Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611. With 3 figures
No evidence for an indirect benefit from female mate
preference in Arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus, but
female ornamentation decreases offspring viability
MATTI JANHUNEN1*, JUKKA KEKÄLÄINEN2,3, RAINE KORTET3,
PEKKA HYVÄRINEN4 and JORMA PIIRONEN1
1
Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute, Joensuu Game and Fisheries Research,
Yliopistonkatu 6, FI-80100 Joensuu, Finland
2
Department of Biological and Environmental Science, University of Jyväskylä, PO Box 35, FI-40014
Jyväskylä, Finland
3
Department of Biology, University of Eastern Finland, PO Box 111, FI-80101 Joensuu, Finland
4
Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute, Kainuu Fisheries Research, Manamansalontie 90,
FI-88300 Paltamo, Finland
Received 19 January 2011; revised 19 February 2011; accepted for publication 19 February 2011
bij_1659
602..611
Female mate choice is considered an important evolutionary agent, but there has been an ongoing debate over the
fitness consequences it produces, especially in species that have a resource-free mating system. We examined a
potential fitness benefit resulting from the pre-spawning mate preference in Arctic charr Salvelinus alpinus, a
salmonid fish with no parental care. The females were first allowed to discriminate behaviourally between two
males presented to them in a free choice test. We then tested with controlled fertilizations whether the females
would accrue indirect genetic benefits for their offspring, as measured by embryonic viability, if they had mated
with the male they preferred. Both parental identities influenced offspring survivorship, but the females did not
consistently prefer the male which gave her the higher reproductive success. Neither was the degree of male red
breeding coloration associated with female preference or the observable genetic quality. In contrast, there was a
negative relationship between female coloration and her offspring survivorship, suggesting a significant trade-off
in resource investment between sexual ornamentation and reproduction. To conclude, the potential indirect fitness
consequences arising from females’ pre-spawning mate preference seem to be negligible in early stages of
development of Arctic charr. © 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society,
2011, 103, 602–611.
ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: carotenoid coloration – genetic quality – mate choice – maternal effects –
salmonids.
INTRODUCTION
In general, mate choice has been considered an important evolutionary agent, and has a major influence on
individual fitness (Andersson, 1994; Puurtinen,
Ketola & Kotiaho, 2009). Owing to the disparity of
*Corresponding author. E-mail: matti.janhunen@rktl.fi
602
reproductive inputs between sexes, females are generally predicted to be more selective of their mates
than are males (Andersson, 1994; Reynolds, 1996).
When there are no direct, material benefits available
to females from mate choice, females should theoretically obtain indirect genetic benefits for mate choice
to evolve (Tregenza & Wedell, 2000; Kokko et al.,
2003; Nordeide, 2007). In such resource-free mating
systems, female preference may be based on male
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
ARCTIC CHARR MATE PREFERENCE
traits that signal heritable quality (e.g. additive ‘good
genes’ effects), or alternatively on genetic compatibility (i.e. non-additive genetic benefits) (Tregenza &
Wedell, 2000; Mays & Hill, 2004; Neff & Pitcher,
2005; Puurtinen et al., 2009). As far as the mate
choice is predominantly based on ‘good genes’,
females are expected to show congruence in their
preference for sexually selected ornamental traits,
which reveal the differences in individuals’ quality. In
other words, genes of certain high-quality (and highly
ornamented) males are expected to be good for all
females (Møller & Alatalo, 1999; Hunt et al., 2004).
Instead, genetic compatibility differs from mate
choice for ‘good genes’ in that the variation in offspring viability should be dependent on how well the
alleles from two parents function together (Tregenza
& Wedell, 2000; Mays & Hill, 2004; Reid, 2007;
Kekäläinen et al., 2009).
As males are able to produce a large number of
microgametes (sperm), they are likely to maximize
their reproductive effort by mating as many times as
possible (Bateman, 1948). Females, by contrast, have
to expend more energy to produce more expensive
macrogametes (eggs), and thus females can be
expected to favour mate quality over quantity
(Trivers, 1972). This also appears to be the case in
most salmonid species for which the operational sex
ratio in breeding systems is usually male-biased, and
the total energy investment in gamete production is
considerably higher for females than for males
(reviewed by Fleming & Reynolds, 2004). Given that
in many species males provide virtually no resources
other than sperm, they are likely to be chosen
mainly for indirect genetic benefits (e.g. Forsberg
et al., 2007; Consuegra & Garcia de Leaniz, 2008).
Nevertheless, it remains unclear to what extent
female preference actually determines the outcome
of reproduction in wild populations. In the brown
trout Salmo trutta L., for example, females seem to
choose their mates based on the male’s body size
and/or on certain external structures (Petersson
et al., 1999; Labonne et al., 2009), but this supposedly adaptive mating behaviour may be over-ridden
in importance by male–male competition (Petersson
et al., 1999; see also Petersson & Järvi, 2007). Furthermore, the sexual conflict theory suggests that
the manipulative behaviour among males may
rather be a hindrance than a help for inter-sexual
mate choice (Moore et al., 2001; Wong & Candolin,
2005; Garner et al., 2010). Thus, the effects of female
mate preference may often be manifested only when
the confounding forms of reproductive competition
are eliminated or controlled for (e.g. Bluhm &
Gowaty, 2004; Anderson, Kim & Gowaty, 2007).
In the lek-like breeding system of Arctic charr
Salvelinus alpinus L., sexually mature individuals
603
aggregate at the spawning sites (Sigurjónsdóttir &
Gunnarson, 1989; Figenschou, Folstad & Liljedal,
2004), and the most dominant (or largest) males tend
to establish and defend territories frequently visited
by females (Fabricius & Gustafson, 1954). The expression of mutual, yet sexually dichromatic carotenoidbased red breeding coloration is assumed to be a
sexually selected attribute that may provide important information in mate choice (reviewed in Møller
et al., 2000). Red intensity can, for example, be linked
to immune function (Skarstein & Folstad, 1996) and
milt characteristics (Måsvær, Liljedal & Folstad,
2004; Janhunen et al., 2009), but there is also somewhat controversial information on whether it is a
useful indicator of individual condition and health
state (Skarstein, Folstad & Rønning, 2005; Nordeide
et al., 2008). To our knowledge, no one has tested the
functional significance of breeding coloration in
respect of assortative mating preference in this
species. However, a recent crossing experiment demonstrated a positive relationship between paternal
redness and offspring endogenous growth (Eilertsen
et al., 2009), suggesting that red coloration may
signal, at least in some males, the underlying genetic
quality as well. In females, on the other hand, the
allocation of carotenoid pigments occurs, in addition
to their own body maintenance and skin coloration,
also to their eggs (through the yolk) (Blount, Houston
& Møller, 2000; Nordeide et al., 2008), which may
further complicate the informative content and evolution of this ornament. Yolk carotenoids are known
to be highly responsible for egg-quality maternal
effects in fish (e.g. Torrissen, 1984; Ahmadi et al.,
2006), and according to a recent study on Arctic charr,
the carotenoid-mediated effects may have implications on phenotype for fitness in mothers and their
offspring (Janhunen et al., 2010b).
In this study, we present the results of two interrelated experiments. We first followed the prespawning behaviour of Arctic charr females in a
simplified ‘free choice’ situation (a population-level
preference test). Reproductively active females were
provided an opportunity to choose between two nest
sites, each of which was ‘guarded’ by a male in the
cage. Use of a remote monitoring system and passive
integrated transponder (PIT) tags enabled us to continuously track female visits in the vicinity of both
prospective mates. First, our aim was to determine
whether the females display a pre-spawning mate
preference that is consistently associated with the
male’s red spawning coloration when the potential
social constraints, such as intra-sexual contests or
male harassment of females, are eliminated. Second,
we assessed with controlled fertilizations whether the
observed, freely expressed mate preference is selectively advantageous to a female, i.e. yields genetic
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
604
M. JANHUNEN ET AL.
benefits that are revealed via offspring’s enhanced
viability during embryonic development. In other
words, we were primarily interested in females that
clearly preferred either of the two males, so that we
could experimentally pair these females with preferred and non-preferred mates (maternal half-sibling
broods) and test for offspring’s early survival differences between these individuals. The potential effects
of parental coloration on offspring viability were also
examined.
MATERIAL AND METHODS
STUDY FISH AND THEIR SAMPLING
The studied Arctic charr originated from the Lake
Inari population and represented a hatchery brood
stock held at the nearby Sarmijärvi Aquaculture
station (Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute; 68°47′N, 28°8′E), in north-eastern Finland. The
experimental fish were 7 years old (year-class 2002)
and derived from a rearing lot produced by tens of
paired fertilizations (i.e. 22 wild-caught males and
26 first-generation hatchery females had been used
as founding individuals). The fish were fed continuously (ad libitum) with carotenoid-rich salmonid
food (Rehuraisio Emo-Vital; astaxanthin content
80 mg kg-1).
The experiment was conducted following the principles of animal treatment and welfare for scientific
experimentation according to permission given by the
National Animal Experiment Board (Project licence:
ESLH-2008-04178/Ym-23). Before the spawning
period (14–15 September 2009), 32 females and 56
males were randomly selected among the maturing
fish and anaesthetized with clove oil (10 mL L-1) for
the measuring and tagging operations. The fish were
measured for their total length (to the nearest 5 mm)
and body mass (to the nearest 1 g) and were individually tagged with PITs (half duplex PIT-tags; Texas
Instruments Inc., Dallas, TX, USA). The PIT-tags
were surgically implanted through the belly skin into
the body cavity of the fish. To improve the detection
efficacy of females during the experiment, larger PITtags (32 ¥ 4 mm) were used for females than for males
(23 ¥ 4 mm). Also, males were tagged for later identification. At this stage, the males were ranked by eye
as ‘more colourful’ and ‘less colourful’ individuals,
based on their abdominal red intensity, and accordingly they were also individually tagged with either
yellow or beige Floy (T-bar anchor) tags (Hallprint Pty
Ltd, Hindmarsh Valley, South Australia) mounted on
the base of the dorsal fin. In order to have a quantitative measure of the abdominal coloration, the skin
coloration of each male was quantified with a handheld Minolta CR-10 colorimeter (Konica Minolta
Sensing Americas Inc., Ramsey, NJ, USA) on the left
flank from two skin areas: behind the tip of the
pectoral fin and above the anal fin. The instrument
uses parameter values in the CIE 1976 L*a*b* colour
system mode [CIE (International Commission on Illumination), 1986], where L* indicates the reciprocal
difference between black and white, i.e. lightness
(0–100), a* represents the difference between red
(+a*) and green (–a*), and b* represents the difference
between yellow (+b*) and blue (–a*). Furthermore, a
calculatory definition of colour saturation (i.e.
chroma) was obtained as follows: C* = (a*2 + b*2)0.5.
The two values of this parameter were averaged to
provide the assessment of colourfulness for each male.
The C*-values (mean ± SD) of ‘more colourful’ and
‘less colourful’ males were 44.3 ± 4.8 and 30.3 ± 4.1,
respectively, and this difference was also statistically
significant (t-test, t = 11.76, P < 0.001, N = 56). C* was
highly correlated with both a* and b* (Spearman’s
rank correlation, r = 0.82 and 0.93, respectively,
P < 0.001, N = 56, in both cases), and thus its value
can be assumed to inform about the amount of
carotenoid-based pigmentation in the skin (Hatlen,
Jobling & Bjerkeng, 1998).
The fish were allowed to recover from the markings
for 1 day, after which they were transported to the
Kainuu Fisheries Research station (Finnish Game
and Fisheries Research Institute), Paltamo, central
Finland (64°30′N, 27°10′E), where the experiments
were performed.
FEMALE
PREFERENCE TRIALS
The study was conducted in seven circular outdoor
ponds made of concrete (bottom area 38.5 m2, water
depth 60–80 cm; Fig. 1). There were two cages made
of plastic-coated steel wire (160 ¥ 80 ¥ 70 cm, mesh
size 13 ¥ 13 mm) in each pond, and an artificial
spawning ground (hereafter referred to as a nest site)
was placed in front of each cage. The nest sites were
built of two shallow ‘bread boxes’, which were joined
together (72 ¥ 55 ¥ 10 cm) and filled with spawnable
substratum, i.e. gravel and cobbles. Earlier observations have clearly indicated that females find boxes
and substratum suitable for spawning, i.e. dig nests
in them (J. Piironen, pers. observ.). The cages and
nest sites were positioned in each pond so that their
reciprocal differences in terms of water flow conditions were minimal. A stationary PIT system detected
females’ movements continuously between nests
using one flat-bed antenna (a coil inductor loop) on
each nest site. PVC-coated multistrand copper wire
(4 mm2) was used for six loops for each antenna. The
wire was strung through 32-mm PVC pipe. Each
antenna was connected to a Texas Instruments
tuning module which in turn was connected to a
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
ARCTIC CHARR MATE PREFERENCE
Figure 1. Schematic picture of the experimental unit. (1)
Water inlet, from which the water current was conducted
to the experimental pond (2) along the exterior ring. (3)
Water outlet. (4) A pair of spawning grounds (‘nest boxes’)
equipped with circular PIT-antennae. (5) The cages for the
males.
reader with a twin-axial cable (105 W, length
10–50 m). The reader was constructed of reader and
control modules. Readers were connected to two
laptop computers with RS-232 cable through 8 ¥ RS
port adapter in each. The system was configured to
run and save (TIRIS datalogger programme, Citius
Solutions Oy, Helsinki, Finland) ASCII data to computers simultaneously from seven antennas to each.
The system logged detected tag IDs and date and time
(nine times per second from each antenna) of detection from each antenna. Prior to the trials, the system
was tuned and the comparability of the reading distances (c. 20 cm) within each antenna was verified.
The observational trials were carried out in four
consecutive periods during the spawning period of
Arctic charr (26 October–12 November 2009). The
trials comprised seven experimental units (ponds), in
each of which one female and two males were held for
about 4 days (90–96 h). The last observational period
was reserved for the reuse of those females, from
which we had not previously obtained reliable results
(due to technical problems or the passivity of individuals), and then different male pairs were presented to each female from the previous time. The
males used in the experiments had a mean length of
58.0 ± 3.0 cm (SD) (range: 52.0–64.0 cm) and mass of
605
3536 ± 742 g (range: 2065–5362 g). The females had a
mean length of 62.0 ± 4.0 cm (SD) (range: 51.5–
69.5 cm) and mass of 3910 ± 991 g (range: 1874–
6169 g). The males within each pair were sizematched (a maximum difference in body length was
5%), but they belonged to different coloration categories, i.e. were previously ranked as ‘more colourful’
and ‘less colourful’ individuals. A male was placed in
each of the two cages, after which a randomly
assigned, ovulated female was put into the pond. The
female was allowed to swim freely and inspect both
males in their cages as well as the nest sites close to
them. There was no physical contact between the
three individuals, and moreover the opposite ends of
the cages were covered with black plastic to prevent
the males from seeing each other.
Each time a female was detected on either nest, the
identification code was relayed to a computer in the
observation room and saved on an antenna-specific
file with date and time stamps. After each trial, the
recorded PIT-tag data were filtered and then summarized using a special software package (PIT-Data,
N. Vuokko, 2007–2010, http://users.ics.tkk.fi/ntvuok/
rktl/). For each female, the seconds per minute spent
in the detection range of each antenna were summed
up for the entire observation period. Female preference for either male was interpreted on the basis of
the overall time they were on each nest site. Finally,
following the criteria of Drickamer, Gowaty & Holmes
(2000) and Drickamer, Gowaty & Wagner (2003), only
those cases for which the time ratio between the two
antennae was 60:40 or more (in percentages) were
used for further analyses [the average total time
spent on the two nest sites was 315 ± 117 min (SE),
N = 12 females].
IN
VITRO FERTILIZATIONS AND INCUBATION
After the behavioural trials, we tested, using a halfsib breeding design, whether female preference or
male/female coloration is linked to offspring viability
during incubation. The fertilizations were conducted
in four sequential stages after each experimental
period. The eggs of each female were stripped and
divided into two portions. One portion was then fertilized with milt from the preferred male and the
other one with milt from the non-preferred male,
resulting in 12 maternal half-sib family pairs. In
addition, seven extra half-sib family pairs were produced from females for which the mate preference
result had remained unavailable. These extra families were included in the data set when we examined
the effect of parental coloration on progeny traits.
Excess quantities of milt were used to secure
maximal fertilizations. The abdominal coloration of
each parental fish (both males and females) was measured again as described previously.
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
606
M. JANHUNEN ET AL.
Following fertilizations, the swollen eggs from
each family were divided into four batches of 100
eggs (N = 400 per family). Each batch was placed
into an individual floating cylinder (depth 20 cm,
diameter 10 cm) with plastic grid bottom. The cylinders were introduced into four, independent, circular 3.5-m2 rearing tanks with a flow-through
water supply (water inflow c. 24 L min-1). Each
family was thereby replicated once in each of the
four tanks, resulting in 38 containers per tank. The
temperature of the inflow water varied between 1.6
and 3.4 °C and oxygen level varied between 6.3 and
11.2 mg L-1 during the incubation period (from
November 2009 to early April 2010). The number of
dead eggs (i.e. those turning partially or completely
white) was counted for the first time after about 1
week after the fertilizations [grand mean across all
families = 3.1 ± 0.7% (SE), N = 38]. Thereafter, dead
eggs were counted and removed weekly to minimize
the risk of fungal infections.
Offspring survivorship was assessed after the eggs
in each container had developed visible eye pigmentation (3–13 February; post-fertilization degree-days
c. 235 °C). The egg batches were then poured out at
least twice from the incubators into a plastic cup at a
height of 20 cm. After this mechanical standard treatment, the eggs that contained non-viable embryos
could be recorded. The exact family-specific survival
rates to eyed stage were then determined as the
number (proportion) of remaining live eggs per replicate. Inferring from the low mortality rates at the
start of the incubation, perhaps only a minor proportion of the total variation in this viability measure is
attributable to variation in fertilization success (see
also Johnston, 2002; Janhunen et al., 2011).
STATISTICAL
ANALYSES
Wilcoxon’s signed-rank test was used to identify
whether females consistently preferred, or spent more
time (in total amount of seconds) on, the nest site
close to a male classified as the more colourful
(redder). A linear mixed model (REML procedure) was
used to assess whether female mate preference or
male coloration (ranked within pairs) affect offspring
early survivorship. Here, the number of embryos surviving to eyed stage per replicate was used as a
response variable. Female preference (preferred vs.
non-preferred) or male coloration status (‘more colourful’ vs. ‘less colourful’) was included in the model as a
two-levelled fixed factor and female identity and male
identity (nested within female identity) as random
factors. Fixed effect was tested using F-tests and
random effects using likelihood ratio test. Finally,
linear regression was used to determine whether
female coloration could explain variation in offspring
viability. The survival data were incorporated into
the regression model by entering the grand means
of females (obtained from two half-sib family means
per female) against the average log-transformed
C*-values. The possible confounding effect of female
length proved to be insignificant in explaining variation in progeny survivorship (P = 0.951), and it was
therefore excluded from the final regression model.
The repeatability of family replicates was tested
using AV Bio-Statistics software (A. Vainikka, http://
personal.inet.fi/koti/ansvain/avbs/). All other statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 15.0,
for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).
RESULTS
FEMALE
PREFERENCE EXPERIMENT
Twelve females presented with two males in our mate
preference trials provided an interpretable selectivity
result, i.e. the overall time ratio they spent on the two
nest sites was 60:40 or larger (range 60:40–98:2).
Further, the observed time ratio was larger than
70:30 in ten cases.
In each of the 38 males used in the crossings the
saturation of red abdominal coloration was lower
after the trials, compared with that measured in
connection with tagging (a probable consequence of
altered environmental conditions). The mean withinindividual decrease in the average C*-value was 32%
(range 14–49%). As a consequence, the relative colour
difference narrowed within 17 male pairs, although
the previous within-pair rankings into ‘more colourful’ and ‘less colourful’ individuals remained
unchanged. The mean within-pair difference in the
C*-values was 8.9 ± 0.9 (SE) after the matepreference trials. Females did not consistently show a
preference for the more colourful individual in male
pairs (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: Z = -0.55, P = 0.583;
N = 12).
OFFSPRING
VIABILITY
We found large variation in offspring survival rates
from fertilization to eyed stage across all families,
although there was virtually no difference in the
proportions of survived offspring from females mated
with preferred males compared with offspring from
females mated with non-preferred males (0.39 ± 0.27
and 0.41 ± 0.30, respectively; F1,11 = 0.49, P = 0.498;
Fig. 2). Both parental identities contributed to
progeny viability [female: c21 = 26.12, P < 0.001; male
(female): c21 = 20.95, P < 0.001], but the independent
effect of females appeared to be considerably larger
than that of males (Fig. 2). We did not find a statistically significant difference between the survival
rates of half-sibships sired by ‘more colourful’ and
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
ARCTIC CHARR MATE PREFERENCE
Figure 2. The proportions of embryos survived to eyeembryo stage. The egg batches of each female (N = 4) were
fertilized with sperm from a preferred male (black bar)
and a non-preferred male (grey bar). Error bars indicate
one standard error.
Figure 3. The relationship between female red coloration
(chroma, C*) and offspring’s early survival rates in Arctic
charr (adjusted r2 = 0.181, P = 0.039). Survival values represent the grand means obtained from two maternal halfsib families.
‘less colourful’ males (F1,18 = 0.95, P = 0.34). Instead,
the regression analysis revealed a negative relationship between female abdominal colourfulness (C*)
and offspring survivorship (adjusted r2 = 0.181;
F1,17 = 4.99, P = 0.039; Fig. 3). The family-specific
replicates were highly comparable (repeatability,
r = 0.97, F37,112 = 110.24, P < 0.001).
DISCUSSION
We did not find evidence that the pre-spawning mate
preference of female Arctic charr was consistently
607
associated with the strength of male carotenoid-based
breeding coloration, when social constrains, such as
male–male competition and coercive and manipulative interaction between the sexes, were experimentally eliminated. Hence, the present study indicates
that in our study population, male abdominal colourfulness (redness) was not the single most important
signal predicting female preference, but there may be
other traits, or perhaps combinations of them (multiple cues; see Candolin, 2003), that mattered as well.
Based on the results of our crossing experiment,
females did not gain apparent genetic benefits in the
form of offspring’s early viability when they were
paired with males they preferred versus with males
they did not prefer. The saturation of red coloration in females, instead, showed a negative relationship with embryo survival, suggesting a significant
trade-off in resource allocation between offspring and
ornamentation.
Mate choice experiments are often difficult to carry
out reliably due to many potential problems that
could affect and disturb animals in such situations.
Furthermore, the use of female association preference
as a proxy for female pre-spawning preference has
been criticized (Wagner, 1998; Gabor, 1999; Shackleton, Jennions & Hunt, 2005). However, it has been
recently demonstrated in fish that female preference
in a dichotomous choice experiment, where the sexes
are separated by a partition, actually is a good predictor of ultimate mate choice (Cummings & Mollaghan, 2006; Lehtonen & Lindström, 2008; Walling
et al., 2010). In such laboratory-based experimental
set-ups, the ornamental characteristics of the male
have also been shown to be important determinants
of female preference (e.g. Milinski & Bakker, 1990;
Walling et al., 2010; Kekäläinen et al., 2010b). As we
assigned males to preference tests at random with
respect to other phenotypic traits besides body size
(similar) and breeding coloration (divergent), we
cannot further comment on the cue basis of the
observed female preference. Although the importance
of carotenoid-based signals in directional mate choice
has been recognized in several vertebrate species
(Møller et al., 2000), individual differences in mate
preferences are highly probable (e.g. Lehtonen &
Lindström, 2008). Furthermore, a female’s mate preference may be a very plastic character, varying in
response to how a male’s attractiveness is dependent
on his genetic quality in a particular ecological
context (Qvarnström, 2001). For example, female
preference for carotenoid coloration may be, to a great
extent, environmentally induced, varying in respect of
carotenoid availability (Grether et al., 2005). Our
study fish originated from a culture environment,
where the supply of dietary carotenoids is abundant.
In such conditions, the association between red col-
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
608
M. JANHUNEN ET AL.
oration and some components of male quality (e.g.
foraging ability and parasite resistance) may be weak,
and this in turn could have reduced the responsiveness of females to the carotenoid coloration of potential mates. Thus, the fact that we did not find
preference for more colourful males could simply be
due to the fact that females living in high-carotenoid
environments are likely to put less emphasis on carotenoid coloration when choosing mates, in comparison
with females living in low-carotenoid environments
(see Grether et al., 2005).
Furthermore, one of the possible explanations for
individual, non-uniform preferences is that females
tend to choose genetically compatible mates (Tregenza & Wedell, 2000; Ryan & Altmann, 2001). Offspring viability as an integral component of overall
developmental success obviously involves multiple
genes and hence is per se a complex quantitative trait
that largely relies on parents’ compatibility (i.e. nonadditive effects within the genotype of the individual;
see Wedekind, Müller & Spicher, 2001; Nordeide,
2007; Patton et al., 2007; Pitcher & Neff, 2007; Wedekind et al., 2008; Janhunen, Piironen & Peuhkuri,
2010; Kekäläinen et al., 2010c). Two independent
crossing experiments on Atlantic cod Gadus morhua
L. (Rudolfsen et al., 2005), and Chinook salmon Oncorhynchus tshawytscha Walbaum (Pitcher & Neff,
2007), for example, have demonstrated large variation in offspring survivorship due to interaction
between males and females: an optimal mate selection had the potential to increase early survivorship
by 74 and 19%, respectively. Neither of these studies
could, however, associate offspring survivorship with
male secondary sexual traits. Our present results also
indicate that both female and male identity contribute to embryos’ chances of survival, even though the
female effect was stronger than the male effect. Thus,
a larger part of the observed variation among progenies was probably a result of maternal effects rather
than direct genetic effects (see also Heath, Fox &
Heath, 1999; Nagler, Parsons & Cloud, 2000; Kortet
et al., 2004; Perry et al., 2004; Jacob et al., 2007).
There were no differences between the half-sib
families related to female mate preference or male
coloration, suggesting that the potential fitness consequences (additive or non-additive genetic benefits)
resulting from mate preference are perhaps less pronounced in the early stages of development in Arctic
charr. However, our results are based on relatively
small sample sizes and the experimental design was
somewhat conservative, as we allowed the females to
discriminate between only two males. If the number
of available males per female had been larger, we
might have increased a priori the probability of
observing some differences, at least between the most
preferred and least preferred ones.
Interestingly, the variation in carotenoid coloration
seems to be informative among females rather than
males when predicting the reproductive success by
the number of live offspring produced (see also Janhunen et al., 2011). The observed negative relationship between a female’s colourfulness and her
offspring viability suggests that colourful females
should be avoided as mating partners due to their
lower fertility. A possible physiological basis for this
is the pre-existing trade-off between ornamentation
and eggs: the development of bright breeding coloration may reduce the availability of valuable
carotenoid pigments (antioxidants) to developing
embryos (Nordeide, Rudolfsen & Egeland, 2006; Nordeide et al., 2008), leading to a lower incubation
success per brood. Egg carotenoids reduce the susceptibility of embryonic tissues to oxidative stress
(Blount et al., 2000), and their positive effects on
fertilization rate, early survival, and growth rates
have been well documented in fishes (e.g. Torrissen,
1984; Salze et al., 2005; Ahmadi et al., 2006; Tyndale
et al., 2008). In addition, a similar finding to our
present results has also been made on another Arctic
charr population (Janhunen et al., 2011), which
strongly suggests that the female carotenoid-based
ornamentation in this species may not have evolved
independently (i.e. in accordance with the mutual
sexual selection hypothesis; Kraaijeveld, KraaijeveldSmit & Komdeur, 2007), but it rather represents a
maladaptive genetic correlation arising from sexual
selection on male coloration (Lande, 1980; see also
Nordeide et al., 2008; Janhunen et al., 2011).
In summary, we applied an automatic PIT-tag detection system as a tool to observe the pre-spawning mate
preference behaviour of female Arctic charr under
captive conditions. Females’ freely expressed mate
preference was not biased towards male carotenoidbased breeding coloration, which is perhaps not surprising given that directional preferences can often
differ among individuals, be context-dependent (e.g.
within-individual changes in preference may occur
in response to changes in environmental or internal
conditions; Wagner, 1998; Qvarnström, 2001; Lehtonen, Wong & Lindström, 2010), and act on more than
only one phenotypic trait. Irrespective of the actual
criteria used by individual females to express their
preference, however, the corresponding matings did
not involve an apparent fitness benefit. Although the
early viability of offspring was largely attributable to
the female parent (also involving the aspect of female
ornamental coloration), we can expect a gradual
strengthening in male-mediated effects (see Perry
et al., 2004). Hence, depending on the magnitude of the
maternal control through egg deposit, the indirect net
fitness consequences connected with female mate preference and/or paternal sexual ornamentation might
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
ARCTIC CHARR MATE PREFERENCE
appear only in later performance measures (e.g.
Kekäläinen et al., 2010a) or under pathogen or parasite infections (Wedekind et al., 2001; Kekäläinen
et al., 2009; Jacob et al., 2010). Further studies on the
potential mechanisms and adaptability of female mate
preference in the current and other Arctic charr populations are therefore needed.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This work was carried out with financial support from
Jenny and Antti Wihuri Foundation (M.J.), the
Finnish Game and Fisheries Research Institute (P.H.,
J.P., project no. 202501), and Academy of Finland
(J.K., project no.121694; R.K., project no. 127398). We
are grateful to Julia Hämäläinen, Lars Figenschou,
Jani Koskimäki, and the staff of the FGFRI’s Sarmijärvi and Kainuu stations (especially Ari Leinonen
and Tapio Laaksonen) for their assistance as well as
to three anonymous referees for their helpful comments on a first version of this manuscript.
REFERENCES
Ahmadi MR, Bazyar AA, Safi S, Ytrestoyl T, Bjerkeng B.
2006. Effects of dietary astaxanthin supplementation on
reproductive characteristics of rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus
mykiss). Journal of Applied Ichthyology 22: 388–394.
Anderson WW, Kim Y-K, Gowaty PA. 2007. Experimental
constraints on mate preferences in Drosophila pseudoobscura decrease offspring viability and fitness of mated pairs.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 104:
4484–4488.
Andersson MB. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Bateman AJ. 1948. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila.
Heredity 2: 349–368.
Blount JD, Houston DC, Møller AP. 2000. Why egg yolk is
yellow? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15: 47–49.
Bluhm CK, Gowaty PA. 2004. Social constraints on female
mate preferences in mallards, Anas platyrhynchos, decrease
offspring viability and mother productivity. Animal Behaviour 68: 977–983.
Candolin U. 2003. The use of multiple cues in mate choice.
Biological Reviews 78: 575–595.
CIE (International Commission on Illumination). 1986.
Colorimetry. CIE Publication No. 15.2. Vienna: CIE.
Consuegra S, Garcia de Leaniz C. 2008. MHC-mediated
mate choice increases parasite resistance in salmon. Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences
275: 1397–1403.
Cummings M, Mollaghan D. 2006. Repeatability and consistency of female preference behaviours in a northern
swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis. Animal Behaviour 72:
217–224.
Drickamer LC, Gowaty PA, Holmes CM. 2000. Free mate
choice in house mice affects reproductive success and
609
offspring viability and performance. Animal Behaviour 59:
371–378.
Drickamer LC, Gowaty PA, Wagner DM. 2003. Free
mutual mate preferences in house mice affect reproductive
success and offspring performance. Animal Behaviour 65:
105–114.
Eilertsen EM, Bårdsen B-J, Liljedal S, Rudolfsen G,
Folstad I. 2009. Experimental evidence for paternal effects
on offspring growth rate in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus).
Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences 276: 129–136.
Fabricius E, Gustafson K. 1954. Further aquarium observations on the spawning behaviour of the char, Salmo
alpinus. Institute of Freshwater Research Report, Drottningholm 35: 58–104.
Figenschou L, Folstad I, Liljedal S. 2004. Lek fidelity of
male Arctic charr. Canadian Journal of Zoology 82: 1278–
1284.
Fleming IA, Reynolds JD. 2004. Salmonid breeding
systems. In: Hendry AP, Stearns SC, eds. Evolution illuminated: salmon and their relatives. New York: Oxford University Press, 264–294.
Forsberg LA, Dannewitz J, Petersson E, Grahn M. 2007.
Influence of genetic dissimilarity in the reproductive success
and mate choice of brown trout – females fishing for optimal
MHC dissimilarity. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:
1859–1869.
Gabor C. 1999. Association patterns of sailfin mollies
(Poecilia latipinna): alternative hypotheses. Behavioral
Ecology and Sociobiology 46: 333–340.
Garner SR, Bortoluzzi RN, Heath DD, Neff BD. 2010.
Sexual conflict inhibits female mate choice for major histocompatibility complex dissimilarity in Chinook salmon. Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences
277: 885–894.
Grether GF, Kolluru GR, Rodd FH, de la Cerda J, Shimazaki K. 2005. Carotenoid availability affects the development of a colour-based mate preference and the sensory
bias to which it is genetically linked. Proceedings of the
Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences 272: 2181–2188.
Hatlen B, Jobling M, Bjerkeng B. 1998. Relationships
between carotenoid concentration and colour of fillets of
Arctic chair, Salvelinus alpinus (L.), fed astaxanthin.
Aquaculture Research 29: 191–202.
Heath DD, Fox CW, Heath JW. 1999. Maternal effects on
offspring size: variation through early development of
Chinook salmon. Evolution 53: 1605–1611.
Hunt J, Bussière L, Jennions MD, Brooks R. 2004. What
is genetic quality? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19:
329–333.
Jacob A, Nusslé S, Britschgi A, Evanno G, Müller R,
Wedekind C. 2007. Male dominance linked to size and age,
but not to ‘good genes’ in brown trout (Salmo trutta). BMC
Evolutionary Biology 7: 207.
Jacob A, Evanno G, von Siebenthal BA, Grossen C,
Wedekind C. 2010. Effects of different mating scenarios on
embryo viability in brown trout. Molecular Ecology 19:
5296–5307.
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
610
M. JANHUNEN ET AL.
Janhunen M, Rudolfsen G, Kekäläinen J, Figencshou L,
Peuhkuri N, Kortet R. 2009. Spawning coloration and
sperm quality in a large lake population of Arctic charr
(Salmonidae: Salvelinus alpinus L.). Biological Journal of
the Linnean Society 98: 794–802.
Janhunen M, Piironen J, Peuhkuri J. 2010. Parental
effects on embryonic viability and growth in Arctic charr
Salvelinus alpinus at two incubation temperatures. Journal
of Fish Biology 76: 2558–2570.
Janhunen M, Peuhkuri N, Primmer C, Kolari I,
Piironen J. 2011. Does breeding ornamentation signal
genetic quality in Arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus? Evolutionary Biology 38: 68–78.
Johnston G. 2002. Arctic charr aquaculture. Oxford: WileyBlackwell.
Kekäläinen J, Vallunen JA, Primmer CR, Rättyä J,
Taskinen J. 2009. Signals of major histocompatibility
complex overdominance in a wild salmonid population. Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences
276: 3133–3140.
Kekäläinen J, Huuskonen H, Tuomaala M, Kortet R.
2010a. Both male and female sexual ornaments reflect
offspring performance in a fish. Evolution 64: 3149–3157.
Kekäläinen J, Valkama H, Huuskonen H, Taskinen J.
2010b. Multiple sexual ornamentation signals mate quality
and predicts female preference in minnows. Ethology 116:
895–903.
Kekäläinen J, Rudolfsen G, Janhunen M, Figencshou L,
Peuhkuri N, Tamper N, Kortet R. 2010c. Genetic and
potential non-genetic benefits increase offspring fitness of
polyandrous females in non-resource based mating system.
BMC Evolutionary Biology 10: 20.
Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Morley J. 2003. The
evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of
the Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences 270: 653–664.
Kortet R, Vainikka A, Rantala MJ, Myntti J, Taskinen J.
2004. In vitro embryo survival and early viability of larvae
in relation to male sexual ornaments and parasite resistance in roach, Rutilus rutilus L. Journal of Evolutionary
Biology 17: 1337–1344.
Kraaijeveld K, Kraaijeveld-Smit FJL, Komdeur J. 2007.
The evolution of mutual ornamentation. Animal Behaviour
74: 657–677.
Labonne J, Augery M, Parade M, Brinkert S, Prevost E,
Héland M, Beall E. 2009. Female preference for male body
size in brown trout, Salmo trutta: is big still fashionable?
Animal Behaviour 77: 129–137.
Lande R. 1980. Sexual dimorphism, sexual selection, and
adaptation in polygenic characters. Evolution 34: 292–305.
Lehtonen TK, Lindström K. 2008. Repeatability of mating
preferences in the sand goby. Animal Behaviour 75: 55–61.
Lehtonen TK, Wong BBM, Lindström K. 2010. Fluctuating
mate preferences in a marine fish. Biology Letters 6: 21–
23.
Måsvær M, Liljedal S, Folstad I. 2004. Are secondary sex
traits, parasites and immunity related to variation in
primary sex traits in the Arctic charr? Proceedings of the
Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences 271: 40–42.
Mays HL, Hill GE. 2004. Choosing mates: good genes versus
genes that are a good fit. Trends in Ecology and Evolution
19: 554–559.
Milinski M, Bakker TCM. 1990. Female sticklebacks use
male coloration in mate choice and hence avoid parasitized
males. Nature 344: 330–333.
Møller AP, Alatalo RV. 1999. Good-genes effects in sexual
selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, Biological
Sciences 266: 85–91.
Møller AP, Biard C, Blount JD, Houston DC, Ninni P,
Saino N, Surai PF. 2000. Carotenoid-dependent signals:
indicators of foraging efficiency, immunocompetence or
detoxification ability? Avian and Poultry Biology Reviews
11: 137–159.
Moore AJ, Gowaty PA, Wallin WG, Moore PJ. 2001.
Sexual conflict and the evolution of female mate choice and
male social dominance. Proceedings of the Royal Society
Series B, Biological Sciences 268: 523–530.
Nagler JJ, Parsons JE, Cloud JG. 2000. Single pair mating indicates maternal effects on embryo survival in
rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Aquaculture 184:
177–183.
Neff BD, Pitcher TE. 2005. Genetic quality and sexual
selection: an integrated framework for good genes and compatible genes. Molecular Ecology 14: 19–38.
Nordeide JT. 2007. Is there more in ‘gamete quality’ than
quality of the gametes? A review of effects of female mate
choice and genetic compatibility on offspring quality. Aquaculture Research 38: 1–16.
Nordeide JT, Rudolfsen G, Egeland ES. 2006. Ornaments
or offspring? Female sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus
L.) trade off carotenoids between spines and eggs. Journal
of Evolutionary Biology 19: 431–439.
Nordeide JT, Mohus Å, Nicolaisen O, Volden R, Egeland
ES. 2008. Offspring or ornaments? Is carotenoid-based
ornamentation in female Arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus
(L.), condition-dependent and traded off against offspring?
Ecology of Freshwater Fish 17: 328–339.
Patton SJ, Kane SL, Wheeler PA, Thorgaard GH. 2007.
Maternal and paternal influence on early embryonic survival of androgenetic rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss):
implications for measuring egg quality. Aquaculture 263:
26–34.
Perry GML, Audet C, Laplatte B, Bernantchez L. 2004.
Shifting patterns in genetic control at the embryo–alevin
boundary in brook charr. Evolution 58: 2002–2012.
Petersson E, Järvi T. 2007. Characteristics of brown trout
males influence growth and survival of their offspring.
Journal of Fish Biology 71: 493–509.
Petersson E, Järvi T, Olsén H, Mayer I, Hedenskog M.
1999. Male–male competition and female mate choice in
brown trout. Animal Behaviour 57: 777–783.
Pitcher TE, Neff BD. 2007. Genetic quality and offspring
performance in Chinook salmon: implications for supportive
breeding. Conservation Genetics 8: 607–616.
Puurtinen M, Ketola T, Kotiaho JS. 2009. The good-genes
and compatible-genes benefits of mate choice. The American
Naturalist 174: 741–751.
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611
ARCTIC CHARR MATE PREFERENCE
Qvarnström A. 2001. Context-dependent genetic benefits
from mate choice. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 16: 5–7.
Reid JM. 2007. Secondary sexual ornamentation and nonadditive genetic benefits of female mate choice. Proceedings
of the Royal Society Series B, Biological Sciences 274: 1395–
1402.
Reynolds JD. 1996. Animal breeding systems. Trends in
Ecology and Evolution 11: 68–72.
Rudolfsen G, Figenschou L, Folstad I, Nordeide JT,
Søreng E. 2005. Potential fitness benefits from mate selection in the Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18: 172–179.
Ryan KK, Altmann J. 2001. Selection for male choice based
primarily on mate compatibility in the oldfield mouse, Peromyscus polionotus rhoadsi. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50: 436–440.
Salze G, Tocher DR, Roy WJ, Robertson DA. 2005. Egg
quality determinants in cod (Gadus morhua L.): egg performance and lipids in eggs from farmed and wild broodstock.
Aquaculture Research 36: 1488–1499.
Shackleton MA, Jennions MD, Hunt J. 2005. Fighting
success and attractiveness as predictors of male mating
success in the black field cricket, Teleogryllus commodus:
the effectiveness of no-choice tests. Behavioral Ecology and
Sociobiology 58: 1–8.
Sigurjónsdóttir H, Gunnarson K. 1989. Alternative mating
tactics of Arctic charr, Salvelinus alpinus, in Thingvallavatn, Iceland. Environmental Biology of Fishes 26: 159–176.
Skarstein F, Folstad I. 1996. Sexual dichromatism and the
immunocompetence handicap: an observational approach
using Arctic charr. Oikos 76: 359–367.
Skarstein F, Folstad I, Rønning H. 2005. Spawning
611
colouration, parasites and habitat selection in Salvelinus
alpinus: initiating speciation by sexual selection? Journal of
Fish Biology 67: 969–980.
Torrissen OJ. 1984. Pigmentation of salmonids – effects of
carotenoids in eggs and startfeeding diet on survival and
growth rate. Aquaculture 43: 185–193.
Tregenza T, Wedell N. 2000. Genetic compatibility, mate
choice and patterns of parentage: invited review. Molecular
Ecology 9: 1013–1027.
Trivers RL. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection.
In: Campbell B, ed. Sexual selection and the descent of man.
Chicago, IL: Aldine, 136–179.
Tyndale ST, Letcher RJ, Heath JW, Heath DD. 2008.
Why are salmon eggs red? Egg carotenoids and early life
survival of Chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha).
Evolutionary Ecology Research 10: 1187–1199.
Wagner WE Jr. 1998. Measuring female mating preferences.
Animal Behaviour 55: 1029–1042.
Walling CA, Royle NJ, Lindström J, Metcalfe NB. 2010. Do
female association preferences predict the likelihood of reproduction? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64: 541–548.
Wedekind C, Müller R, Spicher H. 2001. Potential genetic
benefits of mate selection in whitefish. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 14: 980–986.
Wedekind C, Evanno G, Urbach D, Jacob A, Müller R.
2008. ‘Good genes’ and ‘compatible genes’ effects in an
Alpine whitefish and the information content of breeding
tubercles over the course of the spawning season. Genetica
134: 21–30.
Wong BBM, Candolin U. 2005. How is female mate choice
affected by male competition? Biological Reviews 80: 559–
571.
© 2011 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2011, 103, 602–611