America has always been one of the most culturally diverse nations on earth. Americans have benefited from the rich mix of traditions, foods, music, and technology. Without ever leaving the u.s., you are likely to communicate with people from dozens of different ethnic and cultural traditions.
America has always been one of the most culturally diverse nations on earth. Americans have benefited from the rich mix of traditions, foods, music, and technology. Without ever leaving the u.s., you are likely to communicate with people from dozens of different ethnic and cultural traditions.
America has always been one of the most culturally diverse nations on earth. Americans have benefited from the rich mix of traditions, foods, music, and technology. Without ever leaving the u.s., you are likely to communicate with people from dozens of different ethnic and cultural traditions.
America has always been one of the most culturally diverse nations on earth. Americans have benefited from the rich mix of traditions, foods, music, and technology. Without ever leaving the u.s., you are likely to communicate with people from dozens of different ethnic and cultural traditions.
Copyright:
Attribution Non-Commercial (BY-NC)
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 32
4
Cultural Cues
Nonverbal Communication in a Diverse World
MERICA HAS ALWays been one of the most culturally diverse nations on
earth, As Garreau (1981) maintains in his book The Nine Nations of North
America, “It is no surprise that the second most commonly asked question after
“What's your name?’ is ‘Where are you from?”” (p. xvi). Although some Ameri-
cans grow up in ethnically homogeneous communities, most of us live and work
in communities that are ethnically and culturally diverse, Look around your class-
room or your workplace. Chances are there are people who grew up in many
regions of the United States, as well as people from different ethnic and racial
backgrounds than your own. Intolerance and prejudice against various ethnic
groups has been a problem throughout much of U.S. history, yet diversity has
served as one of our nation’s greatest strengths. Americans have benefited from
the rich mix of traditions, foods, music, and technology contributed over the
years by people of many backgrounds.
Americans are the most mobile people on earth. According to Dinnerstein
and Reimers (1975), “Never before—and in no other country—have as many
varied ethnic groups congregated and amalgamated as they have in the United
States” (p. 1). Moreover, once immigrants settle in America, they and their fami
lies frequently move to new states and regions. More than 20 percent of Ameri-
cans move annually (Fielding, 1974), and this has been true for centuries (Lewis,
1972; Zelinsky, 1973). One of the biggest communication challenges you will face
is effectively interacting with people from different cultural backgrounds than
your own, Indeed, without ever leaving the United States, you are likely to com-
‘municate with people from dozens of different ethnic and cultural traditions.
Lustig and Koester (1993) conclude:
On this, the threshold of the twenty-first century, competence in intercultural
‘communication is an absolute necessity. In both your private and public lives,
74‘Cultural Cues
in all of your personal and professional endeavors, itis imperative that you
learn to communicate with people whose cultural heritages make them vastly
different from yourself. (p. xv)
Furthermore, people are traveling today in record numbers, and interna-
tional trade is at an all-time high (Brown, Kane, & Rodman, 1994). Never before
have so many people from so many cultures had so much contact with one an-
other. Language differences constitute a major barrier to effective intercultural
communication, yet they are just the tip of the iceberg, Nonverbal behaviors are
both biological (for discussions on the biological basis of nonverbal communi-
cation see Chapters 1,5, and 7) and cultural in origin. Because cultural differences
exist in each channel of nonverbal behavior and because nonverbal behaviors are
multichanneled, the chance for intercultural misunderstandings in nonverbal
communication is substantial
Culture is mainly an implicit, spontaneous, nonverbal phenomenon. Most
aspects of one’s culture are learned through observation and imitation rather than,
through explicit verbal instruction or expression. The primary level of culture is
communicated implicitly, without awareness, by primarily nonverbal means (An-
dersen, 1986, 1988; E. T. Hall, 19845 Sapir, 1928), For example, cultural customs
such as how to wait in line, what foods to eat, how to treat one’s elders, and how
to greet other people are communicated nonverbally from adults to children,
often with no verbal instruction. In fact, one of the most basic and obvious functions
of nonverbal communication is to communicate one’s culture. People cling tena-
ously to cultural, religious, and ethnic customs and are unwilling to give up
important aspects of their culture, including language, dress, gestures, tactile be-
haviors, and the many other aspects of nonverbal behavior that you will read
about in this chapter. Indeed, culture is such @ powerful force that throughout
history people have gone to war to preserve cultural customs and behaviors.
Until people communicate interculturally, they are unaware of most of their
‘own nonverbal behaviors, because these behaviors are enacted mindlessly, spon-
taneously, and unconsciously (Andersen, 1986; Burgoon, 1985a; Samovar & Por-
ter, 19855 see also Chapter 1). Indeed, Edward Sapir stated many years ago, “We
respond to gestures with an extreme alertness and, one might almost say, in ac-
cordance with an elaborate and secret code that is written nowhere, known to
none and understood by all” (1928, p. 556). In fact, “culture is so basic, learned at
such a tender age, and so taken-for-granted, that it is often confused with human
nature itself” (Andersen, 1997, p. 224). Because we are not usually aware of our
own nonverbal behavior, which we take for granted, we have great difficulty un-
derstanding or learning the nonverbal behavior of another culture. Frequently,
people feel uncomfortable in other cultures because they intuitively think “some-
thing isn’t right.” “Because nonverbal behaviors are rarely conscious phenomena,
it may be difficult for us to know exactly why they {people from other cultures]
are feeling uncomfortable” (Gudykunst & Kim, 1992, p. 172).
In this chapter, we will focus on six conceptual differences among cultures
7576 Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
that provide a theoretical explanation for why cultures differ in their nonverbal i
communication. Indeed, a multitude of nonverbal differences exist between cul-
tures, and detailing these myriad differences would be impossible and somewhat
trivial. To begin, we will briefly explore intercultural interactions in terms of the
nonverbal codes that were discussed in Chapters 2 and 3. Following that, we will
examine in detail the six cultural dimensions that help explain differences in non- |
verbal communication across cultures.
Cultural Differences in Nonverbal Communication Codes
Chronemics
According to Edward Hall, the founder of the intercultural communication field,
“time talks. It speaks more plainly than words” (1959, p. 15). In the following
extract, Hall (1959) describes an American diplomat in a Latin country arriving
for an appointment to see a cabinet minister of the government:
‘Arriving a little before the hour (an American respect pattern), he waited. The |
hour came and passed;—five minutes—ten minutes—fifteen minutes. At this
point he suggested to the secretary that pethaps the minister did not know |
he was waiting in the outer office... thirty minutes—forty-five minutes (the
insult period)! He jumped up and told the secretary that he had been “cooling
his heels in an outer office for forty-five minutes and he was damned sick and
tired” of this type of treatment. The message was relayed to the minister who
said, in effect, “Let him cool his heels.” The attaché’s stay in the country was
not a happy one. The principal source of misunderstanding lay in the fact that
in the country in question the five-minute delay interval was not significant.
Forty-five minutes, on the other hand, instead of being at the tail end of the
‘waiting scale, was just at the beginning.” (p. 18)
Cultures dance to different drummers. Chronemics is probably the most-
discussed and best-researched nonverbal code in the intercultural literature (Bru-
neat 1979; Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1996; Gudykunst & Kim, 1992; Hall, 1959,
1976, 1984). Although each culture is unique in its use of time, Hall (1984) main- |
tains that the big cultural gulf is between monochronic, or M-time, cultures and
polychronic, or P-time, cultures. M-time cultures, including countries in north-
‘em Europe or with northern European roots (for example, England, Germany,
Finland, Norway, Sweden, Canada, and the United States), believe in doing one |
thing at a time. P-time cultures have little regard for artificial schedules and stress
informality, people-centeredness, multiple simultaneous activities, and the im- |
portance of context. |
Some years ago on a trip to some remote island in the Bahamas (a P-time
culture), I (being from an M-time culture) was surprised to discover that life in :Cultural Differences in Nonverbal Communication Codes
the islands totally stops when it rains. Until the rain stopped, the ground crew
at the airport would not unload our airplane, nor would gas station attendants
pump gas. We American college students who were visiting the island on our
short one-week spring break were outraged by this behavior. But our anger was
useless, for we were up against a cultural (P-time) custom of the Caribbean
people. As this story illustrates, the time frames of cultures vary so substantially
that if only chronemic differences existed, intercultural misunderstandings would
still be considerable. As described in Chapter 3, people in the United States view
time as a commodity that can be wasted, spent, saved, and used wisely. Of course,
many cultures have no concept of time as being segmented into arbitrary units
(seconds, minutes, hours, weeks, months, and so on). In many third world cul-
tures, life moves to the rhythms of nature: the day, the weather, the tides, and
such, Things are experienced polychronically and simultaneously. By contrast, in
Western cultures time is modularized, and events are scheduled sequentially not
simultaneously. It is thus very important to remember when interacting with
people from different cultures that the beat of their life may be to a totally differ-
ent drummer. As an American visiting a third world culture, it would be safe to
assume that all your assumptions about time are false.
Proxemics
Research on proxemics has shown that cultures differ substantially in their use of,
interpersonal space, in their regard for territory, and in the meanings they assign
to proxemic behavior (Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1996; Gudykunst & Kim,
1992; Hall, 1959, 1976; Malandro & Barker, 1983; Scheflen, 1974; see Chapter 2
for more on proxemic behavior). For example, according to Lustig and Koester
(4993), people from colder climates typically maintain larger interpersonal dis-
tances when they communicate, whereas those from warmer climates prefer closer
interpersonal distances (p. 196).
(One Saturday night in Mexico City several years ago, I rode in a subway
train with several other Americans. As thousands of people boarded the train and
the crush became intense, the Americans showed signs of pain and claustropho-
bia, yet the Mexicans remained blasé, though somewhat amused by our discom-
fort. It is common for Americans to perceive Mexicans as rude and Arabs as
pushy because they maintain close personal-space boundaries and expect and
employ closer interpersonal distances than do Americans. Conversely, Arabs and
Mexicans may perceive Americans as aloof and unfriendly because of cultural
differences in proxemic behavior.
Kinesics
Considerable intercultural differences have been observed in all aspects of peo-
ple’s kinesic behavior, including facial expressions, body movements, gestures,
and conversational regulators (Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1989; Gudykunst &
7apter 4+ Cultural Cues
Kim, 19925 Hall, 1976; Jensen, 1985; Samovar, Porter, & Jain, 19813 Scheflen, 1974). i
For example, the A-OK sign, which is formed by making a circle with the thumb |
and index finger, is familiar to all Americans. When Richard Nixon visited Ven- |
ezucla as the US. vice president in 1956 and gave two A-OK signs to a crowd of
demonstrators in Caracas, a huge riot erupted. Nixon was later informed that the
A-OK sign actually had a different meaning in Venezuela, similar to an upraised
middle finger in America. In Italy, China, and Colombia good-bye is signaled by
moving the palm and fingers back and forth toward one’s body, a gesture that
means “come here” in the United States (Jandt, 1995). These differences in em-
blems—wordlike gestures with explicit meanings—really constitute a different |
language as people travel from culture to culture.
Northern Europeans and northeast Asians engage in much more restrained
nonverbal displays than, for example, Africans, southern Buropeans, or Mexi-
cans. Gestures and movements differ dramatically in meaning, extensiveness, |
and intensity. Americans and northern Europeans seem “square” and unexpres-
sive to most Italians and Egyptians. Conversely, the gestural behavior of people
from the Mediterranean seems almost undignified and overly emotional to most
Americans.
Haptics
Research has confirmed that interpersonal patterns of tactile communication, or
haptics, show considerable intercultural variation (Andersen & Leibowitz, 1978;
Malandro & Barker, 1983; Prosser, 1978; Samovar, Porter, & Jain, 1981). Recent
studies reveal differences in international and intercultural touch in amount, lo-
cation, type of touch and whether it is manifested in public or private (Jones,
1994; McDaniel & Andersen, 1998).
Touch behavior represents a major source of diversity among couples from
different cultures. During a recent data-collection session at an international air-
port, a colleague and I observed incredibly diverse tactile behaviors between
people of many cultural backgrounds (see McDaniel & Andersen, 1998). A family
leaving for ‘Tonga formed a circle, wove their arms around each others’ backs, and
prayed and chanted together. A tearful man returning to Bosnia repeatedly tried
to leave his sobbing wife; each time he turned back to her, they would grip each
other by the fingertips and exchange a passionate, tearful kiss and a powerful
embrace. Two Korean couples departed without any touch, despite the prolonged
separation that lay ahead of them.
Physical Appearance |
Physical appearance, the most salient nonverbal code during initial encounters,
is crucial because many intercultural encounters are based on stereotypes and
superficial physical cues. Certainly, what is “beautiful” varies greatly from culture
eee ACultural Differences in Nonverbal Communication
Clothing communicates
culture. This man is
wearing traditional
clothing appropriate to his
culture and climate.
to culture, particularly in terms of clothing, artifacts, body ornamentation, body
type, and body shape (see Burgoon, Buller, & Woodall, 1996; Hatfield & Sprecher,
1986; Samovar, Porter, & Jain, 1981). There is some cross-cultural similarity in
what constitutes an attractive face, but little else is similar. Ubangi women insert
pieces of wood into their mouths to create lips up to ten inches in diameter. In
China during much of this century, women often had their feet bound and de-
formed, which prevented walking outside the home. This culturally imposed de-
formity was considered a very desirable trait in China, although most Americans
would consider such a tradition grotesque and perhaps evil. The amount of body
and facial hair that is desirable also varies greatly from culture to culture. Ameri-
can advertisers have been quite successful in convincing women that hair on their
legs or underarms is unsightly, and they have been somewhat successful in con-
vincing men, particularly in the business world, that facial hair is unprofessional.80
Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
White shirts and ties have become the universal attire of the international
business culture, transcending local cultural dress. Judging by their clothing
alone, it would be difficult to determine what culture these businessmen
are from.
By contrast, European women are much less likely than American women to
remove the hair from their legs and underarms, and businessmen in the Middle
Bast are much more likely to have mustaches or beards than American business-
men. And although T-shirts, blue jeans, business suits, and basketball shoes are
now internationally accepted attire, many cultures still observe local dress cus-
toms. Recently, during the previously mentioned field study at an international
airport, I witnessed Tongans in multicultural ceremonial gowns, Sikhs in white
turbans, Hasidic Jews in blue yarmulkes, and Africans in white dashikis—along-
side Californians in running shorts and halter tops.
Oculesics
Oculesics, the study of messages sent by the eyes, through eye contact, blinks, eye
movements, and pupil dilation, has received only marginal attention by intercul-
tural communication scholars (Gudykunst & Kim, 19925 Jensen, 1985; Samovar,
Porter, & Jain, 1981). Because eye contact has been called an “invitation to com-
‘municate,” its cross-cultural variation is an important communication topic, yet
‘most books on intercultural communication fail to cite any research on this im-
portant topic.
—Cultural Differences in Nonverbal Communication Codes
Several years ago I served as an instructor for a university-wide orientation
for teaching assistants. Among the participants were two young women from
France who were in America for the first time. These very attractive young women
expressed their disappointment to the class that during their brief stay they had
received no attention at all from American men. “They don't even look at us,”
one complained. “Are American men gay?” queried the other. These women were
used to the eye behavior of men in France, where staring at a woman's body is
quite acceptable and, in certain contexts, even flattering, Such staring would be
considered rude in America, where men take fleeting glances at a woman's body
and try not to get caught looking,
Vocalics
Vocalics, or paralinguistics—the tones, pitches, and other nonverbal elements
of the voice—has received relatively little attention in intercultural communica-
tion research (Gudykunst & Kim, 1992; LaBarre, 1985; Rich, 1974; Scheflen, 19735
Samovar, Porter, & Jain, 1981). The sounds of the human voice vary greatly from.
culture to culture, For example, to speakers of other languages, German speakers
seem dominant and angry, Americans often erroneously perceive Saudi Arabians
to be excited or angry due to their intonation patterns (Lustig & Koester, 1993).
Despite the linguistic similarity between the Scandinavian languages Swedish and
Danish, the differences in pronunciation cause Swedes to joke that “Danes don't
spéak, they just make animal sounds!” Italians and Mexicans complain that the
tones of English are unromantic and difficult to use to communicate intimacy.
‘Music and singing, universal forms of nonverbal communication, have been
almost completely overlooked in intercultural research, except for one excellent
study (Lomax, 1968) that identified several groups of worldwide cultures through
differences and similarities in their folk sounds. Lomax’s research is so thorough
that migration and diffusion patterns among cultures can be studied by examin-
ing his data.
‘Accents are one aspect of vocalics that has been widely researched (for a
summary see Giles & Street, 1994). Research has shown that regional, ethnic, and
Dlue-collar accents are preferred by members of one’s own group but thought of
as signs of low intelligence, low education, low status, and low success by the
dominant, or “mainstream,” culture. This research on vocalic stereotyping has
been conducted in a number of countries, with similar patterns emerging regard-
less of the country.
Olfactics
Olfactic differences among cultures have not been widely studied. Reports of
chemical and olfactic communication are given only cursory treatment in con-
temporary intercultural textbooks because of the limited research on this topic
8182
‘Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
(see Gudykunst & Kim, 1992; Jandt, 1995; Lustig & Koester, 1993). One thing we
do know is that North Americans are arguably the most uptight culture on earth
about smell. To them, no body odor is a good body odor. Not so in other places.
In the Middle East communication is so much more intense than in Europe and
in the United States and Canada. According to Hall (1966b), in Arab cultures,
“Not only is the sheer noise level much higher, but the piercing look of the eyes,
the touch of the hands, and the mutual bathing in the warm, moist breath rep-
resents stepped-up sensory inputs to a level Europeans find unbearably intense”
(p. 158). The feel and smell of another's breath, considered obnoxious and offen-
sive by Americans and Canadians, is an intimacy cue in the Middle East, much
like eye contact and nodding in the United States.
Locating and Defining Culture
The word culture is frequently used in a variety of ways. To avoid confusion,
culture is defined in this book as “the enduring influence of the social world
‘on one’s behavior, including interpersonal communication behavior” (Andersen,
1997, p. 246). Culture, along with the social situation and the individual's personal
traits and internal state, is one of the four primary antecedents of interpersonal
behavior (Andersen, 1987, 19975 Figure 4.1). Culture is a learned set of shared
perceptions about belief, values, and needs that affects the behaviors of relatively
large groups of people (Lustig & Koester, 1993). Culture exerts a considerable
force on individual behavior through what Geertz (1973) calls “control mecha-
rnisms—plans, recipes, rules, instructions (what computer engineers call ‘pro-
grams’)—for the governing of behavior” (p. 44). Culture has persuasive effects
on members who live in its sphere, although not all people are affected uni-
formly. Culture is usually an intergenerational phenomenon, because the family
and the community inculcate in children the views, rules, norms, and folkways
of their culture, “Culture can be behaviorally observed by contrasting intragroup
homogeneity with intergroup heterogeneity” (Andersen, Lustig, & Andersen,
1986, p. 1).
Culture is often confused with personal traits because both are enduring
influences on a person's nonverbal behavior (Andersen, 1987, 1997). Personal
traits, however, derive from multiple sources (Andersen, 1987), including ge-
netics, environmental influences, and individual consciousness, as well as cul-
ture. Culture is also sometimes confused with the situation because both are part
of one’s social environment. Culture, however, is an enduring phenomenon,
whereas each situation is a transient experience with an observable beginning and
end. Culture, along with genetics, is the most enduring, powerful, and invisible
shaper of our communication behavior.
ae |Dimensions of Cu
Location of influence FIGURE 4.1 Sources of
Social itterhar influence on interpersonal
environment forces behavior.
Enduring Gs
phenorna | Culture Traits
Time
fame |,
ransient
Prennena | Situation State
Dimensions of Culture
Each culture manifests thousands of nonverbal behavioral differences—some
small and subtle, some large and obvious—that distinguish it from every other
culture. Because there are hundreds of different cultures on earth, the list of these
differences is almost an endless one. Fortunately, there is a way to organize these
differences into meaningful dimensions or groups. Researchers have identified @
set of six dimensions that may explain why cultures differ in their nonverbal be-
havior (Andersen, 1988; Hecht, Andersen, & Ribeau, 1989; Hofstede, 1982). Ar-
guably, most cultural variations in nonverbal behavior, including those discussed
in the first part of this chapter, are the result of these fundamental dimensions.
Immediacy Orientation
Immediacy behaviors are defined as actions that simultaneously communicate
warmth, closeness, and availability for communication and signal approach
rather than avoidance and closeness rather than distance (see Chapter 8 for more
‘on nonverbal immediacy). Cultures that display considerable interpersonal close-
ness or immediacy have been labeled contact cultures; people in these cultures
stand closer to each other, touch more (Hall, 1966b), and are generally more
expressive than are people in noncontact cultures, who tend to stand farther
apart, touch less, and manifest less nonverbal expressiveness. Patterson (1983)
maintains:
These habitual patterns of relating to the world permeate all aspects of every-
day life, but their effects on social behavior define the manner in which people
relate to one another. In the case of contact cultures, this general tendency is
manifested in closer approaches so that tactile and olfactory information may
be gained easily. (p: 145)
Cultures also differ in the degree of sensory stimulation they prefer. Contact cul-
tures create immediacy by increasing sensory input, whereas noncontact cultures
prefer less sensory involvement.84 Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
y
[ Noncontact Cutures [BBD Contact Cultures
Myanmar Taiwan Mediterranean region, Northern Africa
(formerly Burma) Thailand including: Eastem Europe
China Vietnam France (including Russia)
Hong Kong Norway Greece Indonesia
Japan Sweden Israel Middle East
South Korea Finland taly Latin America
Philippines Germany Portugal {including Mexico)
Spain South America
FIGURE 4.2 _Immediacy orientations of selected countries and regions.
Although much has been written about contact and noncontact cultures,
only a handful of studies have examined the immediacy dimension of culture.
As shown in Figure 4.2, research indicates that contact cultures include Indonesia,
most Arab countries (including those in northern Africa), as well as coun-
tries in the Mediterranean region (including France, Greece, Italy, Portugal, and
Spain), the Middle East, eastern Europe (including Russia), and virtually all of
Latin America (including Mexico; Condon and Yousef, 1983; Hecht, Andersen, &
Ribeau, 1989; Jones, 1994; Jones & Remland, 1982; Mehrabian, 1971a; Patterson,
1983; Samovar, Porter, & Jain, 1981; Scheflen, 1972). Australians are moderate in
their cultural-contact level. Noncontact cultures include most of northern Eu-
rope (including Norway, Sweden, Finland, Germany, and Great Britain), Anglo-
Americans (the predominant culture of the United States), as well as virtually
every Asian country (including Myanmar—formerly Burma—China, Hong
Kong, Japan, South Korea, the Philippines, Taiwan, Thailand, and Vietnam;
‘Andersen, Andersen, & Lustig, 1987; Heslin & Alper, 1983; Jones, 1994; Jones &
Remland, 1982; McDaniel & Andersen, 1995; Mehrabian, 19714; Patterson, 19835
Samovar, Porter, & Jain, 1981; Scheflen, 1972).Dimensions of Culture 85
Although, in general, the Japanese touch very little and strive to maintain
their personal space, extreme crowding during rush hour in Tokyo is quite
common and accepted as normal by urban commuters.
Two recent studies (McDaniel & Andersen, 1998; Remland, Jones, & Brick-
man, 1991) suggest that residents of the United States, Canada, and Great Britain
may actually fall under the contact category. According to this research, these
cultures engage in considerable touch and maintain relatively close interpersonal
distances. These studies question Hall’s (1966b) research and suggest that his
original designation of some cultures—such as Europe and North America (ex-
cluding Mexico)—as noncontact cultures was an oversimplification. One expla-
nation for these conflicting findings is that younger people—that is, those born
after World War II—in England and the United States exhibit much higher con-
tact and more nonverbally expressive behaviors than their parents and grandpar-
ents. Members of the baby-boomer generation and generations that followed
have grown up in a culture permeated by international and intercultural media,
intercultural contact, mediated sports celebrations replete with physical contact,
rock and roll, and a counterculture ideologically predisposed to contact. Try giv-
ing a hug or a high five to a person over 55 (with the exception of family mem-
bers) and see if I’m right. A person in his or her 20s—that is, a member of a
contact culture—is much more likely to respond to your high five. It may not be
that Hall’s findings about Europe and North America were wrong; instead, they
may have simply failed to detect the dramatic culture change that was simulta-
neously occurring in our globally mediated world.86
Chapter4 + Cultural Cues
Contact cultures tend to be located in the Middle East, the Mediterranean
region, and in lower latitudes near the equator. Research has identified several
factors—including energy level, climate, sunlight, and metabolism—that may
explain such latitudinal variations (Andersen, Lustig, & Andersen, 1990). Evi-
dently, cultures in cooler climates tend to be more task-oriented and interperson-
ally “cool,” whereas cultures in warmer climates tend to be more interpersonally
oriented and interpersonally “warm.” Even within the United States, more-
immediate cultures tend to be located in the warmer latitudes. Andersen, Lustig,
& Andersen (1990) report a 31 correlation between latitude and touch avoidance
among American university students. ‘These data suggest that students attend-
ing universities located in the sun belt are more touch-oriented and thus consti-
tute contact cultures or subcultures. Furthermore, research suggests that African
‘Americans tend to be warmer, more expressive, and more immediate than Eu-
ropean Americans (Lustig & Koester; 1993).
Recently, Pennebaker, Rime, and Sproul (1994) found a correlation between
latitude and expressiveness within dozens of countries. Their studies show that
northerners are less expressive in Belgium, Croatia, France, Germany, Italy, Ja~
pan, Serbia, Spain, Switzerland, and the United States than are southerners in
these countries; in fact, a difference in expressiveness has been found to exist
actoss the entire northern hemisphere. Pennebaker et al. (1994) conclude:
Logically, climate must profoundly affect social processes. People living in
cold climates devote more time to dressing, to providing warmth, to planning
ahead for food provisions during the winter months. ... In warm climates,
people are more likely to see, hear, and interact with neighbors year round.
Emotional expressiveness, then, would be more of a requirement. (pp. 15-16)
Similarly, Andersen, Lustig, and Andersen (1990) conclude:
In Northern latitudes societies must be more structured, more ordered, more
constrained, and more organized if the individuals are to survive harsh weather
forces. ... In contrast, southern latitudes may attract or produce a culture
characterized by social extravagance and flamboyance that has no strong incli-
nation to constrain or order their world. (p. 307)
McDaniel and Andersen's (2998) data on public touch suggest the biggest
cultural difference in immediacy is between Asians, who rarely touch in public,
and people from virtually every other culture, all of whom manifest higher de-
grees of public touching, These findings are consistent with other research that
suggests that China and Japan are distinctly nontouch cultures (Barnland, 19785
Jones, 1994). Research by Klopf and Thompson (1991) reveals that Japanese stu-
dents reported themselves to be significantly less immediate than either Finnish
or American students, This provides additional evidence that Asia is a low-
immediacy, noncontact region and that northern Europe and North America—
excluding Mexico—may be higher-immediacy regions than previously believed,Dimensions of Culture 87
ri
‘The United States is the most individualistic culture on earth. Despite traffic
jams and air pollution, individualistic Americans typically commute one
person to a car.
though not as highly immediate as cultures located in Latin America, the Medi-
terranean, or the Middle East.
Little other research has been conducted on the differences in immediacy
and contact between Asia and the rest of the world. Why is Asia such a low-
immediacy culture? McDaniel and Andersen (1995) speculate that Confucianism,
with its emphasis on self-control, respect, decorum, and proper behavior, may
restrain the expressiveness of cultures under its influence. Perhaps the Asian em-
phasis on collectivism (discussed in the following section) is a primary force that
emphasizes respect of others at the expense of individual expressiveness.
To summarize the cultural differences in immediacy: in general, north-
ern countries, northern parts of individual countries, traditional cultures, and
Asian countries are the least immediate and expressives southern regions, modern
countries, and non-Asian cultures are the most expressive and immediate. Obvi-
ously, these findings are painted with a fairly broad brush and must await a more
detailed cultural portrait.
Individualism Versus Collectivism
Perhaps the most fundamental dimensions along which cultures differ is their
degree to which the core values of a culture center on individualism versus col-[Bi coltectvist Cultures [Ed inividuatstic Cuttures
(11 highest counties, in orden (I highest courres, in order) |
1. Venezuela 7. Singapore 1. United States 7. aly |
Zcolombia 8. Chile 2Ausialia—&. Belgium j
SiPakistan 9. Hong Kong 3. Great Briain 8: Denmark
Pera 10, Yugoslavia XGanada 101 France
S.Taiwan 17, Portugal S.Netherlands 11; Sweden
6. Thaland 6. New Zealand
FIGURE 4.3 _Individualistic versus collectivist rankings of selected countries
(Hofstede, 1982).
lectivism. This dimension explains the ways in which people live (for example, i
alone, in families, in villages or tribes, see Hofstede, 1982), their values, and how |
‘they communicate, including how they communicate nonverbally. Individualistic
cultures value space, privacy, emotional expression, and personal choice of non-
verbal behavior. Collectivist cultures value contact, togetherness, and restrained
individual expression of emotion, particularly the expression of negative emo-
tion. Collectivists engage in nonverbal behaviors that benefit the group. |
In his landmark study of forty noncommunist countries, Hofstede (1982) |
reports that the nine most individualistic nations are (in order) the United States,
Australia, Great Britain, Canada, the Netherlands, New Zealand, Italy, Belgium,
and Denmark—all of which are Western, developed cultures with predominantly
Furopean roots (Figure 4.3). The nine most collectivist nations are (in order)
Venezuela, Colombia, Pakistan, Peru, Taiwan, Thailand, Singapore, Chile, and
Hong Kong—all Asian or South American cultures. Similarly, Sitaram and Cod-
gell (1976) reported individuality to be of primary importance in Western cul-
tures, of secondary importance in African cultures, and of lesser importance in
Eastern and Muslim cultures.
oe| ee
Dimensions of Culture
Individualism is one of the fundamental dimensions that distinguish cul-
tures, particularly Eastern cultures from Western cultures. “There is little doubt
that Western culture is individualistic, so people rely on personal judgments.
Eastern cultures emphasize harmony among people, between people and nature,
and value collective judgments” (Hecht, Andersen, & Ribeau, 1989, p.170). Tom-
kins (1984) argues that an individual’s psychological makeup is the result of this,
cultural dimension. He states, “Human beings, in Western Civilization, have
tended toward self-celebration, positive or negative. In Oriental thought another
alternative is represented, that of harmony between man and nature” (p. 182).
‘According to Lustig and Koester (1993), individualism is characterized by the key
words “independence, privacy, self, and the all-important I” (p. 144). By contrast,
collectivist cultures emphasize we and a sense of connection and belonging. Ac-
cording to Jandt (1995), the Japanese concept of amea best typifies collectivist
cultures. Amea is characterized by nurturing concern, dependence on others, and
a wish to be loved and cared for unconditionally.
For better or worse, Americans are individualists. According to the U.S.
census of 1990, the most common American houschold consisted of one person
and the second most common household comprised two people. Americans see
individualism as a blessing and have elevated it to the status of a national religion
(Andersen, 1988). From the early days of the American republic, individualism
has been celebrated. In his writings about America in the 1830s, the Erench phi-
osopher Alexis de Tocqueville (1835/1945) noted:
Individualism is a mature and calm feeling, which disposes each member of
the community to sever himself from the mass of his fellows and to draw apart
with his family and friends, so that he has thus formed a little circle of his
‘own, ... he willingly leaves society at large to itself (p. 104).
“The best and the worst of American culture is attributable to individualism.
Proponents of individualism have argued that itis the basis of liberty, democracy,
freedom, and economic incentive and that it serves as a protection against tyr-
anny. Conversely, individualism has been blamed for our alienation from one
another, loneliness, selfishness, crime, and narcissism. Indeed, Hall (1976) has
claimed that as an extreme individualist, “Western man has created chaos by
denying that part of his self that integrates while enshrining the parts that frag-
ment experience” (p. 9)
‘Although the United States is the most individualistic country on earth (An-
dersen, 1988; Hofstede, 19825 Prosser, 1978), regions of the United States vary in
their degree of individualism. Elazar (1972) has shown that the central Midwest
and mid-Atlantic regions are the most individualistic political culture, whereas
the Southeast is the most traditionalistic and least individualistic. Remember,
however, that these findings are all relative; by world standards, even Mississippi
is an individualistic culture. Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, & Tipton (1985)
8990
Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
Individualism lies at the very core of American culture. ... Anything that
‘would violate our right to think for ourselves, judge for ourselves, make our
own decisions, live our lives as we see fit, is not only morally wrong, itis sacri-
legious. (p. 142)
Just as geographical regions of the United States differ in their degree of individu-
alism, s0 too do various ethnic groups. African Americans, for example, empha-
size individualism, whereas Mexican Americans emphasize group and relational
solidarity (Hecht, Andersen, & Ribeau, 1989). Indeed, extreme individualism
makes it difficult for most Americans to interact with and understand people
from other cultures. As Condon and Yousef (1983) stated, “The fusion of indi-
vidualism and equality is so valued and so basic that many Americans find it most
difficult to relate to contrasting values in other cultures where interdependence
greatly determines a person's sense of self” (p. 65).
Whether a culture is individualistic or collectivist affects the nonverbal be-
havior of that culture in every way. First, people from individualistic cultures are
more remote proxemically. Collectivist cultures are interdependent, and as a re-
sult, people work, play, ive, and sleep in close proximity to each other. Hofstede
(1982) cites research that suggests that as hunters and gatherers, people lived apart
in individualistic, nuclear families. As agricultural societies developed, the inter-
dependent extended family began living in close proximity in large family or
tribal units, Urban-industrial societies later returned to the norm of individu
alism, nuclear families, and lack of proximity to one’s neighbors, friends, and
coworkers.
Kinesic behavior tends to be more synchronized in collectivist cultures.
Where families work collectively, movements, schedules, and actions need to be
highly coordinated (Argyle, 1975a). In developed, urban cultures, family members
often “do their own thing,” coming and going, working and playing, eating and
sleeping, all on different schedules. People in individualistic cultures also smile
more than in normatively, or collectively, oriented cultures, according to Tom-
kins (1984). This is probably due to the fact that individualists are responsible for
their relationships and their own happiness, whereas collectively oriented people
regard compliance with norms as a primary value and personal or interpersonal
happiness as a secondary value (Andersen, 1988). Matsumoto (1993) contends
that “collective cultures will foster emotional displays of their members that
maintain and facilitate group cohesion, harmony, or cooperation, to a greater
degree than individualistic cultures” (p. 132). Similarly, Lustig and Koester (1993)
maintain that “people from individualistic cultures are more likely than those
from collectivist cultures to use confrontational strategies when dealing with in-
terpersonal problems; those with a collectivist orientation arc likely to use avoid-
ance, third-party intermediaries, or other face-saving techniques” (p. 147). People
in collectivist cultures may suppress both positive and negative emotional dis-
plays that are contrary to the mood of the group, because maintaining the group'sDimensions of Culture 91.
cohesion is a primary value (Andersen, 1988). Bond (1993) reports that the Chi-
nese culture is lower in frequency, intensity, and duration of emotional expres-
sion. Bond contends that “the expression of emotion is carefully regulated out
of a concern for its capacity to disrupt group harmony and status hierarchies”
(p. 245).
People in individualistic cultures are encouraged to express emotions
nonverbally, especially in their face and voice, because individual freedom is a
paramount value. Research suggests that people in individualistic cultures are
nonverbally affiliative. Intuitively, the reason for this is not obvious, because in-
dividualism doesn't require affiliation. However, as Hofstede (1982) explains:
In less individualistic countries where traditional social ties, like those with
extended family members, continue to exist, people have less of a need to
make specific friendships. One's friends are predetermined by the social rela-
tionships into which one is born. In the more individualistic countries, how-
ever, affective relationships are not socially predetermined but must be
acquired by each individual personally. (p. 163)
In individualistic countries, affiliation, dating, flirting, small talk, smiling, and
initial acquaintance are more important than in collectivist countries, where the
social network is more fixed and less reliant on individual initiative. Bellah etal.
(1985) maintain that over the centuries, the individualistic and mobile nature
of US. society has enabled people to meet more easily and maintain more open
communication; at the same time, however, it has caused their relationships to be
more casual and transient.
Finally, in an impressive study of dozens of cultures, Lomax (1968) found
that song and dance styles of a country were related to its level of social cohesion
and collectivism. Collectivist cultures exhibit higher cohesiveness in their sing-
| ing styles and more synchrony in their dance styles (Lomax, 1968). It isn’t sur-
| prising that rock dancing, which emphasizes separateness and “doing your own
thing,” evolved in such individualistic cultures as England and the United States.
| Americans’ style of dancing may serve as a metaphor for the whole USS. culture,
which emphasizes individuality more than any other country in the world (An-
dersen, 1988).
Gender Orientation
‘The gender orientation of culture has a huge impact on many aspects of com-
| munication, including nonverbal behavior. Although gender is a biologically de-
termined trait—each of us is born as a male or female- is also regulated by
the gender rules of each culture. In most cultures, males are socialized into so-
called masculine roles, which emphasize qualities such as power and dominance,
| and females are socialized into traditionally feminine roles, which emphasize
| qualities such as nurturance and compassion. Gender roles regulate (1) the types92
Chapter4 + Cultural Cues
of expressions permitted by each sex, (2) occupational status, (3) nonverbal as-
pects of power, (4) the ability to interact with strangers or acquaintances of the
opposite sex, and (5) all aspects of interpersonal relationships between men and
women, “While numerous studies have focused on gender as an individual char-
acteristic, gender has been neglected as a cultural dimension” (Hecht, Andersen,
& Ribeau, 1989, p. 171).
‘As conceptualized here, gender orientation refers to the degree of mascu-
linity or femininity exhibited by a culture. In rigidly masculine cultures, positive
traits typically include strength, assertiveness, competitiveness, and ambitious-
ness; less valued are typically feminine traits such as affection, compassion, nur-
turance, and emotionality (Bem, 1974; Hofstede, 1982). Masculine cultures also
regard power and materialism as important values (Gudykunst & Kim, 19933
Hofstede, 1982). People in masculine cultures believe in ostentatious manliness
(Lustig & Koester, 1993). Masculine Latino countries such as Mexico and Vene-
zuela emphasize machismo, or macho male characteristics. Similarly, masculine
Germanic countries such as Austria, Germany, and Switzerland value “manly”
‘men—as satirically depicted in the classic “Hans and Frans” routine on Saturday
Night Live, in which Hans and Frans are “all pumped up,” not “ittle girlie guys.”
By contrast, feminine (also called androgynous) cultures emphasize nurtur-
ance, compassion, and quality of life (Hofstede, 1982). These cultures have more
flexible sex roles and allow both men and women to express more diverse, less
stereotyped sex-role behaviors. Not surprisingly, women have more rights and
privileges in feminine cultures.
Hofstede (1982) has measured the degree to which a culture endorses mas-
culine or feminine goals. The masculinity of a culture is negatively correlated with
a high percentage of women in technical and professional jobs and positively
correlated with segregation of sexes in higher education (Hofstede, 1982).
The countries with the nine highest masculinity index scores, according
to Hofstede (1982), are (in order) Japan, Austria, Venezuela, Italy, Switzerland,
Mexico, Ireland, Great Britain, and Germany (Figure 4.4). The majority of the
countries in this ist are located in western and central Europe and the Caribbean
region. The nine countries with the highest feminine scores are (in order) Swe-
den, Norway, the Netherlands, Denmark, Yugoslavia, Finland, Chile, Portugal,
and Thailand—a good percentage of these countries being located in northern
Europe. Compared to low-masculinity countries, high-masculinity countrieshave
fewer women in the labor force, have only recently afforded voting privileges to
women, and are least likely to consider wife rape a crime (Seager & Olson, 1986).
Why don't most South American cultures manifest stronger masculine
orientations and the Latin pattern of machismo? Hofstede (1982) suggests that
machismo is more prevalent in the Caribbean-bordering countries than in the
remainder of South America. In fact, South America, as compared to Central
‘America, has a much higher percentage of working women, much higher school
attendance by gitls, and more women in higher education (Seager & Olson, 1986).Dimensions of Culture
[a Feminine Androgynous) Cultures [Bi Maseutine Cultures
(11 highest countries, inorder (1 highest countries, in order)
1.Sweden 7. Chile 1. Japan 7. eland
2Nonay 8. Portugal ZAtstia 8, Great Brain
2 Netherlands 9. Thailand Svenezuela 9. Germany
4. Denmark 10. Spain 4 tly 10. Philippines
5. Yugoslavia 11. Feru 5. Switzeriand 11, Colombia
6. Finland 6. Mexico
FIGURE4.4 Gender orientations of selected countries (Hofstede, 1982).
Furthermore, Brazil—the largest country in South America—manifests consid-
erable Portuguese influence in its language and culture. Because Portugal tends to
be feminine in its gender orientations, its values may still be evident in contem-
porary Brazilian behavior.
Research suggests that androgynous patterns of behavior (that are both
feminine and masculine) result in higher levels of self-esteem, social competence,
success, and intellectual development for both males and females (Andersen,
1988) and may even lead to greater physical health, because both masculine
strength and fitness and feminine expressiveness and nurturance are associated
with good health. Is ita coincidence that the more androgynous (feminine) coun-
tries are associated with longer life expectancies? Perhaps not. Nonverbal styles
that allow both men and women alike to express both masculine traits (such as,
dominance and strength) and femninine attributes (such as warmth and emotion-
ality) are likely to contribute to physical and emotional well-being. Buck (1984)
hhas demonstrated that many males may harm their health by internalizing emo-
tions rather than externalizing them, as women usually do. Internalized emotions
that aren't expressed result in more stress and higher blood pressure. Interest-
ingly, more-masculine countries show higher levels of stress (Hofstede, 1982).
93
“om94 —— Chapter4 + Cultural Cues
Considerable research has demonstrated significantnonverbal vocalic differ-
‘ences between egalitarian and nonegalitarian countries, Countries where women
are economically important and where sexual standards for women are permissive
show more relaxed vocal patterns than less-egalitarian countries (Lomax, 1968).
Moreover, these egalitarian countries show less tension between the sexes, more
vocal solidarity and coordination in their songs, and more synchrony in their
overall movement (Lomax, 1968).
Tn feminine countries, as compared to masculine countries, same-sex touch
is more common. Masculine cultures tend to have a more rigid and unaccepting
view toward same-sex touch, particularly among males, Feminine countries have
more permissive attitudes about sexual behavior, particularly for women (Hof-
stede, 1982). Indeed, many of the most feminine countries, such as Denmark and
the Netherlands, are criticized by people of other nationalities for their liberal
attitudes and laws regarding sexual behavior.
Itis important to note that, according to Hofstede (1982), the United States
tends to be a masculine country, although it is not among the very most mascu-
line. Intercultural communicators should keep in mind that other countries may
be either more or less sexually egalitarian than the United States. Because most
countries are more feminine than the United States (that is, they are more nur-
turing and compassionate), Americans of both sexes frequently seem dominant,
oud, aggressive, and competitive by world standards, conveying the image of the
“ugly American.” Likewise, Americans’ attitude toward women may seem sexist
in extremely feminine cultures such as Scandinavia.
Power-Distance Orientation
A fourth basic dimension of intercultural communication is power distance.
Power distance —the degree to which power, prestige, and wealth are unequally
distributed in a culture—has been measured in a number of cultures using
Hofstede’s (1982) Power Distance Index (PDI). In cultures with high PDI scores,
power, wealth, and influence are concentrated in the hands of a few rather than
being more equally distributed throughout the population. Condon and Yousef
(2983) distinguish among three cultural patterns: democratic, authority-centered,
and authoritarian. The PDI is highly correlated (.80) with authoritarianism (Hof-
stede, 1982).
‘According to Hofstede (1982), the highest-PDI countries are (in order)
the Philippines, Mexico, Venezuela, India, Yugoslavia, Singapore, Brazil, France,
Hong Kong, and Colombia (Figure 4.5). This list is dominated by countries lo- |
cated in the low latitudes of Asia and the Americas. Gudykunst and Kim (1992) |
report that both African and Asian cultures usually establish hierarchical relation
ships. Asian students are expected to be modest and deferent nonverbally in the
presence of their instructors. Likewise, the Vietnamese consider teachers and em- |
ployers to be their mentors and will not question orders. The nine lowest PDI |
countries are (in order) Austria, Israel, Denmark, New Zealand, Ireland, Sweden,
ae|
[i Low-Power-Distance Cultures [EI] High-Power-Distance Cultures
(17 lowest countries, in order) (17 highest counties, in order)
1. Austria 7.Nonway 1. Philippines 7. Brazil
2 Israel 8 Finland 2.Mexico 8 France
3. Denmark 9. Switzerland 3. Venezuela _9. Hong Kong
4. New Zealand 10. Great Britain 4. India 10, Colombia
5. ireland 11. Germany 5, Yugoslavia 11. Turkey
6, Sweden 6. Singapore
FIGURE4.5 Power-distance orientations of selected countries (Hofstede, 1982).
Norway, Finland, and Switzerland (Hofstede, 1982). Most of the countries in this
list are middle-class, developed democracies located at high latitudes.
A primary determiner of power distance is weather and climate (for a dis-
cussion of these macroenvironmental nonverbal factors see Chapter 3). Hofstede
(1982) claims that latitude and climate are major forces that shape a culture. He
maintains that the key intervening variable is climate. For example, in colder
climates, technology is needed for survival. This produces a chain of events in
which people come to rely on each other rather than on authority figures. Edu-
cation, teamwork, and individual initiative are necessary to survive in harsh, vari-
able climates, as are creative solutions to problems and changing conditions,
rather than simple obedience to authority. The result is that most low-PDI cul-
tures are found in high latitudes and harsh climates; in fact, Hofstede (1982)
reports a .65 correlation between PDI and latitude! In a study conducted at
forty U.S. universities, Andersen, Lustig, and Andersen (1990) reported a —.47
correlation between latitude and intolerance for ambiguity and a —.45 correlation
between latitude and authoritarianism. This suggests that residents of the north-
ern United States are less authoritarian and more tolerant of ambiguity than those
95
Ne Zeta a96
(Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
in the southern United States. Northern US. cultures, like their international
counterparts, may have to be less autocratic and more cooperative to ensure col-
laboration and survival in harsher climates.
‘The United States scores slightly lower than the median on the PDI, indi-
cating lesser status differentials than many countries but more inequality than
many others. Cultures differ in terms of how status in acquired. In many coun-
tries, such as India, one’s status is historically ascribed according to class. In the
United States, however, power and status are typically achieved through mone-
tary success and manifested by conspicuous material displays of materialism (An-
dersen & Bowman, 1990).
‘The population of a country or a culture may be another predictor of power
distance. Generally, larger cultures score higher on the PDI (Lustig & Koester,
1993). As the size of any group increases, it becomes unwieldy and difficult to
‘manage informally. This is even true of larger classrooms, larger governments,
and larger organizations. In larger aggregations, informal relationships must yield
to formal rules, bureaucracies, and hierarchical relationships. For cultures with
large populations to function effectively, social and political hierarchies must be
created, causing the power-distance factor to increase, The fact that most low-
power-distance countries are small, well-managed democracies is probably not
coincidental.
Power distance affects the nonverbal behavior of a culture. In high-PDI cul-
tures such as India, with its rigid caste system, interaction among the classes is
severely limited, More than 20 percent of India’s population are identified as “un-
touchables”; these individuals languish at the bottom of India’s five-caste system
(Chinoy, 1967). Any contact with untouchables by members of other castes is
strictly forbidden and considered “polluting,” Certainly, tactile communication
between castes is greatly curtailed in Indian culture. High-PDI countries with less
rigid stratification than India may still prohibit free interclass dating, marriage,
and contact—opportunities that are taken for granted in low-PDI countries.
Low-PDI countries are much more likely to permit nonverbal communication of
all types, including liberal interpersonal association, close interpersonal distances,
and greater tactile contact and visual exchanges.
Civilizations with large power discrepancies produce different kinesic be-
havior. High-PDI cultures foster and encourage emotions that underscore status
differences. In high-PDI cultures, people are expected to show only positive emo-
tions to others with high status and to display negative emotions to others with low
status (Matsumoto, 1991; Porter & Samovar, 1998). According to Andersen and
Bowman (1990), the bodily tension of subordinates is more obvious in power-
discrepant relationships. Similarly, Andersen and Bowman (1990) report that in
power-discrepant circumstances, subordinates smile more in an effort to appease
superiors and appear polite. The ever present smiles on many Asians’ faces con-
stitute a culturally inculcated behavior of appeasing superiors and smoothing
social relations; this nonverbal behavior is in keeping with a high-PDI cultural
profile,Dimensions of Culture
Vocalic, or paralinguistic, cues also vary according to the power-distance
dimension of a given culture. Citizens of low-PDI cultures are generally less aware
that vocal loudness may be offensive to others. American vocal tones are often
perceived as noisy, exaggerated, and childlike (Condon & Yousef, 1983). Lomax
(1968) has shown that in countries where political authority is highly centralized,
singing voices are tighter and the voice box is more closed; by contrast, more
permissive societies produce more relaxed, open, and clear sounds.
Uncertainty Orientation
People in all cultures face change, unpredictability, and uncertainty. Uncertainty
orientation is a cultural predisposition toward risk and ambiguity (Hecht, Ander-
sen, & Ribeau, 1989). Cultures with high uncertainty avoidance dislike ambiguity
and uncertainty and believe that certain rules and beliefs will reduce uncertainty;
at the individual level, this predisposition is often called tolerance or intolerance
for ambiguity (Martin & Westie, 1959). People with an intolerance for ambiguity
or with high levels of uncertainty avoidance want clear, black-and-white answers,
fear change, and dread the future. People with a high tolerance for ambiguity and
with low levels of uncertainty avoidance are more tolerant, accept ambiguous
answers, see many shades of gray, and embrace future change. Hofstede (1982)
demonstrates that a country’s uncertainty avoidance is highly correlated with a
high incidence of neuroticism and anxiety in its population. High uncertainty
avoidance is negatively correlated with risk-taking and positively correlated with
fear of failure.
Countries vary greatly in their tolerance for uncertainty. In some cultures,
freedom leads to uncertainty, which leads to stress and anxiety. Hofstede (1982)
‘maintains that intolerance of ambiguity and dogmatism are primarily a function
of the uncertainty-avoidance dimension rather than the power-distance dimen-
sion, According to Hofstede (1982), the nine countries with the highest levels of
uncertainty avoidance are (in order) Greece, Portugal, Belgium, Japan, Yugosla-
via, Peru, Argentina, Chile, and France (Figure 4.6); this list is dominated by
southern European and South American countries. The nine countries with the
highest levels of uncertainty tolerance are (in order) Singapore, Denmark, Swe-
den, Hong Kong, Ireland, Great Britain, India, the Philippines, and the United
States; this list is dominated by northern European and Southeast Asian cul-
tures. Hofstede (1982) also reports that Catholic countries are high in uncertainty
avoidance, whereas Protestant, Hindu, and Buddhist countries tend to be more
accepting of uncertainty; Eastern religions and Protestantism tend to be less
“absolute” than Catholicism. Andersen, Lustig, and Andersen (1990) report that
intolerance for ambiguity is much higher in the southern United States than in
the north; this tendency reflects the international pattern of tolerance at higher
latitudes.
Fewer studies have examined the uncertainty dimension of nonverbal be-
havior than the other dimensions of culture. However, some research has shown
9798
United States
Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
Gre Bain
ireland
France
Portugal
spain
wm
[a Uncertainty-Tolerant Cultures LUncertainty-Avoidant Cultures
(12 highest countries, in order) (12 highest countries, in order)
1. Singapore 7. India 1.Greece 7. Argentina
2. Denmark 8 Philippines 2.Portugal 8 Chile
3. Sweden 9, United States 3.Belgium 9. France
4.Hong Kong 10. Canads Japan 10. Spain
5 lreland 11, South AMtica 5. Yugoslavia 11. Turkey
6. Great Britain 12, New Zealand 6. Peru 12, Mexico,
FIGURE 4.6 Uncertainty orientations of selected countries (Hofstede, 1982).
that emotional displays are associated with uncertainty avoidance. Hofstede (1982)
maintains that uncertainty-avoidant countries tend to display emotions more
than do uncertainty-tolerant countries. Furthermore, he reports that uncertainty-
avoidant countries are less tolerant in their attitudes toward young people and
their emotional displays. According to Gudykunst and Kim (1992), when consen-
sus or uniformity breaks down in high-uncertainty-avoidant countries, people
become upset and show their emotions more than do residents of uncertainty-
tolerant countries, who are more likely to tolerate change or nonconformity.
Residents of high-uncertainty-avoidant cultures, such as Greece or France, are
most likely to experience and exhibit stress and anxiety cues (Hofstede, 19825 see
Chapter 7 for a complete discussion of these behaviors).
Certainly, disagreement and nonconformity are not appreciated in high-
uncertainty-avoidant countries. Moreover, such cultures seem to prefer verbal
behavior over nonverbal behavior. Indeed, Hofstede (1982) reports that in high-
uncertainty-avoidant countries, rituals are created to provide “pseudocertainty,”
or the illusion of certainty. Such cultures place great importance on memos and
reports and structured information collection and dissemination procedures—
all with the intent of creating the illusion, if not the reality, of certainty.Dimensions of Culture
Similarly, nonverbal behavior is more likely to be codified and rule-
governed in high-uncertainty-avoidant countries. This means that kinesic and
proxemic behaviors, for example, follow a strict set of rules and rituals, Such a
profile seems to fit a high-uncertainty-avoidant country like Japan but remains
to be tested and is somewhat speculative. Hofstede (1982) found that high-
tuncertainty-avoidant nations report more stylized and ritualized behavior, so we
should expect nonverbal behavior to be more proscribed in these cultures. When
communicating with people from a high-uncertainty-avoidant country such as
Japan or France, Americans may come across as excessively unconventional and
lacking in manners; at the same time, Americans may view their Japanese or
French counterparts as too controlled and rigid (Lustig & Koester, 1993).
High Context Versus Low Context
Context is the final important dimension of intercultural communication. Con-
text provides situational or environmental cues that frame any communication
and influence its meaning (see Chapter 3 for more about the contextual codes of
nonverbal communication). E. T. Hall (1976, 1984) has described high- and low-
context cultures in considerable detail. “A high context (HIC) communication or
message is one in which most of the information is either in the physical context
or internalized in the person, while very litte isin the coded, explicit, transmitted
parts of the message” (Hall, 1976, p. 93). Lifelong friends often use high-context,
or implicit, messages that are nearly impossible for an outsider to understand.
The situation itself, a smile, or a glance provides implicit meaning that does not
need to be articulated. “In a high-context culture, much more is taken for granted
and assumed to be shared, and consequently the overwhelming preponderance
of messages are coded in such a way that they don’t have to be explicitly and
verbally transmitted” (Lustig & Koester, 1993, p. 133). In high-context situations
or cultures, information is gleaned from the environment, the context, the situ-
ation, and from nonverbal cues that give the message meaning in the absence of
explicit verbal utterance.
Low-context messages are just the opposite of high-context messages;
most of the information is provided in the explicit code (Hall, 1976). Low-
context messages must be elaborated, clearly communicated, and highly spe-
cific. Unlike personal relationships, which tend to utilize high-context message
systems, institutions such as courts of law and formal systems such as math-
ematics and computer language require explicit, low-context message systems
(E.T. Hall, 1984).
Ina high-context country, the power structure is well known, and people
behave with great deference toward their superiors. By contrast, in a low-context
culture, people are oblivious to the power structure until they are told who is
powerful and who is not. In a high-context culture, a negative facial expression
or lack of enthusiasm may signal the cancellation of a business deal, whereas
written notification would be required in the same case in a low-context culture.
99[i Low context Cultures [EB High-Context Cultures
(6 lowest countries, in order) (@ highest countries, in order)
1.Germany 5. Norway 1.China 5. Native Americans
Switzerland 6. Finland 2. Japan 6. African Americans
3. United States. 7. Denmark 3, South Korea. 7. Mexican Americans
4: Sweden 8. Canada 4 Taiwan 8, Latinos
FIGURE 4.7 High-context versus low-context rankings of selected countries
and groups.
Cultures vary considerably in the degree of context used in communication.
‘As shown in Figure 4.7, the lowest-context cultures are Germany, Switzerland,
the United States, Sweden, Norway, Finland, Denmark, and Canada (Hall, 1976,
1984; Gudykunst & Kim, 1992). These cultures are preoccupied with specifics,
details, literalness, and precise time schedules at the expense of context. They
utilize behavior systems built around Aristotelian logic and linear thinking (E. T.
Hall, 1984). These cultures also tend to be pathologically verbal, that is, they may
miss subtle cues from other people or from the environment that are more mean-
ingful than words (see Chapter 1 for more on the differences between verbal and
nonverbal communication). Countries that exhibit characteristics of both high-
context and low-context message systems include France, Great Britain, and Italy
(Gudykunst & Kim, 1992); these countries are somewhat less explicit than far-
northern European and Scandinavian cultures.
The highest-context cultures are found in Asia. China, Japan, South Korea,
and Taiwan are extremely high-context cultures (Eliot, Scott, Jensen, & McDon-
ough, 19825 Gudykunst & Kim, 19925 E. T. Hall, 1976, 1984). Native Americans,
‘African Americans, Mexican Americans, and Latinos are also fairly high-context
groups (Lustig & Koester, 1993). Usually, language implies an explicit messageDimensions of Culture
system, but the Chinese language is unique in that it is an implicit, high-context
system. To use a Chinese dictionary, one must understand thousands of charac
ters, which change meaning when used in varying combinations with other char-
acters. Pethaps the best example of high-context communication is the Japanese
tea ceremony, in which every behavior of both the host and the guests is carefully
prescribed. The tea “ceremony is a highly scripted and ritualized experience that
is almost incomprehensible to those who have not been trained to understand
the meanings of even the most subtle behaviors” (Lustig & Koester, 1993, p. 132).
According to Jandt (1995), in the tea ceremony “nothing is spoken; all meanings
are in context. A typical response from a low-context observer is ‘hurry up and
drink your tea’” (p. 203)
Asian cultures are so reliant on context partly because they have been highly
influenced by Buddhism, which places a high value on silence, lack of emotional
expression, and the unspoken, nonverbal parts of communication (Burgoon,
Buller, & Woodall, 1989; McDaniel & Andersen, 1998). Americans frequently
complain that the Japanese never “get to the point,” yet they fail to recognize that
high-context cultures rely on the context and setting and let the point evolve
(E. T. Hall, 1984). In a recent study of airport departures, McDaniel and Andersen
(2998) found Asians to be the least tactile of any cultural group on earth. The
influences of Confucianism and Buddhism and the value placed on context rather
than on emotional expression probably explain this finding. Native American
cultures with ancestral migratory roots in East Asia are remarkably similar to
contemporary Asian cultures in several ways, especially in their use of high
context communication (E. T. Hall, 1984).
Before meeting with a small group of native Americans on their reservation
near my home in Southern California, I instructed my Anglo-American friends
to sit silently and not make a lot of idle small talk. Complying with my instruc-
tions, my friends and I sat in a circle under a pepper tree and listened to the wind
and the birds before the meeting commenced. From our perspective, consider-
able time passed (probably only about five minutes). The Native Americans sub-
sequently welcomed us back to the reservation for other ceremonies, projects,
and meetings. By observing their high-context rules for communication, we had
gained their trust.
Not surprisingly, most Latin American cultures, a fusion of Iberian (Por-
tuguese-Spanish) and Native American traditions, are also high-context cultures.
Eastern Mediterranean (including Greece and Turkey) and Arab cultures tend to
fall under the high-context category as well.
Obviously, communication is quite different in high- and low-context cul-
tures. First, explicit forms of communication such as verbal codes are more
prevalent in low-context cultures such as the United States and northern Eu-
rope, Research by Ting-Toomey (1991) shows that U.S. respondents are more
likely to express love overtly and explicitly through verbal communication than
are French or Japanese respondents, These findings are consistent with low-
101102
Chapter4 + Cultural Cues
context communication patterns. People from low-context cultures are often per-
‘ceived as excessively talkative, belaboring of the obvious, and redundant. People
from high-context cultures may be perceived as nondisclosive, wasteful of time,
sneaky, and mysterious. Second, high-context cultures do not value verbal com-
munication as highly as low-context cultures. Elliot et al. (1982) found that more-
verbal people were perceived as more attractive by people in the United States (a
low-context culture), but that less-verbal people were perceived as more attractive
in Korea (a high-context culture). Third, high-context cultures are more likely to
tune into and utilize nonverbal communication. Low-context cultures, particu-
larly men in low-context cultures, fail to perceive as much nonverbal communi-
cation as members of high-context cultures. Nonverbal communication provides
the context for all communication (Watzlawick, Beavin, & Jackson, 19675 see also
Chapter 3), but people from high-context cultures are particularly affected by
these contextual cues. Thus, facial expressions, tension, movements, speed of in-
teraction, location of interaction, and other subtle “vibes” are likely to be per-
ceived by and have more meaning for people from high-context cultures. Finally,
people in high-context cultures expect more from their interaction partners than
do people in low-context cultures (Hall, 1976): people in high-context cultures
expect their interaction partners to pick up on unarticulated feelings, subtle ges-
tures, and environmental cues, which people from low-context cultures simply
do not process. Unfortunately, both cultural extremes fail to recognize these basic
differences in behavior, communication, and context and are quick to misattri-
bute the causes for their behavior.
The Practical Benefits of Understanding Intercultural
Communication Differences
‘Simply reading about these six cultural dimensions of nonverbal communication
will not ensure intercultural communication competence. Combining research-
based knowledge from intercultural articles and courses with actual encounters
with people from other cultures is the best way to gain intercultural communi-
cation competence. International travel, even travel within the United States, pro~
vvides an opportunity to gain perspective on one’s own behavior and the behavior
of others that will help lead to intercultural communication competence.
‘A full, practical understanding of the dimensions along which cultures
differ, as well as knowledge of how specific communication acts differ cross-
culturally, has several practical benefits. First, such knowledge will highlight and
challenge assumptions about our own behavior. The structure of our own behav-
jor is invisible and taken for granted until it is exposed and challenged through
actual intercultural encounters. Indeed, Hall (1976) stated that diversity in inter-
ethnic communication can be a source of strength and self-discovery.‘The Practical Benefits of Understanding Intercultural Communication Differences
International tourism,
migration, study, and
business has greatly
increased intercultural
encounters,
Second, this chapter should convince you that conclusions about the non-
verbal communication of people from other cultures are generalizd observations,
not ironclad rules. No dictionary or code of intercultural behavior exists. You
cannot read people like books, not even people from your own culture. However,
understanding that another person is from a masculine, collectivist, or high-
context culture may help shape your perception of her or his behavior as less
confusing, more interpretable, and less bizarre.
Finally, understanding intercultural communication and actually engag-
ing in intercultural encounters are bound to reduce ethnocentrism and make
strangers from other cultures seem less threatening. Fear is often based on igno-
rance and misunderstanding. In your future life, T guarantee that you will con-
tinue to meet and communicate with many people from other cultures than your
own. Learn from these differences. Intercultural diversity is a source of joy and
optimism about the number of possible ways to be human.
103104 Chapter 4 + Cultural Cues
Summary
The diversity of U.S. culture requires that Americans understand intercultural
communication in order to communicate effectively. Differences in language are
only the tip of the intercultural iceberg; many of the most important cultural
differences are in nonverbal communication.
‘The codes of nonverbal communication, discussed in Chapters 2 and 3, are
used differently by people in various cultures. Chronemically, the more devel-
oped cultures, such as countries in Europe and North America, employ mono-
chronic time, a time orientation in which one thing gets done at a time and time |
is treated like a commodity. People in lesser-developed countries do not struc-
ture time so rigidly; such cultures employ polychronic time, a time orientation
that emphasizes informality and multiple simultaneous activities. Proxemically, j
people from cooler, northerly climates tend to be more distant and remote inter-
personally, whereas people from tropical regions tend to prefer closer interper-
sonal distances. Kinesically, cultures differ as greatly in their use of emblems
(gestures with verbal meanings) as in their languages. People in warmer climates
use more expressive gestures and movements than do people in cooler climates.
Haptic behavior varies dramatically from one culture to the next; for example,
Asian cultures engage in very little touch, whereas Mediterranean cultures em-
ploy a great deal of touch in their interpersonal interactions. The physical ap-
pearance of people around the world in terms of hair, clothing, artifacts, and
body decorations is extremely varied. Cultures also differ greatly in their use of
oculesics. For example, many Mediterranean cultures use much more direct eye
contact than do Americans or Asians. Vocalics also shows considerable cultural
variation in intonation, loudness, pitch, and tone. In terms of olfactics, Ameri-
cans are much more tuned in to smell than are most of the world’s people.
Nonverbal differences across cultures center on six dimensions of human
behavior. Cultures differ in immediacy, the degree to which nonverbal behavior
communicates warmth, closeness, and availability for communications. Cultures
high in immediacy—contact cultures—stand close to each other, touch more, |
and are generally more expressive. Cultures low in immediacy—noncontact |
cultures—tend to stand farther apart, touch less, and manifest less nonverbal
expressiveness. Individualistic cultures value independence and privacy and
‘manifest these values in their nonverbal behavior. Conversely, collectivist cultures |
value interpersonal connection and group behavior and reflect these values in
their nonverbal behavior. Gender orientation is the degree to which a culture |
emphasizes typically masculine traits or typically feminine traits. The nonverbal
behavior of a masculine culture emphasizes assertiveness, competitiveness, and |
ambitiousness. Cultures that are more feminine exhibit nonverbal behavior that
stresses equality, nurturance, emotionality, and compassion. Cultures also differ
in power distance, the degree to which wealth and prestige are equally or un-
= A.Summary
equally distributed in a culture, High-power-distance cultures have more formal,
hierarchical relationships and show only positive emotions to authority figures.
Low-power-distance cultures exhibit nonverbal behavior that is more informal
and not structured along class lines. Uncertainty orientation is a cultural predis-
position toward risk and ambiguity. Uncertainty-avoidant cultures are more con-
trolled and ritualized in their behavior but exhibit more nonverbal emotional
displays when under stress. Uncertainty-tolerant cultures tend to exhibit fewer
prescribed nonverbal behaviors and greater tolerance toward young people and
their emotional displays. Context is the final dimension of intercultural com-
munication. In low-context cultures, verbal communication is emphasized and
nonverbal communication is more informal. In high-context cultures, nonverbal
‘communication is highly stylized and replete with meaning.
105