Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

NICU

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 11

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.

com

Open Access

Research

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants: relationship between breast feeding, early weight gain and neurodevelopment based on results from two cohorts, EPIPAGE and LIFT
,1,2,3,4 Dominique Darmaun,4 Clair-Yves Boquien,4 Jean-Christophe Roze 1,2,3 Cyril Flamant, Jean-Charles Picaud,5 Christophe Savagner,6 Olivier Claris,7 Alexandre Lapillonne,8 Delphine Mitanchez,9,10 Bernard Branger,3 Umberto Simeoni,11 Monique Kaminski,12 Pierre-Yves Ancel12

J-C, Darmaun To cite: Roze D, Boquien C-Y, et al. The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants: relationship between breast feeding, early weight gain and neurodevelopment based on results from two cohorts, EPIPAGE and LIFT. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/ bmjopen-2012-000834
< Prepublication history and

ABSTRACT Context: Supplementation of breast milk is difcult


once infants suckle the breast and is often discontinued at end of hospitalisation and after discharge. Thus, breastfed preterm infants are exposed to an increased risk of nutritional decit with a possible consequence on neurodevelopmental outcome. Objective: To assess the relationship between breast feeding at time of discharge, weight gain during hospitalisation and neurodevelopmental outcome. Design: Observational cohort study. Setting: Two large, independent population-based cohorts of very preterm infants: the Loire Infant Follow-up Team (LIFT) and the EPIPAGE cohorts. Patients: 2925 very preterm infants alive at discharge. Main outcome measure: Suboptimal neurodevelopmental outcome, dened as a score in the lower tercile, using Age and Stages Questionnaire at 2 years in LIFT and Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Test at 5 years in EPIPAGE. Two propensity scores for breast feeding at discharge, one for each cohort, were used to reduce bias. Results: Breast feeding at time of discharge concerned only 278/1733 (16%) infants in LIFT and 409/2163 (19%) infants in EPIPAGE cohort. Breast feeding is signicantly associated with an increased risk of losing one weight Z-score during hospitalisation (LIFT: n1463, adjusted odd ratio (aOR)2.51 (95% CI 1.87 to 3.36); EPIPAGE: n1417, aOR1.55 (95% CI 1.14 to 2.12)) and with a decreased risk for a suboptimal neurodevelopmental assessment (LIFT: n1463, aOR0.63 (95% CI 0.45 to 0.87); EPIPAGE: n1441, aOR0.65 (95% CI 0.47 to 0.89) and an increased chance of having a head circumference Z-score higher than 0.5 at 2 years in LIFT cohort (n1276, aOR1.43 (95% CI 1.02 to 2.02)) and at 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort (n1412, aOR1.47 (95% CI 1.10 to 1.95)).

ARTICLE SUMMARY Key messages


-

The observed better neurodevelopment in spite of suboptimal initial weight gain could be termed the apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants. Regardless of the mechanisms involved, the current data provide encouragement for the use of breast feeding in preterm infants. The same effect was observed in two large distinct cohorts. Observational study: despite the use of propensity score, a few potential confounder can remain.

Strengths and limitations of this study


-

additional table for this paper are available online. To view these les please visit the journal online (http://dx.doi. org/10.1136/ bmjopen-2012-000834). Received 16 January 2012 Accepted 1 March 2012 This nal article is available for use under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial 2.0 Licence; see http://bmjopen.bmj.com

Conclusions: The observed better neurodevelopment in spite of suboptimal initial weight gain could be termed the apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants. Regardless of the mechanisms involved, the current data provide encouragement for the use of breast feeding in preterm infants.

For numbered afliations see end of article. Correspondence to ; Dr Jean-Christophe Roze jcroze@chu-nantes.fr

INTRODUCTION Breast feeding is universally recommended for the feeding of term infants.1 Regarding very preterm infants, <32 weeks of gestation, exclusive breast feeding is a debated topic since supplementation is required to ensure optimal growth during initial hospitalisation but is difcult once the preterm infant can suckle the breast.2 1

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants Suboptimal nutrition with insufcient growth during hospitalisation in neonatal intensive care unit is associated with later cognitive dysfunction.3 4 In extremely preterm infants, growth velocity during hospitalisation exerts a signicant, and possibly independent, effect on neurodevelopment and growth outcomes at 18e22 months of corrected age.3 In preterm infants, follow-up studies showed that at 7 years of age, preterm infants fed standard formula demonstrated neurocognitive impairment with a signicant reduction in IQ compared with infants fed with enriched formula.4 Breast feeding with supplementation during initial hospitalisation improves cognitive outcome at 30 months of corrected age in extremely preterm infants.5 Human milk indeed requires nutrient fortication to meet the protein and mineral needs of the rapidly growing preterm infant.6 During hospitalisation, as the baby receives mothers milk through a gastric tube, it is easy to use a milk fortier to maintain adequate growth. Once the preterm infant can suckle the breast, however, the use of a milk fortier is not easy, as it disrupts the routine of breast feeding. As a consequence, mother milk supplementation is often discontinued at the end of hospitalisation and hospital discharge, and this discontinuation exposes the infants to an increased risk of nutritional decit.7 Thus, breast feeding at time of discharge could be associated with less weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation and during the weeks following discharge. This is why exclusive breast feeding remains a matter of debate in preterm infants. The aim of the current study was to assess the complex relationship between breast feeding at time of discharge, weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation and neurodevelopment at 2 or 5 years using data from two independent large cohorts of very preterm infants of <33 weeks of gestation covering the late 1990s and mid2000s. The secondary objective was assessment of growth (weight, height, head circumference) at 2 and 5 years. METHODS Data source and patients Date source is constituted of two cohorts: EPIPAGE1 and LIFT (Loire Infant Follow-up Team)8 cohorts with recruitment over two distinct periods. EPIPAGE is a prospective population-based cohort study including all infants born between 22 and 32 weeks of gestation in 1997 in the maternity wards of nine French regions accounting for about one-third of all births in France. Among infants born in 1997, who survived and eligible for the follow-up (n2282), we included all infants whose status regarding breast feeding at time of discharge was known (n2163). LIFT cohort is a cohort of infants born in one region (Pays de la Loire, a region in Western France) and enrolled in the regional followup network. Among surviving very preterm infants with a gestational age <33 weeks of gestation, born between 1 January 2003 and 30 June 2008 and enrolled in LIFT cohort (n1857), we included all children whose status regarding breast feeding at time of discharge was known (n1733) (gure 1). Each cohort was registered to the French CNIL. For EPIPAGE cohort, parents were told about the study and given written information in the maternity or neonatal unit, and verbal consent was provided to the medical team in charge of the study at recruitment. For LIFT cohort, a written consent was obtained at enrolment. Developmental assessment In the EPIPAGE cohort, the neuropsychological assessment was performed using the Kaufman Assessment

Figure 1

Flow charts.

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants Battery for Children (K-ABC) at 5 years of age.9 Neuropsychological assessment was performed by trained psychologists when appropriate for the patients condition and when accepted by the patient. The K-ABC yields four global test scores. The Mental Processing Composite (MPC) Scale, which is considered to be equivalent to IQ, is a global measure of cognitive ability. This scale is standardised to a mean of 100 (SD 15). An MPC in the lower tercile (score of <85) was considered as an index of suboptimal neurodevelopment. In the LIFT cohort, neurodevelopmental assessment was performed using Age and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ),10 a questionnaire completed by parents at a corrected age of 2 years. This questionnaire includes ve parts, among which three are related to cognitive development: communication, problem solving and personal social interactions. The sum of the ve partial scores was calculated, and subject population was split in three terciles of global ASQ score. Being in the lower tercile (ASQ score <220) was considered an index of suboptimal neurodevelopment. Growth assessment To assess growth, we used measurements performed at birth, discharge and 6 months, 2 years and 5 years of age in EPIPAGE cohort and birth, discharge, 9 months and 2 years in LIFT cohort. We calculated Z-score by using LMS method.10a We used reference growth curves for which LMS parameters have been published for weight, height and head circumference. For birth and discharge (up to 41 weeks of postmenstrual age) measurements, we used Olsens preterm infant growth chart.11 For the few preterm infants discharged after 41 weeks of postmenstrual age and for follow-up period measurements, we used WHO growth curves.12 Weight gain during hospitalisation was assessed as the difference of weight Z-score between discharge and birth. Statistical analysis Means and SDs are reported for continuous variables and frequencies for categorical variables. ANOVA and c2 or Fisher test if necessary were used to compare infant characteristics and 5-year outcome between the groups of infants who were breast fed or formula fed at time of discharge. The propensity score method was used to reduce bias in assessing the relation between breast feeding at discharge and cognitive outcomes.13 14 The propensity score is dened as a conditional probability, between 0 and 1, that a subject will be breast fed at discharge based on an observed group of covariates. This score is then used just as if it were the only confounding covariate. Thus, the collection of predictors is collapsed into a single predictor, which may better adjust covariates between the groups and reduce bias. Two full nonparsimonious logistic regression models were developed to derive a propensity score for breast feeding at discharge, one for each cohort. These models included true confounders: variables that are potentially associated with both mode of feeding and with outcome.15 16 These variables included for the EPIPAGE cohort: characteristics of the mothers (age, maternal body mass index,17 level of maternal education, socioeconomic status, number of children at home), characteristics of the newborns (gestational age, Z-score of birth weight, presence of a congenital malformation), characteristics of pregnancy (antenatal corticosteroids, multiple pregnancy, complication of pregnancy), place of birth (inborn/outborn, region of birth) and characteristics of neonatal hospitalisation (cranial ultrasound abnormalities, patent ductus arteriosus, necrotising enterocolitis, neonatal surgery, bronchopulmonary dysplasia dened by supplemental oxygen requirement at 36 weeks, duration of mechanical ventilation, length of hospital stay). Patients with missing data were excluded from multivariable analysis. For the LIFT cohort, the variables included in propensity score calculation were similar but less numerous, including characteristics of the mother, characteristics of the newborn and neonatal hospitalisation. The HosmereLemeshow goodness-of-t test and the area under the curve were used to assess each model t. First, we studied crude associations between breast feeding at discharge and suboptimal neurodevelopment assessment and then the same associations after adjustment for the propensity score, gestational age and birthweight Z-score. Due to the known relationship between gestational age, weight gain and suboptimal development, these variables were included as confounding factors in the multivariable model even if they are already included in the calculation of propensity score, as suggested by other authors.14 We used logistic regression models for univariate and multivariate analyses.16 We estimated the crude and adjusted OR, and its 95% CI, of a suboptimal neurodelopmental score, that is, to be in the lower tercile of MPC (<85) at 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort or ASQ (< 220) at 2 years corrected age in LIFT cohort, associated with breast feeding at discharge. Second, we studied crude associations between breast feeding and postnatal weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation and thereafter up to 2 years in LIFT cohort and 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort. Finally, we studied the relationship between weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation and suboptimal neurodevelopment assessment. Moreover, we assessed growth (weight, height, head circumference) at 6 months, 2 and 5 years in EPIPAGE and at 9 months and 2 years in LIFT cohort. A supplementary propensitydscore matching analysis was performed comparing matched pairs of breast fed and not breast fed very preterm infants in each cohort. Matched pairs were created by their propensity score, and the outcomes of the two groups were compared in each cohort. All p values were based on two-sided tests. All analyses were performed using SPSS V.15.0 (SPSS Inc.). 3

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants RESULTS Patients characteristics The populations enrolled in EPIPAGE (n2163) and LIFT cohorts (n1733) were very similar with a small albeit signicant difference in gestational age (29.962.0 vs 29.862.1 weeks, p0.01), birth weight (13806395 vs 13406396 g, p0.01) and postnatal weight gain (0.9860.79 vs 0.8360.82 loss of weight Zscore, p0.001) but not in length of hospital stay (60.2631.7 vs 58.7632.2 days, p0.13). The proportion of breastfed infants was a little bit higher in EPIPAGE cohort (19%) than in LIFT cohort (16%), p0.02. In both cohorts, breast feeding was associated with some characteristics of the mothers, pregnancy, newborns and neonatal hospitalisation (table 1). Among the 2163 children of EPIPAGE cohort, 1753 (81%) were followed up to 5 years and 1462 (68%) assessed by trained psychologists. Among the 1733 of LIFT cohort, 1587 (85%) were followed up to 2 years of corrected age and we obtained ASQ for 1463 children (79%). Propensity score was calculated in each cohort. In EPIPAGE cohort, it was possible to calculate the propensity score for 2130 of the 2163 very preterm infants who were alive at hospital discharge and whose status regarding breast feeding at time of discharge was known. The propensity scores ranged from 0.0005 to 0.722. The HosmereLemeshow test was 7.2, p0.51. The receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve area was 0.7260.01. In LIFT cohort, the propensity score was calculated for all preterm infants. For each variable, if necessary, a subgroup with unknown data was constituted. The propensity scores ranged from 0.007 to 0.747. The HosmereLemeshow test was 6.2, p0.63. The ROC curve area was 0.7160.01. Variables signicantly associated with breast feeding at discharge were very similar in both cohorts (table 2). Exposure to breast feeding at time of discharge and neurodevelopmental outcome In both cohorts, breast feeding was associated with a signicant reduction of risk for a suboptimal

Table 1 Demographic and clinical characteristics of very low gestational age infants breast fed or not at time of discharge EPIPAGE cohort Characteristics Characteristics of newborns Gestational age, weight Birth weight, g Birth weight, Z-score Male gender Characteristics of mothers Age <25 years Two or more children at home Single Upper level education No professional activity Health insurance for low nancial income Upper socio-demographic level Characteristics of pregnancy Hypertension during pregnancy Multiple pregnancy Neonatal hospitalisation Duration of mechanical ventilation, days Length of hospital stay, days Change in weight Z-score during neonatal hospitalisation Follow-up and outcome Lost to follow-up Follow-up Incomplete neurodevelopmental evaluation Normal neurodevelopment Non-optimal neurodevelopment Breast fed (n[409) 30.4 1460 0.23 212 49 57 18 178 38 e 211 (51.6) 99 (24.2) 101 (24.7) 3.1 (7.5) 52.5 (24.1) 1.00 (0.63) (1.8) (400) (1.15) (51.8) (12.0) (13.9) (4.4) (43.5) (9.3) e 527 (30.0) 381 (21.7) 572 (32.6) 5.6 (11.5) 62.2 (32.6) 0.95 (0.63) 0.001 0.276 0.002 0.001 0.001 0.170 Not breast fed (n[1754) 29.8 1360 0.28 952 372 349 157 430 338 (2.1) (390) (1.05) (54.3) (21.2) (19.9) (9.0) (24.5) (19.3) p Value 0.001 0.001 0.26 0.36 0.001 0.005 0.002 0.001 0.001 LIFT cohort Breast fed (n[278) 30.3 1430 0.26 155 e 121 (43.5) 20 (7.2) e 52 (18.7) 12 (4.3) 135 (48.6) 31 (11.2) 61 (21.9) 4.0 (10.4) 54.2 (29.2) 1.02 (0.60) e 451 (31.0) 146 (10.0) 385 (26.5) 253 (17.4) 415 (28.5) 6.8 (11.2) 59.5 (32.7) 0.80 (0.70) 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.010 0.024 0.002 0.013 0.001 (1.8) (280) (0.81) (55.8) Not breast fed (n[1455) 29.8 1330 0.34 788 e 675 (46.3) 150 (10.3) 0.013 0.012 (2.1) (400) (0.81) (54.2) p Value 0.001 0.001 0.120 0.62

49 (12.0) 360 (88.0) 44 (12.2) 252 (70.0) 64 (17.8)

361 (20.6) 1393 (79.4) 247 (17.7) 725 (52.0) 421 (30.2)

0.001 0.001

22 (7.9) 256 (92.1) 14 (5.5) 183 (71.5) 59 (23.0)

124 (8.5) 1331 (91.5) 110 (8.3) 761 (57.2) 460 (34.5)

0.738 0.001

Bold values represent the denominator in each group to calculate percentage of each outcome. Data are means (SD) or n (%). Non-optimal neurodevelopment was dened as Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Mental Composite Processing <85 at 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort and Age and Stages Questionnaires score <220 at 2 years of corrected age in LIFT cohort. LIFT, Loire Infant Follow-up Team.

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants


Table 2 Signicant associations between the variables included in calculation of propensity score and breast feeding EPIPAGE cohort (n[2130) aOR (95% CI) Characteristics of the mothers Age <25 years Upper socio-demographic level Higher education No professional activity Mother of foreign origin Characteristics of pregnancy Multiple pregnancy Characteristics of the newborns Duration of neonatal hospitalisation (by week) Gestational age (per week) Birth weight (per Kg) 0.613 (0.42 to 0.89) 1.77 (1.34 to 2.34) 1.557 (1.17 to 2.06) 0.669 (0.44 to 1.00) 1.50 (1.05 to 2.14) 0.51 (0.39 to 0.67) 0.89 (0.84 to 0.94) 1.03 (0.92 to 1.16) 1.17 ( 0.66 to 2.08) p Value 0.010 0.001 0.002 0.052 0.024 0.001 0.001 0.650 0.590 LIFT cohort (n[1733) aOR (95% CI) p Value

e 2.32 (1.708 to 3.17) e 0.57 (0.35 to 0.75) e 0.69 (0.48 to 0.99) 0.91 (0.85 to 0.97) 0.98 (0.86 to 1.10) 1.13 (0.701 to 1.83)

e 0.001 e 0.001 e 0.047 0.004 0.714 0.612

aOR, adjusted odd ratio; LIFT, Loire Infant Follow-up Team.

neurodevelopmental assessment at 2 years of corrected age (LIFT cohort) and at 5 years (table 3). Moreover, The K-ABC Mental Processing Composite score increased as a function of the corrected age at which infants were weaned off breast feeding (gure 2). In each cohort, breast feeding was consistently associated with a reduction in the risk for suboptimal neurodevelopmental assessment before and after adjustment for gestational age, birthweight Z-score and sex, and propensity score (table 3). We observed the same results in propensitydscore matching analysis (supplemental table). Weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation and exposure to breast feeding Breast feeding was associated with an increased risk of losing one weight Z-score during hospitalisation, before adjustment in LIFT cohort and after adjustment in both cohorts (table 4). The loss in weight Z-score during the NICU hospitalisation was similar in both cohorts in breastfed infants but not in non-breastfed infants: the loss was less pronounced in the most recent cohort (table 1). We observed a greater loss in weight Z-score in breastfed group in propensitydscore matching analysis and this for each cohort (supplemental table).

Weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation, exposure to breast feeding and neurodevelopmental outcome Restricted intrauterine growth (ie, lower birthweight Z-score) was associated before and after adjustment for gestational age and sex with a suboptimal neurodevelopmental assessment in both cohorts. Postnatal weight gain during neonatal hospitalisation was signicantly associated with a suboptimal neurodevelopmental assessment only in LIFT cohort but not in EPIPAGE cohort (table 5), with and without adjustment for gestational age, birthweight Z-score, sex, breast feeding and propensity score. Growth after neonatal hospitalisation In contrast with data on initial growth, from 2 years of corrected age, weight, height and head circumference were signicantly higher in preterm infants who had been breast fed at the time of discharge in both LIFT and EPIPAGE cohorts (gure 3). After adjustment for weight Z-score at birth, sex and propensity score, breast feeding at discharge was signicantly associated to an increased chance of having a head circumference Z-score higher than 0.5 at 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort (n1412, adjusted odd ratio (aOR)1.47 (95% CI 1.10 to 1.95)) and at 2 years of corrected age in LIFT cohort (n1276, aOR1.43 (95% CI 1.02 to 2.02)).

Table 3 Association between breast feeding at time of discharge and non-optimal neurodevelopmental performance EPIPAGE cohort (n[1462) Adjustment No adjustment Adjusted for gestational age, birthweight Z-score and sex Adjusted for gestational age, birthweight Z-score, sex and propensity score* OR (95% CI) 0.44 (0.33 to 0.60) 0.46 (0.34 to 0.62) 0.65 (0.47 to 0.89) p Value 0.001 0.001 0.008 LIFT cohort (n[1463) OR (95% CI) 0.53 (0.39 to 0.73) 0.57 (0.41 to 0.78) 0.63 (0.45 to 0.87) p Value 0.001 0.001 0.005

Non-optimal neurodevelopment was dened as Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Mental Composite Processing <85 at 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort and Age and Stages Questionnaires score <220 at 2 years of corrected age in LIFT cohort. *n1443. LIFT, Loire Infant Follow-up Team. J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants during hospitalisation is a poor predictor of the quality of growth, as it does not provide any insight into the changes in body composition. In infants with a very low birth weight, enhanced postnatal growth is also associated with a better later neurodevelopmental outcome,18 especially regarding postnatal growth in head circumference, an index of brain growth.19 20 Yet the neuroprotective effects of improved growth may only be mild and mainly concern growth over the rst few weeks of hospital stay.21 In the two cohorts used in the current analysis, the role of postnatal weight gain was slightly different. In the EPIPAGE cohort, we did not observe any association between postnatal weight gain during hospitalisation and suboptimal neurodevelopmental score at 5 years of age (table 5). In the LIFT cohort, after adjustment for propensity score, a signicant association was found between initial weight gain rate and neurodevelopment. In contrast, in both cohorts, a negative association was obvious between birthweight Z-score and suboptimal neurodevelopment (table 5). Such difference between the two cohorts could be in relation with the signicantly better initial neonatal weight gain rate in the LIFT cohort compared with the EPIPAGE cohort, as a matter of fact, the loss in weight Z-score was less in the most recent cohort, presumably due to improvement in care routines between the two periods (1997 vs 2003) in the NICUs in France.22 Thanks to the improvement in nutritional management in the second period, the role of postnatal nutrition may have become detectable in the more recent LIFT cohort before and after adjustment for breast feeding. Benecial effects of breast feeding on cognitive skills and behavioural scores have been demonstrated previously in term,23 24 preterm25 and extremely preterm infants.5 Multiple biases may, however, interfere, particularly maternal socioeconomic and educational status: in the two cohorts, upper social status was obviously associated with a higher chance for being breast fed at discharge and for an optimal neurodevelopmental score. In term infants, after control for biases26dparticularly using the method of sibling comparison which automatically controls for any confounding factors that are the same for each of the siblings in a pair27dthe role of breast feeding has been found to be either not signicant or modest. In preterm infants, observational studies

Figure 2 The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) Mental Processing Composite score (mean, SD) at 5 years as a function of breastfeeding status at time of discharge and corrected age (*) at which infants were weaned off breast feeding, in EPIPAGE cohort. zp adjusted for propensity score.

DISCUSSION In two independent cohorts of preterm infants, breast feeding at discharge was associated with a reduction in the risk for a suboptimal neurodevelopmental assessment at 2 (LIFT cohort) or 5 years of age (EPIPAGE cohort) despite a higher risk for suboptimal weight gain (loss of one weight Z-Score) during neonatal hospitalisation in these breastfed infants. The observed improved neurodevelopment in spite of suboptimal initial weight gain could be termed the apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants. Such an observation is indeed a paradox as several earlier studies have documented an association between suboptimal early postnatal nutrition with insufcient weight gain during hospital stay and later cognitive dysfunction. In extremely preterm infants, faster weight gain in the NICU was associated with improved outcome in terms of neurodevelopment and growth at 18e22 months of corrected age.3 Nevertheless, this paradox is probably only an apparent paradox because weight gain

Table 4 Association between breast feeding at time of discharge and slow postnatal growth dened as a loss of one Z-score unit for weight during neonatal hospitalisation EPIPAGE cohort (n[1430) Adjustment No adjustment Adjusted for gestational age, sex, birthweight Z-score Adjusted for gestational age, sex, birthweight Z-score and propensity score*
*n1417. LIFT, Loire Infant Follow-up Team.

LIFT cohort (n[1463) OR (95% CI) 1.92 (1.49 to 2.49) 2.66 (1.99 to 3.52) 2.51 (1.87 to 3.36) p Value 0.001 0.001 0.001

OR (95% CI) 1.12 (0.87 to 1.43) 1.32 (0.98 to 1.76) 1.55 (1.14 to 2.12)

p Value 0.38 0.07 0.006

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants


Table 5 Association between birth weight or postnatal growth and non-optimal neurodevelopment EPIPAGE cohort OR (95% CI) Birth weight (per one Z-score unit below the mean) No adjustment Adjusted for gestational age, sex Postnatal growth for weight (per one Z-score unit lost between birth and discharge) No adjustment Adjusted for gestational age, sex, birthweight Z-score Adjusted for gestational age, sex, birthweight Z-score, breast feeding and propensity score* n1460 1.19 (1.04 to 1.35) 1.23 (1.07 to 1.40) n1430 0.93 (0.78 to 1.11) 0.97 (0.79 to 1.19) 1.06 (0.85 to 1.31) p Value 0.01 0.003 LIFT cohort OR (95% CI) n1463 1.12 (0.99 to 1.28) 1.24 (1.08 to 1.43) n1463 1.29 (1.10 to 1.51) 1.21 (1.01 to 1.44) 1.28 (1.07 to 1.53) p Value 0.08 0.001

0.44 0.74 0.63

0.002 0.038 0.008

Non-optimal neurodevelopment was dened as Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children Mental Composite Processing <85 at 5 years in EPIPAGE cohort and Age and Stages Questionnaires score <220 at 2 years of corrected age in LIFT cohort. *n1417. LIFT, Loire Infant Follow-up Team.

have shown that preterm infants whose mothers chose to breast feed reached a higher IQ at 8 years of age. Interestingly, infants whose mothers were willing to breast feed but failed to do so had the same IQ as those whose mothers elected not to breast feed.25 In preterm infants, observational studies found feeding with human milk to be associated with a better outcome.5 Eight randomised studies were included in a Cochrane review.28 Only two old randomised studies using unfortied donor human milk reported a comparison of developmental outcome between unfortied human milk and formula feeding. Follow-up of the infants who participated in these two trials did not nd a signicant effect on long-term growth parameters or neurodevelopmental outcomes. Most of other parameters reported by the randomised studies comparing formula to human milk were in favour of breastmilk, except for weight gain during hospitalisation. Moreover, among children who had been enrolled in one of such studies, Lucas et al observed 15 years later that breastmilk consumption was associated with a lower blood pressure in children born prematurely, suggesting a long-term benecial effects of breastmilk in preterm infants.29 Taken together, data from the literature and from the current study are consistent with a benet of breastmilk consumption on developmental outcome. At the time of discharge, very preterm infants have accumulated decits in energy, protein and minerals, and, due to their early discharge, still have higher nutrient requirements than healthy appropriate-forgestational age term infants. Despite the vast body of published recommendations on the nutrition of preterm infants, there is little data on optimal nutritional management after discharge of breasfed preterm infants.30 No randomised studies can be ethically performed to determine whether feeding preterm infants following hospital discharge with nutrientenriched formula milk versus human breast milk affects growth and development. The conclusion of a Cochrane review2 is that mothers who wish to breast feed, and their healthcare advisors, would require very clear

evidence that feeding with a nutrient-enriched formula milk had major advantages for their infants before electing not to feed (or to reduce feeding) with maternal breast milk, and that evidence from trials that compared feeding preterm infants following hospital discharge with nutrient-enriched versus standard formula milk demonstrating an effect on growth or development. The need to contribute an answer to the nagging disturbing question, Does exclusive breastfeeding at the time of discharge inuence outcome? was the very incentive to perform the current study. Our analysis of two large cohorts of preterm infant with relatively longterm follow-up used the propensity score method as a means to control as much as possible for potential confounders. From such analysis, we obtained clear-cut evidence that despite an increased risk for suboptimal early weight gain (increased risk for the loss of one weight Z-score) during neonatal hospitalisation, breast feeding at discharge is associated with a better outcome after adjustment for potential confounders by using propensity score. The main weakness of our study is the observational design. Despite the use of propensity score, we cannot ensure all potential confounders were eliminated. For instance, the very ability of an infant to suckle may be associated with a less sick infant, and the variables used in adjustment may not be entirely independent either of each other or of the outcome. Moreover, how much fortier was given could not, unfortunately, be retrieved from either of the two databases, and thus, some important variable such as caloric intake during neonatal intensive care unit stay could not be included in the analysis. Similarly, we did not address the mechanisms. For instance, the effect of breast feeding for term infants has been attributed to bias,31 including the complex relationship between weight gain and timing of weaning. Such bias is unlikely to be relevant for our population since the preterm infants with the slower weight gain rate were those who were not weaned at discharge. The slower weight gain during neonatal intensive care unit stay therefore likely reects 7

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants


Figure 3 Weight, head circumference and height expressed as Z-score at discharge, 6 or 9 months and 2 years of corrected age (EPIPAGE and Loire Infant Follow-up Team (LIFT) cohorts) and at 5 years (EPIPAGE cohort) according to breast feeding at discharge. Weight measurements was known for 1460, 1430, 973, 873 and 1447 infants at birth, discharge, 6 months, 2 and 5 years, respectively, in EPIPAGE and for 1463, 1463, 1341 and 1297 infants at birth, discharge, 9 months and 2 years, respectively, in LIFT cohort.

a biological effect rather than bias. Several potential mechanisms can be proposed to explain the better outcome in breastfed infants: motherechild interaction (better bonding, better care given by parents), the effect of specic nutrients contained in breastmilk such as polyunsaturated fatty acids, prebiotic oligosaccharides, etc. Regardless of mechanisms, we did observe the same fact in two distinct cohorts, with the same magnitude. By adjustment for propensity score, sex and birthweight Z-score, we were indeed able to observe the apparent paradox of a better neurodevelopmental outcome, despite a lower early weight gain. These adjustments are necessary because a complex relationship exists between breast feeding, birth weight, gestational age, birthweight Z-score and postnatal weight gain during hospitalisation. In conclusion, the neurodevelopment of premature infants is likely to benet from feeding supplemented mother milk during hospital stay and unsupplemented mothers milk after discharge, and these data from two cohorts are indeed reassuring because in spite of a lesser 8

weight gain during hospitalisation, we observed a better neurodevelopmental outcome in the breastfed groups. The present report suggests that breast feeding should be recommended at the time of discharge. As the rate of exclusive breast feeding at time of discharge is lowd<30% in Europe32dstrategies to facilitate breast feeding at discharge must be developed.33 Moreover, when adjusted on breast feeding, postnatal weight gain has a positive effect on neurodevelopmental outcome as observed in LIFT cohort; so the question about the putative benet of human milk supplementation after discharge remains open. Supplementation must be continued as long as possible, according to the state of knowledge, and this suggests also that more research is warranted about human milk composition and the potential benet of human milk supplementation at time of discharge is warranted in the future.
Author afliations 1 Department of Neonatal Medicine, Nantes University, CHU of Nantes, Nantes, France

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants


Centre dInvestigation Clinique INSERM CIC004, CHU de Nantes, Nantes, France 3 INRA UMR 1280, Physiologie des Adaptations Nutritionnelles, Nantes University, Nantes, France 4 Loire Infant Follow-up Team (LIFT) Network, Ppays de Loire, France 5 Department of Neonatal Medicine, Hopital de la Croix-Rousse, CHU de Lyon, Lyon, France 6 Department of Neonatal Medicine, CHU of Angers, Angers, France 7 pital Edouard Herriot, Lyon, France Department of Neonatal Medicine, Ho 8 pitaux de Paris, Necker Paris Descartes University, Assistance Publique-Ho hospital, Paris, France 9 pitaux de Paris, Re animation Ne onatale et Pe diatrique, Assistance Publique-Ho pital Trousseau, Paris, France Ho 10 Paris, Paris, France UPMC, Universite 11 Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, La Conception University Hospital, Marseille, France 12 INSERM, UMR 953, Epidemiological Research in Perinatal Health and pital Tenon, Paris, France Womens and Children Health, Ho
2

Contributors J-CR, DD, MK and P-YA conceived and designed the experiments. J-CR, DD, MK, P-YA and C-YB analysed the data. J-CR, DD and P-YA wrote the paper. J-CR, CF, J-CP, CS, DM, BB and US acquired data. J-CR, DD, J-CP, AL, OC, DM, US, CB, FF, CS, BD, MK and PA revised and critical reviewed the manuscript. Funding EPIPAGE cohort was supported by grants from INSERM (French National Institute of Health and Medical Research), the Directorate General for Health of the Ministry for Social Affairs. Lift cohort was supported by grants from Regional Health Agency of Pays de la Loire. No support from any organisation for the submitted work, no nancial relationships with any organisations that might have an interest in the submitted work in the previous 3 years and no other relationships or activities that could appear to have inuenced the submitted work. Competing interests None. Patient consent Obtained. Ethics approval Ethics approval was provided by the French CNIL. Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed. Data sharing statement Data set is available by emailing P-YA pierreyves. ancel@inserm.fr for EPIPAGE cohort and jcroze@chu-nantes.fr for LIFT cohort.

REFERENCES
1. 2. 3. Fewtrell MS, Morgan JB, Duggan C, et al. Optimal duration of exclusive breastfeeding: what is the evidence to support current recommendations? Am J Clin Nutr 2007;85(Suppl):635e8. Henderson G, Fahey T, McGuire W. Nutrient-enriched formula milk versus human breast milk for preterm infants following hospital discharge. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2007;(4):CD004862. Ehrenkranz RA, Dusick AM, Vohr BR, et al. Growth in the neonatal intensive care unit inuences neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics 2006;117: 1253e61. Lucas A, Morely R, Cole TJ. Randomised trial of early diet in preterm babies and later intelligence quotient. BMJ 1998;317:1481e7. Vohr BR, Poindexter BB, Dusick AM, et al. Persistent benecial effects of breast milk ingested in the neonatal intensive care unit on outcomes of extremely low birth weight infants at 30 months of age. Pediatrics 2007;120:e953e9. Schanler RJ, Shulman RJ, Lau C. Feeding strategies for premature infants: benecial outcomes of feeding fortied human milk versus preterm formula. Pediatrics 1999;103:1150e7. Greer FR. Post-discharge nutrition: what does the evidence support? Semin Perinatol 2007;31:89e95. JC, Bureau-Rouger V, Beucher A, et al; Follow-up network for Roze newborns at risk for handicap in a French region. Arch Pediatr 2007;14(Suppl):65e70.

4. 5.

6. 7. 8.

Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Assessment Battery for children. Circle Pines. Minnesota: American Guidance Service, 1983. 10. Flamant C, Branger B, Nguyen The Tich S, et al. Parent-completed developmental screening in premature children: a valid tool for followup programs. PLoS One 2011;6:e20004. 10a. Cole TJ. The LMS method for constructing normalized growth standards. Eur J Clin Nutr 1990;44:45e60. 11. Olsen IE, Groveman SA, Lawson ML, et al. New intrauterine growth curves based on United States data. Pediatrics 2010;125: e214e24. 12. WHO Child Growth Standards. Methods and Development: Length/ height-for-age, Weight-for-age, Weight-for-length, Weight-for-height and Body Mass Index-for-age. Geneva: World Health Organization, http://www.who.int/childgrowth/standards/Technical_report.pdf (accessed 10 Sep 2010). 13. Rubin DB. Estimating causal effects from large data sets using propensity scores. Ann Intern Med 1997;127:757e63. 14. Dagostino RB Jr. Propensity score methods for bias reduction in the comparison of a treatment to a non-randomized control group. Stat Med 1998;17:2265e81. 15. Newgard CD, Hedges JR, Arthur M, et al. Advanced statistics: the propensity score, a method for estimating treatment effect in observational research. Acad Emerg Med 2004;11:953e61. 16. Cummings P. Propensity scores. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008;162:734e7. 17. Mok E, Multon C, Piguel L, et al. Decreased full breastfeeding, altered practices, perceptions, and infant weight change of prepregnant obese women: a need for extra support. Pediatrics 2008;121: e1319e24. 18. Latal-Hajnal B, von Siebenthal K, Kovari H, et al. Postnatal growth in VLBW infants: signicant association with neurodevelopmental outcome. J Pediatr 2003;143:163e70. 19. Cooke RW, Foulder-Hughes L. Growth impairment in the very preterm and cognitive and motor performance at 7 years. Arch Dis Child 2003;88:482e7. 20. Hack M, Breslau N, Weissman B, et al. Effect of very low birth weight and subnormal head size on cognitive abilities at school age. N Engl J Med 1991;325:231e7. 21. Franz AR, Pohlandt F, Bode H, et al. Intrauterine, early neonatal, and postdischarge growth and neurodevelopmental outcome at 5.4 years in extremely preterm infants after intensive neonatal nutritional support. Pediatrics 2009;123:e101e9. 22. Zeitlin J, Ancel PY, Delmas D, et al. EPIPAGE and MOSAIC Ile-deFrance Groups. Changes in care and outcome of very preterm babies in the Parisian region between 1998 and 2003. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2010;95:F188e93. 23. Lanting CI, Fidler V, Huisman M, et al. Neurological differences between 9-year-old children fed breast-milk or formula-milk as babies. Lancet 1994;344:1319e22. 24. Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT. Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr 1999;70:525e35. 25. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, et al. Breast milk and subsequent intelligence quotient in children born preterm. Lancet 1992;339:261e4. 26. Gale CR, Martyn CN. Breastfeeding, dummy use, and adult intelligence. Lancet 1996;347:1072e5. 27. Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. BMJ 2006;333:945. 28. Quigley M, Henderson G, Anthony MY, et al. Formula milk versus donor breast milk for feeding preterm or low birth weight infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2007;(4):CD002971. 29. Singhal A, Cole TJ, Lucas A. Early nutrition in preterm infants and later blood pressure: two cohorts after randomised trials. Lancet 2001;357:413e19. 30. De Curtis M, Rigo J. Extrauterine growth restriction in very-lowbirthweight infants. Acta Pediatr 2004;93:1563e8. 31. Kramer MS, Guo T, Platt RW, et al. Breastfeeding and infant growth: biology or bias? Pediatrics 2002;110:343e7. 32. Bonet M, Blondel B, Agostino R, et al. Variations in breastfeeding rates for very preterm infants between regions and neonatal units in Europe: results from the MOSAIC cohort. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2011;96:F450e2. 33. Renfrew MJ, Craig D, Dyson L, et al. Breastfeeding promotion for infants in neonatal units: a systematic review and economic analysis. Health Technol Assess 2009;13:1e146.

9.

PAGE fraction trail=9

J-C, Darmaun D, Boquien C-Y, et al. BMJ Open 2012;2:e000834. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834 Roze

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

The apparent breastfeeding paradox in very preterm infants: relationship between breast feeding, early weight gain and neurodevelopment based on results from two cohorts, EPIPAGE and LIFT
Jean-Christophe Roz, Dominique Darmaun, Clair-Yves Boquien, et al. BMJ Open 2012 2:

doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2012-000834

Updated information and services can be found at:


http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/2/2/e000834.full.html

These include:

Data Supplement References

"Supplementary Data"
http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/suppl/2012/04/10/bmjopen-2012-000834.DC1.html

This article cites 30 articles, 13 of which can be accessed free at:


http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/2/2/e000834.full.html#ref-list-1

Article cited in:


http://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/2/2/e000834.full.html#related-urls

Open Access

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial License, which permits use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non commercial and is otherwise in compliance with the license. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/ and http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/legalcode. Receive free email alerts when new articles cite this article. Sign up in the box at the top right corner of the online article.

Email alerting service

To request permissions go to:


http://group.bmj.com/group/rights-licensing/permissions

To order reprints go to:


http://journals.bmj.com/cgi/reprintform

To subscribe to BMJ go to:


http://group.bmj.com/subscribe/

Downloaded from bmjopen.bmj.com on August 13, 2013 - Published by group.bmj.com

Topic Collections

Articles on similar topics can be found in the following collections Nutrition and metabolism (77 articles) Paediatrics (144 articles)

Notes

To request permissions go to:


http://group.bmj.com/group/rights-licensing/permissions

To order reprints go to:


http://journals.bmj.com/cgi/reprintform

To subscribe to BMJ go to:


http://group.bmj.com/subscribe/

You might also like