A Case of Unusual Autobiographical Remembering
A Case of Unusual Autobiographical Remembering
A Case of Unusual Autobiographical Remembering
Department of Neurology, University of California, Los Angeles, Irvine, and Psychiatry & Neurology, University of Southern California,
California, USA
2
Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory and Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, University of California, Irvine,
California, USA
This report describes AJ, a woman whose remembering dominates her life. Her memory is nonstop, uncontrollable, and automatic. AJ
spends an excessive amount of time recalling her personal past with considerable accuracy and reliability. If given a date, she can tell you
what she was doing and what day of the week it fell on. She differs from other cases of superior memory who use practiced mnemonics to
remember vast amounts of personally irrelevant information. We propose the name hyperthymestic syndrome, from the Greek word
thymesis meaning remembering, and that AJ is the first reported case.
Introduction
What would it be like to live with a memory so powerful that
it dominates ones waking life? We present here the case of
AJ, a woman who told us:
My memory has ruled my life . It is like my sixth sense
There is no effort to it I want to know why I remember
everything. I think about the past all the time . Its like a
running movie that never stops. Its like a split screen. Ill be
talking to someone and seeing something else Like were
sitting here talking and Im talking to you and in my head Im
thinking about something that happened to me in December
1982, December 17, 1982, it was a Friday, I started to work at
Gs (a store) . Its all about dates . I just know these
things about dates . I used to spend all my time thinking
about dates . When I hear a date, I see it, the day, the
month, the year . I can go back over the years since 1980
on a date When I hear a date I see the day . I see it as I
saw it that day I get to a portion of the day so I can see
what day it was and whatever sticks out in my mind . .I
only have to experience something one time and I can be
totally scarred by it I cant let go of things because of my
memory Happy memories hold my head together I treasure these memories, good and bad . I cant let go of things
because of my memory, its part of me When I think of
these things it is kind of soothing I knew a long time ago I
36
standardized tests, and shared her personal experiences with
considerable openness. It became clear, early on, that her
memory was different from other cases of superior memory
reported in the literature. She was not gifted at encoding and
retrieving long strings of digits and numbers. Over and over
again, she told us that her memories are deeply personal, tied
to what she is interested in, closely linked to dates that are
within her personal experience and uncontrollable. So unique
is her presentation that we decided to let the subject herself
guide this inquiry.
In contrast to the vast literature on impaired memory and
the amnesic syndrome, relatively little is known about
forms of superior memory. Previously reported cases of
superior memory seem to have in common the ability to
perform memory feats with meaningless information such
as learning long displays of words or digits and repeating
them back. None were reported to have superior autobiographical memory or to be bothered by constant remembering of personal experiences. Luria (1987) wrote in detail
about the superior memory of S, who became a professional mnemonist and earned his living by amazing audiences with his ability to recall almost limitless amounts of
information. S was not bothered by an extraordinary autobiographical memory, indeed he was described as living his
personal life as in a haze (p. 159). Nor did S think of
himself as having an exceptional memory. When sent to
Luria for examination by the editor of the newspaper where
he was then working as a journalist, S was perplexed as to
why he had been singled out. He did not see his memory as
unusual or remarkable. Nor is there evidence of remarkable
autobiographical memory in VP, another case of superior
memory described by Hunt and Love (1972, 1983). VP was
a man who could play up to 60 games of correspondence
chess without notes, and who, by the age of five, had memorized the street map of Riga, his home town of 500,000
people. VP memorized Bartletts tale, War of the Ghosts
producing it nearly verbatim after two readings and a year
later without warning was able to recall the tale as he had
the year before. VP did not see his memory as above the
norm. In fact he told Hunt and Love that many of his fellow
classmates had better memories than his, as the educational
system in Riga placed such strong emphasis on rote learning. TE reported by Gordon et al. (1984) began studying
mnemonics at the age of 15 and could memorize eight
rows of six digits using complex strategies. In their pathbreaking book on superior memory, Wilding and Valentine
(1997) have written in depth about ten cases. The majority
of their cases had either performed or been spectators at the
World Memory Championships where people perform
feats such as learning and remembering long strings of
binary digits. Wilding and Valentine found that their ten
cases each had very different but highly domain-specific
forms of superior memory. For example, their Subject A
had worked as a telephone operator and was able to recall
nearly all the telephone exchange codes of the British
Isles in response to town names (p. 119). According to
E. S. Parker et al.
Wilding and Valentine (1997, p. 159), none of the ten cases,
despite superior ability on laboratory memory tasks, had
more than average ability to recall autobiographical detail
from their past.
The current view of superior rememberers studied to date
is that their skills are due to the application of strategies
acquired through practice, and not to innate abilities (see
Ericsson et al. 2004). AJ seemed to differ from this characterization as she told us she could not consciously apply
strategies to help her learn and retain new information. She
was not able to use her talents to memorize in school, telling
us she had great difficulty with rote memorization. She
repeatedly told us her memory was automatic, not strategic.
Luria studied, in considerable depth, the mnemonic strategies used by S, including visual images and his complex synesthetic constructions that allowed him to retain prodigious
amounts of information for years. Maguire et al. (2003)
asked their ten cases with superior memory about mnemonic
strategies they used to remember memory tasks during a
brain fMRI. All ten reported using mnemonics and nine
reported using the method of loci for some or all of the
tasks. The method of loci is attributed to the Greek poet,
Simonedes of Kos in 447 BC (Yates, 1966) and basically
consists of taking a mental walk through a familiar route,
attaching to-be-remembered items to places along the route
which in turn serve as retrieval cues during another mental
walk at retrieval. Lurias case S, with his rich capacity for
visual imagery, used a variation of the method of loci to
recall strings of 50 to 70 words and digits, which he could
recall both forward and backward depending on where he
started his mental walk at retrieval. Ericsson et al. (2004)
undertook extensive reevaluation of the famous Rajan
(Thompson et al. 1991), who had superior abilities to
remember digits and letters. They concluded that his memory skill was due to encoding techniques he had acquired
through extensive practice, and not to an innately superior
memory.
Lurias classical study of S influenced our approach to AJ.
Luria carefully described Ss memory skills and abilities in
the context of the total person, considering his memory skills
as part of and causally related to his overall psychological
makeup. Most psychological studies of superior rememberers
have focused on laboratory tasks to document subjects memory performance, with less attention to the cases broader
psychological makeup. Surprisingly little has been written
about how a superior memory relates to the persons inner
makeup or how it affects their everyday functioning. In addition, very little is known about neuropsychological domains
outside memory in cases of superior memory. General intellectual functioning, as measured by tests of IQ (e.g., Wilding
and Valentine, 1997; Maguire et al., 2003), has not been
found to correlate with superior memory. Interestingly, Luria
reported that S was viewed by people as disorganized and
dimwitted, and had trouble with abstraction. We therefore
undertook an extensive neuropsychological evaluation of AJ,
to examine her memory performance on standard clinical
37
Methods
Research data were collected through a protocol approved by
the University of California, Irvine IRB and informed written
consent was obtained from AJ. Much of the research methodology was guided by what AJ told us and we observed. We
interviewed her extensively about her history and her memory. We made video recordings to study her in depth. AJ was
never told what she would be asked about or tested on in
advance. She arrived at each visit completely nave about
dates and events that would be queried on that day. Her recall
of dates and events from her own life were noted for their
high reliability. As AJ kept a diary from the age of 10 to the
age of 34, a subset of her personal recollections was verified
with her diary entries. This verification occurred by asking
her to bring a box of her diaries and then checking in the diaries a subset of dates for which she had, in earlier meetings,
provided us an account of events. Her mother verified some
facts. Her ability to recall dates and newsworthy events was
tested from a book of news events over the past 100 years
(Lucas, 2000). Her ability to tell what day of the week was a
certain date was checked from calendars. Results are presented in a manner to protect AJs confidentiality and
privacy.
Formal neuropsychological testing was conducted by a
clinical neuropsychologist (ESP) according to standard clinical procedures and interpreted according to relevant norms.
Each test is described in Lezak (1995) except for tests from
the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System [(D-KEFS)
Delis et al. 2001]. Clinical tests of memory are designed to
measure memory deficits, not superior memory, and can suffer from ceiling effects. It is noted where AJs scores were at
the maximum possible. Memory tests included: global memory indices (Wechsler Memory Scale Revised (WMS-R));
memory tests requiring organizational strategies [word-list
recall (California Verbal Learning Test-Research Edition
(CVLT)]; and recall of complex visual information (ReyOsterrieth Complex Figure Test (Rey-O CFT)); memory tests
with strong external cues (Warringtons Word and Face Recognition Tests; paired associates from the WMS-R; recognition on the CVLT); working memory (Letter-Number
Sequencing subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
III (WAIS-III), maximum number of digits in the reverse
directions and maximum number of items in reverse visual
span from the WMS-R); semantic memory (Information subtest of WAIS-R); and autobiographical memory (Autobiographical Memory Index (AMI)).
To measure neuropsychological domains beyond memory,
the following domains and tests were selected: General intellectual functioning (Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleRevised (WAIS-R); lateralization (Edinburgh Handedness
Inventory, Grooved Pegboard Test and Finger Tapping Test);
Results
Background Information
Family History
AJ was born in 1965 after a normal pregnancy and delivery to
a family she describes as close and protective. She has one
younger sibling, a left-handed brother who is nearly four
years her junior. Her father is a businessman and her mother
works as an assistant in a medical office. The family is Jewish
with her fathers parents coming from Hungary and Russia,
and her mothers parents coming from Germany and Romania.
Family history is positive for depression, anxiety and ocular
migraines. According to AJ, several other members of her
family have good memories but nothing like hers with her
phenomenal memory for dates. These other relatives are a
paternal aunt, paternal cousin and paternal great uncle. She
was married for the first and only time at the age of 37.
Development of her Memory
AJ reports that her earliest memory is being in her crib,
around the age of 18 to 24 months, and frightened when
woken by her uncles dog. She says she can recall her
brothers birth when she was three years, nine months old.
According to AJ she had always had a richly detailed memory for episodes but there was a change in her memory when
at age eight her family moved from the east coast to the west.
She reports she had loved their life in the east and did not
want to move. She says she was traumatized by the move
and that after the move she started to organize her memories, making lists of friends from back east, looking at pictures of her house and thinking about the past a lot. She
states that after the move, her memories became clearer. In
1976 when she was ten years old she began keeping a diary.
38
She says that she first became aware of her detailed memory
in 1978 at age twelve. She told us the same story on several
different occasions. She recalls that she was studying with
her mother and started to drift off thinking about the year
before in school, which she had loved. It was then she says
she became aware that she was able to vividly recall the
details of the year before and exact dates. From 19741979,
ages eight to thirteen, she can remember many days but not
every day. She often has to think about things (for a few seconds). As the years went by, she remembered more and more
dates and events. She says that from 1980, age 14 onward,
her recall became automatic . give me the day and I see
it. I go back to the day and I just see the day and what I was
doing. Her mother reports that the family noticed AJs superior memory when she was in her twenties.
Diaries
From the age of 10 to the age of 34, AJ kept diaries, nearly
every day. Her diaries were various forms of scheduling calendars with small entry areas, some just one inch by one inch.
Some years, her entries were completely filled with writing
so micrographic that even AJ read them to us with great difficulty. Other years, her entries were less detailed, and more
readable, with 67 brief entries per day. She said that she was
obsessed with writing things down because things would
stay in her mind if she didnt write them down in her diary. It
made her feel better to have things written down. She said she
rarely went back to review them. These diaries provided a
resource for our verification of her recollections.
Educational History
Despite her remarkable memory, AJ reports that she never
excelled in school and says she hated it. She insists that she
must be interested in something in order to remember it. She
said her grades were mostly Cs with some Bs and an A here
and there. When asked why she didnt apply her great memory to school, she said It (meaning her memory) doesnt
work that way. I had to study hard. Im not a genius. She
reports she had trouble memorizing dates in history, arithmetic, foreign languages, sciences and got Ds in geometry.
She told us she received an A in algebra which came much
more easily to her. She said she had math tutors from second
grade to help her memorize math facts. She readily admits
that she hates authority to which she attributes her dislike
of school. Memorizing poetry was painfully difficult. Yet she
can recall with ease every one of her teachers since kindergarten. She did complete a bachelors degree in a social science, graduating at the age of 23.
Occupational History
AJ has had periods where she has been unemployed by
choice. Upon returning home from university, she worked for
a year in the entertainment field. This was followed by a
three-year period, when she was 25 to 27, where she did not
E. S. Parker et al.
work and lived at home with what she describes as an
extreme bout of depression. She then worked as an assistant
in a law office where her memory for events and dates was a
significant asset. After that, she worked as an executive assistant for the next six years until she married. AJs goal is to be
a wife and have a family, rather than having a career outside
the home. That is all she has wanted since she was three years
old. She is working on plans to start her own business that
involves selling a product she will make.
39
40
E. S. Parker et al.
Table 1. AJs recollections of every Easter from 19802003, produced November 21, 2003
AJs unexpected recall of all Easters since 1980. Next to each day, she also wrote a personal
event for that day. This list was produced within 10 minutes. There is one error and it is off
by two days. We have not found anyone who can find the error without resorting to a printed
calendar. Nor have we found anyone who can produce this list of dates.
Date
Personal entry
April 6,1980
9th Grade-Easter vacation ends
April 19,1981
10th Grade-new boyfriend, H
April 11,1982
11th Grade, Grandparents visiting for Passover
April 3,1983
12th Grade, just had second nose reconstruction
April 22, 1984
Freshman at (school), Cs (friend) parents visiting
April 7,1985
Just returned from a week in AZ, sick as a dog
March 30,1986
Parents in Palm Springs, W, G, A (friends) staying at house
April 17, 1987
B (friend) for Easter, vomit up carrots
April 3, 1988
personal
March 26, 1989
Bs (friend) for Easter
April 15, 1990
make cookies, S breaks up with me next day
March 31, 1991
R (friend) visiting, gets carded for cigs
April 19, 1992
Easter Dinner at T(friend) comes over
April 11, 1993
hang all day, spaghetti dinner with R
April 3, 1994
wake up at Hs house
April 16, 1995
rainy day, brunch with H (friend)
April 7, 1996
personal
March 30, 1997
dinner with J and C (friends)
April 12, 1998
house smells like ham, M (friend) over
April 4, 1999
hang, describes specific event at work
April 23, 2000
Las Vegas for weekend
April 15, 2001
personal
March 31, 2002
hang
April 20, 2003
hang with J (husband) and family
41
the week a particular date fell on if it falls within her memorized mental calendar. She readily admits she has trouble
with dates outside her calendar span. When asked what day
of the week was April 3, 1955 she told us she didnt know, as
she couldnt see it. She spent several minutes trying to figure this out but was uncertain of the answer.
When given a date within her mental calendar, she says I
see it. I dont see the whole day at one time. I get to a portion
of the day so I can see what day it was and whatever sticks
out in my mind. She places herself in the day or event and
associates to it. She reports that her facility with dates starts
around 1974, when she was eight, strengthens around 1978
when she is 12 and was phenomenally accurate from 1980 on
when she was fourteen. She enjoys dates, dating events and
going over them in her mind. AJ talks about dates in terms
consistent with the mental calendar as being part of her
semantic memory. When asked about her knowledge of dates
she says I just know it. She says she has no idea how her
knowledge of dates developed. All she can tell us is I have
always been about dates. I just know these things. This quality of knowing is characteristic of information in semantic
memory (Tulving, 1983, p. 49).
Table 2. Examples of AJs excellent memory for events and dates if within her areas and time
period of interest. Answers given below so reader can self-test
Name the day of the week and the significant event on this date
August 16, 1977; June 6, 1978; May 25, 1979; November 4, 1979 (book wrong date, AJ
correct); May 18, 1980; October 5, 1983; January 17, 1994; December 21, 1988; May 3, 1991;
May 4, 2001
Name the date for the event
Plane crash in San Diego? Who shot JR episode? Persian Gulf War begins?
Rodney King beating? OJ Simpson verdict? Bombing at Atlanta Olympics?
Death of Princess Diana? Concorde Crash? Election dates for G.W. Bush and Clinton?
Answers (events) AJ gave to dates:
8/16/77 Tuesday, Elvis died
6/6/78 Proposition 13 passed in CA
5/25/79 plane crash, Chicago
11/4/79 Iranian invasion of US Embassy
5/18/80 Sunday, Mt. St Helens erupted
10/5/83 Wednesday, bombing in Beirut, killed 300
1/17/94 Monday, Northridge earthquake
12/21/88 Lockerby plane crash
5/3/91 last episode of Dallas
5/4/01 Robert Blakes wife killed
Answers (dates) A.J. gave to events:
San Diego crash September 25, 1978
JR November 21, 1980
Gulf War Wednesday, January 16, 1991
Rodney King beating March 3, 1991
OJ Simpson verdict Tuesday, October 3, 1995
Atlanta bombing July 26, 1996
Princess Diana August 30 or 31, 1997 (depending on France or US)
Concorde July 25, 2000
Elections date G.W. Bush November 7, 2000, Clinton Nov 3, 1992 and November 5, 1996.
42
On four occasions we asked AJ to draw her mental calendars
and she drew virtually the same calendars each time and in the
same way. She drew one for years and another for months. Her
calendar for years was drawn from left to right and at 1970
changed orientation from top to bottom. She told us her demarcation of years is based on her internal schema that she cannot
explain. She said this is how I see it. 1965 is when I was born.
1963 is when everything changed for the world I am interested in all that even though I wasnt born yet. 1965 is when I
was born. 1960 I feel that with President Kennedy the world
changed, and these are significant things the way I see my own
life. 1974 is when we moved to California. She had no idea
why she turned at 1970 and simply repeated this was they way
she saw it, the way she had always seen it.
For months she first drew January in the 11 oclock position, then counterclockwise filled in the rest of the months.
She has no idea why she drew the months this way but
insisted again it was how she saw it and always had.
Observations on AJs Memory Weaknesses
In contrast to AJs strong autobiographical memory, and her
ability to recall dates and events, she is not a gifted memorizer. For example, she told us she has five keys on her keyring and can never recall which key is for what. She says she
has to make lists to help her remember. She reports always
having a terrible time with rote memorization for things such
as learning history dates and poetry. Although she describes
her mind like having a movie running, she is not recording
her world verbatim in its totality. One day after several hours
together, she was asked to close her eyes and tell what her
two interviewers were wearing. She was unable to do so.
After making a videotape of AJ, we brought her back a
month later and, without warning, interviewed her about specific events from the month before. When we asked her specific questions such as, had she talked about her second grade
teacher, or had she talked about the date April 27, 1994, she
told us she did not recall. We devised a recognition test to see
if recognition would be better than free recall. She was asked
about dates and events that had been covered during the
video (targets) mixed with ones that had not been discussed
on the video but had been at another time (distractors). She
was very uncertain when answering questions on the recognition test and had to be prompted to answer. She only recognized one of four target dates and she mistakenly thought that
three of the distractor dates might have been discussed on
that date. She had similar difficulty recognizing which events
had been discussed on the video. How paradoxical that someone with such a powerful autobiographical memory, and
extraordinary ability to recall dates and events, was unable to
recall or recognize the details of a videotaping from the
month before. Her autobiographical memory, while incredible, is also selective and even ordinary in some respects.
The neuropsychological test data reported next also demonstrate AJs strengths and weaknesses in different forms of
E. S. Parker et al.
memory, adding more evidence for the selectivity in her
superior memory.
43
122
128
Z-score
1.5
1.9
18/18
8/8
50/50
16/16
27/27
63/63
15
122
Comment
near ceiling
near ceiling
perfect
perfect
perfect
perfect
perfect
perfect
1.67
1.5
near ceiling
near ceiling
27/27
perfect
40/40
perfect
perfect
44
E. S. Parker et al.
Table 5. AJs deficits on neuropsychological tests
Deficits: Defined as performance more than 1.5 sd above/below average
Raw Score
Executive Function and Reasoning Tests
Concept Formation and Shifting from WCST
Perseverative Responses
Executive Functions from HCT
Analogical Reasoning from WAIS-R
Anterior Left Hemisphere Tests
Motor Speed, Right Dominant Hand
Dysnomia from Boston Naming
Organizationally-Demanding Memory Tests
Recall of Word-List from CVLT
Recall of Complex Figure, both delays
Face Memory Test
Face Recognition, Warrington Test
Z-score
Comment
38
78 errors
5
<2.0
2.3
1.67
impaired
impaired
impaired
36.5
51/60
1.6
2.7
impaired
impaired
>2.0
impaired
impaired
1.6
impaired
45
Manual Dexterity
Motor Speed
Language
Visual Spatial
Calculations
Visual-motor
Test
Score
Z = 0.93
Z = 1.0
Z = 0.0
Z = 0.6
Z = 0.3
Z = +1.3
Z = 0.8
Z = 0.6
Z = +0.5
Z = +0.5
Z = 0.6
Z = +0.1
Z = 0.0
Z = +0.2
Z = +0.7
Z = 0.0
Z = 0.33
Z = 1.33
Z = 0.33
Z = 0.33
Z = 1.3
35/36
Z = +0.2
Z = 0.6 eighth grade
Z = 0.0
Z = 0.33
Z = +1.0
never in depressed range
her superior Visual Memory Index (see Table 4) supporting her claims of vivid visual recollections. Semantic
memory as measured by the Information subtest of the
WAIS-R is entirely average, and measures an individuals
acquired knowledge of common facts. The measures of
executive function on which she performs in the normal
range listed on Table 6 differ from those areas of executive functioning where she exhibits impairments listed on
Table 5. The tests on which she is impaired are unstructured and require considerable abstraction, hypothesis
formulation and conceptual shifting. The tests on which
she is normal are tests of fluency, processing speed and
novelty, particularly Trails A and the Stroop Test. Other
areas of normal performance indicate that she has developed average reading, spelling, and arithmetic skills,
despite her complaints of difficulties in school. Her level
of self-reported depression on the Beck Depression Inventory was measured on multiple occasions and was never in
the clinically significant range.
Discussion
46
past. Her recollections were highly reliable and accurate
where verification was possible. She obtained a perfect score
on the Autobiographical Memory Test. We have documented
her claim that, when given a date, she can go back to that date
through the years, particularly from age 14 to the present, and
recall what she was doing on that date. Her recollections are
fast, seemingly automatic as she claims. Data include AJs
self-reports, our observations, documentation of her memory
with novel testing procedures developed specifically for AJ,
and standardized neuropsychological testing. Each approach
revealed different but complementary information that, taken
together, offers a profile of her phenomenal memory in the
context of other neuropsychological functions and how it
impacts her life. The following discussion weaves threads
from these typically separate approaches to offer the reader
hypotheses about AJs form of extraordinary memory.
First, it is important to address what several of our colleagues have suggested, namely, that AJ is simply pulling the
wool over our eyes and in fact there is nothing particularly
remarkable about her memory. How do we know that she
isnt rehearsing, practicing, and preparing in order to fool us
into believing her memory is extraordinary? The reader is
reminded that AJ never knew in advance the hundreds of
dates and events she was going to be asked by us, so she
would not know what to prepare. She had no idea we were
going to ask her about Easters from 1980 on before she was
asked to write them down. Nor did she know two years later
that she would be asked to do the same thing again, which she
did entirely accurately again and reliably. When she brought
a box with four years of her diaries, she had no idea which of
the 1,460 days we would be verifying. When we queried her
from the book of news events (Lucas, 2000), often we did not
know in advance which dates we were going to select. It is
also noteworthy that AJ herself told us what she could and
could not recall. Dates and events before 1980 but within her
lifespan were fuzzy but from 1980 on were crystal clear.
She could recall public events only if she was interested in
them, or if something important had happened to her on the
day they occurred. She told us what she could accurately
remember and what she did not remember. She repeatedly
told us she was a terrible memorizer. Her difficulties with
some types of remembering have been described on formal
tests such as the CVLT. Her neuropsychological testing provided no evidence of malingering or invalid performance and
she did have supernormal scores on a subset of standard
memory tests. If anyone can tell us how such a profile could
be faked and why, our position about the validity of AJs
presentation is open to modification.
We know of no other reported case of someone who recalls
personal memories over and over again, who is both the
warden and the prisoner of her memories, as AJ reports. We
took seriously what she told us about her memory. She is
dominated by her constant, uncontrollable remembering,
finds her remembering both soothing and burdensome, thinks
about the past all the time, lives as if she has in her mind a
running movie that never stops and has memories tied
E. S. Parker et al.
closely with dates. We relied on her own reports when it
came to describing her awareness of her memory and the
meaning of her memory to her life, as there are no objective
tests to measure this. Whereas previously reported cases of
superior memory have been described, they are of individuals
who are capable of encoding and reciting prodigious amounts
of new information, using practiced mnemonic strategies
(Hunt and Love, 1972; Luria, 1987; Gordon et al., 1984;
Thompson et al., 1991; Wilding and Valentine, 1997; Maguire
et al., 2003; Ericsson et al., 2004).
One way to conceptualize this phenomenon is to see AJ as
someone who spends a great deal of time remembering her
past and who cannot help but be stimulated by retrieval cues.
Normally people do not dwell on their past but they are oriented to the present, the here and now. Yet AJ is bound by
recollections of her past. As we have described, recollection
of one event from her past links to another and another, with
one memory cueing the retrieval of another in a seemingly
unstoppable manner. According to one theory, it takes a
special neurocognitive state to enable present stimuli to be
interpreted as such cues. Such a state is called episodic
retrieval mode and refers to the orientation of the subject as
she focuses on past happenings (Tulving 1983, 1999). Studies
using positron emission tomography have identified regions
in the right and left prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate
gyrus that are activated with the maintenance of episodic
retrieval mode (Lepage et al., 2000). AJ seems unable to turn
off episodic retrieval mode as in normal individuals. She has
trouble inhibiting her constant remembering of her personal
past at will. Inhibition is an important cognitive function that
has been associated with the right inferior frontal cortex
based on human lesion-mapping, leading to speculation the
same regions may be important for memory retrieval (Aron
et al., 2000).
This view of AJ is not sufficient to capture fully the nature
of her unique remembering. While she does spend a great
deal of time remembering from her personal past, as do some
older people, there is another remarkable feature which is her
rich repertoire of personal memories that is available and
accessible to her. It is quite possible for someone to be in a
state of remembering ones personal past without vivid, specific recollections as to what one was doing on a particular
date. Like us all, AJ has a rich storehouse of memories latent,
awaiting the right cues to invigorate them. The memories are
there, seemingly dormant, until the right cue brings them to
life. But unlike AJ, most of us would not be able to retrieve
what we were doing five years ago from this date. Given a
date, AJ somehow goes to the day, then what she was doing,
then what she was doing next, and left to her own style of
recalling, what she was doing next. Give her an opportunity
to recall one event and there is a spreading activation of recollection from one island of memory to the next. Her retrieval
mode is open, and her recollections are vast and specific.
There has been research on brain regions involved with episodic retrieval mode, but not on superabundant autobiographical memory as it has not been identified before.
47
AJ may have a variant of a neurodevelopmental, frontostriatal disorder putting her at risk for her hyperthymestic syndrome. Deficits in executive functioning and anomalous lateralization are both found in neurodevelopmental frontostriatal
disorders which include autism, obsessive-compulsive disorder
(OCD), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Tourettes syndrome and schizophrenia (Bradshaw and Sheppard, 2000). The
frontostriatal system (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, lateral orbitofronal cortex, cingulate, supplementary motor area and
associated basal ganglia structures) is vulnerable to neurodevelopmental disorders and this is consistent with AJs history
and presentation. For example, AJ reports that from an early age
she became upset when order in her external environment was
disturbed, a sign of early obsessive-compulsive tendencies.
There are signs of atypical developmental features before she
started to write diaries at the age of ten and become aware of her
detailed memory around the age of twelve and a half, in 1978.
With the exception of some autistic savants (OConner and Hermalin, 1989), superior memory abilities are not characteristic of
neurodevelopmental fronto-striatal disorders. And although AJ
is not autistic, nor do savants remember autobiographical information, there are certain similarities between them. Like autistic
savants, AJ had an interest in dates from an early age, has strong
domain-specific areas of knowledge, has repetitive and obsessive tendencies (Heavey et al., 1999) and has a highly variable
neuropsychological profile with areas of superiority co-existing
with areas of deficit (Winner, 2000). We suggest that it might be
fruitful to examine frontostriatal symptoms in other cases of
superior memory, as they too may have deficits in executive
functions associated with their unusual memory abilities.
The hypothesis that AJs superior memory may be caused
by atypical neurodevelopment is based on her unusual profile
of performance on neuropsychological tests taken in conjunction with hints from published research. It is, however, quite
possible that there is no causal relationship and that the overall parallels between her memory and her neuropsychological
weaknesses are simply correlative. Since there are no previous studies of superior rememberers that have simultaneously
examined neurocognitive domains outside memory, particularly frontal lobe functions, there are no comparative data at
this point. The findings from AJ are sufficiently compelling
to warrant further exploration of the relationships among
forms of superior memory and other cognitive domains in
future research.
AJs highly developed mental calendar provides her with a
structured, concrete framework to encode and retrieve information. It should be noted, however, while dates are linked to
her memories, they are not the only retrieval cues for personal recollections. We have described how she can recall
dates when given an event, and she can recall events when
given a date. Her recollection of dates and public events
was verified from independent sources. Her recollection of
personal events was verified where possible with her diaries.
When given a date, she can tell you what day of the week it
fell on. She told us she places herself in the date, recalling
where she was and what she was doing on it. When given the
48
same date on different occasions, she reliably told us the
same story as to what she recalls doing on that day.
Her mental calendar and tendency to encode and retrieve
information by dates can be viewed as a mnemonic strategy;
however, unlike previously reported cases of superior memory, AJ does not consciously rehearse the use of dates to
facilitate her memory, nor is she able to apply this or any
strategy to learn new information such as word lists. However, she does rehearse dates, but she says this occurs automatically and obsessively. Her calendar can be thought of as
a mnemonic that has become automatized with extensive use.
It is particularly interesting that AJ uses dates to organize
her memories as dates are typically very poor recall cues in
laboratory studies of normal subjects and days of the week
are poorly remembered (see Friedman, 1993).
AJ is not a calendrical calculator, the rare ability found in
people with autism who provide dates based on calculation
rather than memory (Heavey et al., 1999). For example, Horwitz et al. (1965) describe a calendrical calculator who could
provide the day of the week of a given date for a span of some
40,000 years. AJs knowledge of dates is constrained to a
period linked to her personal experience. It is, we suggest, part
of her semantic memory, providing a structured but somewhat
inflexible context for encoding and retrieving personal experiences. She told us over and over that dates are things she
knows, and that sense of knowing characterizes retrieval from
semantic memory. The retrieval of a date can then produce
retrieval of personal experiences from episodic memory, which
is characterized by a sense of remembering, exactly how she
describes such memories (Tulving, 1983; Gardiner et al., 2002).
Current theoretical accounts of autobiographical memory
distinguish between memories for personal facts or personal
knowledge and memories of specific personally experienced
events that are accompanied by rich sensory-perceptual recollections with a special awareness of remembering. According
to one theory, semantic autobiographical memory pertains to
general knowledge of the self and episodic autobiographical
memory pertains to recollections of a specific event with
reexperiencing of contextual details and a sense of awareness
of the self in ones past (Wheeler et al., 1997). A similar distinction has been made between event-specific sensory and
perceptual episodic memory and higher level knowledge of
self that covers a larger time period (Conway, 2001). According to such theoretical distinctions, AJs superiority seems
particularly striking for semantic or general autobiographical
memory. She has a developed self-knowledge particularly
surrounding dates and she just knows these things. Moreover, her episodic recollections are relatively sparse. For
example, she does dismally on word-list recall tasks which
are heavily dependent on episodic memory. She was unable
to recall the details of a video-taping episode from a month
earlier, a clear episodic or event-specific recollection. Yet
when shown the tape again, she was able to reproduce the
same recollection when she heard the same question again.
AJs hyperthymistic syndrome may be a case of highly superior semantic autobiographical memory.
E. S. Parker et al.
It is somewhat surprising that superior memory does not
necessarily facilitate other aspects of everyday life, and in
fact, in the case of AJ her memory was not helpful in school,
and causes her to spend much of her time recollecting the
past instead of orienting to the present and the future. Given
the considerable emphasis placed today on techniques to
facilitate memory skills, particularly with children, but also
with aging adults, it may be worth questioning just what
forms of superior memory benefit everyday functioning.
Who would expect that VP, who could play seven simultaneous chess games blindfolded, and had an estimated IQ of
136, would be employed as a store clerk (Hunt and Love,
1972)? How paradoxical that Lurias case S, who could recall
seemingly unlimited amounts of materials for years, had trouble capturing the meaning of what he read and moved from
one job to another, eventually becoming a professional
mnemonist. In addition, there was nothing consistently noteworthy about the occupations of the ten cases of superior
memory reported by Wilding and Valentine (1997), although
this was not a focus of their investigations. Occupations
included a telephone operator, mathematician working in a
financial institution, an airport cleaning supervisor, a journalist (the only female), business consultant, hypnotist/magician, student of English, nurse, memory improvement teacher
and a 13-year-old too young to be employed.
Just how unique is AJs memory, whether or not there are
other cases of hyperthymesia out there in the world, and
whether there are other forms of superior memory yet-to-be
described, are questions that must await further research for
answers. There is a large literature on patients with amnesic
syndromes that has enriched our understanding of how
memory can fail and the brain regions involved. By comparison, research on forms of superior memory is miniscule,
leaving much to be explored. We suggest that work with AJ
has demonstrated the importance of not only exploring her
memory as she describes it, but also looking at memory in
the broader context of other neuropsychological domains.
Previous research on cases of superior memory have focused
primarily on subjects performance on laboratory memory
tasks, but have yet to examine other neuropsychological
functions in depth. Answers to questions about the relationships between superior memory and other neurocognitive
functions can only be addressed by investigating these cases
of superior memory in the broader context of other neuropsychological functions.
References
Alexander MP, Stuss DT, Fansabedian N. California Verbal Learning Test:
performance by patients with focal frontal and non-frontal lesions.
Brain 2003; 126: 14931503.
Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience 2000; 8: 170177.
Bradshaw JL, Sheppard DM. The neurodevelopmental frontostriatal disorders: Evolutionary adaptiveness and anomalous lateralization. Brain
and Language 2000; 73: 297320.
49
Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment: Third edition. New York:
Oxford University Press, 1995.
Lucas S, editor. 20th Century day-by-day: 100 years of news. New York:
DK Publishing, 2000.
Luria AR. The mind of a mnemonist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 1987.
Maguire EA, Valentine ER, Wilding JM, Kapur N. Routes to remembering:
The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience 2003; 6:
9095.
Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.
Annual Reviews in Neuroscience 2001; 24: 1672002.
Milner B. Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting: The role of the
frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology 1963; 9: 100110.
OConner N, Hermalin B. The structure of the autistic idiot-savant mnemonists. British Journal of Psychology 1989; 80: 97111.
Stuss DT, Levine B. Adult clinical neuropsychology: Lessons from
studies of the frontal lobes. Annual Review in Psychology 2002; 53:
401433.
Stuss DT, Levine B, Alexander MP, Hong J, Palumbo C, Hamer J, Murphy
KJ, Izukawa D. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients
with focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38: 388402.
Thompson CP, Cowan TM, Frieman J, Mahadevan RS. Rajan: A study of a
memorist. Journal of Memory and Language 1991; 30: 702704.
Tulving E. Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University
Press, 1983.
Tulving E. On the uniqueness of episodic memory. In: LG Nilsson, HJ
Markowitsch, editors. Cognitive neuroscience of memory. Gottingen:
Hogrefe & Huber, 1999; 1142.
Wheeler MA, Stuss DT, Tulving E. Toward a theory of episodic memory:
The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychological Bulletin 1997; 121: 331354.
Wilding J, Valentine E. Superior memory. Hove, East Sussex: Psychology
Press, 1997.
Winner E. Giftedness: Current theory and research. Current Directions in
Pyshological Science 2000; 9: 153156.
Yates FA. The art of memory. London: Pimlico, 1966.