Abstract
Humans can physically interact with other humans adeptly. Some overground interaction tasks, such as guiding a partner across a room, occur without visual and verbal communication, which suggests that the information exchanges occur through sensing movements and forces. To understand the process of motor communication during overground physical interaction, we hypothesized that humans modulate the mechanical properties of their arms for increased awareness and sensitivity to ongoing interaction. For this, we used an overground interactive robot to guide a human partner across one of three randomly chosen paths while occasionally providing force perturbations to measure the arm stiffness. We observed that the arm stiffness was lower at instants when the robotâs upcoming trajectory was unknown compared to instants when it was predicable - the first evidence of arm stiffness modulation for better motor communication during overground physical interaction.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Humans can physically interact with one another without verbal communication1,2, such as when assisting an elder to walk or performing a partnered dance. Even without explicitly shared goals, two human partners can physically interact with each other to perform collaborative tasks. Prior work suggests that sophisticated motor communication strategies exist in physically interacting human dyads that can lead to improved performance of the joint task3,4,5,6, distinction of skill levels7,8 or roles9, or even motor learning and adaptation to the task10,11,12,13. Understanding this biomechanical strategy of physical interaction may be the first step in facilitating better physical human-human interaction (pHHI) and ultimately in developing safe and effective physical human-robot interaction (pHRI)14.
A promising focal point for observing such communication during pHHI or pHRI may be human arm impedance15. During physical interaction, human arms are used as a medium for two-way motor communication6,16,17. Since interactions through arms can be characterized by their movements and the interaction forces, the causal relationship between these two physical quantities, which is mechanical impedance, may reveal human motor control strategies. Indeed, previous works investigated arm stiffness (the spring-like component of impedance) to study motor control in humans for stability tasks18,19,20 or in seated reaching tasks21,22,23,24,25.
Moreover, it was recently suggested that the arm stiffness may affect perception of small forces and/or movements, which is a crucial factor for effective motor communication26. This implies that during practical overground interaction tasks, such as walking assistance, humans may modulate their arm stiffness for increased sensitivity depending on the ongoing interaction dynamics, particularly in the absence of verbal communication or visual feedback. This further motivates the investigation of the arm stiffness during pHRI or pHHI to uncover the underlying motor communication strategy.
To this end, this work presents the first measurement and analysis of human arm stiffness during overground physical interaction between a robot leader and a human follower. The recently developed overground interactive robot, Ophrie27,28, was used in an overground pHRI experiment. The task was designed to simulate a guided walking task such that the human subjects were compelled to focus on the movement of the robot to understand its movement intention. We applied perturbations to measure arm stiffness at two importantly different time points during the trials: either at the point where three potential robot trajectories diverge such that the subjects are least certain of their path, or near the end of the trial when the subjects are most certain that the robot always continues straight forward. By comparing the human arm stiffness in these two conditions that require different levels sensitivity of motor communication, we present the first evidence of human arm stiffness modulation for effective overground pHRI.
Results
Subjects walked with an interactive robot while closing their eyes and holding the handle on the robot arm. The robot moved on one of three trajectories that involved straight and deviated segments. When asked to guess which trajectory the robot moved on, the subjects were able to respond correctly in 386 out of 400 total trials, implying that the subjects were paying close attention to the movement of the robot throughout the experiment. The average interaction force among all the subjects and all trials due to background stiffness right before the perturbation onset was observed to be a pushing force of 2.215±0.976N. That is, on average, the subjects were pushing the robot towards their front-right direction and the resulting reaction force from the robot was in the opposite direction. This can be seen in Fig. 1 where +y is towards the robot and -x is the forward movement direction. When perturbation was applied, the perturbation forces of 3 N were imposed on top of the current interaction force at that moment, briefly increasing the interaction force magnitude in either -x or in -y direction (at 6 s, Fig. 2). As a result, the subjectsâ hands were displaced approximately towards -x or -y direction as can be seen in Fig. 3.
The interaction dynamics were estimated using linear regression, as discussed in the âdata acquisition and analysisâ section. Out of 400 trials, 28 trials were excluded from analysis due to the estimated stiffness being outliers. The average stiffness across all subjects and conditions is 106.819±63.741 N/m. The stiffness values for different conditions and perturbation direction can be found in the Table 1, which is consistent with the literature on unconstrained voluntary arm movements22. The mean arm stiffness and the standard deviation for the individual subjects based on the perturbation direction (forward or lateral) is shown in Fig. 4, and based on the condition (âat deviationâ or âafter deviationâ) is shown in Fig. 5.
The acceptability of the linear regression result was evidenced by the low positive estimates of endpoint inertia (0.207±0.275 kg) and damping (9.114±8.321 Ns/m) parameters, which were expected of relaxed human arms and with the low intrinsic mechanical impedance of the robot arm. The mean equilibrium position of the hand was estimated to be at 0.027±0.045 m (averaged over all trials and all subjects) away from the workspace center towards the robot. To further address the reliability of the interaction dynamics estimation, the interaction forces were estimated using the estimated inertia, damping, stiffness, and the measured hand trajectory. This was then compared against the actual interaction force. The mean and standard deviation of the R2 values between the estimated and measured interaction forces of all 40 trials were 0.568±0.108 (subject 1), 0.567±0.087 (subject 2), 0.533±0.078 (subject 3), 0.578±0.084 (subject 4), 0.528±0.047 (subject 5), 0.559±0.062 (subject 6), 0.539±0.072 (subject 7), 0.516±0.040 (subject 8), 0.520±0.071 (subject 9), and 0.582±0.175 (subject 10).
The skewness and the excess kurtosis of the estimated stiffness values were 1.275 and 1.472, which indicated that the distribution cannot be considered substantially non-normal29. Therefore, parametric tests can be utilized for analysis. The stiffness data between the two curved trajectories (curved towards the subject or away from the subject, Fig. 6) were compared with a paired t-test and were shown to be not significantly different (p>0.780; least square means of 120.099 N/m and 127.174 N/m). Hence, stiffness measurements from all curved trajectory trials were grouped and labeled as a single âafter deviationâ condition. All stiffness measurements from the straight trajectory trials were labeled as âat deviationâ condition.
We performed a three-way ANOVA with the following factors: subjectâ10 levels, conditionsâ2 levels (âat deviationâ and âafter deviationâ), and perturbation directionâ2 levels (âlateralâ and âforwardâ). The omnibus test (Table 2) showed that the mean of at least one combination in the ANOVA model is significantly different from others (p<0.001). Subsequent interaction effect analysis (Table 3) revealed that the three-way interaction of the included factors is not significant (p>0.090). Likewise, there is no significant interaction between the subjects and the conditions (p>0.179, see Fig. 5). In contrast, a strong interaction between the subjects and the perturbation direction was observed to affect the arm stiffness (p<0.0001), which implies that the trend in variation of the arm stiffness in lateral to forward direction is not consistent among subjects (see Fig. 4). Likewise, a significant interaction between the experiment design parameters (perturbation direction and experiment conditions) is observed (p<0.0001). This suggests that the arm stiffness in different directions varies differently based on when it was measuredâthat is, before or after the deviation in Fig. 7.
The main effect of the conditions on stiffness estimates was significant. The stiffness of the arm was significantly higher in the âafter deviationâ condition than in the âat deviationâ condition (118.604±70.247 N/m and 95.530±54.661 N/m, respectively, p<0.0001). That is, the arm stiffness was lower when the robot trajectories may or may not make a curve (B in Fig. 6) compared to when the robot trajectories were always straight (E or H in Fig. 6). The main effect of subject was also significant (p<0.001) showing strong inter-subject variability. The main effect of the perturbation direction was also significant (p<0.0001), consistent with the well-known elliptical shape of 2-D stiffness reported in the literature22,30.
Discussion
The movement trajectories of the robot were specially designed to capture the contrast in overground arm stiffness between (a) when the subjects would be compelled to pay attention to the haptic cues from the robot and (b) when they are not compelled to. The former is implemented as the âat deviationâ trials where the subjects were perturbed at the exact location at which the robot turns in 50% of the trials (B in Fig. 6). In this condition, we expected the subjects to actively âlistenâ to the robot through their hand because they were asked to guess the trajectory of the robot and follow its lead. In addition, the subjects had their eyes closed and were not told whether the robot would turn or not for any specific trial, and hence they had to rely on the information coming through their arm and hand which is holding the robotâs handle. On the other hand, once the robot has passed the first half of the trajectory, the remainder of the path was always straight in the forward direction. In these âafter deviationâ conditions, the arm stiffness was measured long after the robot completed the turn, when the subjects did not have any compelling reason to listen to the haptic information from the robot because itâs motion was predictable. The subjects reported the trajectory correctly 96.5% of the time (386 out of 400 trials) which indicates that they were actively sensing the robot movement, at least up to the trajectory deviation.
In general, the subjects pushed the robot towards their front-right (-x and +y in Fig. 1). This may be because the eyes-closed subjects expected the robot which is leading them to be slightly ahead of them to the right, and that they preferred to keep a safe distance from the robot by pushing it away. While maintaining this push towards the front-right, the subjects may have sensed the forward movement of the robot by sensing a decrease in the intensity of the push in the forward direction.
We observed that the arm stiffness was significantly different in âat deviationâ condition trials compared to âafter deviationâ condition trials (p<0.0001). This indicates that humans modulate their arm stiffness during overground physical interaction tasks. The modulation was related to whether they were compelled to listen to haptic information (i.e. âat deviationâ condition) or not (i.e. âafter deviationâ condition). Note that the arm stiffness modulation was completely voluntary - the subjects were free to choose how stiff or compliant their arm could be during the entire experiment. The results imply that the subjects may have lowered their arm stiffness in the forward direction due to the need for more sensitivity in haptic communication âat deviationâ. When less sensitivity was required âafter deviationâ, their forward arm stiffness was higher. In the lateral direction, this modulation did not occur, which may imply that the need for communication was less in that direction.
Our result is consistent with the hypothesis in our prior study26 that low arm stiffness may facilitate motor communication. According to26, lower arm stiffness may allow larger movements to occur from small interaction forces, which will then be better sensed by the proprioceptors in the musculoskeletal system. Our results reinforce this observation. The key distinction is that, while in26 the stiffening of the arm influenced the sensitivity to small interaction forces, in this work, the need for better sensitivity caused the subjects to modulate the stiffness of their arms. Also, in26 the arm was heavily constrained such that the arm configuration was predetermined and fixed with respect to the position of the robot. In contrast, in this work, the arm configuration and the hand position with respect to the robot were allowed to deviate. This allowed the subjects to modulate their arm movement as desired in a more naturalistic way. Hence the state of the arm (such as how stiff it is) was decided freely by the subject as needs arose. This study implies that the said âneedâ could be the need for higher sensitivity during times when the robotâs movement was more unpredictable.
The modulation of the stiffness in the forward direction was significant, whereas in the lateral direction, the modulation was not significant (Fig. 7). This may imply that the subjects were more concerned about sensing the forward movement rather than the lateral movement of the robot. Indeed, due to the non-holonomic constraint, the wheeled base of our robot cannot generate strictly sideways movement. Hence the subjects may not be as compelled to modulate the arm stiffness in the lateral direction as much as in the forward direction.
Another possibility is that the modulation of stiffness may have been a result of the change in the arm posture30. The shape and size of the 2-D stiffness ellipse depends on the posture15,22, where the ellipse eccentricity is higher as the hand gets further away from the chest. In our study, the forward stiffness is higher than the lateral stiffness in the âafter deviationâ condition, indicating high eccentricity and suggesting that the hand may have been further away from the chest than it was during the âat deviationâ condition. The modulation of stiffness with posture (instead of muscle activity) is more likely given the low stiffness values that are comparable to the stiffness of the relaxed arms in22. Future experiments with arm kinematics and electromyography may address this hypothesis.
The main effect of the subjects was significant, which was expected because every individual is different and might respond to similar perturbation differently. It is not uncommon to have higher inter-subject variability in experiments involving humans, especially with regards to bio-physiological variables such as arm stiffness. More variance could have been added due to the nature of the experiment having high degrees of freedom as an overground experiment (as opposed to constrained, seated settings). Factors such as the subjectsâ walking habits (posture), height, or arm muscle cross-section all would contribute to this variability.
While the robotâs velocity setpoint did not differ by trials in this experiment, future usage of Ophrie may include applications with varying velocity profiles including the maximum speed. Our observations suggest that the human userâs arm stiffness would be lower when predictability is lower with the robot partner. The robotâs speed, by itself, does not increase or decrease predictability and therefore may not affect the arm stiffness of the user. However, the rate at which the robot reaches its setpoint speed (fast or slow) may motivate the human user to change their arm stiffness values, since higher acceleration may be perceived as lower predictability. For example, an overall slower robot with frequent periods of high acceleration and deceleration may be perceived as less predictable than a faster robot with a slow rate of change of speed.
The arm stiffness modulation observed in this study has further implications in the design and control of robots for overground pHRI31. It may be argued that the robot partner would feel more human-like to the user if the robot armâs mechanical impedance was similar to that of a typical human. In this view, the low impedance of the robot arm may provide an impression of attentiveness to the userâs intent. The robot may also selectively lower the stiffness in specific directions to communicate its movement intent or role-taking to the user12.
Methods
Participants
A total of 10 healthy young adults (1 female, 1 left-handed, 27.6±2.41 years) without any self-reported neurological disorder took part in the study. Subjects signed a written informed consent form before participating in the experiment. The research protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Missouri System, and was performed in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.
Experiment protocol
A guided walking experiment was designed wherein the human participants were asked to walk alongside the robot Ophrieâthat acted as a leaderâwhile holding the robotâs handle. Ophrie was specially designed for the overground pHRI experiments. Its manipulator possesses unique characteristics that are critical to overground physical interaction experiments involving humans, such as low inherent endpoint impedance, the ability to apply and measure small force perturbations (<10 N), and the capability of endpoint movement speed of 0.845m/s assuming cyclic motion of 2 Hz within the workspace27,28.
In the experiment, subjects were asked to walk following the lead of the robot. During each trial, the human participants were asked to stand upright next to Ophrie and held its handle using their right hand as shown in Fig. 6b. The robotâs height was adjusted beforehand such that the height of the interaction handle was aligned to the subjectâs elbow level. The subjects were informed that the position-based background stiffness controller (explained below) would be activated after they are ready, and hence they should relax their arm to let the robotic arm position the handle to the center of its workspaceâa 0.15x0.15 m2 2-dimensional space parallel to the ground plane (see Fig. 6a). The subjects were informed that, after the experimenter issues a verbal cue, the robot would start to move, and they had to follow the robot with their eyes-closed throughout the entire trial.
The background stiffness controller provided an interaction force to the human user such that the subjectsâ hands were maintained near the center of the workspace, such that
where (x, y) is the location of the end point with respect to the center of the workspace (0, 0), K is the scalar level of background stiffness, and (\(F_x\), \(F_y\)) are the forces generated by the robot.
The robot was programmed to go on a straight trajectory for the initial 1.5 m, then the trajectory would deviate either to the left or right, or remain straight in the forward direction (Fig. 6a). The deviation (BD or BG in Fig. 6a) took 3.8 s. After that, the trajectories were always straight. The paths for these three different trajectories were marked on the floor using colored tapes to ensure that, prior to the beginning of the experiment, the subjects clearly understood (a) where the robot might deviate from straight trajectory and (b) the experiment design that âthe latter half of the trajectory will always be straightâ. Given their eyes were closed, the subjects were expected to listen to haptic information felt from holding the interaction handle to sense the movement of the robot and the deviation in the trajectory when the robot turned. After the robot stopped, the subjects were asked to tell in which direction the robot deviated based on their judgment before opening their eyes.
The top view sketch of the entire experimental setup and the ideal starting pose for each trial is shown in Fig. 6. Throughout the entire trial, the robot was programmed to ramp up to and maintain a constant linear velocity of 0.5 m/s. When turning, the angular velocity ramped up to 0.45 rad/s and then ramped down to zero. In straight trajectory trials (ABC in Fig. 6a, 50% of the total number of trials), the robotic arm applied a force perturbation of 3 N (lasting 0.8 s) when it reached B. In trials with deviated trajectories (the other 50% of the trials equally divided between ABDEF or ABGHI in Fig. 6a), the perturbation was applied at either E or H. The perturbation was applied either towards the -x or the -y direction. Subjects were aware that the robot might turn and that it will also apply a slight push at some point on the trajectory, but they were unaware of the direction of the turn or the direction of perturbation for any specific trial. The presence of a background stiffness controller helped the subjects to maintain their hand inside the workspace; the stiffness controller was briefly turned off during the perturbation, so that the estimated stiffness within the 0.8 period would be solely due to the arm stiffness. Each trial took approximately 20 s.
In this work, we applied perturbations at one of two different time points during the trials to measure stiffness at different levels of participant attention to the robot movement. The first time point is âat deviationâ, which happens at point B (Fig. 6a), where the three potential paths deviate. In all of the trials that used this early perturbation, participants continued on the straight path BC after perturbation, since the perturbation would interfere with information required to signal a turn. Since subjects did not know ahead of the trial whether the trajectory would be straight or curved, they would be uncertain to which trajectory the robot would follow after point B - to go straight, or to curved to the left or right, and thus paying close attention to the robotâs movements. The other time point is âafter deviationâ, which happens at the end of the trial at point E or H (Fig. 6a), when the robot always continues straight for the remainder of the trial. These perturbations always occurred when the robots took one of the curved paths, since the straight path trials used the earlier perturbation. Since subjects already knew this, there was no uncertainty about the robotâs trajectory at point E or H. The label âafter deviationâ came from the fact that the perturbation occurred many seconds after the robot has chosen its trajectory. For any given trial, there was exactly one perturbation, occurring either at point B (âat deviationâ condition) or at E/H (âafter deviationâ condition).
The arm stiffness measurements were made in two orthogonal directions (-x and -y) to capture the directional differences in the arm stiffness. The choice between +x and -x or between +y and -y could be made arbitrarily since the model used in this study and in the literature15 assumes linearity. It is well known that the 2D-constrained arm presents a 2D stiffness ellipse15. Since arm stiffness is affected by the arm configuration15,30, the measurement of stiffness ellipse requires that perturbation is provided at the same arm configuration across multiple trials. This was possible for seated experiments such as22 where the subjectâs trunk location was fixed with respect to the robot. However, this is not possible for overground pHRI experiments where the trunk location with respect to the robot must be allowed to vary (ex. natural lateral sway due to walking). Thus the two directional arm stiffness were measured and reported as individual scalar values. The number of directions was limited to two to avoid excessively long experiments that could result from having three or more perturbation directions. The varying arm configuration in overground pHRI experiments was also why force perturbation was chosen over position perturbation in this study, since the latter requires a knowledge of the expected position or trajectory of the arm movement at the time of perturbation32.
In summary, there were six different trial types depending on the direction of trajectory deviation (towards F, C, or I, 3 levels) and the direction of force perturbation (2 levels). The experiment was designed as a Randomized Complete Block (RCB) design. Each block consisted of eight randomly distributed trials where there were two ABDEF, two ABGHI, and four ABC trajectories. The ABC trajectory constituted the âat deviationâ condition and the two deviated trajectories constituted the âafter deviationâ condition. There were the same number of trials for straight versus deviated trajectories such that the same number of measurements could be made for âat deviationâ and âafter deviationâ conditions. Each of these groups had an equal number of forward and lateral perturbations. There were 5 blocks totaling the number of trials per subject to 40. Each experiment on one subject took approximately 2.5 hours.
Data acquisition and analysis
The interaction dynamics is the function of arm movement and its derivative. A widely used simplified model was adopted in this study where we assume the interaction dynamics in task space as passive, linear, and time-invariant second-order dynamics33,34,35,36.
where f is the interaction force measured after the onset of force perturbation, x, \(\dot{x}\), and \(\ddot{x}\) are the position, velocity and acceleration of the hand after the perturbation, respectively, and m, b, and k are the inertia, damping, and stiffness from the interaction dynamics, respectively. \(x_0\), \(\dot{x}_0\), and \(\ddot{x}_0\) are the mean hand position, velocity, and acceleration of the interaction point just before the perturbation, and likewise, \(f_0\) is the interaction force just before the perturbation, averaged over the 0.1 second interval prior to the perturbation onset to reduce the effect of high-frequency noise in force measurements.
The hand position with respect to the robot can be calculated using the motorsâ encoder positions and forward kinematics. The motor shaft position was measured with the single-turn absolute Biss sine encoder (2048 LPR) embedded in the servo motors (AKM32E-ANCNAA00: Kollmorgen Corp, VA, USA). The kinematic details of the robot arm can be found in27. The interaction forces were measured using the mini45 force/torque sensor (ATI Industrial Automation, NC, USA), which is lodged between the endpoint of the robotic arm and the interaction handle. All data were recorded with a sampling rate of 1 KHz. After recording, the hand position and force data were filtered in Matlab (MathWorks Inc., MA, USA) using a Butterworth low-pass zero-lag filter with 40 Hz cutoff frequency.
From the measured hand position data during perturbation, we used the earlier portion of the arm movement response that shows the characteristics of a passive second-order system. First, the hand velocity versus time graph was plotted in Matlab and the time instant of the second peak (local maximum) was manually identified, since the general trend of the arm movement up to this instant were similar across subjects and also resembled the second-order response. Then, the time interval from the perturbation onset to the time of second peak was selected as the period of data for analysisâwhich was on average 438.5±113.7 milliseconds. The perturbation force, hand position, and hand velocity during the 800 ms perturbation for a representative subject can be seen in Fig. 8. Multi-variable linear regression (Matlab fitlm function) was applied to this data to estimate m, b, and k in Equation 2. The stiffness (k) is the main outcome of interest, whereas the inertia (m) and the damping (b) were only used to check the validity of the estimation.
Each trial from the experiment fetched one scalar stiffness value in either forward or lateral direction. It can be considered as the armâs resistance to an external perturbation towards the axis of the perturbation. Out of 400 trials performed by 10 different subjects, 17 were discarded because the stiffness estimates were negative. From the remaining 383 trials, 11 outliersâdefined as values more than 3 times the interquantile range past the 25th or 75th quantilesâwere screened out using JMP statistical software (SAS Institute Inc., NC, USA). ANOVA was performed using JMP on the remaining 372 stiffness values. A significance level of p=0.05 was used.
Data availibility
The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available in the Harvard Dataverse repository, https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/XEKVUF.
References
Franklin, D. W., So, U., Burdet, E. & Kawato, M. Visual feedback is not necessary for the learning of novel dynamics. PLoS ONE 2, e1336 (2007).
Batcho, C., Gagné, M., Bouyer, L., Roy, J. & Mercier, C. Impact of online visual feedback on motor acquisition and retention when learning to reach in a force field. Neuroscience 337, 267â275 (2016).
Reed, K. B., Peshkin, M., Hartmann, M. J., Colgate, J. E. & Patton, J. Kinesthetic interaction. In 9th International Conference on Rehabilitation Robotics, 2005. ICORR 2005, 569â574 (IEEE, 2005).
Takagi, A., Ganesh, G., Yoshioka, T., Kawato, M. & Burdet, E. Physically interacting individuals estimate the partnerâs goal to enhance their movements. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 1â6 (2017).
Feth, D., Groten, R., Peer, A., Hirche, S. & Buss, M. Performance related energy exchange in haptic human-human interaction in a shared virtual object manipulation task. In World Haptics 2009-Third Joint EuroHaptics conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, 338â343 (IEEE, 2009).
Sylos-Labini, F., dâAvella, A., Lacquaniti, F. & Ivanenko, Y. Human-human interaction forces and interlimb coordination during side-by-side walking with hand contact. Front. Physiol. 9, 179 (2018).
Sawers, A. et al. Small forces that differ with prior motor experience can communicate movement goals during human-human physical interaction. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 14, 1â13 (2017).
Mireles, E. J. A., Zenzeri, J., Squeri, V., Morasso, P. & De Santis, D. Skill learning and skill transfer mediated by cooperative haptic interaction. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 25, 832â843 (2017).
Mörtl, A. et al. The role of roles: Physical cooperation between humans and robots. Int. J. Robot. Res. 31, 1656â1674 (2012).
Groten, R., Feth, D., Klatzky, R. L. & Peer, A. The role of haptic feedback for the integration of intentions in shared task execution. IEEE Trans. Haptics 6, 94â105 (2012).
Zivotofsky, A. Z. & Hausdorff, J. M. The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 4, 1â5 (2007).
Mojtahedi, K., Whitsell, B., Artemiadis, P. & Santello, M. Communication and inference of intended movement direction during human-human physical interaction. Front. Neurorobot. 11, 21 (2017).
Takagi, A., Usai, F., Ganesh, G., Sanguineti, V. & Burdet, E. Haptic communication between humans is tuned by the hard or soft mechanics of interaction. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14, e1005971 (2018).
Küçüktabak, E. B., Kim, S. J., Wen, Y., Lynch, K. & Pons, J. L. Human-machine-human interaction in motor control and rehabilitation: A review. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 18, 1â18 (2021).
Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A., Hogan, N. & Bizzi, E. Neural, mechanical, and geometric factors subserving arm posture in humans. J. Neurosci. 5, 2732â2743 (1985).
Reed, K. B. & Peshkin, M. A. Physical collaboration of human-human and human-robot teams. IEEE Trans. Haptics 1, 108â120 (2008).
Stefanov, N., Peer, A. & Buss, M. Role determination in human-human interaction. In World Haptics 2009-Third Joint EuroHaptics conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, 51â56 (IEEE, 2009).
Burdet, E., Osu, R., Franklin, D. W., Milner, T. E. & Kawato, M. The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance. Nature 414, 446â449 (2001).
Burdet, E. et al. Stability and motor adaptation in human arm movements. Biol. Cybern. 94, 20â32 (2006).
Franklin, D. W. et al. Endpoint stiffness of the arm is directionally tuned to instability in the environment. J. Neurosci. 27, 7705â7716 (2007).
Lacquaniti, F., Carrozzo, M. & Borghese, N. Time-varying mechanical behavior of multijointed arm in man. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 1443â1464 (1993).
Gomi, H. & Kawato, M. Human arm stiffness and equilibrium-point trajectory during multi-joint movement. Biol. Cybern. 76, 163â171 (1997).
Scott, S. H. Apparatus for measuring and perturbing shoulder and elbow joint positions and torques during reaching. J. Neurosci. Methods 89, 119â127 (1999).
Selen, L. P., Beek, P. J. & Van Dieën, J. H. Impedance is modulated to meet accuracy demands during goal-directed arm movements. Exp. Brain Res. 172, 129â138 (2006).
Piovesan, D., Pierobon, A., DiZio, P. & Lackner, J. R. Experimental measure of arm stiffness during single reaching movements with a time-frequency analysis. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 2484â2496 (2013).
Rashid, F., Burns, D. & Song, Y. S. Sensing small interaction forces through proprioception. Sci. Rep. 11, 1â10 (2021).
Regmi, S. & Song, Y. S. Design methodology for robotic manipulator for overground physical interaction tasks. J. Mech. Robot.12(4), 1942â4302. https://doi.org/10.1115/1.4045688 (2020).
Regmi, S. & Song, Y. S. Estimation of endpoint impedance of a 2d parallel manipulator using numerical simulation experiments. In ASME International Mechanical Engineering Congress and Exposition, vol. 84522, V005T05A066 (ASME, 2020).
Kim, H.-Y. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor. Dent. Endod. 38, 52â54 (2013).
Trumbower, R. D., Krutky, M. A., Yang, B.-S. & Perreault, E. J. Use of self-selected postures to regulate multi-joint stiffness during unconstrained tasks. PLoS ONE 4, e5411 (2009).
Chen, T. L. et al. Evaluation by expert dancers of a robot that performs partnered stepping via haptic interaction. PLoS ONE 10, e0125179 (2015).
Burdet, E. et al. A method for measuring endpoint stiffness during multi-joint arm movements. J. Biomech. 33, 1705â1709 (2000).
Dolan, J. M., Friedman, M. B. & Nagurka, M. L. Dynamic and loaded impedance components in the maintenance of human arm posture. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. 23, 698â709 (1993).
Tsuji, T., Morasso, P. G., Goto, K. & Ito, K. Human hand impedance characteristics during maintained posture. Biol. Cybern. 72, 475â485 (1995).
Rahman, M., Ikeura, R. & Mizutani, K. Control characteristics of two humans in cooperative task and its application to robot control. In 2000 26th Annual Conference of the IEEE Industrial Electronics Society. IECON 2000. 2000 IEEE International Conference on Industrial Electronics, Control and Instrumentation. 21st Century Technologies, vol. 3, 1773â1778 (IEEE, 2000).
Bennett, D., Hollerbach, J., Xu, Y. & Hunter, I. Time-varying stiffness of human elbow joint during cyclic voluntary movement. Exp. Brain Res. 88, 433â442 (1992).
Funding
This work was supported in part by the National Science Foundation Grants #1843892 and #2046552.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
S.R., D.B., and Y.S.S. designed the experiment. S.R. performed the experiment. S.R., D.B., and Y.S.S. analyzed and interpreted the data. S.R. produced the manuscript draft. All authors reviewed and edited the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Regmi, S., Burns, D. & Song, Y.S. Humans modulate arm stiffness to facilitate motor communication during overground physical human-robot interaction. Sci Rep 12, 18767 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-23496-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-23496-z