (1991) Issues in the Moraic Structure of Spanish. Doctoral dissertation, University of
Massachusetts, Amherst.
Duryea, David (1991) Issues in Thai template morphology: Word chains and word reversal. Phonology at Santa
Cruz, 2, 33–58.
Emeneau, Murray (1955) The Kolami Language. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Everett, Dan & K. Everett (1984). On the relevance of syllable onsets to stress placement. Linguistic Inquiry,
15, 705– 711.
Everett, Dan & Lucy Seki (1985) Reduplication and CV skeleta in Kamaiurá. Linguistic Inquiry, 16, 326–330.
Finer, Daniel (1985) Reduplication and verbal morphology in Palauan. The Linguistic Review, 6, 99–130.
Flemming, Edward & Michael Kenstowicz (1995) Base-identity and uniform exponence: Alternatives to
cyclicity. Ms., MIT.
Fleisch, Henri (1968) L'arabe classique. Beirut: Khayats.
Freeland, Lynn S. (1951) The Language of the Sierra Miwok. International Journal of American Linguistics,
Memoir 6. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University.
French, Koleen Matsuda (1988) Insights into Tagalog Reduplication, Infixation and Stress from Non-Linear
Phonology. MA Thesis, University of Texas, Arlington.
Gafos, Adamantios (1995) On the proper characterization of ‘nonconcatenative’ languages. Ms., Johns Hopkins
University. ROA-106, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Gafos, Adamantios (1996) The Articulatory Basis of Locality in Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, The Johns
Hopkins University.
Goldsmith, John (1976) Autosegmental Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Goldsmith, John (1990) Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Golston, Chris (1991) Minimal word, minimal affix. In Tim Sherer (ed.), Proceedings of the North East
Linguistic Society 21 (pp. 95–110). Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association, University
of Massachusetts.
Goodman (1983) Revised and published as Goodman (1988).
Goodman, Beverley (1988) Takelma verbal morphology. In James Blevins & Juli Carter (eds.), Proceedings of
NELS 18 (pp. 156–174). Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association, University of
Massachusetts.
Goodman, Beverley (1995) Features in Ponapean Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca,
NY.
Goswami, Upendra (1955-6) Onomatopoetic and echo-words in Kamrupi. Indian Linguistics 17, 161–4.
Grimshaw, Jane (1985) [On dative verbs]. Ms., Brandeis University.
Guerssel, Mohamed & Jean Lowenstamm (1994) Talk presented at Second Colloquium on Afro-Asiatic
Linguistics, Sophia Antipolis, France.
Haeberlin, H. (1918) Types of reduplication in the Salish dialects. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 1, 239-253.
Hale, Kenneth (1973) Deep-surface canonical disparities in relation to analysis and change: An Australian
example. Current Trends in Linguistics, 11, 401–458.
Hale, Kenneth & Abanel Lacayo Blanco (1989) Diccionario Elemental del Ulwa (Sumu Meridional).
Cambridge, MA: Center for Cognitive Science, MIT.
Hale, Kenneth & J. White Eagle (1980) A preliminary metrical account of Winnebago accent. International
Journal of American Linguistics, 46, 117– 132.
Halle, Morris & G.N. Clements (1983) Problem Book in Phonology: A Workbook for Introductory Courses in
Linguistics and in Modern Phonology. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Halle, Morris & Michael Kenstowicz (1991) The Free Element Condition and cyclic versus noncyclic stress.
Linguistic Inquiry, 22, 457– 501.
Halle, Morris & Jean-Roger Vergnaud (1978) Metrical structures in phonology. Ms., MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
89
Halle, Morris & Jean-Roger Vergnaud (1980) Three-dimensional phonology. JLR, 1, 83– 105.
Hammond, Michael (1988) Templatic transfer in Arabic broken plurals. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory,
6, 247–270.
Hammond, Michael (1990) The `name game' and onset simplification. Phonology, 7, 159–162.
Hammond, Michael (1991) Morphemic circumscription. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of
Morphology (pp.195-210). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Hammond, Michael (1993) Language games and prosodic circumscription. Ms., University of Arizona, Tucson.
Harms, Robert T. (1966) The measurement of phonological economy. Language, 42, 602-611.
Harris, James (1979) Some observations on ``Substantive principles in Natural Phonology''. In Daniel Dinnsen
(ed.), Current Approaches to Phonological Theory. Bloomington: Indiana Univ. Press.
Harris, James (1985) Spanish word markers. In F. H. Nuessel, Jr. (ed), Current Issues in Spanish Phonology
and Morphology. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club.
Harrison, Sheldon (1976) Mokilese reference grammar. With the assistance of Salich Y. Albert. Honolulu:
University of Hawaii Press.
Harrison, Sheldon & Salich Y. Albert (1976) Mokilese-English Dictionary. University Press of Hawaii,
Honolulu.
Hart, George (1981) Nasality and the organization of autosegmental phonology. Bloomington: Indiana University
Linguistics Club.
Hayes, Bruce (1980) A metrical theory of stress rules. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA. Distributed
by the Indiana University Linguistics Club. New York: Garland, 1985.
Hayes, Bruce (1982a) Extrametricality and English stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 13, 227-276.
Hayes, Bruce (1982b) Metrical structure as the organizing principle in Yidiny phonology. In Harry van der Hulst
& Norval Smith (eds.), The Structure of Phonological Representations, Part 1 (pp. 97–110).
Dordrecht: Foris.
Hayes, Bruce (1983) Talk presented at Sloan Conference on Phonology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Hayes, Bruce (1985) Iambic and trochaic rhythm in stress rules. In M. Niepokuj et. al. (eds.), Proceedings of the
Thirteenth Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 429–446). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley
Linguistics Society.
Hayes, Bruce (1986) Inalterability in CV phonology. Language, 62, 321– 251.
Hayes, Bruce (1987) A revised parametric metrical theory. In J. McDonough & B. Plunkett (eds.), Proceedings
of NELS 17 (pp. 274–289). Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association, University of
Massachusetts.
Hayes, Bruce (1989) Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 253–306.
Hayes, Bruce (1991) Published in expanded form as Hayes (1995).
Hayes, Bruce (1995) Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Hayes, Bruce & May Abad (1989) Reduplication and syllabification in Ilokano. Lingua, 77, 331–374.
Hayward, R. J. (1988) In defence of the skeletal tier. Studies in African Linguistics, 19, 131–172.
Healey, P. M. (1960) An Agta Grammar. Manila: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Healey, Alan, Ambrose Isoroembo, & Martin Chittleborough (1969) Preliminary Notes on Orokaiva Grammar,
Papers in New Guinea Linguistics 9, 33-64.
Heath, Jeffrey (1981) Tübatulabal phonology. In G. N Clements (ed.), Harvard Studies in Phonology II.
Bloomington: Indiana University Lingustics Club.
Henderson, John (forthcoming) Number-marking in Arrernte verbs. Ms., University of Western Australia. Paper
presented at Australian Linguistic Society Annual Conference, Macquarie University, North Ryde, 1990.
Hess, T. (1966) Snohomish chameleon morphology, International Journal of American Linguistics, 32,
350-356.
90
McCarthy & Prince
Hewitt, Mark (1992) Vertical Maximization and Metrical Theory. Doctoral dissertation, Brandeis University,
Waltham, MA.
Hewitt, Mark (1995) Single segment reduplication in Optimality Theory. Ms., UBC/Brandeis University.
Hewitt, Mark & Alan Prince (1989) OCP, locality, and linking: The N. Karanga verb. In E. Jane Fee & K. Hunt
eds., Proceedings of the Eighth WCCFL (pp. 176–191). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics
Association.
Hill, Jane (1970) A peeking rule in Cupeño. Linguistic Inquiry, 1, 534–539.
Hill, Jane & Ofelia Zepeda (1992) Derived words in Tohono O'odham. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 58, 355–404.
Hoberman, Robert (1988) Local and long-distance spreading in Semitic morphology. Natural Language &
Linguistic Theory, 6, 541–550.
Hoeksema, Jack (1985) Categorial Morphology. New York: Garland Publishing.
Hohepa, Patrick W. (1967) A Profile Generative Grammar of Maori. International Journal of American
Linguistics, Memoir 20, Indiana University Publications in Anthropology and Linguistics. Bloomington,
IN: Indiana University.
Hualde, Jose (1988). Affricates are not contour segments. In H. Borer (ed.), Proceedings of WCCFL 7 (pp. 77–
89). Stanford: Stanford Linguistics Association.
Huffman, Franklin (1972) The boundary between the monosyllable and the disyllable in Cambodian. Lingua, 29,
54–66.
Hung, Henrietta (1992) Relativized suffixation in Choctaw: A constraint-based analysis of the verb grade system.
Ms., Brandeis University.
Hyman, Larry (1970) How concrete is phonology? Language, 48, 58-76.
Hyman, Larry (1972) A Phonological Study of FeFe Bamileke. Studies in African Linguistics, Supplement
4.
Hyman, Larry (1985) A Theory of Phonological Weight. Dordrecht: Foris.
Ingria, Robert (1980) Compensatory lengthening as a metrical phenomenon. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 465– 495.
Inkelas, Sharon (1989) Prosodic Constituency in the Lexicon. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University,
Stanford, CA.
Inkelas, Sharon (1990) Prosodic replacement in Modern Hebrew. In Michael Ziolkowski et al. (eds.), Papers
from the 26th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Volume 2: The Parasession on the
Syllable in Phonetics and Phonology (pp. 197–212). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Innes, Gordon (1966) An Introduction to Grebo. The Hague: Mouton.
Itô, Junko (1986) Syllable Theory in Prosodic Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts,
Amherst.
Itô, Junko (1989) A prosodic theory of epenthesis. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 7, 217–260.
Itô, Junko (1991) Prosodic minimality in Japanese. In Karen Deaton, Manuela Noske & Michael Ziolkowski
(eds.), CLS 26-II: Papers from the Parasession on the Syllable in Phonetics and Phonology (pp.
213–240). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Itô, Junko & Jorge Hankamer (1989) Notes on monosyllabism in Turkish. Phonology at Santa Cruz, 1, 61–70.
Itô, Junko, Yoshihisa Kitagawa & R. Armin Mester (1992) Prosodic type preservation in Japanese: Evidence
from zuuja-go. SRC–92–05, Syntax Research Center, UC Santa Cruz. ROA-99, Rutgers Optimality
Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. Published in revised form as Itô, Kitagawa, Mester (1996).
Itô, Junko, Yoshihisa Kitagawa & R. Armin Mester (1996) Prosodic Faithfulness and Correspondence: Evidence
from a Japanese Argot. ROA-146, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Journal of East Asian Linguistics 5, 217-294.
Itô, Junko & Ralf-Armin Mester (1992) Weak layering and word binarity. Ms., University of California, Santa
Cruz.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
91
Jacobsen, William. (1964) A Grammar of the Washo Language, Doctoral dissertation, University of California,
Berkeley.
Jakobson, Roman (1962) Selected Writings I: Phonological Studies. Expanded Edition. The Hague: Mouton.
[Reference is to: Typological studies and their contribution to historical comparative linguistics. Report
in the First Plenary Session of the Eighth International Congress of Linguists, Oslo, 5 August 1957.]
Janda, Richard & Brian Joseph (1986) One rule or many? Sanskrit reduplication as fragmented affixation. Ohio
State University Working Papers in Linguistics, 34, 84–107.
Jensen, John T. (1977). Yapese Reference Grammar. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.
Jespersen, Otto (1928-) A modern English grammar on historical principles. London: G. Allen & Unwin, ltd.
Jun, Jongho (1994) Metrical weight consistency in Korean partial reduplication. Phonology 11, 69–88.
Kager, René (1989) A Metrical Theory of Stress and Destressing in English and Dutch. Dordrecht: Foris.
Kager, René (1992) Are there any truly quantity-insensitive systems?. In Laura Buszard-Welcher, Lionell Wee
& William Weigel (eds.) Proceedings of BLS 18 (pp. 123-132). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics
Society.
Kager, René (1993a) Alternatives to the iambic-trochaic law. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory.
Kager, René (1993b) Consequences of Catalexis. Proceedings of HILP-1.
Kager, René (1993c) Shapes of the generalized trochee. In Jonathan Mead (ed.), Proceedings of WCCFL 11 (pp.
298-312). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics Association.
Kager, René (1996a) Stem disyllabicity in Guugu Yimidhirr. OTS-WP-TL-96-008. ROA-70, Rutgers Optimality
Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. To appear in HIL Phonology Papers II, H. van der Hulst and
Jeroen van de Weijer, eds. HIL: Leiden.
Kager, René (1996b) On affix-allomorphy and syllable counting. OTS-WP-TL-96-006. ROA-88, Rutgers
Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. To appear in Interfaces in Phonology, T. Hall and
U. Kleinhenz, eds.
Katz, Graham (1991) A prosodic circumscription analysis of /gi/-words in Kannada. Term paper for Linguistics
240, 1991 LSA Linguistic Institute Course, University of California, Santa Cruz.
Kaufman, Terrence (1971) Tzeltal Phonology and Morphology. University of California Publications in
Linguistics 61. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Kaye, Jonathan & Jean Lowenstamm (1981/1984) De la syllabicité. In Forme sonore du langage : structure des
representations en phonologie, F. Dell, D. Hirst, J.-R. Vergnaud, directeurs de la publication. Paris:
Hermann, 1984. [Ms. UQAM 2, 1981].
Kenstowicz, Michael (1977) Rule application in pregenerative American phonology. In Andreas Koutsoudas
(ed.), The ordering and application of phonological rules (pp. 259-282). The Hague: Mouton. [Repr.
in F. Koerner, ed., (1976) The transformational-generative paradigm and modern linguistic theory.
Amsterdam: Benjamins, 141–174.]
Kenstowicz, Michael (1979). Chukchee vowel harmony and epenthesis. ROA-30, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. In P. Clyne, W. Hanks & C. Hofbauer (eds.), CLS 15: The Elements:
Parasession on Linguistic Units and Level (pp. 402– 412). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Kenstowicz, Michael (1981). Vowel harmony in Palestinian Arabic: A suprasegmental analysis. Linguistics, 19,
449-465.
Kenstowicz, Michael (1995) Base-identity and uniform exponence: Alternatives to cyclicity. Ms., MIT. ROA105, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Kim, Young-Seok (1984) Aspects of Korean morphology. Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
Kiparsky, Paul (1982) Lexical phonology and morphology. In I. S. Yang (ed.), Linguistics in the Morning Calm
vol. 2 (pp. 3– 91). Seoul: Hanshin.
Kiparsky, Paul (1986) The Phonology of Reduplication. Ms., Stanford University.
Kiparsky, Paul (1992) Catalexis. Ms., Stanford University.
92
McCarthy & Prince
Kirchner, Robert (1992) Lardil truncation and augmentation: A morphological account. Ms., University of
Maryland, College Park.
Kitagawa, Yoshihisa (1985) Published as Kitagawa (1987).
Kitagawa, Yoshihisa (1987) Redoing reduplication: A preliminary sketch. In Geoffrey J. Huck & Almerindo E.
Ojeda (eds.), Discontinuous Constituency (Syntax and Semantics, Volume 20). Orlando: Academic
Press.
Klokeid, Terry (1976) Topics in Lardil Grammar. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Kroeger, Paul (1989a) Discontinuous reduplication in vernacular Malay. Paper presented at Berkeley Linguistics
Society Meeting, February 20, 1989.
Kroeger, Paul (1989b) On the nature of reduplicative templates. Ms., Stanford University.
Lamontagne, Greg (1989) Suffix-triggered variation in Southern Sierra Miwok. Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Lamontagne, Greg & Keren Rice (1995) A correspondence account of coalescence. In Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh
Dickey, & Suzanne Urbanczyk, eds., University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics
18: Papers in Optimality Theory. Pp. 211-224.
Laycock, D. (1969) Sublanguages in Buin: Play, poetry, and preservation. In D. Laycock, R. Lloyd & P. Staalsen
(eds.), Papers in New Guinea Linguistics 10, Pacific Linguistics, Series A, No. 22. Canberra: Australian
National University.
Lee, Borim (1984) A Study on the Autosegmental Tiers - through Akan Reduplication. Master’s paper,
University of Texas, Austin.
Lee, Borim (forthcoming [1991]) Prosodic Structures in Takelma Phonology and Morphology. Doctoral
dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
Lee, Jin-Seong & Stuart Davis (1993) A prosodic analysis of infixing reduplication in Korean ideophones. Ms.,
Indiana University.
Leslau, Wolf (1950) Ethiopic Documents: Gurage. Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology 14. New York:
Viking Fund.
Leslau, Wolf (1964) The jussive in Chaha. Language, 40, 53–57.
Levelt, Clara (1990) Reduplication in Samoan: how to behave as a plural and other good manners. In John van
Lit, René Mulder & Rint Sybesma (eds), Proceedings of the 2nd Leiden Conference for Junior
Linguists, University of Leiden, Leiden.
Levergood (1983) Ms., University of Texas, Austin.
Levergood, Barbara (1987) Topics in Arusa Phonology and Morphology. Doctoral dissertation, University of
Texas, Austin.
Levin, Juliette (1983) Reduplication and prosodic structure. Ms., MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Levin, Juliette (1985) A Metrical Theory of Syllabicity. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Levin, Juliette (1989) The autonomy of the skeleton: Evidence from Micronesian. Ms., University of Texas,
Austin.
Lichtenberk, František (1983) A Grammar of Manam. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
Lombardi, Linda (1990). The nonlinear organization of the affricate. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory,
8, 375– 425.
Lombardi, Linda & John McCarthy (1991) Prosodic circumscription in Choctaw morphology. Phonology, 8,
37–71.
Lowenstamm, Jean & Jonathan Kaye (1986) Compensatory lengthening in Tiberian Hebrew. In L. Wetzels &
E. Sezer (eds.), Studies in Compensatory Lengthening (pp. 97–132). Dordrecht: Foris.
Marantz, Alec (1982) Re reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry, 13, 435–482.
Marantz, Alec (1987) Phonologically induced bracketing paradoxes in full morpheme reduplication. In Megan
Crowhurst (ed.), Proceedings of the Sixth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp.
203–212). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics Association.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
93
Marlett, Stephen & Joseph Stemberger (1983). Empty consonants in Seri. Linguistic Inquiry, 5, 617– 639.
Marsack, C. C. (1962) Teach Yourself Samoan. London: Hodder and Stoughton.
Martin, Jack (1989) Infixation and extrametricality in prosodic morphology: Three rules from Creek. Ms.,
University of North Texas.
McCarthy, John (1979) Formal Problems in Semitic Phonology and Morphology. Doctoral dissertation, MIT,
Cambridge, MA.
McCarthy, John (1981) A prosodic theory of nonconcatentive morphology. Linguistic Inquiry, 12, 373–418.
McCarthy, John (1982) Prosodic templates, morphemic templates, and morphemic tiers. In H. van der Hulst &
N. Smith (eds.), The Structure of Phonological Representations, Part 1. Dordrecht: Foris.
McCarthy, John (1983a) A prosodic account of Arabic broken plurals. In Ivan Dihoff (ed.), Current Trends in
African Linguistics 1 (pp. 289–320). Dordrecht: Foris.
McCarthy, John (1983b) Morpheme Form and Phonological Representation. Paper presented at Sloan
Conference on Hierarchy and Constituency in Phonology. University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
McCarthy, John (1984a) Ponapean reduplication. Ms., University of Texas, Austin.
McCarthy, John (1984b) Speech disguise and phonological representation in Amharic. In H. van der Hulst & N.
Smith (eds.), Advances in Nonlinear Phonology (pp. 305–312). Dordrecht: Foris.
McCarthy, John (1984c) Prosodic organization in morphology. In M. Aronoff and R. Oehrle (eds.), Language
Sound Structure (pp.299-317). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
McCarthy, John (1985) Features and tiers: The structure of Semitic roots. Talk presented at MIT, Cambridge,
MA.
McCarthy, John (1986) OCP Effects: Gemination and antigemination. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 207– 263.
McCarthy, John (1989) Linear order in phonological representation. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 71-99.
McCarthy, John (1991) L'infixation réduplicative dans les langages secrets. Langages, 101, 11–29.
McCarthy, John (1993) Template form in prosodic morphology. In Laurel Smith Stvan et al. (eds.), Papers from
the Third Annual Formal Linguistics Society of Midamerica Conference. Bloomington, IN: Indiana
University Linguistics Club.
McCarthy, John (1996) Extensions of faithfulness: Rotuman revisited. Ms., University of Massachusetts,
Amherst. ROA-110, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1988) Quantitative transfer in reduplicative and templatic morphology. In
Linguistic Society of Korea (ed.), Linguistics in the Morning Calm, 2 (pp. 3–35). Seoul: Hanshin
Publishing Co.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1990a) Foot and word in prosodic morphology: The Arabic broken plurals.
Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 8, 209–282.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1990b) Prosodic morphology and templatic morphology. In M. Eid & J.
McCarthy (eds.), Perspectives on Arabic Linguistics: Papers from the Second Symposium (pp. 1–54).
Amsterdam: Benjamins, Amsterdam.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1991a) Prosodic minimality. Paper presented at University of Illinois Conference
The Organization of Phonology.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1991b) Linguistics 240: Prosodic Morphology. Lectures and handouts from
1991 LSA Linguistic Institute Course, University of California, Santa Cruz.
McCarthy, John and Alan Prince (1993a) Prosodic Morphology I: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction.
Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst, and Rutgers University.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1993b) Generalized alignment. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.),
Yearbook of Morphology 1993 (pp. 79-153). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1994a) The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in Prosodic Morphology.
ROA- 13. Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. In Mercè Gonzàlez (ed.),
Proceedings of the North East Linguistics Society 24 (pp. 333-379). Amherst, MA: Graduate
Linguistic Student Association.
94
McCarthy & Prince
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1994b) Two Lectures on Prosodic Morphology. The Utrecht Workshop on
Prosodic Morphology. ROA-59, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1995a) Prosodic Morphology. In John Goldsmith (ed.), A Handbook of
Phonological Theory (pp. 318-366). Basil Blackwell, Oxford.
McCarthy, John & Alan Prince (1995b) Faithfulness and Reduplicative Identity. ROA-60, Rutgers Optimality
Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. In Jill Beckman, Suzanne Urbanczyk, & Laura Walsh, (eds.)
University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18: Papers in Optimality Theory (pp.
249-384). Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.
McCawley, James (1969) Length and voicing in Tübatulabal.Proceedings of the CLS 5.
McDonough, Joyce (1990) Topics in the Phonology and Morphology of Navajo. Doctoral dissertation,
University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
McNally, Louise (1990) Multiplanar reduplication: Evidence from Sesotho. In Aaron Halpern (ed.), The
Proceedings of the Ninth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 331–346). Stanford, CA:
Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics Association, CA.
Merlan, Francesca (1982) Mangarayi. Lingua Descriptive Series 4. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Mester, Ralf-Armin (1986) Studies in Tier Structure. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts,
Amherst.
Mester, Ralf-Armin (1990) Patterns of truncation. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 478–485.
Mester, Ralf-Armin (1995) The quantitative trochee in Latin. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 12, 1-61.
Michelson, Karin (1981) Stress, epenthesis, and syllable structure in Mohawk. In G. N. Clements (ed.), Harvard
Studies in Phonology II. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.
Miner, Kenneth L. (1979) Dorsey’s Law in Winnebago-Chiwere and Winnebago Accent. International Journal
of American Linguistics, 45, 25-33.
Montler, Timothy (1989) Infixation, reduplication, and metathesis in the Saanich actual aspect. Southwest
Journal of Linguistics, 9(1).
Montler, Timothy R. & Heather K. Hardy (1988) Imperfective gemination in Alabama. International Journal
of American Linguistics, 54, 399–415.
Montler, Timothy R. & Heather K. Hardy (1991) The phonology of negation in Alabama. International Journal
of American Linguistics, 57, 1–23.
Moore, John (1989) Doubled verbs in Modern Standard Arabic. Phonology at Santa Cruz, 1, 93–124.
Moravcsik, Edith (1977) On Rules of Infixing. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistics Club.
Moravcsik, Edith (1978) Reduplicative constructions. In Joseph Greenberg, Charles Ferguson & Edith
Moravcsik (eds.), Universals of Human Language 3: Word Structure (pp. 297–334). Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Morris, Mitzi (1989) Swedish nickname formation. Phonology at Santa Cruz, 1, 125–171.
Munro, Pamela & P. Benson (1973) Reduplication and rule ordering in Luiseño. International Journal of
American Linguistics, 39, 15–21.
Mutaka, Ngessimo & Larry Hyman (1990) Syllable and morpheme integrity in Kinande reduplication.
Phonology, 7, 73–120.
Myers, Scott (1987) Tone and the Structure of Words in Shona. Doctoral dissertation, University of
Massachusetts, Amherst.
Nash, David (1979-80) Yidiny stress: a metrical account. Cunyforum, 7/8, 112–130.
Nash, David (1980) Topics in Warlpiri Grammar. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA. New York:
Garland, 1986.
Newman, Stanley (1944) The Yokuts Language of California. New York: Viking Fund Publications in
Anthropology.
Newman, Stanley (1947) Bella Coola I: Phonology. International Journal of American Linguistics, 13,
129-134.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
95
Newman, Stanley (1965) Zuni Grammar. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press.
Nicklas, Thurston (1974) The Elements of Choctaw. Doctoral dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
Nicklas, Thurston (1975) Choctaw morphophonemics. In James Crawford (ed.), Studies in Southeastern Indian
Languages. Athens, GA: University of Georgia Press.
Nivens, Richard (1992) A lexical phonology of West Tarangan. In Donald Burquest & Wyn Laidig (eds.),
Phonological Studies in Four Languages of Maluku. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Noske, Manuela (1991) Metrical structure and reduplication in Turkana. In M. Lionel Bender (ed.), Proceedings
of the Fourth Nilo-Saharan Linguistics Colloquium (pp. 245-262). Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag.
Odden, David & Mary Odden (1985) Ordered reduplication in Kihehe. Linguistic Inquiry, 16, 497–503.
Ola, Olanike Olajumoke (1995) Optimality in Benue-Congo Prosodic Phonology and Morphology. Doctoral
Dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.
Orgun, C. Orhan (1995) Correspondence and Identity constraints in two-level OT. WCCFL. ROA-62, Rutgers
Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Orgun, C. Orhan (1996) Sign-based morphology: A declarative theory of phonology-morphology interleaving.
Ms. UC Berkeley. ROA-62, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Orgun, C. Orhan & Sharon Inkelas (1992) Turkish prosodic minimality. Paper presented at 6th International
Conference on Turkish Linguistics, Anadolu University, Eski·ehir, Turkey. August, 1992.
Padgett, Jaye & Máire Ní Chiosáin (1995) Labial attraction in Igbo. Handout of talk presented at Santa Cruz
phonology workshop, April 15, 1995.
Pater, Joe (1995) Austronesian nasal substitution and other NC effects. Ms., McGill Univesity. To appear in
René Kager, Harry van der Hulst, and Wim Zonneveld, eds., The Prosody Morphology Interface. ROA92, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Payne, David (1981) The Phonology and Morphology of Axininca Campa. Arlington, TX: Summer Institute
of Linguistics.
Perlmutter, David (1992) Pervasive word formation patterns in a grammar. Paper presented at BLS.
Piggott, Glyne (1992) Satisfying the minimal word. Ms., McGill University, Montréal.
Plénat, Marc (1984) Toto, fanfa, totor et même guiguitte sont des anars. In François Dell et al. (eds.),
Forme sonore du langage (pp. 161–182). Paris: Hermann.
Plénat, Marc (1985) Morphologie du Largonji des loucherbems. Langages, 78, 73-122.
Plénat, Marc, Ed., (1991) Les javanais. Langages, 101.
Poser, William (1982) Why cases of syllable reduplication are so hard to find. Ms., MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Poser, William (1984a) Hypocoristic formation in Japanese. In Mark Cobler et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the
Third West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 218–229). Stanford, CA: Stanford
Linguistics Association.
Poser, William (1984b) The Phonetics and Phonology of Tone and Intonation in Japanese, Doctoral
dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Poser, William (1986) Diyari stress, metrical structure assignment, and the nature of metrical representation. In
M. Dalrymple et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 5.
Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics Association.
Poser, William (1989) The metrical foot in Diyari. Phonology, 6, 117–148.
Poser, William (1985 [1990]) Evidence for foot stucture in Japanese. Language, 66, 78–105.
Prentice, D. J. (1971) The Murut Languages of Sabah. Pacific Linguistics, Series C, no. 18. Canberra:
Australian National University.
Priest, Perry (1980) Estudios sobre el idioma siriono. Riberalta, Bolivia: Instituto Linguistico de Verano.
Prince, Alan (1975). The phonology and morphology of Tiberian Hebrew. Doctoral dissertation, MIT,
Cambridge, MA.
Prince, Alan (1980) A metrical theory for Estonian quantity. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 511–562.
Prince, Alan (1983) Relating to the grid. Linguistic Inquiry, 14, 19–100.
96
McCarthy & Prince
Prince, Alan (1984) Phonology with tiers. In M. Aronoff & R. Oehrle (eds.), Language Sound Structure (pp.
234-244). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Prince, Alan (1985) Improving tree theory. Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 11, 471-490.
Prince, Alan (1987) Planes and copying. Linguistic Inquiry, 18, 491–510.
Prince, Alan (1991) Quantitative consequences of rhythmic organization. In Karen Deaton, Manuela Noske &
Michael Ziolkowski (eds.), CLS 26–II: Papers from the Parasession on the Syllable in Phonetics and
Phonology (pp. 355–398). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Prince, Alan & Paul Smolensky (1991a) Optimality. Paper given at Arizona phonology conference.
Prince, Alan & Paul Smolensky (1991b) Notes on connectionism and harmony theory in linguistics. In Technical
Report CU–CS–533–91. Department of Computer Science, Univerisity of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado.
Prince, Alan & Paul Smolensky (1992) Optimality: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Paper read at
12th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, Los Angeles.
Prince, Alan & Paul Smolensky (1993) Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar.
Ms., Rutgers University, New Brunswick, and University of Colorado, Boulder.
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1983) Tone in Lexical Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
[Dordrecht: Reidel, 1986.]
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1988) Vocalic underspecification in Yoruba. Linguistic Inquiry, 19, 233-270.
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1994) Underlying mora structure. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 344-353.
Rehg, Kenneth L. & Damien G. Sohl (1981) Ponapean Reference Grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii
Press.
Reichard, Gladys A. (1959) A comparison of five Salish languages: V. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 59, 239-253.
Robins, R. H. (1959) Nominal and verbal derivation in Sundanese. Lingua, 8, 337–369.
Rosenthall, Samuel (1995) The prosodic base of the Hausa plural. In Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, &
Suzanne Urbanczyk, eds., University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18: Papers
in Optimality Theory. Pp. 421-438. Also to appear in René Kager, Harry van der Hulst, and Wim
Zonneveld, eds., The Prosody Morphology Interface.
Rubach, Jerzy (1994) Affricates as strident stops in Polish. Linguistic Inquiry, 25, 119-143.
Sagey, Elizabeth (1986). The representation of features and relations in nonlinear phonology. Doctoral
dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA. New York: Garland Press, 1991.
Samek-Lodovici, Vieri (1992) Universal constraints and morphological gemination: A crosslinguistic study. Ms.,
Brandeis University.
Samek-Lodovici, Vieri (1994) A Unified Analysis of Crosslinguistic Morphological Gemination. In Peter
Ackema & Maaike Schoorlemmer (eds.), Proceedings of CONSOLE I, The Hague: Holland
Academic Graphics, pp.265-283. ROA-149, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Sapir, Edward (1922) The Takelma language of southwestern Oregon. In Franz Boas (ed.), Handbook of
American Indian Languages. Bulletin of American Ethnology, 40(2), 1–296.
Sapir, Edward (1930) Southern Paiute, a Shoshonean Language. Proceedings of the American Academy of
Arts and Sciences, 65, no. 1-3.
Schachter, Paul & Victoria Fromkin (1968) A Phonology of Akan: Akuapem, Asante, and Fante. UCLA
Working Papers in Phonetics 9.
Schein, Barry & Donca Steriade (1986). On geminates. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 691– 744.
Schlindwein, Deborah (1988) The Phonological Geometry of Morpheme Concatenation. Doctoral dissertation, University of Southern California.
Schlindwein, Deborah (1991) Reduplication in lexical phonology: Javanese plural reduplication. The
Linguistic Review, 8, 97–106.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
97
Scullen, Mary Ellen (1993) The Prosodic Morphology of French. Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University,
Bloomington.
Selkirk, Elisabeth (1980a) Prosodic domains in phonology: Sanskrit revisited. In Mark Aronoff & MaryLouise Kean (eds.), Juncture (pp. 107–129). Saratoga, CA: Anma Libri.
Selkirk, Elisabeth (1980b) The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 11,
563–605.
Selkirk, Elisabeth (1981) ``Epenthesis and Degenerate Syllables in Cairene Arabic,'' in H. Borer & J. Aoun
eds., Theoretical Issues in the Grammar of the Semitic Languages, MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Selkirk, Elisabeth (1988) A two-root theory of length. Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Sharvit, Yael (1994) Issues in the Phonology and Morphology of the Modern Hebrew Verbal System:
Alignment Constraints in Verb Formation. ROA-23, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Shaw, Patricia (1980) Dakota Phonology and Morphology. New York: Garland.
Shaw, Patricia (1987) Non-conservation of melodic structure in reduplication. In Anna Bosch et al. (eds.),
Papers from the 23rd Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Part Two:
Parasession on Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology (pp. 291–306). Chicago: Chicago
Linguistic Society.
Shaw, Patricia (1992) Templatic evidence for the syllable nucleus. NELS 23 handout, University of Ottawa.
Sherer, Tim D. (1994) Prosodic Phonotactics. ROA-54, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.
Sherzer, Joel (1970) Talking backwards in Cuna: The sociological reality of phonological descriptions.
Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 26, 343– 353.
Sherzer, Joel (1975) A problem in Cuna phonology. The Journal of the Linguistic Association of the
Southwest, 1, 45– 53.
Sherzer, Joel. (1982) Play languages: With a note on ritual languages. In L.K. Obler and L. Menn (eds.),
Exceptional Language and Linguistics (pp. 175-199). New York: Academic Press.
Shetler, J. (1976) Notes on Balangao Grammar. Summer Institute of Linguistics, Huntington Beach, CA.
Sietsema, Brian (1988) Reduplication in Dakota. In Lynn MacLeod et al. (eds.), Papers from the 24th
Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (pp. 337–352). Chicago: Chicago
Linguistic Society.
Silverman, Daniel (1991) The asymmetrical application of phonology and morphological principles in
reduplication. Ms., UCLA, Los Angeles, CA.
Sloan, Kelly (1988) Bare-consonant reduplication: Implications for a prosodic theory of reduplication. In
Hagit Borer (ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp.
319–330). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistic Association.
Sloan, Kelly (1991) Syllables and Templates: Evidence from Southern Sierra Miwok. Doctoral dissertation,
MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Smith, Norval (1985) Spreading, reduplication, and the default option in Miwok nonconcatenative morphology. In Harry van der Hulst & Norval Smith (eds.), Advances in Nonlinear Phonology (pp.
363–380). Dordrecht: Foris.
Smith, Norval (1986) Reduplication, spreading and/or empty suffix slots in Sierra Miwok associative
morphology. In Hans Bennis & F. Beckema (eds.), Linguistics in the Netherlands 1986 (pp.
235–243). Dordrecht: Foris.
Smith, Norval & Ben Hermans (1982) Nonconcatenatieve woordvorming in het Sierra Miwok. Glot, 5,
263–284.
Sommer, Bruce (1981) The shape of Kunjen syllables. In D. L. Goyvaerts (ed.), Phonology in the 1980's.
Ghent: E. Story-Scientia.
98
McCarthy & Prince
Spring, Cari (1990a) Implications of Axininca Campa for Prosodic Morphology and Reduplication.
Doctoral dissertation, University of Arizona, Tucson.
Spring, Cari (1990b) How many feet per language? In Aaron Halpern (ed.), The Proceedings of the Ninth
West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (pp. 493–508). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics
Association.
Spring, Cari (1990c) Unordered morphology: The problem of Axininca reduplication. In David J. Costa (ed.),
Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 137–157).
Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.
Spring, Cari (1992) Maximization as association. Ms., Ohio State University.
Steriade, Donca (1982). Greek prosodies and the nature of syllabification. Doctoral dissertation, MIT,
Cambridge, MA.
Steriade, Donca (1985) Reduplication, ablaut, and syllabicity in Sanskrit. Ms., MIT, Cambridge, MA.
Steriade, Donca (1986) Yokuts and the vowel plane. Linguistic Inquiry, 17, 129–146.
Steriade, Donca (1987) Reduplication and syllable transfer. Ms., MIT, Cambridge, MA.
.Steriade, Donca (1988) Reduplication and syllable transfer in Sanskrit and elsewhere. Phonology, 5, 73–155.
Steriade, Donca (1990) Gestures and Autosegments: comments on Browman and Goldstein’s paper. In John
Kingston & Mary Beckman, eds., Between the grammar and the physics of speech: Papers in
laboratory phonology 1, pp. 382-397. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Steriade, Donca (1993). Closure, release and nasal contours. In M. Huffman & R. Krakow (eds.), Nasality.
San Diego: Academic Press.
Stevens, Alan (1968) Madurese Phonology and Morphology. American Oriental Society, New Haven.
Stonham, John (1990) Current Issues in Morphological Theory. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University,
Stanford, CA.
Swadesh, Morris & Charles Voegelin (1939) A problem in phonological alternation. Language, 15, 1–10.
Tateishi, Koichi (1985 [1989]) Theoretical implications of the Japanese musician's language. In E. Jane Fee
& Katherine Hunt (eds.), Proceedings of the Eighth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics
(pp. 384–398). Stanford, CA: Stanford Linguistics Association.
Thompson, Laurence & M. Terry Thompson (1981) Affective derivatives in Thompson River Salish.
University of Hawaii Working Papers in Linguistics, 13, 119-127.
Thun, Nils (1963) Reduplicative Words in English; a study of formations of the types tick-tick, hurly-burly,
and shilly-shally. Uppsala.
Topping, D. (1973) Chamorro Reference Grammar. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.
Topping, D. (1980) Spoken Chamorro. Honolulu: University Press of Hawaii.
Tuggy, David H. (1979) Tetelcingo Nahuatl. In Ronald W. Langacker (ed.), Studies in Uto-Aztecan
Grammar Volume 2: Modern Aztec Grammatical Sketches (pp. 1-140). Arlington, TX: SIL.
Turner, Margaret Mary & Gavan Breen (1984) Akarre Rabbit Talk. [NB: Breen identifies the language as
Eastern Arrernte rather than Akarre, p.c.]. Language in Central Australia, no.1, 10-13.
Uhrbach, Amy (1985) Some Facts of Reduplication in Bahasa Indonesia with Implications for a Theoryof
Morphology. Ms., University of Texas, Austin.
Uhrbach, Amy (forthcoming) Published as Uhrbach (1987).
Uhrbach, Amy (forthcoming [1987]) A Formal Analysis of Reduplication and its Interaction with Phonological and Morphological Processes. Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
Ulrich, Charles (1986) Choctaw Morphophonology. Doctoral dissertation, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA.
Ulrich, Charles (1992) A unified account of Choctaw intensives. To appear in Proceedings of the 1992
Canadian Linguistic Association Annual Conference, Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics.
Urbanczyk, Suzanne (1992) Infixing and melodic circumscription. Ms., University of Massachusetts,
Amherst.
Prosodic Morphology — 1986
99
Urbanczyk, Suzanne (1995) Double reduplications in parallel. In Jill Beckman, Laura Walsh Dickey, &
Suzanne Urbanczyk, eds., University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics 18:
Papers in Optimality Theory. Pp. 499-532. Also to appear in René Kager, Harry van der Hulst, and
Wim Zonneveld, eds., The Prosody Morphology Interface. ROA-73, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Urbanczyk, Suzanne (1996a) Morphological templates in reduplication. Proceedings of NELS 26. Amherst,
MA: GLSA.
Urbanczyk, Suzanne (1996b) Patterns of Reduplication in Lushootseed. Doctoral dissertation, UMass
Amherst.
Vago, Robert (1985) The treatment of long vowels in word games. Phonology Yearbook, 2, 327–342. van
der Hulst, Harry (1984) Syllable Structure and Stress in Dutch. Dordrecht: Foris.
Voegelin, Charles (1935) Tübatulabal grammar. University of California Publications in American
Archaeology and Ethnology, 34, 55-190.
Walsh, Laura (1992) Tone in reduplication. In C.P. Canakis, G.P. Chan & J.M. Denton (eds.), Papers from
the 28th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society: Main Session (pp.543-553).
Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
Weeda, Donald (1986 [1987]) Formal properties of Madurese final syllable reduplication. In Anna Bosch et
al. (eds.), Papers from the 23rd Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, Part
Two: Parasession on Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology (pp. 403–417). Chicago: Chicago
Linguistic Society.
Weeda, Donald (1992) Word Truncation in Prosodic Phonology. Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas,
Austin.
Wehr, Hans (1971) A Dictionary of Modern Written Arabic. Trans. by J Milton Cowan. Ithaca, NY: Spoken
Language Services.
Whitney, W. D. (1889). Sanskrit Grammar. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wiesemann, Ursula (1972) Die phonologische und grammatische Struktur der Kaingang-sprache. The
Hague: Mouton.
Wilbur, Ronnie (1974) The Phonology of Reduplication, Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois,
Urbana.
Wilkins, David (1984) Nominal reduplicationin Mparntwe Arrernte. Language in Central Australia, no. 1,
16-22.
Wilkins, David (1989) Mparntwe Arrernte (Aranda): studies in the structure and semantics of grammar,
Doctoral Dissertation, Australian National University: Canberra.
Wilkinson, Karina (1986) Syllable structure and Lardil phonology. Ms., University of Massachusetts,
Amherst.
Wilkinson, Karina (1988) Prosodic structure and Lardil phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 19, 325–334.
Williams, Jeffrey (1984) Phonological conditioning in Nakanai reduplication. Linguistic Analysis, 14, 173190.
Williams, Jeffrey (1991) A note on echo word morphology in Thai and the languages of South and SouthEast Asia. Australian Journal of Linguistics, 11, 107–112.
Winter, Werner (1970) Reduplication in Washo: A restatement. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 36, 190-198.
Woodbury, Anthony (1985) Meaningful phonological processes: A consideration of Central Alaskan Yupik
Eskimo prosody. Ms., University of Texas, Austin.
Yin, Yuen-Mei (1989) Phonological aspects of word formation in Mandarin Chinese. Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas, Austin.
Yip, Moira (1982) Reduplication and CV-skeleta in Chinese secret languages. Linguistic Inquiry, 13,
637–662.
100
McCarthy & Prince
Yip, Moira (1983) Redundancy and the CV-skeleton. Ms., Brandeis University, Waltham, MA.
Yip, Moira (1988) Template morphology and the direction of association. Natural Language & Linguistic
Theory, 6, 551–577.
Yip, Moira (1991) Prosodic morphology of four Chinese dialects. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 1(1).
Yip, Moira (1992) Reduplication with fixed melodic material. In Kimberley Broderick (ed.), NELS 22.
Amherst, MA: Graduate Linguistic Student Association.
Yip, Moira (1993) The interaction of ALIGN, PARSE-Place, and *ECHO in reduplication. ms., University of
California, Irvine. Pp. 17.
Yip, Moira (1995a) Identity avoidance in phonology and morphology. Ms., UC Irvine. To appear in the
Proceedings of the Conference on Morphology and its Relation to Syntax and Phonology, UC Davis,
May, 1995. ROA-82, Rutgers Optimality Archive, http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Yip, Moira (1995b) Repetition and its avoidance: The case of Javanese. Ms., UC Irvine. To appear in K.
Suzuki and D. Elzinga, eds., Arizona Phonology Conference 4: Features in Optimality Theory,
University of Arizona Coyote Working Papers in Linguistics. ROA-83, Rutgers Optimality Archive,
http://ruccs.rutgers.edu/roa.html.
Zec, Draga (1988) Sonority Constraints on Prosodic Structure. Doctoral dissertation, Stanford University,
Stanford, CA.