J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
DOI 10.1007/s10802-011-9533-3
Socialization of Social Anxiety in Adolescent Crowds
Nejra Van Zalk & Maarten Herman Walter Van Zalk &
Margaret Kerr
Published online: 22 June 2011
# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011
Abstract In this study, we looked at whether social anxiety
is socialized, or influenced by peers’ social anxiety, more in
some peer crowds than others. Adolescents in crowds with
eye-catching appearances such as Goths and Punks (here
termed Radical), were compared with three comparison
groups. Using data from 796 adolescents (353 girls and 443
boys; Mage =13.36) at three timepoints, the results show that
adolescents affiliating with the Radical crowd tended to
select peers from the same crowd group. Being a member
of a crowd in itself did not predict socialization of social
anxiety, but adolescents in the Radical crowd were more
influenced by their peers’ social anxiety than adolescents
who did not affiliate with the Radical crowd group. The
results suggest that through a bidirectional process,
adolescents affiliating with Radical crowds may narrow
their peer relationship ties in time, and in turn socialize
each other’s social anxiety.
Keywords Social anxiety . Peer crowds . Selection .
Socialization . Early adolescence
During adolescence, most young people seek an identity of
their own. Part of this process involves affiliating with
peers whose values and interests are compatible with their
own (Newman and Newman 1976). Many young people
adopt identities that separate them from others, but make
them similar to one another. Such constellations of youths
are sometimes categorized as crowds. Crowds are large
groups of individuals, who are similarly stereotyped by
N. Van Zalk (*) : M. H. W. Van Zalk : M. Kerr
Center for Developmental Research, JPS:Psychology,
Örebro University,
Örebro, Sweden
e-mail: nejra.van-zalk@oru.se
themselves and others, and may or may not be friends
(Brown 1990). Even though crowd members do not have to
be friends in order to be assigned by themselves or others to
a certain crowd, studies indicate that adolescents tend to
have relationships with peers who endorse a similar, or the
same crowd identity (Brown 1990; La Greca et al. 2001;
Urberg et al. 2000). Crowds are important for many young
people, as they provide a sense of identity and prospects for
social interactions with other similar-minded adolescents
(Brown 1990). Hence, affiliating with peer crowds might
serve as a gateway to gaining an identity in adolescence,
and might fulfill different needs for adolescents.
Previous research has shown that crowd members tend
to take on similar crowd characteristics such as values,
attitudes, and clothing styles. The majority of research on
similarity of crowd characteristics has identified typical
crowds such as the higher-status Jocks and Cheerleaders,
and the lower-status Nerds, Brains, Druggies, and Burnouts
(see eg., Heaven et al. 2005; La Greca et al. 2001; Prinstein
and La Greca 2002; Stone and Brown 1998; Thurlow 2001;
Urberg et al. 2000). Affiliating with high-status crowds in
adolescence has been linked to higher self-esteem and
lower depression (Brown and Lohr 1987; Brown et al.
2008; Prinstein and La Greca 2002), whereas affiliating
with low social-status crowds has been shown to result in
increases in social anxiety, depression, and loneliness
(Prinstein and La Greca 2002). Crowd affiliation has also
been linked to health-risk behavior (Kobus 2003; La Greca
et al. 2001), as well as eating, exercise, and weight control
behaviors (Mackey and La Greca 2007). How can this
similarity be explained?
There are a number of possible reasons for similarity
between adolescents and their peers. According to the
similarity-attraction hypothesis, people are attracted to
others who are similar to themselves because this enhances
1240
interaction between them (Byrne 1971), and this selection is
one reason for similarity. Similarity, however, could also be
a result of influence, or socialization (Kandel 1978), a
process in which individuals become similar over time
because they influence each other. Concerning crowds,
there is evidence for selection into peer crowds, but less is
known about socialization within peer crowds. Previous
research indicates that crowd members are likely to select
each other as friends, as 55–85% of adolescents befriend
someone from their own or a similar crowd (Brown 1990;
La Greca et al. 2001; Urberg et al. 2000), and adolescents
are most friendly to members of their own crowd (Urberg et
al. 2000). Although socialization has not been studied, we
suggest that this preference for similar peers within crowd
contexts could be a basis for socialization, as adolescents
might have opportunities to model behavior or even take
direct instructions from crowd members on how to behave.
In addition, peer crowds are thought to comprise cliques, or
tight-knit groups of a smaller number of adolescents
(Brown 1990). As adolescents interact with each other
within these contexts as well, cliques—especially if
embedded in crowds—may serve as an additional context
for socialization of behaviors. Through possibly intense
social interactions with other clique members, adolescents
might encourage or model behaviors from each other.
Hence, there are some reasons to expect socialization
within peer crowds, and this could be an important issue,
because some of the characteristics on which crowd
members are similar concern risk behaviors and internal
adjustment.
Similarities on internal adjustment have been shown for
alternative crowds such as Punks, Goths, and Metal Heads
(Bešić and Kerr 2009; Delsing et al. 2007). For example,
one study focused on shy, socially fearful behavior in peer
crowds with extremely eye-catching, or even shocking
appearances such as Punks and Goths (termed Radical). In
these crowds adolescents might, for example, wear white
face paint, faked bloodstains around the eyes or mouth, or
long, black hair shaved off of one side of the head. The
authors hypothesized that creating the unusual appearances
that define these crowds might be a way of limiting
anxiety-arousing social contacts or a self-handicapping
strategy, where social failures could be ascribed to one’s
extreme appearance rather than one’s social anxiety (Bešić
and Kerr 2009). Radicals were compared with three other
groups. The first one, Academics, consisted of Computer
Nerds and Brains, which are often considered by their peers
to be more socially anxious than adolescents in other
crowds (Kinney 1993). The second, the Independents,
consisted of adolescents who answered “None of the above
[identities]” to the crowd question. These adolescents have
previously been found to have high levels of social anxiety
(Prinstein and La Greca 2002). These two groups were used
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
in order to compare the Radicals with crowds that were known
to be relatively high on social anxiety, but without the startling
appearances of the Radicals. The final comparison group
comprised crowds that might be recognizable by their
appearances, but whose appearances were not deemed
shocking or off-putting, for example “Sports,” “Role-Players,”
“Vegans,” and “Skaters.” The socially anxious adolescents in
the Radical crowd group were found to be more socially
anxious and depressed and to have worse self-esteem than
socially anxious adolescents in all other crowd groups. The
study was cross-sectional, however, so there was no way of
teasing apart selection and socialization effects. Thus, whether
social anxiety socialization occurs after adolescents join these
crowds remains untested.
Very little is known about whether social anxiety might
be socialized in Radical crowds more than other crowds.
Socially anxious individuals tend to conform more easily
than others (Cohen and Prinstein 2006; Santee and Maslach
1982). Scholars have suggested that conformity might be a
vital part of being in a crowd that maintains a specific
clothing style, and that being in one of these crowds might
increase adolescents’ susceptibility to peer influence
(Brown et al. 1990). Going beyond clothing, the Radical
crowds, more than others, might value a reserved, withdrawn, or even seemingly depressed way of behaving, and
adolescents in these crowds might intensify their socially
fearful behavior and/or depressed mood to conform to the
standard. Then, through self-perception (Bem 1967), a
process in which people infer their own attitudes or
characteristics from their behaviors, they might come to
see themselves as increasingly socially anxious. In addition,
even though crowds in themselves might be abstract
phenomena, they do affect adolescents’ social relationships
(Brown et al. 1994), as adolescents are more likely to be
friends if they are part of the same crowd (Brown et al.
1994; La Greca et al. 2001; Urberg et al. 2000), and peers
influence each other on both delinquency (Burk et al. 2007;
Dishion et al. 1996; Kiesner et al. 2002) and depression
(Hogue and Steinberg 1995; Prinstein 2007; Rose 2002;
Van Zalk et al. 2010). As Radical crowds have been shown
to comprise fewer individuals than other crowds (Bešić and
Kerr 2009), one could assume that these adolescents might
have more tight-knit, close peer relationships than adolescents in larger crowds. If so, adolescents in the Radical
crowds may run a greater risk than adolescents in other,
larger crowds of being influenced by their peers on social
anxiety. Previous research supports this notion, showing
that socially anxious girls in close peer relationships, albeit
not peer crowds, were at higher risk of peer contagion of
social anxiety than less socially anxious girls (Prinstein
2007). To our knowledge, however, no studies have looked
at differential socialization of social anxiety within peer
crowds in adolescence.
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
1241
An important factor to take into account regarding
socialization of social anxiety within contexts of peer crowds
are gender differences. First, differences on levels of social
anxiety between boys and girls are well documented (e.g., La
Greca and Lopez 1998), and scholars have also suggested
that social anxiety may be more socially accepted in girls
than in boys (Bruch and Cheek 1995). This may make girls’
social fears more prone to influence than boys’. Some studies
support this notion, as socially anxious girls have been found
to be more susceptible than boys to influence from their best
friends on depression (Prinstein 2007). Second, girls are
more likely to be part of friendship cliques (Urberg et al.
2000), whereas boys tend to have larger peer groups
(Benenson 1990). In addition, adolescent girls’ relationships
with peers are characterized by high levels of psychological
intimacy (Selfhout et al. 2009). Even though this process is
assumed to function on the dyadic level, it may have relevant
implications for why peer crowds would influence the
development of social anxiety in girls more than boys. Thus,
as we may expect different outcomes for boys and girls
regarding socialization of social anxiety, it is of consequence
to consider possible gender differences in these processes.
In this study, we examined the socialization of social
anxiety within peer crowds in early adolescence, using
longitudinal data on early adolescents. We controlled for
depression and self-esteem in the analyses, as depression
(Elovainio et al. 2004; Smith and Betz 2002; Stednitz and
Epkins 2006) and poor self-esteem (Crozier 1981; Pilkonis
1977) are well-known correlates of social anxiety, and
should be controlled to determine the unique role of social
anxiety in adolescent peer relationships (Romero and
Epkins 2008). In so doing, we excluded the likelihood of
depression and self-esteem being reasons for selection and
socialization processes on social anxiety. We hypothesized,
first, that adolescents would select peers based on social
anxiety. Second, we hypothesized that adolescents would
also select peers based on crowd membership, and that
adolescents affiliating with Radical crowds would have
relationships with peers who affiliate with Radical crowds.
Third, we hypothesized that peers would socialize each
other’s social anxiety more in the Radical crowds than in other
crowd groups. Finally, we hypothesized that social anxiety
may be socialized among peers in the crowds differently for
boys and girls, in that girls in Radical crowds may be more
strongly influenced by peers’ social anxiety than boys.
26,000 in Sweden. The data collections took place annually
at roughly one-year intervals, with five waves of data
collected. More than 90% of all adolescents in the
community who were in grades 4–12 participated (aged
approximately 10 to 18 years). Measures of social anxiety,
depression, and crowd affiliation were included at Waves 3,
4, and 5, so these waves were used in this study and
referred to as Times 1, 2, and 3. At Wave 3 of the study,
the unemployment rate (8%) was comparable to that in
the rest of the country, as was the percentage of singleparent households for the entire community (10.0%). The
mean individual income was around 21,000 USD, which
was about 5.0% lower than in the rest of the country
(Statistics Sweden 2004).
To form the target sample that would be included in the
analyses, we began with all 7th-graders who participated in
at least two consecutive measurements (N=329; 148 girls,
and 181 boys; Mage =13.27). After inclusion of the peers
these 7th-graders nominated at each time point, the final
network consisted of 796 adolescents (353 girls and 443
boys; M = 13.36). The peer relationship nominations
included both in- and out-of-school peers, and several
adolescents named older or younger peers. Hence, the
final social network included 4th through 12th graders,
with adolescents spread over 27 classes and 4 different
schools. Two and a half percent of the target sample were
4th-graders (about 10 years old), 6% were 5th-graders,
14.7% were 6th-graders, 44.3% were 7th-graders, 17.3%
were 8th-graders, 6.9% were 9th-graders, 2.3% were 10thgraders, 0.4% were 11th-graders, and 0.1% were 12th-graders.
Regarding the overall missingness of the peer relationship variables in the target sample, all of the target
participants had at least one peer nomination at one
timepoint. Regarding missingness for other variables (e.g.,
social anxiety), the program we used imputes or estimates
information about covariates based on other existing
participant data (Huisman and Snijders 2003). Eighty-nine
percent of the adolescents in the sample completed 1
measurement, 78.3% completed 2 measurements, whereas
65.7% completed all 3 measurements of data on social
anxiety. Roughly 4% of the adolescents in the target sample
were first-generation immigrants at Time 1. Sixty-eight
percent of the sample lived in households with both
biological parents; 19% lived in single-parent households,
and 13% lived with one stepparent and one biological
parent.
Method
Procedure
Participants
Adolescents filled out questionnaires during school time.
They were informed about the types of questions they
would answer, and how long it would take to answer them.
They were told that participation in the study was voluntary,
Participants were from a large, community-based study
conducted in a community with a total population of about
1242
and that they might choose to do something else should
they decide not to participate. They were also guaranteed
that their answers would be strictly confidential, and never
shown to parents, teachers, or other people. Trained
research assistants administered the questionnaires, with
no teachers present during the sessions. Before the data
collections, parents were informed about the study
through community-based meetings and via letters. With
the letter, they received a postage-paid card to return
without cost if they did not want their adolescent to
participate (only 1% of the parents did so). The parents
were also informed that they could withdraw their
adolescent from the study at any time. No participant
was paid for taking part in the study. In each of the
classes we held a drawing for movie tickets, however, for
which all adolescents were eligible, regardless of participation. All of the measures and procedures were approved by the
University Ethics Review Board.
Measures
Peer Relationships Adolescents were asked to identify up
to “three important peers, someone you talk with, hang
out with, and do things with” (Kerr et al. 2007). In
addition, they identified up to 10 peers with whom they
spent time in school and up to 10 peers with whom they
spent time outside of school. Up to 23 reciprocal and nonreciprocal nominations of peers each participant spent
time with in school and in free-time were collected
annually. That is, adolescents were allowed to nominate
peers regardless of whether these peers reciprocated the
peer relationship or not. For the current study, when
participants nominated siblings or romantic partners as
important peers, these were excluded from the analyses so that we could focus only on other peers. We
combined the three different types of peer relationship
nominations by identifying the unique peers who were
nominated at each measurement. The mean number of
nominated peers ranged between 3.2 and 4.6 across the
measurements. Of all peer relationships in the network,
36–41% were formed from one measurement to the
next, 26–31% were ended from one measurement to the
next, and 30–33% remained stable from one measurement to the next.
Self-Perceptions of Crowd Affiliation Before developing a
peer crowd affiliation measure, we collected information
about existing crowds by talking to teachers, students,
and adolescent leaders in the community. We then
developed a list of peer crowds that, based on the
information we received, existed in the community. This
list was included in the questionnaires, and adolescents
were asked to choose the peer crowd they most identified
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
with from the list. They were also given the option to
indicate that they did not identify with any crowd, or
they could add a crowd if the one they identified with
was not on the list.
Formation of Crowd Groups We combined crowds into
crowd groups in the same way as was done in a previous
study that examined Radical crowds and socially anxious
behavior (Bešić and Kerr 2009). The idea was to compare
adolescents in the Radical crowds with those in other
crowds that have been shown to be high on social anxiety,
thus creating a stringent test of the role of Radical crowds
in the socialization of social anxiety.
The Radical Crowd Group Previously, several crowds were
combined into a Radical crowd, and the predictive validity
of the measure was shown (Bešić and Kerr 2009). Peer
crowds that were considered by those who had spent time
in the schools teaching or collecting data as eye-catching in
a way that seemed calculated to shock, scare, or put people
off were included in the Radical crowd group. The Punks
and Goths appear in other studies of peer crowds, whereas
the Synths (so named for their interest in electronically
synthesized music, and their all-black clothing and oftenblack, dramatically styled hair) and the Aesthetics may be
specific to adolescent culture in Sweden. The Aesthetics
create startling appearances in a large variety of ways, for
example, creating space-alien-type hair sculptures or
wearing welder’s goggles, and are typically found among
adolescents who have chosen or are planning to choose
the music, drama, and art high school track. Thus, the
Punks, Goths, Synths, and Aesthetics formed the Radical
crowd group. Fourteen percent affiliated with the Radicals across all three timepoints; 29% remained affiliated
with the Radicals from Time 1 to Time 2, and 23%
remained affiliated with the Radicals from Time 2 to
Time 3. Thus, Radical crowd membership was somewhat
stable over time.
Comparison Crowd Groups As in a previous study (Bešić
and Kerr 2009), and as explained in the introduction, we
compared Radicals with three crowd groups: Academics,
Independents, and Non-Radicals. Table 1 shows the
numbers of participants in each crowd for Times 1–3, as
well as the entire list of crowds.
Social Anxiety Social anxiety was measured with 8 questions
about fears in different situations (Gren-Landell et al.
2009). Examples of these were speaking in front of the
class, putting a hand up during class, and making a phone
call to someone one does not know. The adolescents rated
themselves on a three-point scale as having No fear (1) to
A lot of fear (3) in these situations. The Cronbach’s alphas
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
1243
Table 1 Numbers and ranges of participants in all crowd groups by gender at times 1–3
Crowd groups
Self-perceptions of
crowd affiliation
Radicals (NT1-T3 =14–45)
Aesthetics
Goths
Punks
Synths
None of the Identities
Computer Nerds
Brains
Sports
Hip-Hoppers
Role-Players
Snobs
Poppers
Skaters
Feminists
Vegans
Ravers
Environmental Activists
Independents (NT1-T3 =111–136)
Academics (NT1-T3 =66–128)
Non-Radicals (NT1-T3 =257–363)
Girls
(N=238)
Time 1
Boys
(N=260)
Girls
(N=268)
Time 2
Boys
(N=357)
Girls
(N=225)
Time 3
Boys
(N=281)
2
2
5
0
98
8
6
85
9
5
5
4
3
4
1
0
1
2
2
1
0
38
46
6
117
17
10
4
4
7
0
1
4
1
11
2
4
0
59
24
24
76
15
6
20
19
0
5
1
0
2
3
6
10
3
40
61
19
132
19
14
16
9
12
3
3
4
3
12
2
4
1
71
13
12
70
12
7
11
7
2
0
0
1
0
5
8
10
3
40
61
7
82
11
12
16
3
8
4
3
5
3
Total N:
were 0.73 for Time 1, 0.71 for Time 2, and 0.76 for Time 3.
The cross-year correlations ranged from 0.34 to 0.60.
Depression Depression was measured with the widely-used
Child Depression Scale from the Center for Epidemiological
Studies, which has shown good validity (Radloff 1977).
There were 20 questions rated on four-point scales from
Not at all (1) to Often (4). The scale assesses depressive
symptoms such as worry, sadness, hopelessness, lethargy,
and poor appetite. The adolescents were asked to think
about the past week. Examples of items were: I have
“worried about things I don’t usually worry about,” “felt
scared,” and “felt down and unhappy.” The Cronbach’s
alphas were 0.84 for Time 1, 0.83 for Time 2, and 0.84 for
Time 3. The cross-year correlations ranged from 0.39 to
0.51. Correlations between depression and social anxiety
ranged from 0.11 to 0.33 (ps<0.001–0.05) at the three
time points.
Self-Esteem Self-esteem was measured with the Rosenberg
scale (Rosenberg 1979). There were ten items, assessing
self-perceptions of one’s own worth. Examples of items
were: “On the whole, you are satisfied with yourself,”
“You think that you have many good characteristics,” and
“Sometimes you feel you are of no use.” The four-point
response scale ranged from Not true at all (1) to Very true
(4). The Cronbach’s alphas were 0.82 for Time 1, 0.86 for
Time 2, and 0.77 for Time 3. The cross-year correlations
498
625
506
ranged from 0.53 to 0.60. Correlations between self-esteem
and social anxiety ranged from 0.15 to 0.32 (ps<0.001) at the
three time points.
Strategy for Analysis
To examine selection and influence processes simultaneously,
we used the software program Simulation Investigation for
Empirical Network Analyses, or SIENA (Snijders 2001;
Snijders et al. 2007). This method has been successfully
applied to assess selection effects for personality in
adolescence (Selfhout et al. 2010), and selection and
influence processes regarding social anxiety (Van Zalk et
al. 2011). SIENA is currently the only program available that
can be used to study influence processes while controlling
for selection within adolescents’ networks of multiple peer
relationships. The analyses take into account that some peer
relationships evolve into reciprocal peer relationships whereas
others do not, and how all these peer relationships in turn
affect adolescents over time. When socialization is studied,
then, reciprocity is controlled for. Age, gender, internalizing,
and whether adolescents selected others based on being in the
same classroom and school were also controlled for in all of
the subsequent analyses. If information about any of the
variables in the current study were missing (>8% across
measurements), scores were imputed according to the
procedure developed by Huisman and Steglich (Huisman
and Steglich 2008). More details about the modeling and
1244
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
specific effects regarding this approach are provided in the
results section.
Results
Control Variables
Several effects of control variables on peer relationship
selection were found, and are shown in Table 2. First,
the negative outdegree parameter showed that participants
were likely to nominate significantly less than 50% of the
total network members as peers on average, thereby
indicating that adolescents were selective when forming
new peer relationships. Second, the reciprocated relationship effects, referring to the tendency to form reciprocated
peer relationships, showed that adolescents tended to
reciprocate their peer relationships. Third, the triadic
relationship effects, referring to the tendency to form
triadic peer relationships, indicated that adolescents
tended to directly select the peers of their peers over time.
Table 2 Selection effects within the social network
Parameter
Social anxiety
Social anxiety: selection similarity
Social anxiety: adolescents select
Social anxiety: peers select
Crowd group affiliation
Radicals: selection similarity
Radicals: adolescents select
Radicals: peers select
Academics: selection similarity
Academics: adolescents select
Academics: peers select
Independents: selection similarity
Independents: adolescents select
Independents: peers select
Non-Radicals: selection similarity
Non-Radicals: adolescents select
Non-Radicals: peers select
Control variables
Outdegree
Reciprocated relationship
Triadic relationship
Gender: selection similarity
Age: selection similarity
Depression: selection similarity
Self-esteem: selection similarity
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001
Unstandardized
effect
s.e.
0.85***
0.09***
−0.01*
0.30
0.01
<0.01
1.25*
−1.39*
−1.30*
−0.09
0.13
−0.19***
0.10
0.04
−0.10
0.13***
0.23***
−0.06
0.60
0.62
0.60
0.07
0.07
0.07
0.06
0.07
0.06
0.04
0.04
0.04
−3.42***
2.84***
0.43***
0.69***
2.23***
1.64***
0.02
0.03
0.04
0.01
0.02
0.11
0.13
0.24
Thus, effects of the network structure on selection were
significant and therefore needed to be controlled for in
further analyses.
In addition, all main effects were controlled for gender,
age, depression, and self-esteem when looking at these
similarity effects, as demonstrated in Table 2. Boys selected
boys more than girls, and girls selected girls more than
boys (as indicated by the gender: selection similarity
effect). Furthermore, adolescents tended to select peers of
the same age (as suggested by the age: selection
similarity effect). Adolescents with similar levels of
depressive symptoms tended to select each other as peers
(depression: selection similarity). Finally, no significant
effects of self-esteem on peer relationship selection were
found, suggesting that mean levels of and similarity in
self-esteem did not affect peer relationship selection.
Selection of Peers Based on Social Anxiety
For selection processes, we examined to what extent
variables predicted that a non-peer dyad at one measurement would change into a peer dyad at the next. The
parameters were calculated across all three measurements,
and controlled for reciprocation and non-reciprocation of
the dyad. This means that when examining changes in the
dependent variable selection (selecting a person as a peer
versus not selecting this person as a peer), the program
calculated whether this selection was reciprocated or
unreciprocated. The information from each wave was used
for the prediction of the subsequent wave.
To test whether similarity in social anxiety predicted
peer selection, we examined to what extent similarity in
social anxiety between all non-peers predicted selecting a
peer or not selecting a peer. This effect, labeled social
anxiety: selection similarity, is shown in Table 2. Higher
social anxiety: selection similarity predicted a higher
probability of peer selection over non-selection. Hence,
adolescents tended to select peers with similar levels of
social anxiety.
Selection of Peers Based on Crowd Group Membership
The Radical Crowd Group As the findings in Table 2 show,
adolescents tended to select peers with the same Radical
crowd membership (indicated by the Radicals: selection
similarity effect). Adolescents from the Radical crowd
group were more likely to select other Radical crowd
group members, whereas the other adolescents were more
likely to select peers who were not from the Radical
crowd group. Adolescents from the Radical crowd group
also tended to select fewer peers (indicated by Radicals:
adolescents select), and tended to be selected less by
others (indicated by Radicals: peers select).
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
The Academic Crowd Group Being in the Academic crowd
group did not significantly predict peer selection (as
shown by a non-significant Academics: selection similarity effect in Table 2). Adolescents in the Academic
crowd group were less selected by others as peers
(indicated by Academics: peers select), and the Academic
crowd group membership (i.e., Academics: adolescents
select) did not significantly predict how often they selected
peers. Hence, Academics did not tend to select each
other as peers, nor were they selected often by other
adolescents in the network.
The Independent Group Being in the Independent group
did not significantly predict peer selection (as shown by
Independents: selection similarity in Table 2). In a similar
vein, adolescents’ Independent status did not predict how
often they selected peers (i.e., Independents: adolescents
select), or how often they were selected by others as peers
(Independents: peers select). Thus, Independent status was
not predictive of peer relationship selection.
The Non-Radical Crowd Group For adolescents in all other
crowds, the results showed that Non-Radicals: selection
similarity predicted peer selection, implying that adolescents in Non-Radical crowds preferred peer relationships
with other adolescents from Non-Radical crowds. Additionally, adolescents from the Non-Radical crowd group
tended to select a larger number of peers (as shown by the
significant effect for Non-Radicals: adolescents select). In
addition, being from the Non-Radical crowd group did not
affect being selected as a peer by others (indicated by NonRadicals: peers select). In sum then, even though NonRadicals tended to select each other as peers and tended to
select a larger number of peers, they did not tend to be
selected particularly often by others in the network.
Socialization of Social Anxiety
Examining Socialization of Social Anxiety For socialization
processes, the dependent variable was represented by
changes in adolescents’ social anxiety. SIENA treats
changes in social anxiety as ordinal categorical values
(Snijders et al. 2007) and models changes in peer relationships and social anxiety as two interdependent processes,
thereby controlling each for the other. This means that
positive significant effects of the variables of interest on
these change scores of social anxiety indicated that higher
scores on the variable in question predicted a larger increase
in adolescents’ social anxiety than expected by chance,
holding all other effects of independent variables (including
those in selection) constant. When examining these effects,
individuals are assumed to be mutually dependent because
each individual figures simultaneously as an adolescent
1245
who is being influenced, and as a peer who influences
others. This mutual dependence is mediated by effects of
the network structure, such as reciprocated relationships
and triadic relationships. These effects, respectively, pertain
to the tendency to form reciprocated relationships (John
selects Mark and Mark selects John) and the tendency to
form triadic relationships (John selects Mark, Mark
selects Sue, and John selects Sue). This also controls
for the effect of small cliques on socialization, in that our
results then do not pertain to small clique formations of
up to three individuals. This way, all selection and
influence effects were adjusted for overlap in the network
(for full details, see Snijders 2001). We also controlled for
the trend in social anxiety by including both the linear
tendency of change and quadratic tendency of change in
the categorical scores of social anxiety (for more details,
see Snijders et al. 2010). Hence, prior levels of social
anxiety were controlled for. We will discuss effects of
targets‘ characteristics, peers’ characteristics, and interactions
between effects.
Effect of Peers’ Social Anxiety Our findings demonstrated
that peers’ social anxiety significantly predicted changes
in adolescents’ social anxiety, as shown in Table 3.
Adolescents showed a tendency to become similar to their
peers’ average social anxiety over time, thus supporting
the idea of socialization processes in peer relationships for
social anxiety.
Was Social Anxiety Socialized among Peers in the Radical
Crowd Group more than Others? To answer this, our
primary question of the study, we looked at the interactions
in the model. They are shown in Table 3. In particular, the
Peers’ social anxiety X Targets’ Radical affiliation interaction indicates whether social anxiety was socialized more
among peers in the Radical crowd group than in the other
crowd groups. As shown in the table, this was the only
significant interaction in the model, suggesting that the
answer is yes. We explored the interaction by using
post-hoc multiple group tests (Snijders et al. 2010) to
compare effects for adolescents who affiliated with the
Radical crowd group at at least one measurement (n=82)
with those who never affiliated with the Radical crowd
group (n=714). Figure 1 displays the differences between
the Radical crowd groups (unstandardized effect=1.13,
p<0.01) and the other crowd groups (unstandardized
effect = 0.33, p < 0.01) in the socialization of socially
anxious behavior over time. Although peers’ social
anxiety predicted changes in adolescents’ social anxiety
for both groups (unstandardized effect=0.87, p>0.05), the
effect was significantly larger (unstandardized effect=1.13,
p<0.05) for adolescents who affiliated with the Radical
crowd group than for adolescents who did not. Thus, our
1246
Table 3 Socialization effects
within the social network
* p<0.05; ** p<0.01; ***
p<0.001
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
Parameter
Linear shape social anxiety
Quadratic shape social anxiety
Peers’ social anxiety
Peers’ social anxiety * Targets’
Peers’ social anxiety * Targets’
Peers’ social anxiety * Targets’
Peers’ social anxiety * Targets’
Radical affiliation
Non-Radical affiliation
Academic affiliation
Independent affiliation
findings suggest that adolescents in the Radical crowd
group were more influenced by their peers’ social anxiety
than adolescents who were not members of the Radical
crowd group.
Is Social Anxiety Socialized Differently for Boys and Girls
among Peers in the Crowds? To answer this question, we
computed a three-way interaction between gender, Radical
crowd affiliation, and friend’s social anxiety predicting
changes in social anxiety. We controlled for all other main
effects, as well as two-way interaction effects (Gender X
Radical Crowd; Gender X Peers’ Social Anxiety, and
Radical Crowd X Social Anxiety) in the model. The
interaction between gender, Radical crowd, and friends’
social anxiety when predicting target’s changes in social
anxiety was not significant (unstandardized effect=0.01,
p>0.10). This implies that there were no gender differences
regarding the extent to which Radical crowd affiliation
interacted with friend’s social anxiety when predicting targets’
social anxiety.
Fig. 1 Interaction effects of Radical crowd affiliation by peers’ social
anxiety on adolescents’ social anxiety. Change in social anxiety was
controlled for previous levels of social anxiety
Unstandardized effect
s.e.
−0.24***
−0.02
0.87***
0.58***
0.04
0.06
0.19
0.05
<0.01
0.23
0.06
0.04
0.05
0.58
Discussion
Adolescents tend to affiliate with reputation-based collectives called peer crowds, and they are likely to form peer
ties with others who affiliate with similar crowds. Previous
research suggests, however, that membership in some peer
crowds is linked to depression (Hogue and Steinberg 1995;
Prinstein 2007; Rose 2002), and membership in crowds that
adopt eye-catching, sometimes extreme clothing, hair, and
makeup styles, in particular, is linked to social anxiety,
depression, and low self-esteem (Bešić and Kerr 2009).
In this study, we expanded on that knowledge by showing
that, controlling for depression and self-esteem, adolescents with Radical crowd affiliation were more influenced
by their peers’ social anxiety than were adolescents in
other crowds. We also showed that peers within the
Radical crowds became more and more isolated from the
rest of the social network, as they not only selected others
as peers to a lesser degree over time, but were also being
selected less over time. This might point to a bidirectional
process, where adolescents start affiliating with Radical
crowds, narrow their peer relationship ties in time, and
become less popular in the entire peer relationship
network. Over time, they may in turn socialize each other
into becoming more socially anxious, and these processes
seemed to function similarly for boys and girls. To our
knowledge, this is the first study to examine socialization
processes of socially anxious behavior within crowds in
early adolescence.
There are several possible ways in which adolescents
might socialize each others’ socially anxious behaviors.
First, peers could strengthen one another’s social anxiety by
signaling empathy or approval in situations that boost social
fears, or via discussions of awkward, fearful feelings. A
similar process, termed deviancy training, has been identified in delinquent adolescents’ peer relationships (Dishion
et al. 1996). Talking or commenting about antisocial
behavior has been linked to subsequent increases in the
actual antisocial behaviors, and this process might be
extended to social anxiety. As peer relationships are often
embedded in crowds, this may be a viable explanation.
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
A second way in which adolescents’ socially anxious
behaviors could be influenced by their peers is through
dwelling on the problems of social anxiety in discussions
with peers. This process, called co-rumination, has been
linked to peer socialization of depression (Rose 2002). It
refers to extensive discussions and focus on negative
emotions in problems with close reciprocal friends. By
strengthening the peer relationship bonds in dyads through
extensive self-disclosure, co-rumination increases feelings
of depression and anxiety (Rose 2002). Both depression
and anxiety are well-known correlates of social anxiety
(Cox et al. 2005; Elovainio et al. 2004), so co-rumination
might also apply to social anxiety. Further, even though
co-rumination is assumed to occur in close peer relationships, such interactions might also exist within the larger
contexts of peer crowds (Brown 1990; La Greca et al.
2001; Urberg et al. 2000). Regarding socially anxious
peers then, they might evoke even more socially fearful
behaviors over time due to frequent, intense discussions
about their social fears.
A third way in which adolescents’ socially anxious
behaviors could be affected by their peers‘ social anxiety is
through modeling behavior to garner approval. Adolescents
might model peers’ taciturn or withdrawn behaviors in
order to gain status or acceptance in the peer group, and
those who are already socially anxious might be more
prone to doing so. In one study, socially anxious girls
were found to be at higher risk for developing depressive
symptoms over time due to a greater susceptibility to peer
contagion (Prinstein 2007). As socially anxious individuals
usually have poor self-esteem (McCarroll et al. 2009), they
might be more vulnerable to peer contagion―both in
close relationships and in the crowd social network.
Consequently, socially anxious adolescents may be at
increased risk to reinforce their social anxiety because
they could be more vulnerable to peer contagion than
less socially anxious adolescents.
Extending this reasoning one step further, one may
wonder why social anxiety would be socialized more in the
Radical crowds than in others. One idea suggested in the
literature is that conformity may be an important part of
affiliating with a crowd that maintains a specific style of
clothing, hair, and makeup, and this emphasis on conformity might increase adolescents’ susceptibility to peer
influence (Brown et al. 1990). As previous research has
shown, socially anxious adolescents tend to conform more
easily than others (Cohen and Prinstein 2006; Santee and
Maslach 1982). As was suggested previously, adolescents
affiliating with the Radical crowds, more than others, could
value taciturn, withdrawn social interactional styles and
behaviors in addition to their eye-catching appearances.
Adolescents in these crowds might intensify their socially
anxious behaviors to conform to the group standard,
1247
without necessarily being aware of it. This might lead to
perceptions of the self as more socially anxious over time,
through a process of observing one’s own behavior and
concluding what attitudes or characteristics must have
caused it (Bem 1967).
Another explanation for social anxiety being socialized
more in the Radical crowds than in other crowds might lie
in the way adolescents’ peer relationships are constructed.
Studies show that adolescents are likely to have peers
within the same crowd (Brown et al. 1994; La Greca et al.
2001). Radical crowds tend to be small (Bešić and Kerr
2009), and adolescents in these crowds might have more
intimate, close peer relationships than adolescents in larger
crowds. As close peers tend to influence each other on both
externalizing (e.g., delinquency, see Burk et al. 2007;
Dishion et al. 1996; Kiesner et al. 2002) and internalizing
behavior (e.g., depression, see Hogue and Steinberg 1995;
Prinstein 2007; Rose 2002), Radical adolescents might be
more influenced by their peers’ social anxiety because their
relationships may be closer than those in larger crowds.
Because peer relationships in this study were partly
measured in terms of important peers in adolescents’ lives,
this seems a viable explanation.
This study is not without limitations. First, we only
included adolescents in our data if they had two consecutive waves of data with peer relationship nominations to
fulfill the criteria of the SIENA software. Thus, adolescents
who did not nominate any peers, which might indicate
social isolation, were not part of the study. Hence, it
remains unanswered whether they were affected by their
peers. In addition, whether adolescents had stable or
intimate, high-quality peer relationships was not examined
in this study and is another limitation. Previous research
shows, however, that the stability of peer relationships
might not be the most important factor in children’s
adjustment, but rather acquiring a peer (Laursen et al.
2007; Wojslawowicz Bowker et al. 2006). Peer relationships during adolescence also show high instability across
short periods of time (Cairns et al. 1995). Also, we cannot
rule out the possibility that our results might have been
due to clique formations rather than crowds, as cliques are
often embedded in peer crowds (Brown 1990). The size of
typical cliques has been disputed, with a lack of
consistency between how many individuals constitutes a
clique (Henrich et al. 2000). In our study, nonetheless, we
controlled for the effect of dyadic and triadic peer
relationship structures, implying that the results were true
over and above smaller clique formations. A further
limitation is that we did not have peer reports of crowd
affiliation, in order to verify adolescents’ self-perceptions
of crowd membership, which is nonetheless a limitation
shared by most previous studies. Finally, we concentrated
on one cohort of 7th-grade adolescents (about age 13) and
1248
their peers. Perhaps adolescents are more open to socialization
during this period than others, but we cannot say.
Despite these limitations, there are several strengths of
this study, as well. By using SIENA, we were able to
control for various selection effects on adolescents’ peer
relationships and various network effects, which have been
shown to be important (Haynie 2001). Thus, our results
provide unique insight into the selection and socialization
effects of social anxiety, over and above other important
effects. In addition, we included an entire cohort of
adolescents in one community, followed over three years,
and they were allowed to select peers both in and outside of
their classrooms and schools. This provides an ecologically
valid view of adolescents’ peer relationships. Finally, the
reports of peers’ characteristics were independent of the
target adolescents, as we included peers’ independent
reports of their own behavior. This gives us confidence
that our findings are not biased by adolescents’ projecting
their own behavior onto their peers (see Bauman and Ennett
1994) or believing that their peers are more similar to
themselves than they actually are (Prinstein and Wang
2005). Thus, despite its limitations, the study provides a
new way of viewing socialization of socially anxious
behaviors within adolescent crowds.
Developing a sense of identity is one of the major
developmental tasks for most young people. During some
part of their teenage years, many adolescents explore their
identities by adopting different ways of looking and
behaving, and by affiliating with different crowds. For a
smaller number of adolescents this may be taken to the
extreme, as they might adopt startling or even shocking
appearances. Besides being influenced by these crowds
in terms of clothing or music, however, it seems that
some behaviors may be socialized as well. Our data
showed that over time, peers in Radical crowds
socialized each other into becoming more socially
anxious. More research is needed in order to probe the
exact processes behind the connection between Radical
crowd affiliation and seemingly poor internal adjustment. Perhaps creating tolerance, openness, and an
inviting atmosphere in settings where crowds are most
salient, such as schools, may help prevent or diminish
some of these problems in the first place.
References
Bauman, K. E., & Ennett, S. T. (1994). Peer influence on adolescent
drug use. American Psychologist, 49, 820–822.
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: an alternative interpretation of
cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74,
183–200.
Benenson, J. F. (1990). Gender differences in social networks. The
Journal of Early Adolescence, 10, 472–495.
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
Bešić, N., & Kerr, M. (2009). Punks, Goths, and other eye-catching
peer crowds: do they fulfill a function for shy youths? Journal of
Research on Adolescence, 19, 113–121.
Brown, B. B. (1990). Peer groups and peer cultures. In S. S. Feldman
& G. R. Elliott (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing
adolescent (pp. 171–196). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Brown, B. B., & Lohr, M. J. (1987). Peer-group affiliation and
adolescent self-esteem: an integration of ego-identity and
symbolic interaction theories. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 52, 47–55.
Brown, B. B., Lohr, M. J., & Trujillo, C. (1990). Multiple crowds and
multiple life styles: adolescents’ perceptions of peer-group stereotypes. In R. E. Muus (Ed.), Adolescent behavior and society: A
book of readings, vol. 4 (pp. 30–36). NY: McGraw-Hill.
Brown, B. B., Mory, M. S., & Kinney, D. (1994). Casting adolescent
crowds in a relational perspective: Caricature, channel and
context. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Guillota
(Eds.), Personal relationships during adolescence, vol. 6 (pp.
123–167). London: Sage Publications.
Brown, B. B., Von Bank, H., & Steinberg, L. (2008). Smoke in the
looking glass: effects of discordance between self- and peer rated
crowd affiliation on adolescent anxiety, depression and selffeelings. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 1163–1177.
Bruch, M. A., & Cheek, J. M. (1995). Developmental factors in
childhood and adolescent shyness. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R.
Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia:
Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 163–182). NY:
Guilford.
Burk, W. J., Steglich, C. E. G., & Snijders, T. A. B. (2007). Beyond
dyadic interdependence: actor-oriented models for co-evolving
social networks and individual behaviors. International Journal
of Behavioral Development, 31, 397–404.
Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic.
Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Buchanan, L., & Cairns, B. D. (1995).
Friendships and social networks in childhood and adolescence:
fluidity, reliability, and interrelations. Child Development, 66,
1330–1345.
Cohen, G. L., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Peer contagion of aggression
and health risk behavior among adolescent males: an experimental
investigation of effects on public conduct and private attitudes.
Child Development, 77, 967–983.
Cox, B. J., MacPherson, P. S. R., & Enns, M. W. (2005). Pscychiatric
correlates of childhood shyness in a nationally representative
sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1019–1027.
Crozier, W. R. (1981). Shyness and self-esteem. British Journal of
Social Psychology, 20, 220–222.
Delsing, M. J. M. H., ter Bogt, T. F. M., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Meeus,
W. H. J. (2007). Adolescents’ peer crowd identification in the
Netherlands: structure and associations with problem behaviors.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 467–480.
Dishion, T. J., Spracklen, K. M., Andrews, D. W., & Patterson, G. R.
(1996). Deviancy training in male adolescents friendships.
Behavior Therapy, 27, 373–390.
Elovainio, M., Kivimäki, M., Puttonen, S., Heponiemi, T., Pulkki, L.,
& Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2004). Temperament and depressive
symptoms: a population-based longitudinal study on Cloninger’s
psychobiological temperament model. Journal of Affective
Disorders, 83, 227–232.
Gren-Landell, M., Tillfors, M., Furmark, T., Bohlin, G., Andersson,
G., & Svedin, C. G. (2009). Social phobia in Swedish
adolescents. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology,
44, 1–7.
Haynie, D. L. (2001). Delinquent peers revisited: does network structure
matter? American Journal of Sociology, 106, 1013–1057.
Heaven, P. C. L., Ciarrochi, J., Vialle, W., & Cechavicuite, I. (2005).
Adolescent peer crowd self-identification, attributional style and
J Abnorm Child Psychol (2011) 39:1239–1249
perceptions of parenting. Journal of Community & Applied
Social Psychology, 15, 313–318.
Henrich, C. C., Kupermine, G. P., Sack, A., Blatt, S. J., & Leadbeater,
B. J. (2000). Characteristics and homogeneity of early adolescent
friendship groups: a comparison of male and female clique and
nonclique members. Applied Developmental Science, 4, 15–26.
Hogue, A., & Steinberg, L. (1995). Homophily of internalized distress in
adolescent peer groups. Developmental Psychology, 31, 897–906.
Huisman, M., & Snijders, T. A. B. (2003). Statistical analysis of
longitudinal network data with changing composition. Sociological
Methods Research, 32, 253–287.
Huisman, M., & Steglich, C. (2008). Treatment of non-response in
longitudinal network studies. Social Networks, 30, 297–308.
Kandel, D. B. (1978). Homophily, selection, and socialization in
adolescent friendships. The American Journal of Sociology, 84,
427–436.
Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Kiesner, J. (2007). Peers and problem
behavior: Have we missed something? In R. C. M. E. Engels
(Ed.), Friends, lovers and groups: Key relationships in adolescence. Hot topics in developmental research (pp. 125-153). New
York: John Wiley & Sons.
Kiesner, J., Cadinu, M., Poulin, F., & Bucci, M. (2002). Group
identification in early adolescence: Its relation with peer
adjustment and its moderator effect on peer influence. Child
Development, 73, 196–208.
Kinney, D. A. (1993). From Nerds to Normals: the recovery of identity
among adolescents from middle school to high school. Sociology
of Education, 66, 21–40.
Kobus, K. (2003). Peers and adolescent smoking. Addiction Special Issue:
Contexts and Adolescent Tobacco Use Trajectories, 98, 37–55.
La Greca, A., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents:
linkages with peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 26, 83–94.
La Greca, A. M., Prinstein, M. J., & Fetter, M. D. (2001). Adolescent
peer crowd affiliation: linkages with health-risk behaviors and
close friendships. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 26, 131–143.
Laursen, B., Bukowski, W. M., Aunola, K., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2007).
Friendship moderates prospective associations between social
isolation and adjustment problems in young children. Child
Development, 78, 1395–1404.
Mackey, E. R., & La Greca, A. M. (2007). Adolescents’ eating,
exercise, and weight control behaviors: does peer crowd
affiliation play a role? Journal of Pediatric Psychology Special
Issue: Pediatric Overweight, 32, 13–23.
McCarroll, E. M., Lindsey, E. W., MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Campbell
Chambers, J., & Frabutt, J. M. (2009). Health status and peer
relationships in early adolescents: the role of peer contact, selfesteem, and social anxiety. Journal of Child & Family Studies,
18, 473–485.
Newman, P. R., & Newman, B. M. (1976). Early adolescence and
its conflict: group identity versus alienation. Adolescence, 11,
261–274.
Pilkonis, P. A. (1977). The behavioral consequences of shyness.
Journal of Personality, 45, 596–611.
Prinstein, M. J. (2007). Moderators of peer contagion: a longitudinal
examination of depression socialization between adolescents and
their best friends. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Psychology, 36, 159–170.
Prinstein, M. J., & La Greca, A. M. (2002). Peer crowd affiliation and
internalizing distress in childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal follow-back study. Journal of Research on Adolescence,
12, 325–351.
Prinstein, M. J., & Wang, S. S. (2005). False consensus and adolescent
peer contagion: examining discrepancies between perceptions
1249
and actual reported levels of friends’ deviant abd health risk
behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 293–306.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale
for research in the general population. Applied Psychological
Measurement, 1, 385–401.
Romero, L. E., & Epkins, C. C. (2008). Girls’ cognitions of
hypothetical friends: are they related to depression, loneliness,
social anxiety and perceived similarity? Journal of Social and
Personal Relationships, 25, 311–332.
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys.
Child Development, 73, 1830–1843.
Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic books.
Santee, R. T., & Maslach, C. (1982). To agree or not to agree: personal
dissent amid social pressure to conform. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 42, 690–700.
Selfhout, M. H., Branje, S. J., & Meeus, W. H. (2009). Developmental
trajectories of perceived friendship intimacy, constructive problem solving, and depression from early to late adolescence.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 251–264.
Selfhout, M., Burk, W., Branje, S., Denissen, J., van Aken, M., &
Meeus, W. (2010). Emerging late adolescent friendship networks
and Big Five personality traits: a social network approach.
Journal of Personality, 78, 509–538.
Smith, H. M., & Betz, N. E. (2002). An examination of efficacy and
esteem pathways to depression in young adulthood. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 49, 438–448.
Snijders, T. A. B. (2001). The statistical evaluation of social network
dynamics. Sociological methodology, 31, 361–395.
Snijders, T. A. B., Steglich, C. E. G., Schweinberger, M., & Huisman,
M. (2007). Manual for SIENA version 3. Groningen: University
of Groningen.
Snijders, T. A. B., van de Bunt, G. G., & Steglich, C. E. G. (2010).
Introduction to actor-based models for network dynamics. Social
Networks, 32, 44–60.
Stednitz, J. N., & Epkins, C. C. (2006). Girls’ and mothers’ social
anxiety, social skills, and loneliness: associations after accounting
for depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 35, 148–154.
Stone, M., & Brown, B. B. (1998). In the eye of the beholder:
adolescents’ perceptions of peer crowd stereotypes. In R. E.
Muus (Ed.), Adolescent behavior and society: A book of readings
(pp. 158–169). NY: McGraw-Hill.
Sweden, S. (2004). Köping municipal facts. Retrieved from: http://
www.koping.se/kommunfakta/2004/Municipal_facts_2004.pdf.
Thurlow, C. (2001). The usual suspects? A comparative investigation of crowds and social-type labelling among young British
teenagers. Journal of Youth Studies, 4, 319–334.
Urberg, K. A., Değirmencioğlu, S. M., Tolson, J. M., & HallidayScher, K. (2000). Adolescent social crowds: measurement and
relationship to friends. Journal of Adolescent Research, 15,
427–445.
Van Zalk, M. H. W., Kerr, M., Branje, S. J. T., Stattin, H., & Meeus, W.
H. J. (2010). It takes three: selection, influence, and de-selection
processes of depression in adolescent friendship networks.
Developmental Psychology, 46, 927–938.
Van Zalk, N., Van Zalk, M., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2011). Social
anxiety as a basis for friendship selection and socialization in
adolescents’ social networks. Journal of Personality, 79, 499–
526.
Wojslawowicz Bowker, J. C., Rubin, K. H., Burgess, K. B.,
Booth-LaForce, C., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (2006). Behavioral
characteristics associated with stable and fluid best friendship patterns in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,
52, 671–693.