Antisocial and Prosocial
Antisocial and Prosocial
Antisocial and Prosocial
Pro-social
behaviour
Humans
are
social
beings
and
seek
interac-ons
with
others.
They
will
form
many
social
rela-onships
throughout
their
life-me.
The
term
social
rela*onship
is
dened
as
the
connec-on
or
associa-on
between
two
or
more
people,
especially
with
regard
to
how
they
think,
feel
and
behave
towards
each
other.
Pro-social
behaviour
The
term
social
behaviour
can
be
dened
as
any
behaviour
where
interac-on
occurs
between
two
or
more
people.
Eg:
smiling
at
someone.
Social
behaviour
can
be
classied
as
either
posi%ve
or
nega%ve.
Posi%ve
social
behaviour
is
referred
to
as
pro-social
behaviour.
Nega%ve
social
behaviour
is
dened
as
an*-social
behaviour.
Pro-social
behaviour
Pro-social
behaviour
is
any
behaviour
intended
to
help
or
benet
another
person,
group
or
society.
To
determine
whether
behaviour
is
pro-social
the
goal
that
drives
the
behaviour
is
a
more
important
considera-on
than
the
actual
outcome
of
the
behaviour.
True
pro-social
behaviour
is
inten%onal,
the
individual
deliberately
tries
to
provide
assistance.
Eg.
Opening
the
door
for
someone
when
they
have
their
arms
full
Situa-onal
Factors
Bibb
Latane
and
John
Darley
(1968)
conducted
several
experiments
to
help
understand
why
so
many
people
failed
to
help
Ki]y.
They
iden-ed
3
key
factors
associated
with
the
specic
situa-on
that
inuences
whether
people
will
be
prosocial
and
help.
These
include
whether
we
no-ce
the
situa-on,
interpret
the
situa-on
as
one
where
help
is
needed,
and
whether
we
are
prepared
to
take
responsibility
for
helping
in
that
situa-on.
Social
Norms
Onen
we
help
others
because
we
believe
that
we
ought
to
help;
for
example,
we
ought
to
return
a
lost
wallet
that
we
nd
and
we
ought
to
help
a
new
student
nd
their
way
to
a
classroom.
In
such
cases,
our
desire
to
help
is
inuenced
by
social
norms
Social
norms
are
standards,
or
rules,
that
govern
what
people
should
or
should
not
do
in
dierent
social
situa-ons.
Two
social
norms
that
can
inuence
us
are
the
reciprocity
norm
and
the
social
responsibility
norm.
Reciprocity
norm
The
reciprocity
norm
is
based
on
the
reciprocity
principle,
an
unwri]en
rule
that
we
should
give
what
we
receive
or
expect
to
receive.
The
word
reciprocal
means
to
give
mutually
and
the
saying
Do
unto
others
as
you
would
have
them
do
unto
you
reects
the
reciprocity
principle.
In
accordance
with
the
reciprocity
principle,
the
reciprocity
norm
prescribes
that
we
should
help
others
who
help
us.
Reciprocity
norm
For
example,
if
you
help
a
friend
with
their
Maths
homework,
you
would
expect
them
to
return
the
favour
and
assist
you
with
another
subject
when
you
need
help.
This
expecta%on
is
both
reasonable
and
socially
acceptable.
To
receive
without
giving
in
return
goes
against
or
breaks
the
reciprocity
norm.
Personal
Factors
There
is
empirical
evidence
which
suggests
various
personal
factors
can
inuence
pro- social
behaviour
demonstrated
through
helping.
Such
factors
include:
our
ability
to
empathize
with
others
the
mood
we
are
in
when
help
is
needed
whether
we
feel
competent
to
give
the
help
which
is
required.
Empathy
We
are
more
likely
to
help
someone
in
need
of
help
if
we
feel
empathy
for
them.
Empathy
is
the
ability
to
iden-fy
with
and
understand
another
persons
feelings
or
dicul-es.
Empathic
people
can
understand
the
distress
of
others,
feel
concern
for
them
and
can
imagine
what
it
must
be
like
to
be
in
need
of
help.
When
we
empathise
with
someone,
we
usually
want
their
suering
to
end
and
this
can
be
a
powerful
mo-ve
for
us
to
help
in
some
way
Empathy
Research
ndings
indicate
that,
in
an
emergency
situa-on
where
people
appear
to
be
in
distress,
the
more
distressed
and
upset
bystanders
become
from
observing
the
distress
experienced
by
others,
the
more
likely
they
are
to
provide
help
(Schroeder
&
others,
1995;
Dovidio,
1984).
Mood
Are
we
more
likely
to
help
someone
if
we
are
in
a
good
mood
or
a
bad
mood?
For
example,
do
you
believe
that
you
would
be
more
likely
to
help
someone
just
aOer
nding
out
that
you
achieved
an
A+
for
an
important
exam?
What
if
you
just
had
a
huge
ght
with
your
best
friend?
Would
you
be
more
or
less
likely
to
help
a
stranger
who
asked
for
your
assistance
at
that
%me?
Research
studies
indicate
that
a
good
mood
increases
helping,
whereas
a
bad
mood
will
some-mes
increase
and
some-mes
decrease
helping
behaviour.
Mood
Because
helping
makes
us
feel
good,
people
some-mes
help
in
order
to
stay
in
a
good
mood.
People
may
also
help
in
order
to
escape
from
a
bad
mood.
Consequently,
people
who
feel
guilty
are
onen
helpful
people.
Psychologists
believe
that
the
posi-ve
inuence
of
mood
on
helping
behaviour
results
from
a
number
of
interrelated
factors.
Helping
can
sonen
a
bad
mood
and
maintain
or
promote
a
good
mood.
Read
pp
Mood
Competence
You
are
holidaying
beside
the
Murray
River
and
observe
a
swimmer
in
the
middle
of
the
river
calling
out
for
help
as
they
are
swept
downstream.
You
have
no%ced
the
situa%on,
have
correctly
interpreted
it
as
one
in
which
help
is
required
and
have
also
accepted
responsibility
for
helping.
You
quickly
look
around
and
realise
that
there
is
no-one
else
around.
Would
you
help
the
swimmer
if
you
didn't
know
how
to
swim?
you
would
be
extremely
unlikely
to
do
this.
You
would
probably
realise
that
diving
into
a
river
to
rescue
someone
if
you
are
not
a
strong
swimmer
could
lead
to
your
own
drowning
as
well
as
that
of
the
other
person
Competence
Clearly,
we
can't
help
someone
if
we
don't
have
the
skills
required
or
don't
know
how.
Consequently,
our
actual
or
perceived
ability
to
help
can
inuence
whether
or
not
we
help
in
a
specic
situa-on,
as
well
as
the
type
of
help
we
may
oer.
Research
ndings
indicate
that
people
with
abili%es
or
training
that
are
relevant
to
a
situa%on
in
which
help
is
required
are
more
likely
to
help.
Furthermore,
relevant
training
makes
help
not
only
more
likely
to
be
oered,
but
also
more
likely
to
be
eec-ve
Altruism
Some-mes,
pro-social
behaviour
such
as
voluntary
work
for
the
disabled,
dona-ng
money
to
a
charity
is
labelled
as
altruism.
Altruism
is
a
specic
kind
of
pro-social
behaviour
where
the
mo-ve
to
help
is
totally
seless.
Altruism
refers
to
pro-social
behaviour
focused
on
the
wellbeing
or
benet
of
others
without
any
thought
to
personal
gain
or
reward
Altruism-
danger?
Some
psychologists
have
argued
that
altruism
also
diers
from
ordinary
helping
behaviour
in
that
it
involves
an
element
of
personal
risk
Consequently,
genuine
altruism
would
be
demonstrated
by
a
passer-by
who,
for
example,
puts
themself
at
risk
by
running
into
a
blazing
house
to
rescue
a
stranger
trapped
inside
Most
social
psychologists
adopt
the
more
conven-onal
deni-on
of
altruism,
but
does
not
necessarily
involve
an
element
of
personal
risk
Reviewing
Altruism
Complete
learning
ac-vity
10.10
Review
ques-ons
on
page
432
Qs
1-4
Diusion
of
responsibility
People
onen
fail
to
help
when
others
are
around
because
of
the
diusion
of
responsibility
across
all
the
people
who
are
present.
Diusion
of
responsibility
is
the
belief
that,
in
a
situa-on
where
help
is
required
and
others
are
present,
one
or
more
other
people
will
or
should
take
responsibility
for
helping.
When
other
people
are
present,
responsibility
is
divided
up
or
spread
(diused)
across
the
whole
group.
Diusion
of
responsibility
Thus,
diusion
of
responsibility
helps
explain
why
no-one
helps
when
many
people
are
present
in
a
situa-on
where
help
is
required.
The
fact
that
there
are
a
lot
of
other
people
around,
actually
decreases
the
likelihood
that
any
one
person
will
help.
In
the
Genovese
murder,
the
responsibility
for
helping
may
have
diused
across
all
38
neighbours
who
witnessed
the
event.
Ul-mately,
responsibility
diused
to
the
level
where
only
one
person
felt
any
personal
responsibility
to
act
Audience
inhibi-on
The
presence
of
others
at
the
scene
provides
an
audience
and
this
increases
the
chance
of
being
embarrassed
or
feeling
foolish.
Consequently,
these
aspects
of
the
situa-on
can
inhibit,
or
prevent,
someone
from
helping.
This
reason
for
failing
to
help
is
called
audience
inhibi*on
Audience
Inhibi-on
Audience
inhibi*on
can
be
dened
as
not
helping
another
human
being
because
of
a
fear
of
appearing
foolish
in
the
presence
of
others.
It
typically
leads
to
bystanders
to
keep
calm
in
an
emergency
and
check
to
see
how
others
present
are
reac-ng.
The
problem
is
that
if
people
observe
that
everyone
else
is
keeping
calm
they
will
conclude
that
no-one
else
is
concerned
or
upset
and
therefore
help
is
not
needed
Latane and Darley (1968)- smoke-lled room experiment smoke-lled room experiment
Cost-benet
analysis
Suppose
you
had
a
close
friend
with
kidney
failure
A
kidney
transplant
would
enable
them
to
resume
a
normal
lifestyle
and
avoid
the
regular
hospital
visits.
If
they
asked
you
to
donate
one
of
your
kidneys,
what
would
you
do?
In making the decision about whether to help, you might weigh up the costs of dona-ng (considera%ons about your own health) against those of not dona-ng (guilt). You may also consider the benets of dona-ng (feeling good about helping someone), compared with not dona-ng (no interrup%ons to your own life)
Cost-benet
analysis
A
cost-benet
analysis
involves
an
individual
weighing
up
the
personal
and
social
costs
of
helping
against
the
benets
of
helping.
Benets
of
helping
may
be
monetary
rewards,
the
gra-tude
of
the
vic-m,
feeling
good,
and
increase
in
self
esteem,
social
approval,
or
the
thrill
of
making
the
news.
Costs
may
include
the
eort
and
-me
it
takes
to
help,
risks
such
as
personal
injury,
feeling
guilty
or
embarrassed,
or
loss
of
resources.
An--social
behaviour
An*-social
behaviour
is
any
behaviour
that
is
disrup-ve
or
harmful
to
the
well
being
or
property
of
another
person
or
the
func-oning
of
a
group
or
society.
It
typically
involves
ac-ons
which
break
the
law,
rules
or
social
norms
concerning
personal
and
property
rights
of
others.
At
the
core
of
many
an--social
acts
is
aggression.
Aggression
In
psychology,
aggression
is
dened
more
precisely
than
the
deni-on
used
in
our
everyday
language.
eg,
an
asser%ve
and
persistent
sales
assistant,
whose
behaviour
might
be
considered
aggressive
by
customers,
may
not
be
considered
aggressive
according
to
a
psychological
deni%on.
However,
ghts,
armed
conicts,
terrorist
bombings,
sexual
assault,
domes%c
violence,
suicide,
racism,
harassment,
bullying
and
brutality
of
all
kinds
are
all
considered
by
psychologists
to
be
demonstra-ons
of
aggression.
Aggression
Aggression
is
dened
as
any
behaviour
intended
to
cause
physical
or
pyschological
harm
to
a
person
(including
self),
animal
or
object.
For
an
ac-on
to
be
considered
aggressive
there
must
be
an
inten%on
to
harm,
regardless
of
whether
or
not
harm
is
actually
done.
Reviewing Aggression
Explana-ons
of
aggression
Most
of
the
theories
on
aggression
can
be
organised
into
one
of
four
perspec-ves
or
approaches
to
explaining
aggression.
Psychodynamic
perspec*ve:
aggression
in
an
inner
urge
or
force
that
builds
up
within
us
un-l
it
needs
to
be
released.
Ethological
perspec*ve:
aggression
is
ins-nc-ve
and
has
adap-ve
and
survival
func-ons.
Biological
perspec*ve:
aggression
has
a
biological
basis
and
is
therefore
inuenced
by
our
genes,
biochemistry,
brain
and
nervous
system
Social
learning
perspec*ve:
aggression
is
a
learned
behaviour
and
most
of
the
learning
occurs
through
observing
aggressive
behaviour
and
copying
what
we
see.
Biological
perspec-ve
Are
we
all
born
with
genes
for
aggression?
Are
we
born
aggressive,
but
learn
to
control
our
aggression?
Psychologists
who
have
studied
the
rela-onship
between
the
body
and
behaviour
have
developed
various
biologically
based
theories
to
explain
aggression
The
biological
theories
suggest
that
there
are
important
physiological
factors
that
combine
in
various
ways
to
inuence
whether
an
aggressive
response
will
be
made
in
a
par-cular
situa-on.
Biological-
Gene-cs
Some
biological
theories
have
focused
on
the
role
of
genes
in
aggression.
Eg.,
successful
a]empts
at
breeding
highly
aggressive
strains
of
rats,
mice
and
rabbits
in
the
laboratory
suggest
that,
among
these
animals
at
least,
individuals
can
inherit
an
aggressive
tendency
(Cologer-Cliord,
Simon
&
Jubilan,
1992).
Biological-
Gene-cs
In
one
study
by
Finnish
psychologist
Kirs-
Lagerspetz
(1979),
the
most
aggressive
mice
were
taken
from
a
group
and
mated
with
each
other,
and
the
least
aggressive
mice
were
also
interbred.
This
procedure
was
repeated
for
26
genera-ons.
In
the
nal
group
of
ospring,
the
mice
that
had
been
bred
for
aggressive
tendencies
showed
extreme
aggression;
eg,
they
immediately
a]acked
any
other
mouse
placed
in
their
cage.
On
the
other
hand,
the
docile
(non-aggressive)
group
were
so
placid
that
when
other
mice
a]acked
them
they
did
not
ght
back
or
defend
themselves
in
any
way.
Biological-
Gene-cs
Generally,
psychologists
believe
that
our
gene-c
make-up
may
make
us
more
likely
to
become
or
be
an
aggressive
person,
but
environmental
factors
play
a
crucial
role
in
determining
whether
or
not
we
do
in
fact
become
an
aggressive
person
or
are
pre- disposed
to
use
aggressive
behaviour.
Biological-
Neural
Because
aggression
is
a
complex
behaviour,
onen
involving
thoughts
and
feelings
as
well
as
ac-ons,
the
brain
has
a
crucial
role
in
aggression.
However,
psychologists
believe
that
it
is
unlikely
that
one
par-cular
structure
or
area
of
the
brain
is
solely
responsible
for
aggression
Biological-
Neural
The
hypothalamus
and
the
amygdala
are
two
structures
located
deep
within
the
brain.
These
structures
are
made
up
mostly
of
neurons,
as
are
all
other
brain
structures.
The
neurons
communicate
with
each
other
with
signals
that
are
a
combina-on
of
electrical
and
chemical
ac-vity.
Biological-
Neural
Neurons
that
form
brain
structures
can
be
manipulated
using
a
weak
electrical
current.
Researchers
can
do
this
using
a
device
called
an
electrode.
Using
an
electrode,
researchers
can
ac-vate
(switch
on)
or
block
(switch
o)
a
brain
structure
and
stop
it
from
func-oning
as
it
normally
does.
Biological-
Neural
Researchers
have
found
that
when
the
hypothalamus
and
the
amygdala
of
certain
animals
are
electrically
s-mulated,
aggressive
responses
increase,
and
when
the
electrical
ac-vity
is
blocked,
aggressive
responses
decrease.
Biological-
Biochemical
Aggressive
behaviour
can
result
from
the
presence
and
ac-vity
of
certain
chemical
substances
in
the
body.
These
substances
may
occur
naturally
in
the
body
(such
as
hormones)
or
they
may
be
introduced
into
the
body
by
inges-ng
them
(such
as
alcohol
and
other
drugs).
Biological-
Biochemical
Biochemical
inuences
have
been
used
to
explain
dierences
in
the
incidence
of
aggressive
behaviour
between
the
sexes,
as
well
as
dierences
between
individual
males
and
females.
Compared
with
females,
human
males
tend
to
behave
more
aggressively
in
everyday
life
and
commit
more
violent
crimes.
One
explana-on
for
the
sex
dierence
in
aggressive
behaviour
is
the
diering
levels
of
the
hormone
testosterone
in
males
and
females.
Biological-
Biochemical
Experiments
with
animals
have
shown
that
aggressive
behaviour
increases
signicantly
when
high
levels
of
testosterone
are
present
and
low
levels
of
testosterone
result
in
less
aggressive
behaviour
Experiments
have
also
shown
that,
irrespec-ve
of
species,
when
aggressive
male
animals
are
castrated
(de-sexed),
thus
reducing
their
level
of
testosterone,
the
animal
becomes
more
placid.
However,
aner
castra-on,
if
they
are
injected
with
testosterone,
the
animals
tend
to
become
aggressive
again.
Biological-
Biochemical
Various
research
studies
in
prisons
have
found
that
prisoners
who
were
convicted
of
violent
crimes,
such
as
assault,
rape
and
murder,
tended
to
have
a
higher
level
of
testosterone
In
comparison
prisoners
convicted
of
non-violent
crimes,
such
as
trac
oences,
fraud
or
embezzlement
had
lower
testosterone
Biological-
Biochemical
Other
kinds
of
chemicals
that
can
increase
the
frequency
of
aggressive
and
other
an--social
behaviour
include
alcohol,
some
prescribed
medica-ons
and
some
illegal
drugs
such
as
heroin
and
ice.
Research
evidence
suggests
that,
even
with
only
a
small
quan-ty
of
alcohol
in
their
blood,
many
people
become
disinhibited,
having
less
control
over
their
feelings
and
reac-ons
than
usual.
While
many
researchers
are
uncertain
why
alcohol
increases
aggressive
behaviour,
some
believe
that
the
drug
may
aect
the
amygdala
and
other
areas
of
the
brain
involved
in
aggression
Reviewing
social-learning
Complete
Qs
1,2,4
&5