Opinion Polarization in Human Communities Can Emerge as a Natural Consequence of Beliefs Being Interrelated
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. The Background: Fundamental Features of Human Belief Systems
- First and fore-most, it seek to be consistent. This means that people try to maintain a belief system in which the elements mutually support each other, or are independent [48,52]. In the case of holding conflicting beliefs, people experience discomfort called cognitive dissonance [53,54] which people will try to reduce. By this time, cognitive dissonance has become one of the most influential and well-researched theories in social psychology [54,55,56].
- Secondly, beliefs are not equally important: those that are more personal, closer to the “self”, “identity” or “ego”, trigger more intense feelings and are more difficult to change [43,46,54,57]. In this sense, beliefs have a hierarchical property: the ones that are higher in rank define or constrain the ones that are lower in rank [58]. For example, the belief or disbelief in God is a central (high-ranking) element in one’s belief system, while the belief that “an egg should be boiled for seven minutes in order to get the best soft-boiled egg” is a low ranking one, and, accordingly, can be changed more easily [43]. Furthermore, beliefs that people hold in high regard tend to cause greater dissonance in case of contradiction with other beliefs [54].
- Finally, beliefs belonging to the same broader topic (e.g., health, art-related topics, religion, political issues, etc.) are more strongly interrelated than beliefs belonging to different topics. For example, attitudes towards “freedom of speech”, “religious freedom” and “freedom to choose spouse” are more closely related than beliefs regarding “freedom of speech” and, say, homeopathic treatments. In mathematical (graph-theoretical) terms, belief systems are “modular” or “compartmentalized” [43,50].
3. The Model
- (i)
- Rejecting new information that conflicts with the already existing ones;
- (ii)
- Re-evaluating the attitudes;
- (iii)
- A tendency of “explaining things away”, that is, finding alternative explanations (developing new beliefs) which supplement the original information in a way that the primordial contradiction is dissolved.
3.1. Modelling the Re-Evaluation of Beliefs
- The more extreme an attitude value a is, the lower the probability that it will change. Equation (3) expresses the most simple mathematical formulation of this relation.
- The more extreme an attitude value a is, the smaller the magnitude with which it can change.
3.2. Modelling the Inclusion of New Beliefs in Order to Relieve Cognitive Dissonance
4. Results
4.1. Re-Evaluating Beliefs
4.2. Finding Relief in New Ideas
5. Discussion
- (i)
- Real belief systems have a tremendous amount of elements (instead of two or three), that are interconnected and embedded into each other in a complicated manner [39,43], and, accordingly, the “optimization process”—the attempt to minimize the contradictions among the components—refers to the entire system. From a physicist’s point of view, this process is in close relation to physical structures aiming to reach an energy minimum. In this approach, “different realities” [57] can be different local energy minimums of similar systems. However, it is imperative to understand the elementary relation between two elements of the system before considering the entire structure. The present manuscript focuses on this elementary relation. Graph representation is important because, and only because, it serves as a mathematical tool for handling interrelated entities (which are the “beliefs” or “concepts” in our case). Since in the human mind a vast amount of concepts and beliefs are interrelated densely and intricately, any of its graph representations must also assume a vast amount of intricately interrelated (linked) nodes. However, from the viewpoint of the present study, the specific type of the graph does not play any role, because we focus on the elementary process altering the characteristics of two nodes (namely the “attitude values”) due to a newly appearing link between them. (If a link appears, it is due to a certain piece of information connecting the two, originally unconnected beliefs/concepts). The nodes whose values alter are selected by the link (representing a piece of information).
- (ii)
- The present model does not assume that the repeated information is exactly the same, only that the type of connection between two concepts (say a political party and a public issue, such as immigration or environmental topics) is tenaciously either positive or negative. Hence, it also explains how attitudes can become extreme due to the continuous repetition of information, and as such, it serves as a complementary explanation [70] for the reason why, throughout history, the most diverse regimes found it useful to repeat the same messages over and over again (despite the fact that everybody had already heard them many times).
Supplementary Materials
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Flowchart and Parameters
Notation | Meaning | Values |
---|---|---|
N | Number of individuals (population size) | 1000 (SI: 100) |
T | Number of iterations (level of exposure) | 100,000–300,000 |
Noise on the magnitude of attitude change | ||
(SI: , | ||
, | ||
, | ||
) | ||
Positive or negative: Type of the connection between the two original concepts | or | |
Type of the connection between the newly accepted concept and the one it is connected to. | or |
References
- Streitfeld, D. The Internet is broken. The New York Times, 20 May 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Douven, I.; Hegselmann, R. Mis- and disinformation in a bounded confidence model. Artif. Intell. 2021, 291, 103415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berekméri, E.; Derényi, I.; Zafeiris, A. Optimal structure of groups under exposure to fake news. Appl. Netw. Sci. 2019, 4, 101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Altmire, J. Dead Center: How Political Polarization Divided America and What We Can Do About It; Sunbury Press: Mechanicsburg, PA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Carothers, T.; O’Donohue, A. Democracies Divided: The Global Challenge of Political Polarization; Brookings Institution Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson, N. The Square and the Tower: Networks and Power, from the Freemasons to Facebook; Penguin Press: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- McPherson, M.; Smith-Lovin, L.; Cook, J.M. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2001, 27, 415–444. Available online: http://xxx.lanl.gov/abs/https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.415 (accessed on 13 September 2022). [CrossRef]
- Dandekar, P.; Goel, A.; Lee, D.T. Biased assimilation, homophily, and the dynamics of polarization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2013, 110, 5791–5796. Available online: http://xxx.lanl.gov/abs/https://www.pnas.org/content/110/15/5791.full.pdf (accessed on 13 September 2022). [CrossRef]
- Perra, N.; Rocha, L.E.C. Modelling opinion dynamics in the age of algorithmic personalisation. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 7261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berekméri, E.; Zafeiris, A. Optimal collective decision making: Consensus, accuracy and the effects of limited access to information. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 16997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noorazar, H. Recent advances in opinion propagation dynamics: A 2020 Survey. Eur. Phys. J. Plus 2020, 135, 521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lorenz, J. Continuous Opinion Dynamics under Bounded Confidence: A Survey. Int. J. Mod. Phys. C 2007, 18, 1819–1838. Available online: http://xxx.lanl.gov/abs/https://doi.org/10.1142/S0129183107011789 (accessed on 13 September 2022). [CrossRef]
- Hegselmann, R.; Krause, U. Opinion dynamics and bounded confidence models, analysis, and simulation. J. Artifical Soc. Soc. Simul. (JASSS) 2002, 5, 1–33. [Google Scholar]
- Häggström, O.; Hirscher, T. Further results on consensus formation in the Deffuant model. Electron. J. Probab. 2014, 19, 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deffuant, G.; Neau, D.; Amblard, F.; Weisbuch, G. Mixing beliefs among interacting agents. Adv. Complex Syst. 2000, 03, 87–98. Available online: http://xxx.lanl.gov/abs/https://doi.org/10.1142/S0219525900000078 (accessed on 13 September 2022). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Albi, G.; Pareschi, L.; Zanella, M. Boltzmann-type control of opinion consensus through leaders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2014, 372, 20140138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castellano, C.; Fortunato, S.; Loreto, V. Statistical physics of social dynamics. Rev. Mod. Phys. 2009, 81, 591–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mäs, M.; Flache, A. Differentiation without Distancing. Explaining Bi-Polarization of Opinions without Negative Influence. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e74516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edmonds, B. Assessing the Safety of (Numerical) Representation in Social Simulation. In Proceedings of the European Social Simulation Association (ESSA) 2005, Koblenz, Germany, 5–9 September 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Mäs, M.; Flache, A.; Helbing, D. Individualization as Driving Force of Clustering Phenomena in Humans. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2010, 6, e1000959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pineda, M.; Toral, R.; Hernandez-Garcia, E. Noisy continuous-opinion dynamics. J. Stat. Mech. Theory Exp. 2009, 2009, P08001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, Q.; Wang, X. Opinion dynamics with similarity-based random neighbors. Sci. Rep. 2013, 3, 2968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schawe, H.; Hernández, L. When open mindedness hinders consensus. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 8273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schawe, H.; Fontaine, S.; Hernández, L. When network bridges foster consensus. Bounded confidence models in networked societies. Phys. Rev. Res. 2021, 3, 023208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berelson, B. Events as an Influence upon Public Opinion. J. Q. 1949, 26, 145–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atkeson, L.R.; Maestas, C.D. Extraordinary Events and Public Opinion. In Catastrophic Politics: How Extraordinary Events Redefine Perceptions of Government; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2012; pp. 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Condie, S.A.; Condie, C.M. Stochastic events can explain sustained clustering and polarisation of opinions in social networks. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 1355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toscani, G.; Tosin, A.; Zanella, M. Opinion modeling on social media and marketing aspects. Phys. Rev. E 2018, 98, 022315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pareschi, L.; Toscani, G.; Tosin, A.; Zanella, M. Hydrodynamic Models of Preference Formation in Multi-agent Societies. J. Nonlinear Sci. 2019, 29, 2761–2796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalege, J.; Borsboom, D.; van Harreveld, F.; van den Berg, H.; Conner, M.; van der Maas, H.L.J. Toward a formalized account of attitudes: The Causal Attitude Network (CAN) model. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 123, 2–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Maas, H.L.J.; Kolstein, R.; van der Pligt, J. Sudden Transitions in Attitudes. Sociol. Methods Res. 2003, 32, 125–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, E.B. Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 5th ed.; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Seth, A. Being You: A New Science of Consciousness; Dutton: New York, NY, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Pinker, S. How the Mind Works, Illustrated ed.; W. W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Galotti, K.M. Cognitive Psychology In and Out of the Laboratory, Fifth ed.; SAGE Publications: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Lamberts, K.; Shanks, D. (Eds.) Knowledge Concepts and Categories; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Harari, Y.N. Sapiens: A Brief History of Humankind; Harper: Manhattan, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Searle, J. Making the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Shermer, M. The Believing Brain: From Ghosts and Gods to Politics and Conspiracies—How We Construct Beliefs and Reinforce Them as Truths, Illustrated ed.; St. Martin’s Griffin: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Pohl, R.F. (Ed.) Cognitive Illusions: A Handbook on Fallacies and Biases in Thinking, Judgement and Memory, 1st ed.; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Sloman, S.; Fernbach, P. The Knowledge Illusion: Why We Never Think Alone; Riverhead books: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper, J. Cognitive Dissonance: 50 Years of a Classic Theory; SAGE Publications Ltd.: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Tóth, B.; Csányi, V. Our Beliefs—The building blocks of human thoughts (In Hungarian: Hiedelmeink—Az Emberi Gondolatok építokövei); Libri: Budapest, Hungary, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Farrar, Straus and Giroux: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- March, J.G. Primer on Decision Making: How Decisions Happen; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Akerlof, G.A.; Kranton, R.E. Identity Economics: How Our Identities Shape Our Work, Wages, and Well-Being; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Converse, P.E. The nature of belief systems in mass publics. Crit. Rev. 1964, 18, 1–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jost, J.; Federico, C.; Napier, J. Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 307–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DellaPosta, D. Pluralistic Collapse: The “Oil Spill” Model of Mass Opinion Polarization. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2020, 85, 507–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turner-Zwinkels, F.M.; Johnson, B.B.; Sibley, C.G.; Brandt, M.J. Conservatives’ Moral Foundations Are More Densely Connected Than Liberals’ Moral Foundations. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2020, 47, 167–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rokeach, M. The organization and modification of beliefs. Centen. Rev. 1963, 7, 375–395. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, A.; Stein, S.K. Beyond Social Contagion: Associative Diffusion and the Emergence of Cultural Variation. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2018, 83, 897–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Festinger, L. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance; Stanford University Press: Redwood City, CA, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard, J. Cognitive Dissonance: What to Know. 2019. Available online: https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/326738 (accessed on 13 September 2022).
- Harmon-Jones, E. (Ed.) Cognitive Dissonance: Reexamining a Pivotal Theory in Psychology, 2nd ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Perlovsky, L. A challenge to human evolution—Cognitive dissonance. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Galef, J. The Scout Mindset: Why Some People See Things Clearly and Others Don’t; Portfolio: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Zafeiris, A.; Vicsek, T. Why We Live in Hierarchies: A Quantitative Treatise; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Brandt, M.J.; Sleegers, W.W.A. Evaluating Belief System Networks as a Theory of Political Belief System Dynamics. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2021, 25, 159–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wright, R. The Evolution of God; Little, Brown and Company: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Christian, D. This Fleeting World: A Short History of Humanity; Berkshire Publishing Group: Great Barrington, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez, N.; Bollen, J.; Ahn, Y.Y. Collective Dynamics of Belief Evolution under Cognitive Coherence and Social Conformity. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0165910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baldassarri, D.; Goldberg, A. Neither Ideologues nor Agnostics: Alternative Voters’ Belief System in an Age of Partisan Politics. Am. J. Sociol. 2014, 120, 45–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brandt, M.J.; Sibley, C.G.; Osborne, D. What Is Central to Political Belief System Networks? Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 45, 1352–1364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Priest, H. Biased: 50 Powerful Cognitive Biases That Impair Our Judgment; Cognitt Consulting: Vilnius, Lithuania, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Holm, C. The 25 Cognitive Biases: Uncovering The Myth Of Rational Thinking, Kindle ed.; Amazon Media, EU: London, UK.
- Dobelli, R. The Art of Thinking Clearly; Harper Paperbacks: New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- George, J.M.; Jones, G.R. Towards a process model of individual change in organizations. Hum. Relations 2001, 54, 419–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- It Is Not Condolence, Applaud or Awards That We Wish for—The Confessions of a Doctor about the Tribulations of the Covid Pandemic. Available online: https://miapanasz.reblog.hu/nem-sajnalatra-tapsra-vagy-kituntetesre-vagyunk (accessed on 7 January 2012). (In Hungarian).
- Centola, D. How Behavior Spreads: The Science of Complex Contagions; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Zafeiris, A. Source Code of the Simulation on CoMSES Computational Model Library. 2022. Available online: https://www.comses.net/codebases/2ccbae5e-d51c-41ed-80c2-5e4b33c4e9c0/releases/1.0.0/ (accessed on 13 September 2022).
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zafeiris, A. Opinion Polarization in Human Communities Can Emerge as a Natural Consequence of Beliefs Being Interrelated. Entropy 2022, 24, 1320. https://doi.org/10.3390/e24091320
Zafeiris A. Opinion Polarization in Human Communities Can Emerge as a Natural Consequence of Beliefs Being Interrelated. Entropy. 2022; 24(9):1320. https://doi.org/10.3390/e24091320
Chicago/Turabian StyleZafeiris, Anna. 2022. "Opinion Polarization in Human Communities Can Emerge as a Natural Consequence of Beliefs Being Interrelated" Entropy 24, no. 9: 1320. https://doi.org/10.3390/e24091320