NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Published in final edited form as:
Behav Med. 1996 ; 21(4): 171–183.
Methods of Testing for Sleeplessness
Merrill M. Mitler, PhD and James C. Miller, PhD, CPE
is director of research of the Division of Sleep Disorders of the Scripps Clinic and Research
Foundation in LaJolla, California, where Dr Miller, a certified professional ergonomist, is a research
scientist with the Laboratory for Sleep, Fatigue and Safety.
Abstract
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Normal nonrandom fluctuations in daily human perfomzance have been documented for years.
Published research reports have shown patterns of workers' errors in reading gas meters, operators'
delays in answering calls, drivers' drowsiness, sleepy locomotive engineers' automatic breaking,
vehicle crashes, deaths resulting from disease, brief periods of sleep, and sleep latency in structured
naps. The authors summarized these data sets and fitted them with a two-peak-per-day cosine curve
derived from the population growth function used in chaos theory. Median parameters extracted from
the curve fits predicted a sharp peak of sleepiness at 2:30 AM and a secondary peak at 2:30 PM. The
shape of the curve was modfied by a nonlinear sleep-deprivation factor. The model appeared to be
biological rather than behavioral or social because it applied well to disease-related deaths. The
authors also review measurement of sleepiness through electroencephalographic monitoring, selfreports, pupillography, and the Multiple Sleep Latency and the Maintenance of Wakefulness Tests.
Keywords
circadian rhythm; electroencephalographic (EEG) monitoring; fatigue; public safety; sleep
deprivation; sleepiness; testing
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
We are all aware of daily ups and downs in our abilities.Such normal fluctuations in
performance have been documented for many years1,2 and are not random throughout the 24hour day 3,4 In 1955, Bjerner reported on diurnal performance fluctuations for employees at
a Swedish gas company whose work was monitored over a 20-year period.2 Figure 1 shows
the distribution throughout the 24-hour day of 74,927 meter-reading errors made by these
workers. Note that more errors occurred during the night, with a major peak between 1 and 3
AM and a smaller afternoon peak in errors between 1 and 3 PM.
Since these early observations, other, more destructive human error events have also been
shown to occur in this same two-peak pattern.5–8 The 24-hour distribution of 6,052 vehicle
crashes attributable to fatigue (ie, crashes for which the investigation disclosed no mechanical
failure and no alcohol or substance-related causal factors) is shown in Figure 2 (data originally
compiled and plotted in Mitler et al5). Note that the two-peak pattern in this distribution is
similar to that in Figure 1. The number of crashes is elevated between about midnight and 6
AM and again between about 1 and 4 PM
In many studies of measures of human performance and physiologic activity, researchers have
found a two-peak diurnal pattern in 24-hour rhythms.5,9,10 Broughton3,4 was the first to bring
this robust characteristic of human performance to the attention of researchers.
For further information, please address correspondence to Merrill M. Mitler, PhD, Director of Research, Division of Sleep Disorders,
Scripps Clinic and Research Foundation, 10666 North Torrey Pines Road, La Jolla, CA 92037..
Mitler and Miller
Page 2
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Evidence for the biological rather than the behavioral nature of this two-peak temporal pattern
is supported by the identical patterns that also exist in timing of human mortality attributable
to disease.11 The temporal distribution of 437,511 deaths resulting from disease that was
compiled by Smolensky et al12 and Mitler et al11 is shown in Figure 3. The strength of the
two-peak pattern is noteworthy in spite of the imprecision that must be associated with the
process of combining deaths from many types of diseases. Breaking down data on diseaserelated deaths according to type of disease, age, and sex of the individuals indicated that the
two-peak temporal pattern is most prominent in deaths resulting from ischemic heart disease
in both men and women over the age of 65.11 Two peaks, one in the early morning (eg, 3–6
,
AM) and a small peak in the early afternoon, can be seen in Figures 1 2, and 3. Researchers who
applied continuous electroencephalographic (EEG) monitoring techniques for measuring
cycles in sleep and wakefulness13 found that physiologic sleep tendency also has its ups and
downs throughout the 24-hour day.14-19
EEG Monitoring of Sleep
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Few of the multiday studies on humans using continuous electroencephalographic monitoring
of sleep and wakefulness are actually relevant to measurement of sleep tendency. The two main
purposes of such studies are to capture episodic abnormalities in the electroencephalogram,
such as epileptic seizures,20 or to study circadian cycles in sleep or wakefulness.21 Although
all of these studies accomplish their purposes and the findings also show objective and
reproducible diurnal alternations of wakefulness and sleep in patients and normal persons, none
has demonstrated definitive technology for quantitatively differentiating one person's sleep
tendency from that of another or for detecting the presence of an abnormal sleep tendency. In
one of the earliest studies that permitted naps during the “day” in participants living under time
isolation, Schaefer et al22 documented the propensity of humans to sleep for up to 212 minutes
in the “afternoon.” It might seem that circadian EEG studies should be helpful in quantifying
individual sleep tendency, but this has not been the case. For methodological and scientific
reasons, participants in these types of studies are encouraged to have only one sleep period for
each circadian cycle.23
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Thus, most circadian studies are designed to measure overall circadian patterns, not the fine
structure of an individual's sleep cycles or how sleepy one person is compared with another
person. In 1976, however, Kripke24 suggested that circadian studies might yield insights for
understanding sleep disorders, such as narcolepsy. Some clinical studies used round-the-clock
EEG monitoring of sleep tendency in patients with sleep disorders. Findings in several studies
indicated that patients with narcolepsy, who characteristically complain of excessive
sleepiness, do not sleep significantly more than normal persons do when the narcoleptics ani
studied for periods of 24 to 48 hours.25-27 Rather, the sleep of narcoleptics is less consolidated
than that of normal people and occurs in multiple short naps. Pollak and his colleagues28,29
extended this circadian clinical research by evaluating narcoleptic individuals in true temporal
isolation and found that narcoleptics did have more sleep periods per circadian cycle than those
in a control group but that the narcoleptics' total sleep time was not greater than that for the
normal participants.
Modern EEG techniques for measuring sleep tendency did not grow out of circadian studies
but grew, instead, out of sleep research on the properties of the nominal human sleep fraction
of 1 to 2 (ie, 8 hours of sleep for every 16 hours of wakefulness).14–16,30 Sleep researchers
would measure sleep tendency around the clock by allowing 1 hour of sleep for every 3 hours
of time, or 30 minutes for every 90 minutes, or 3 hours for every 9 hours. Lavie and
colleagues17–19,31 extended and refined this approach by preserving the basic sleep fraction
but moving to ultrashort multiples (eg, 7 minutes of every 20). With such ultrashort naps, the
measurement of sleep tendency at any given time in the 24-hour day is not greatly influenced
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 3
by sleep that may have accumulated from previous naps in protocols that allow longer sleeping
times.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The typical result of such studies is depicted in Figure 4, which is a recalculation and replotting
of the amount of sleep that 9 of Lavie's participants obtained each hour when they were allowed
to sleep 7 minutes out of every 20. Note the two-peak pattern is again apparent, with a rapid
rise in sleep tendency (Lavie refers to this as the rapid opening of the nocturnal sleep gate),
beginning at about 8 PM, and a broad plateau from about midnight to 6 AM. A smaller midafternoon increase in sleep tendency occurs between 1 and 3 PM, and almost no sleep occurs at
about 6 to 7 PM, which Lavie refers to as the “forbidden zone.” The general pattern of this elegant
work fits very well with that found by Carskadon et al15 in their 90-minute-day studies (30
minutes of sleep every 90 minutes round the clock) and with the temporal pattern of unintended
sleep episodes described for participants trying to stay awake throughout a 24-hour period.
32,33 The coincidence of the peak times for errors shown in Figure 1, crashes shown in Figure
2, disease-related deaths shown in Figure 3, and sleep tendency shown in Figure 4 is obvious.
With such a degree of interest in sleepiness, it is appropriate to review critically the various
methods of measuring sleep tendency as well as our current concepts regarding what is normal
and what is abnormal in sleep tendency.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Self-Report Techniques
The first attempts to assess sleepiness were probably subjective self-reports that consisted of
documenting the individual's spontaneous thoughts and comments concerning levels of energy,
ability to function, and desire for sleep.34 Interview and questionnaire techniques were
developed to standardize the process and better delineate the time of each measurement.
Among the best known of the questionnaire techniques is the Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS).
35 Respondents complete this measurement by choosing one of seven statements describing
their state of sleepiness from the following list: (1) feeling active and vital, alert, wide awake;
(2) functioning at a high level, but not at peak, able to concentrate; (3) relaxed, awake, not at
a full alertness, responsive; (4) a little foggy, not at peak, let down; (5) fogginess, beginning
to lose interest in remaining awake, slowed down; (6) sleepiness, prefer to be lying down,
fighting sleep, woozy; (7) almost in reverie, sleep onset soon, lost struggle to remain awake.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Major strengths of the SSS are that it can be administered many times per day, that it correlates
with standard measures of performance, and that it reflects the effects of sleep loss. All levels
of the SSS can be recorded throughout the 24-hour day in normal participants, experimentally
sleep-deprived normal participants, and patients with sleep disorders Thus, the SSS provides
no clear procedure for determining what is normal and what is abnormal. Furthermore, Dement
and colleagues36 have documented glaring discordancies between high SSS ratings of 1 or 2
and gross behavioral indicators of sleep, such as closed eyes and snoring in patients with sleep
apnea. These researchers have suggested that such discordancies stem from either the
participant's loss of a proper frame of reference about what normal alertness really is or from
simple denial. An alternative explanation for discordance is that the SSS and behavioral
indicators of sleep may simply reflect different things.
Another common self-report instrument is the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS), which was
devised by M. W. Johns at Epworth Hospital in Melbourne, Australia.37 The ESS asks
respondents to use a number from 0 to 3 corresponding to the likelihood (never, slight,
moderate, and high) that they would fall asleep in eight situations: (1) sitting and reading; (2)
watching TV; (3) sitting, inactive in a public place; (4) as a passenger in a car riding for an
hour without a break; (5) lying down to rest in the afternoon when circumstances permit; (6)
sitting and talking with someone; (7) sitting quietly after a lunch without alcohol; and (8) in a
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 4
car, while stopped for a few minutes in traffic. An ESS score, tallied after the test is
administered, can range between 0 and 24.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The ESS statistically distinguishes normal persons from patients with sleep disorders
characterized by the symptom of excessive sleepiness. The ESS also is correlated with
electroencephalographically determined sleep latencies measured at night or during the day
and does reflect changes in sleepiness associated with treatment-related improvement in sleep
apnea.38 The ESS is considered to be a validated, reliable, self-report measure of sleepiness.
However, the ESS is not designed to be used on various occasions during the day or in the
presence of short-term conditions, such as acute sleep loss, that might influence sleep tendency.
Pupillography
Pupillography is an objective, laboratory-based method of electronically monitoring the size
of a person's pupil.39 Studies have shown that the diameter of the pupil, as well as the degree
of stability of pupil size, is inversely related to the complaint of sleepiness. Pupillography has
been used as a clinical measure of sleep tendency.40,41 The technique has not come into
widespread use because modern pupillographic equipment is not generally available. In
addition, the pupillographic indicators of sleepiness are not specific to whether the sleepiness
is the normal result of circadian variation in alertness or the symptomatic consequence of such
sleep disorders as sleep apnea and narcolepsy.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
EEG-Based Techniques
Common sense dictates that when asked to fall asleep, someone who is not sleepy will take
longer to produce the EEG waves of sleep than someone who is sleepy. In 1976, Carskadon
and her colleagues42 studied the nocturnal sleep of 122 patients who complained of insomnia
and found that the time taken to fall asleep by EEG criteria (sleep latency) varied greatly.
Furthermore, the distribution of sleep latencies for the group of insomniacs did not seem much
different from that of groups of people without insomnia.
Also striking for these insomniacs was their overestimation of how long it took to fall asleep.
Sixty of the study participants (49%) overestimated sleep latency by more than 15 minutes,
and 15 (12%) overestimated by more than 60 minutes. This kind of discrepancy between
subjective and objective measures of sleep tendency is the basic reason why sleep researchers
have pursued and refined EEG-based measures of sleep tendency.
Continuous and Round-The-Clock Measurements
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
We have already mentioned continuous EEG studies of sleep and wakefulness as well as those
that follow the nominal sleep fraction of 1/3 sleep to 2/3 wakefulness. Such approaches
certainly have the advantage of objectivity. These approaches have shown that the factors of
diurnal variation in sleep tendency and sleep deprivation can be objectively separated17–19,
31and that sleep loss, in general, increases overall sleep tendency but does little to the 24-hour
pattern of ups and downs.
Round-the-clock EEG studies, however, are expensive and not easily applicable to the
diagnostic or screening purposes demanded by the clinical and public-safety concerns
associated with the problem of abnormal sleep tendency. Furthermore, no guidelines are
available on how to quantify sleep tendency (in terms of total sleep time, number of sleep
episodes, amount of certain sleep phase, etc) or how to differentiate normal from abnormal
sleep patterns.
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 5
The Multiple Sleep Latency Test
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) is an electroencephalographically based clinical and
research tool that was developed at Stanford University.43,44 The technique grew out of
studies on the human sleep fraction, namely, the 90-minute day, round-the-clock studies of
sleep tendency by Carskadon et al.15 The MSLT was designed to assess sleep tendency only
during the day and is ideally performed after a standard night's sleep in the laboratory.
Details of the MSLT protocol have been published elsewhere.43,45 Essential features are that
a person who has kept regular sleeping and waking hours before the test spends a night in the
sleep laboratory. He or she and is then permitted four, five, or six 20-minute-long opportunities
to sleep at 2-hour intervals after getting up in the morning. The sleep latency (time between
the point at which the lights are turned out and the first sleep is detected on the EEG) is
electroencephalographically determined on each of four or five trials of an MSLT; then a mean
sleep latency is calculated and interpreted.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Other parameters, such as duration and type of sleep, may also be determined, depending on
whether the MSLT is being used for clinical or research purposes. The average sleep latency
for each of the five opportunities to sleep on the MSLT for 17 normal participants originally
is reported by Mitler in Table 1 of his 1990 article in Sleep Research.46,47 The order of
numbers on the vertical axis has been reversed to make increases in sleep tendency (ie,
decreases in sleep latency) go up, thereby facilitating visual comparison among various
measures of sleep tendency and performance. By comparing Figures 4 and 5, one can recognize
that the MSLT, although not a round-the-clock measure of sleep tendency, does measure the
daytime level of sleep tendency and captures the afternoon rise in sleep tendency as well as its
subsequent fall toward what Lavie has termed the forbidden zone in the late afternoon and
early evening.
The MSLT is a well-validated research and clinical tool that is used throughout the world,48
and normative values for participants at various ages have been published.33,49 One of the
most important features of the MSLT is that it clearly reflects the effects of experimental sleep
deprivation,50,51 albeit only for the daytime hours. If prior sleep is reduced from baseline
levels, the MSLT shows an increase from baseline levels in sleep tendency.
Unlike round-the-clock techniques for measuring sleep tendency, the MSLT was the first
objective tool that could be used on a standardized, widespread basis to address the increased
or excessive sleep tendency that arises from sleep loss, drugs, or disease. The MSLT
distinguishes between excessively sleepy patients and normal persons43 and can detect the
daytime carryover effects of a long-acting sleeping pill.52
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Guidelines for the diagnostic use of the MSLT have been -available since 1978; they indicate
that an average MSLT sleep latency of less than 5 minutes may be considered
pathological43 and an average sleep latency of between 5 and 10 minutes may be considered
abnormal or borderline, depending on clinical cornplaints.53 The major drawbacks of the
MSLT are that, although it is less cumbersome to do than a round-the-clock study, it is still
quite cumbersome and expensive. In addition, it is not at all suitable as a screening tool to
identify abnormally sleepy individuals in a population.
The MSLT is also subject to certain interpretative and conceptual problems when it is used to
assess sleep tendency in people who complain of difficulty staying awake: (1) The instructions
of the MSLT (“Try to fall asleep”) are counterintuitive to people who have trouble staying
awake; (2) a person with a vested interest in appearing normal on the MSLT may ignore the
instructions and try to stay awake on each MSLT trial; and (3) the MSLT may not reflect
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 6
changes in sleep tendency that result from treatment of very sleepy patients who have
narcolepsy or sleep apnea, even though the patients report improvement.30
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The Maintenance of Wakefulness Test
The Maintenance of Wakefulness Test (MWT) is a variant of the MSLT that was originally
devised to obviate some of the interpretative and conceptual problems of the MSLT.54 The
person taking the MWT is asked to remain awake and is monitored for electroencephalographic
sleep onset. Findings from early MWT studies demonstrated a prolonged sleep latency as a
result of the instruction to remain awake, compared with asking the subject to go to sleep, as
is done in the MSLT.55 The original MWT used a 20-minute trial conducted five times over
the course of a day, beginning about 2 hours after nocturnal sleep ended.
The MWT has been shown to be useful in evaluating the effect of treatment in patients with
narcolepsy.47 For a graphic summary of MWT sleep latencies for 8 control participants who
were offered five 20-minute-long opportunities to stay awake at 2-hour intervals, beginning at
10 AM, see Figure 6. These data were originally published in Mitler, Gujvarty, and
Bowman54 and are replotted in the same format as that of Figure 4 to make comparisons easier.
Note that the afternoon increase in sleep tendency is positioned similarly in Figures 4 and 5
and that all sleep latencies are longer on the MWT than on the MSLT.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
In 1984, the group at Scripps Clinic lengthened the original MWT's 20-minute trials to 40
minutes because they observed that patients who had histories of daytime sleepiness were too
often able to maintain wakefulness for 20 minutes. This “ceiling effect” was diminished with
trial lengths of 40 minutes. The 40-minute version of the MWT has now been applied to large
numbers of patients with obstructive sleep apnea by at least two independent laboratories,56–
58 and normative values for the MWT are available.59
Because this instrument's instructions more directly assess clinical and public-safety concerns
and because the MWT seems to detect clinical improvement in sleepy patients who are not
completely cured, this test eliminates some of the interpretative problems associated with the
MSLT. Furthermore, Poceta et al56 and Sangal et al57,58 have suggested guidelines for
normalcy: An MWT sleep latency of less than 15 minutes is considered dangerous for driving
an automobile. The Federal Aviation Administration has called for use of the MWT in
determining whether non-commercial pilots can be licensed after the diagnosis of sleep apnea
has been made.60
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Although the MWT may seem to produce a simple prolongation of daytime sleep latencies,
detailed comparisons of the MSLT and the MWT have disclosed that the two tests do not
correlate well in patients who complain of excessive sleepiness. The two tests may actually
measure different abilities.57,58 For example, some patients with very short MSLT sleep
latencies are consistently able to stay awake on 40-minute MWT trials. Others patients cannot
sleep on MSLT trials, but fall asleep quickly on MWT trials. Significant limitations of the
MWT include the following: (1) It is as cumbersome and expensive as the MSLT; (2) there
are no normative data for both sexes at various age groups; (3) there is marked discordance
with the MSLT, the primary and most widely used electroencephalographic measure of sleep
tendency; and (4) no data are available concerning MWT responsiveness to experimental sleep
deprivation.
Other methods for electroencephalographically measuring daytime sleep tendency have been
developed. Erman and colleagues61 attempted to obviate the separate problems of the MSLT
and the MWT by combining both tests. They studied 20 narcoleptic participants with the
Modified Assessment of Sleepiness Test (MAST), which consisted of three sleep-in-bed
conditions interspersed with two reading-in-chair conditions. Although the test is ingenious,
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 7
the problems of expense and lack of normative data are still severe limitations to any laboratorybased strategy.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Computerized EEG Studies
The widespread application of computer technology to EEG monitoring has led to important
new approaches for measuring sleep tendency. For example, the quantitative analysis of EEG
delta activity has long been thought to reflect the physiological tendency for sleep and does
increase in response to experimental sleep deprivation.62 Hasan et al63 have suggested a
method of computer analysis that may automatically detect drowsiness in MSLT-like settings.
Others, such as Broughton,21 have applied eventrelated potential technologies to the
assessment of sleep tendency. Although these technologically advanced approaches to
measuring sleep tendency hold promise, they have not yet been shown to be useful in general
clinical applications or as methods of screening individuals for abnormal sleep tendencies.
Model of the Two-Peak Pattern in Sleep Tendency
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The ideal measure of sleep tendency would have to be physiologically based, rapid, and suitable
for multiple determinations throughout the day. Perhaps a chemical may eventually be found
in blood or urine that correlates satisfactorily enough with sleep tendency to be used as a
universal measure and as a screen. In the meantime, predictive mathematical models offer some
promise, at least as conceptual tools.
The first author of this article (MMM) serendipitiously observed that a combination of the
population growth function and the cosine function could be made to produce a distortion of
the cosine curve that resembled the 24-hour, two-peak pattern in human error and diseaserelated mortality.46,64 In our work to date, this combination, which is expressed as a
mathematical model of sleep tendency in formula 2 in Mitler,64(p72) approximates well the
temporal distributions of a variety of sleep- and performance-related data.
The equation accounted for 79% of the variance in the observed frequency of occurrence
(Figure 7A) for more than 6,000 fatigue-related auto accidents throughout the 24 hours.5
Similarly, for more than 437,000 human deaths,5 the equation accounted for 45% of the
observed variance (Figure 7B).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The variance in some other 24-hour observations may be explained reasonably well by this
equation. It explained 69% of the variance in the frequency of falling asleep while driving65
(Figure 7C), but only 42% of the variance in the duration of switchboard operators' delays in
answering calls1 (Figure 8A). The model fits better for the last two entries in Figure 8,
accounting for 83% of the variance in error-of-omission locomotive brakings66 (Figure 8B)
and 76% of the variance in gas meter reading errors2 (Figure 8C).
It appeared that data from tasks demanding vigilance (accidents, driver drowsiness, locomotive
auto-brakings, and meter-reading errors) produced better curve fits than data from other sources
(mortality and delays in answering calls). For the individual models, see Table 1. The phase
lag constant (Φ–12) shown in the table is the number of hours following midnight at which the
larger of the two peaks occurs. We subtract 12 hours from the actual value (ø) that is used in
the equation so that it is easy for the reader to imagine the placement of the large peak in the
midnight-to-dawn period. The sleep deprivation (SD) constant in the table controls the size of
the secondary peak relative to the larger peak.
We recognize that the SD factor is difficult to evaluate in the 24-hour data available. First, the
data are from normal field populations. Second, few or no 24-hour data are available on
experimentally or systematically varied total sleep times or shift-work patterns. This modeling
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 8
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
approach, however, seems to be consistent with the sleep deprivation data of Lavie and Segal,
31 using the ultrashort sleep-wake cycle protocol. Figure 9 and Figure 10 are from five of Lavie
and Segal's “morning” participants (ie, participants self-characterized as most alert in the
morning) who were studied after normal nocturnal sleep and after 24 hours of sleep deprivation.
Participants were permitted to sleep 7 minutes out of every 20 around the clock, beginning at
7 AM.
Note that sleep deprivation seems to (1) leave unchanged the location of the temporal
distribution of peaks in sleep tendency, (2) increase the amplitude and the breadth of the
nocturnal peak, (3) increase the amplitude and the breadth of the afternoon peak while keeping
constant the relative sizes of the two peaks, and (4) reverse the trend from wakefulness toward
sleepiness between 7 AM and noon.
Accordingly, in our model, the SD's effect on afternoon sleep tendency is asymptotic so that
progressive increases in values of SD have less and less influence on the height of the afternoon
peak. Clearly, further testing of our model must include systematic experiments on extended
and restricted sleep durations to determine whether or not resulting distortions in the two peaks
match those predicted by proportionate changes in SD.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The six activities listed in Table 1 can be linked along some underlying continuum that we
have labeled SD. Consider how the activities differ along several dimensions. With respect to
cause, mortality may differ from the other five examples, being predisposed by disease states
rather than by sleep deprivation. However, we have implied that some factor associated with
sleep is also implicated in the mortality pattern as an acute precipitator of expiration.
It seems that the top two items listed in the table (driver drowsiness, mortality) have more
severe outcomes (injury, death) than the bottom two (inconvenience without or with little
hazard; locomotive auto-brakings seem to be more like an embarrassment to the engineer than
a hazard). The middle two items seem reversed with respect to outcomes, and perhaps accidents
and driver drowsiness should be closer together.
We may consider the levels of susceptibility to sleepiness associated with the six activities.
Perhaps driver drowsiness and death are the least avoidable items in the list. Certainly, turning
back a disease state is difficult. Also, because of the human brain's inability to deal well with
boring tasks, driver drowsiness on the highway appears difficult to deal with.67 The
accompanying article by Rosekind et al (pps 157–165) includes suggestions for prevention and
for countermeasures.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The meter-reading errors, accidents, and operator delays cited here may have been less
susceptible to sleepiness than the first two tasks by virtue of greater human involvement in the
task. That leaves the locomotive cab, where engineeralerting devices may have had at least a
marginal effect in preventing drowsiness. Perhaps analogous devices may be placed in
commercial vehicles on the highway. Compared with the above-mentioned arguments for
cause and effect, the argument for susceptibility to sleepiness seems slightly stronger in
explaining the general rank order of the values assigned to SD in Table 1.
The median time of occurrence of the larger peak for the six curve fits we have mentioned here
was 2.5 hours after midnight. The median standard deviation was 2.75. The mean proportion
of variance explained (mean squared Pearson r via Fisher z transforms) was .684. The
composite model is shown in Figure 11.
Although the ability to fit a function to data relatively successfully is without novelty, the
particular function used here may be instructive. There should be a molecular basis for the
predictive power of this cosine-population function. Both circadian factors and fluctuations in
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 9
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
sleep tendency depend upon the actual numbers of receptor populations in the brain important
for the behavioral expression of sleep. Thus, the function may provide a model for
investigations of neuronal recruiting.
Our equations suggest that the hemicircadian rhythm may involve only a single pacemaker.
Each mathematical function used can be accomplished by neurons associated with and
modifying a single pacemaker. Because sleep deprivation is incorporated into the model, there
is hope to apply the model, not only to normal populations, but also to individuals. This
application to individuals may allow some discrimination of abnormal sleepiness arising from
sleep deprivation or sleep pathology from normal sleepiness at certain times of the day. We
encourage further testing, elaborations, and criticism of the equation by all with appropriate
data.
NOTE
We wish to acknowledge Drs Peretz Lavie, Robert Norman, and Joyce Walsleben for their critical reading of this
article and their helpful suggestions. Dr Miller also wishes to express his thanks to the late Dr Robert R. Mackie for
stimulating his interest in the data discussed in this article.
REFERENCES
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
1. Browne RC. The day and night performance of teleprinter switchboard operators. Occup Psychol
1949;23:1–6. [PubMed: 18113198]
2. Bjerner B, Holm A, Swenson A. Diurnal variation in mental performance: A study of three-shift
workers. Br J Industrial Med 1955;12:103–130.
3. Broughton RJ. Biorhythmic variations in consciousness and psychological functions. Can Psychol
Reviews 1975;16:217–230.
4. Broughton, RJ. Chronobiological aspects and models of sleep and napping. In: Dinges, DF.; Broughton,
RJ., editors. Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological, Behavioral and Medical Aspects of Napping. New
York: Raven; 1989. p. 71-98.
5. Mitler MM, Carskadon MA, Czeisler CA, Dement WC, Dinges DF, Graeber RC. Catastrophes, sleep,
and public policy: Consensus report. Sleep 1988;11:100–109. [PubMed: 3283909]
6. Langlois PH, Smolensky MH, Hsi BP, Weir FW. Temporal patterns of reported single-vehicle car and
truck accidents in Texas, USA during 1980-1983. Chronobiologia 1985;2:131–140.
7. Lavie P, Wollma M, Pollack I. Frequency of sleep-related traffic accidents and hour of the day. Sleep
Research 1986;15:275.
8. Duff, GW. Accidents on The New York State Throughway. Unpublished report to the governor of
New York; 1985.
9. Eastman, C. Are separate temperature and activity oscillators necessary to explain the phenomena of
human ciracdian rhythms?. In: Moore-Ede, MC.; Czeisler, CA., editors. Mathematical Models of the
Circadian Sleep-Wake Cycle. New York: Raven; 1984. p. 81-103.
10. US Congress Office of Technology Assessment. Biological Rhythms: Implications for the Worker.
Washington, DC.: US Government Printing office; 1991.
11. Mitler MM, Hajdukovic RM, Shafor R, Hahn PM, Kripke DF. When people die. Cause of death
versus time of death. Am J Med 1987;82:266–274. [PubMed: 3812519]
12. Smolensky, M.; Halberg, F.; Sargent, F. Chronobiology of the life sequence. In: Ito, S.; Ogata, K.;
Yoshimura, H., editors. Advances in Climatic Physiology. Tokyo: Igaku Shoin; 1972. p. 281-318.
13. Rechtschaffen, A.; Kales, A. A Manual of Standardized Terminology, Techniques and Scoring System
for Sleep Stages of Human Participants. Bethesda, MD: 1968. National Institute of Neurological
Diseases and Blindness (NIH Pub 204)
14. Weitzman ED, Nogeire C, Perlow M, et al. Effects of a prolonged 3-hour sleep-wake cycle on sleep
stages, plasma cortisol, growth hormone and body temperature in man. J Clin Endocrinol Metab
1974;38:1018–1030. [PubMed: 4364804]
15. Carskadon M, Dement W. Sleep studies on a 90-minute day. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol
1975;39:145–155. [PubMed: 50211]
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 10
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
16. Webb WB, Agnew HW Jr. Sleep efficiency for sleep-wake cycles of varied length. Psychophysiology
1975;12:637–641. [PubMed: 1187967]
17. Lavie P, Scherson A. Ultrashort sleep-waking schedule. I. Evidence of ultradian rhythmicity in
“sleepability.”. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1981;52:163–174. [PubMed: 6167426]
18. Lavie P, Zomer J. Ultrashort sleep-waking schedule. II. Relationship between ultradian rhythms in
sleepability and the REM-NONREM cycles and effects of the circadian phase. Electroencephalogr
Clin Neurophysiol 1984;57:35–42. [PubMed: 6198142]
19. Lavie P. Ultrashort sleep-waking schedule. III. Gates and “forbidden zones” for sleep.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1986;63:414–425. [PubMed: 2420557]
20. Offenbacher H, Korner E, Reinhart B, et al. Mobile long-term EEG monitoring in generalized seizure
disorders of different etiology. Eur Neurol 1986;25:146–153. [PubMed: 3758118]
21. Torsvall L, Akerstedt T, Gillander K, Knutsson A. Sleep on the night shift: 24-hour EEG monitoring
of spontaneous sleep1 wake behavior. Psychophysiology 1989;26:352–358. [PubMed: 2756085]
22. Schaefer KE, Clegg BR, Carey CR, Dougherty JHJ, Weybrew BB. Effect of isolation in a constant
environment on periodicity of physiologycal functions and performance levels. Aerospace Med
1967;38:1002–1018. [PubMed: 4383216]
23. Campbell, SS.; Zulley, J. Napping in time-free environments. Sleep and Alertness: Chronobiological,
Behavioral, and Medical Aspects of Napping. Dinges, DF.; Broughton, RJ., editors. New York:
Raven; 1989. p. 121-138.
24. Kripke, DF. Biological rhythms disturbances might cause narcolepsy. In: Guilleminault, C.; Dement,
WC.; Passouant, P., editors. Narcolepsy. New York: Spectrum; 1976. p. 475-483.
25. Baldy-Moulinier, M.; Arguner, A.; Besset, A. Ultradian and circadian rhythms in sleep and
wakefulness. In: Guilleminault, C.; Dement, WC.; Passouant, P., editors. Narcolepsy. New York:
Spectrum; 1976. p. 485-498.
26. Billiard M, Salva MQ, De-Koninick J, Besset A, Touchon J, Cadilhac J. Daytime sleep characteristics
and their relationships with night sleep in the narcoleptic patient. Sleep 1986;9:167–174. [PubMed:
3704438]
27. Broughton R, Dunham W, Newman J, Lutley K, Duschesne P, Rivers M. Ambulatory 24-hour sleepwake monitoring in narcolepsy-cataplexy compared to matched controls. Electroencephalogr Clin
Neurophysiol 1988;70:473–481. [PubMed: 2461281]
28. Pollak CP, Perlick D, Linsner JP, Wenston J, Hsieh F. Sleep problems in the community elderly as
predictors of death and nursing home placement. J Community Health 1990;15:123–135. [PubMed:
2355110]
29. Pollak CP, Wagner DR, Moline ML, Monk TH. Cognitive and motor performance of narcoleptic and
normal participants living in temporal isolation. Sleep 1992;15:202–211. [PubMed: 1621020]
30. Mitler MM, Gujavarty KS, Sampson MG, Browman CP. Multiple daytime nap approaches to
evaluating the sleepy patient. Sleep 1982;5:S119–127. [PubMed: 7156647]
31. Lavie P, Segal S. Twenty-four-hour structure of sleepiness in morning and evening persons
investigated by ultrashort sleep-wake cycle. Sleep 1989;12:522–528. [PubMed: 2595175]
32. Richardson GS, Carskadon MA, Orav EJ, Dement WC. Circadian variation in sleep tendency in
elderly and young adult participants. Sleep 1982;5:s82–s92. [PubMed: 7156658]
33. Carskadon MA. Patterns of sleep and sleepiness in adolescents. Pediatrician 1990;17:5–12. [PubMed:
2315238]
34. Kleitman, N. Sleep and Wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1963.
35. Hoddes E, Zarcone V, Smythe H, Phillips R, Dement WC. Quantification of sleepiness: A new
approach. Psychophysiology 1973;10:431–436. [PubMed: 4719486]
36. Dement, WC.; Carskadon, MA.; Richardson, GS. Excessive daytime sleepiness in the sleep apnea
syndrome. In: Guilleminault, C.; Dement, WC., editors. Sleep Apnea Syndromes. New York: Alan
R. Liss; 1978. p. 23-46.
37. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep
1991;14:540–545. [PubMed: 1798888]
38. Johns MW. Reliability and factor analysis of the Epworth Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 1992;15:376–381.
[PubMed: 1519015]
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 11
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
39. Lowenstein O, Loewenfeld I. Electronic pupillography—A new instrument and some clinical
applications. Arch Opthalmol 1958;59:352–363.
40. Schmidt, HS.; Fortin, L. Electronic pupillography in disorders of arousal. In: Guilleminault, C., editor.
Sleeping and Waking Disorders: Indications and Techniques. Menlo Park, CA: Addison- Wesley;
1982. p. 127-143.
41. Yoss RE. Treatment of narcolepsy. Modem Treatment 1969;6:1263–1274.
42. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Guilleminault C, Zarcone VP, Spiegel R. Self-reports
versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drug-free participants with a complaint of chronic insomnia.
Am J Psychiatry 1976;133:1382–1388. [PubMed: 185919]
43. Richardson GS, Carskadon MA, Flagg W, Van den Hoed J, Dement WC, Mitler MM. Excessive
daytime sleepiness in man: Multiple sleep latency measurement in narcoleptic and control
participants. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1978;45:621–627. [PubMed: 81764]
44. Mitler MM, Van den Hoed J, Carskadon MA, et al. REM sleep episodes during the Multple Sleep
Latency Test in narcoleptic patients. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1979;46:479–481.
[PubMed: 85544]
45. Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Roth T, Westbrook PR, Keenan S. Guidelines for the
multiple sleep latency test (MSLT): A standard measure of sleepiness. Sleep 1986;9:519–524.
[PubMed: 3809866]
46. Mitler MM. Two-peak 24-hour patterns in sleep, mortality and error. Sleep Research 1990;19:399.
47. Mitler MM, Hajdukovic R, Erman M, Koziol JA. Narcolepsy. J Clin Neurophysiol 1990;7:93–118.
[PubMed: 1968069]
48. Thorpy MJ. Report from the American Sleep Disorders Association. The clinical use of the Multiple
Sleep Latency Test. Sleep 1992;15:268–276.
49. Carskadon MA, Harvey K, Duke P, Anders TF, Litt IF, Dement WC. Pubertal changes in daytime
sleepiness. Sleep 1980;2:453–460. [PubMed: 7403744]
50. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Nocturnal determinants of daytime sleepiness. Sleep 1982;5:S73–S81.
[PubMed: 7156657]
51. Carskadon MA, Dement WC. Cumulative effects of sleep restriction on daytime sleepiness.
Psychophysiology 1981;18:107–113. [PubMed: 6111825]
52. Mitler MM, Seidel WF, van den Hoed J, Greenblatt DJ, Dement WC. Comparative hypnotic effects
of flurazepam, triazolam, and placebo: A long-term simultaneous nighttime and daytime study. J
Clin Psychopharmacol 1984;4:2–13. [PubMed: 6141188]
53. Diagnostic Classification Steering Committee. Thorpy, M.; Chairman. International Classification of
Sleep Disorders: Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Rochester, MN: American Sleep Disorders
Association; 1990.
54. Mitler MM, Gujavarty KS, Browman CP. Maintenance of wakefulness test: a polysomnographic
technique for evaluation treatment efficacy in patients with excessive somnolence.
Electroencephulogr Clin Neurophysiol 1982;53:658–661.
55. Hartse KM, Roth T, Zorick FJ. Daytime sleepiness and daytime wakefulness: The effect of instruction.
Sleep 1982;5:s107–s118. [PubMed: 7156646]
56. Poceta JS, Timms RM, Jeong DU, Ho SL, Erman MK, Mitler MM. Maintenance of wakefulness test
in obstructive sleep apnea syndrome [see comments]. Chest 1992;101:893–897. [PubMed: 1555458]
57. Sangal RB, Thomas L, Mitler MM. Disorders of excessive sleepiness. Treatment improves ability to
stay awake but does not reduce sleepiness. Chest 1992;102:699–703. [PubMed: 1355419]
58. Sangal RB, Thomas L, Mitler MM. Maintenance of wakefulness test and multiple sleep latency test.
Measurement of different abilities in patients with sleep disorders [see comments]. Chest
1992;101:898–902.
59. Doghramji K, Mitler M, Sangal RB, et al. A normative study of the Maintenance of Wakefulness
Test (MWT): Preliminary report. Sleep Research 1996;25In press
60. Sleep Apnea Evaluation Specifications. Federal Aviation Administration Specification Letter Dated
October 6, 1992. US Dept of Transportation: 1992.
61. Erman M, Beckham B, Gardner D, Roffwarg H. The modified assessment of sleepiness test (MAST).
Sleep Research 1987;16:550.
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 12
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
62. Borbely AA, Baumann F, Brandeis D, Strauch I, Lehmann D. Sleep deprivation: Effect on sleep
stages and EEG power density in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1981;S1:483–493.
[PubMed: 6165548]
63. Hasan J, Hirvonen K, Varri A, Hakkinen V, Loula P. Validation of computer analysed polygraphic
patterns during drowsiness and sleep onset. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1993;87:117–
127. [PubMed: 7691539]
64. Mitler, MM. Two-peak 24-hour patterns in sleep, mortality and error. In: Peter, JH.; Penzel, T.;
Podszus, T.; von Wichert, P., editors. Sleep and Health Risk. Berlin: Springer-Verlag; 1991. p. 65-77.
65. Prokop O, Prokop L. Ermudung und einschlafen am steuer. Dtsch Z Gerichtl Med 1955;44:343–355.
66. Hildebrandt G, Rohmert W, Rutenfranz J. 12 and 24 h rhythms in error frequency of locomotive
drivers and the influence of tiredness. Int J Chronobiology 1974:2175–180.
67. Mackie, RR.; Miller, JC. Effects Of Hours of Service, Regularity of Schedules, and Cargo Loading
on Truck and Bus Driver Fatigue. Goleta, CA: Human Factors Research; 1978.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 13
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
The temporal distribution of 74,927 meter reading errors recorded for Swedish gas workers
over a 20-year period. Data are plotted from the report of Bjerner.2 The horizontal axis
represents clock time of a 24-hour day beginning at midnight. The vertical axis represents
number of errors per hour.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 14
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 2.
Temporal distribution of 6,052 fatigue-related automobile crashes compiled from Langlois et
al,6 Lavie et al,7 and Duff.8 The horizontal axis is clock time of the 24-hour day beginning at
midnight. The vertical axis represents hourly number of crashes. Also see Figure 4 in Mitler
et al.5
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 15
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 3.
Temporal distribution of 437,511 disease-related deaths compiled from references Mitler et
al11 and Smolensky. 12 The horizontal axis is clock time of the 24-hour day beginning at
midnight. The vertical axis represents hourly number of mortalities. Also see Figure 2 in Mitler.
5
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 16
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 4.
Amount of sleep obtained each hour for 9 participants permitted to sleep up to 7 minutes of
every 20 minutes throughout the 24-hour day. Data were taken from figure 8 in Lavie and
Segal.31 Horizontal axis is clock time of a 24-hour day beginning at midnight. The vertical
axis represents minutes of sleep.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 17
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 5.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Average sleep latency for each of the five opportunities to sleep on the MSLT for 17 normal
participants from Table 1 of Mitler.47 The order of numbers on the vertical axis has been
reversed to make increases in sleep tendency go up on the vertical axis and facilitate comparison
among measures of sleep tendency and performance. The horizontal axis is clock time of a 24hour day beginning at midnight. The vertical axis represents minutes before sleep onset.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 18
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 6.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
MWT sleep latencies for 8 control participants offered five 20-minute-long opportunities to
stay awake at 2-hour intervals, beginning at 10 AM. These data were originally published in
Mitle54 and are replotted in the same format as Figure 4 to facilitate comparisons.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 19
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 7.
Curve fits to (A) accident5 and (B) mortality5 data, and to (C) driver drowsiness data65 with
percentage of variance explained for each curve fit. Parameter values are shown in Table 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 20
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 8.
Curve fits to (A) operators' delays in answering calls,1 (B) locomotive auto-braking data,66
and (C) meter reading error data.2 with percentage of variance explained for each curve fit.
Parameter values in Table 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 21
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 9.
Minutes of sleep each hour for five “morning” participants studied after normal nocturnal sleep.
Participants were permitted to sleep 7 minutes out of every 20 round the clock, beginning at 7
31 for details.
AM. See Lavie and Segal
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 22
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 10.
Minutes of sleep each hour for five “morning” participants studied after 24 hours of total sleep
deprivation. Participants were permitted to sleep 7 minutes out of every 20 round the clock,
beginning at 7 AM. See Lavie and Segal31 for details.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 23
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 11.
“Idealized” curve from medians of parameter values Ф and SD in Table 1.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.
Mitler and Miller
Page 24
TABLE 1
Individual Models of Hemicircadian Rhythm Shown in Various Studies
Data sets
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Accident5
Mortality5
Driver drowsiness65
Operators' delays in
answering calls1
Locomotive autobrakings66
Meter-reading
errors2
Medians
Ф-12 hr
SD
r2
2.7
4.5
1.7
3.4
2.6
4.1
4.8
2.0
.786
.451
.687
.421
1.9
2.4
.832
2.3
2.9
.765
2.5
2.75
.684
Notes. Ф-12 = phase lag in hours past midnight; SD = a sleep deprivation factor used in the equation; Pearson's r2 = amount of variance in the data that
was explained by the model. See Figures 7 and 8. Medians = summary of data sets listed above. Superscripts in data set column indicate references listed
in the article.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Behav Med. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 16.