Handball Coaching
Handball Coaching
Handball Coaching
in handball players
- An analysis of the effects of verbal and non-verbal coaching
behaviors on motivation on the grounds of self-determination
theory
Julian Christopher Bauer
i623946
Masters degree programme: Sport and Physical Activity Interventions
Faculty Supervisor: Stef Kremers, PhD.
Second Examiner: Rob Ruiter, PhD.
External Supervisor: Dr. Ruben Goebel
German Sports University Cologne
Placement period: 1.2.2009 1.6.2009
Faculty of Health Sciences
Universiteit Maastricht
Abstract
Background and Purpose: Using self-determination theory as an analytical framework, the
present study investigated the influence of coaching behaviors in handball on different types
of motivation of the players. As several authors have outlined the importance of the coach as a
social factor which has an influence on motivation and its mediators, the three main goals of
the study were to (1) analyze the correlation between observed coaching behaviors and
motivation of athletes based on self-determination theory, (2) analyze the perceived coaching
behaviors and their relation to motivation, and (3) compare the observed coaching behaviors
with the coaching behaviors as perceived by the players.
Method: Three different instruments were used in order to assess the relationship between
coaching behaviors and motivation. An observational design of two observers was used to
count every displayed coaching behavior of seventeen coaches during one game observation
and one training observation according to an assessment system with ten categories, the
Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS). The perceived coaching behaviors were
measured by a questionnaire, the Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (CFQ). The different
types of motivation were assessed with the sport motivation scale (SMS).
Results: One hundred seventy-seven handball players of seventeen teams returned their
questionnaires (response rate 99.44%). The results indicated few significant relationships
between coaching behaviors and motivation and they were weak and insignificant for most
items. Exceptions were mostly related to the coaching behavior Reinforcement and different
types of motivation.
Conclusion: The results indicate that for the present sample a relationship between coaching
behaviors (perceived and observed) and athletes motivation was not present. Different levels
of assessment of the study design, i.e. observations on the situational level and questionnaires
assessing motivation and perceived coaching behaviors on the contextual level, may have had
an influence on the results. Further research is needed on other social factors that may
influence the different types of motivation as proposed by self-determination theory.
Table of Contents
Abstract .................................................................................................................................... II
Table of Contents .................................................................................................................. III
List of Figures ......................................................................................................................... IV
List of Tables ........................................................................................................................... IV
1 Introduction ......................................................................................................................... 1
1.1 Defining the task ......................................................................................................................... 1
1.2 Review of literature for the study ............................................................................................... 4
1.2.1 Validity of instruments and findings of related studies.................................................. 5
1.2.2 Potential influences on motivation on different levels ................................................... 6
1.3 Coaching assessment tools ......................................................................................................... 7
1.4 Tools to assess sport participation motivation ............................................................................ 9
2 Methods .............................................................................................................................. 12
2.1 Overview of study designs ....................................................................................................... 12
2.2 Participants ............................................................................................................................... 12
2.3 Recording procedure ................................................................................................................ 15
2.3.1 Recording procedure and categorization .................................................................... 16
2.3.2 Distinction between different behaviors and categories .............................................. 17
2.3.3 Measurement of self-reported motivation and perceived coaching behavior ............. 18
2.4 Translation procedure of the questionnaires ............................................................................. 19
2.5 Statistical Analysis ................................................................................................................... 19
3 Results ................................................................................................................................. 21
3.1 Differences in coaching behavior between games and training sessions ................................. 21
3.2 Reliability of Sports Motivation Scale items ............................................................................ 22
3.3 Correlations among Sports motivation scales (SMS) ............................................................... 22
3.4 Relationships between perceived coaching behaviors and observed coaching behaviors ........ 23
3.5 Relationships between observed coaching behaviors and different types of motivation ......... 23
3.6 Relationships between perceived coaching behaviors and different types of motivation ........ 24
3.7 Regression analysis .................................................................................................................. 25
3.7.1 Regression analyses of observed coaching behaviors, potential confounding variables
and different types of motivation ................................................................................. 25
3.7.2 Regression analyses of perceived coaching behaviors, potential confounding variables
and different types of motivation ................................................................................. 27
4 Discussion ........................................................................................................................... 30
4.1 Findings of the study and limitations of the CBAS results ...................................................... 30
4.2 General limitations of the study................................................................................................ 34
4.3 Further research recommendations ........................................................................................... 37
Bibliography ........................................................................................................................... 39
Table of Appendices ............................................................................................................... 54
List of Figures
Figure 2: Mageau and Vallerands (2003) motivational model of the coach-athlete
relationship ................................................................................................................... 2
List of Tables
Table 1: Significant correlational differences between observed coaching behaviors
(CBAS) recorded during training sessions and games ............................................... 21
Table 2: Correlations among Sports Motivation Scales (SMS) ............................................... 22
Table 3: Significant correlations between observed coaching behaviors and perceived
coaching behaviors ..................................................................................................... 23
Table 4: Correlations between observed coaching behaviors and different types of
motivation .................................................................................................................. 24
Table 5: Correlations between perceived coaching behaviors and different types of
motivation .................................................................................................................. 25
Table 6: Regression analyses of the ten observed coaching behaviors, the six potential
confounding variables and the seven different types of motivation .......................... 27
Table 7: Regression analyses of the seven perceived coaching behaviors, the six potential
confounding variables and seven different types of motivation ................................ 28
1 Introduction
1
1 Introduction
1.1 Defining the task
Participation in exercise and sport has been valued as an important way to increase personal
fitness and emotional well-being (Biddle & Mutrie, 2001) by a high number of researchers
and health practitioners. As sport participation is a nearly universally acknowledged way to
improve personal fitness (Vuori, 1995), the question why some people behave in certain ways
while others do not has been the central question of behavioral science for a long time
(Mallett, Kawabata, Newcombe, Otero-Ferero, & Jackson, 2007). Therefore, the reasons for
engaging in any particular behavior are at the center of interest for everyone empowered to
influence others like teachers, sport coaches or parents in order to motivate people to act in
desired ways, for example in the sports or educational context. The determinants for reasons
to act, which can also be named motivation, are of interest mainly because of two reasons:
The explanation of past and actual behavior as well as the prediction and active influence of
future behavior. In order to achieve this, different motivational theories have been proposed.
One of the theories which is especially useful for the context of sport is self-determination
theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985a, 2000), because it implies social and cognitive factors and
different types of motivation as well as behavioral consequences. Self-determination theory
differentiates between intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation and amotivation as well as
the extent to which these different types of motivation are perceived as autonomous and
emanate from the self (Deci & Ryan, 1985b). In line with self-determination theory, a four-
stage causal sequence has been proposed by Vallerand (1997) which contains the sequence of:
Social factors, which have an influence on psychological mediators, which again have an
influence on types of motivation which finally lead to behavioral consequences.
In the context of sport, the coach has been identified as an influential social factor at
all competitive levels (e.g. Horn, 2002; Smoll, & Smith, 2002; Barnett, Smith, & Smoll, 1992;
Bredemeier & Shields, 1993; Scanlan, 1986). The way a coach structures practice and game
situations, his way of making decisions, the quality and quantity of feedback he provides in
response to athletes performances, the relationships he establishes with athletes as well as his
leadership style can all have an impact on athletes behaviors, cognitions, and affective
responses (Amorose, 2007; see also Figure 1).
1 Introduction
2
Figure 1: Mageau and Vallerands (2003) motivational model of the coach-athlete relationship
Different motivational models have been proposed (see Appendix 14; 15; 16).
According to these, coaches can influence the learning processes of their athletes, their
enjoyment during participation and the sense of competence and self-determined motivational
orientation they develop (Chelladurai, 1993; Horn, 1987, 2002; Mageau & Vallerand, 2003;
Murray & Mann, 2001; Smoll & Smith, 2002).
As coaching behaviors can also lead to negative achievement-related and
psychological outcomes (e.g. poor performance, low self-esteem, high levels of competitive
anxiety and burnout) (Amorose, 2007, p. 209), the question arises which coaching behaviors
facilitate and which behaviors decrease the athletes motivation. A sports coach in team sports
as well as in individual sports is in an unequal power situation with his athletes, which gives
him the privilege of making decisions that effect the whole motivational climate (Ames,
1992a). Therefore coaches are made responsible for different goals to be achieved and need to
ensure the development of different aspects as outlined by Martens (2004): Recreational
sports have an emphasis on fun, learning and participation by all, whereas competitive sports
focuses on winning, performance and participation by the best (Martens, 2004, p. 21).
Because of this responsibility, coaches at the recreational level as well as coaches involved in
competitive or elite level sports should have an interest in developing a motivational climate
for their athletes which facilitates the successful achievement of these different outcomes.
Coaching behaviors have been found to have strong implications on the recreational
level in relation to dropout (Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang, 2003),
1 Introduction
3
enjoyment and fun (Simons, Dewitte, & Lens, 2003), and persistence (Gagn, Ryan, &
Bargmann, 2003). However, links between coaching behaviors and concentration (Kowal &
Fortier, 1999) and coaching behaviors and effort and performance (Simons, Dewitte, & Lens,
2003) make clear that for a coach in order to achieve the desired outcomes an effective
usage of his coaching behaviors is equally important in competitive sports where
performance is of higher importance (Martens, 2004).
Keeping the many positive impacts of physical activity on several biological functions
as well as its role in the prevention of overweight and obesity (Wing, 1999; Clark & Blair,
1988) in mind, it becomes clear that a coach can have an important indirect influence on the
aforementioned consequences through his role as a social factor. This influence is also
apparent when an increase in performance is desired as in a sports context where achievement
and performance are prevalent. Acknowledging this influence of the coaching process, it
becomes clear that the high dropout rates in recreational sport participation especially at the
beginning of adulthood (Sarrazin, Boiche, Pelletier, 2007, p. 229) as well as the high levels of
burnout in competitive sports (Klinger, 1975) must lead to the assumption that the responsible
coaches are either not aware of how the motivational climate they create and their displayed
coaching behaviors may influence their athletes motivation, which consequently can lead to
the undesired outcomes described, or, which would be equally bad, they might be unable to
change their coaching behavior.
The aim of the following thesis therefore is to analyze the impact of different coaching
behaviors on different types of motivation which differ in their extent of perceived self-
determination (Deci & Ryan, 1985b). Secondly it will be analyzed whether it is rather the
impact of perceived coaching behaviors or the impact of observed coaching behaviors which
leads to the fulfillment of the three basic needs of relatedness, competence, and autonomy
which may increase or decrease the amount of self-determined forms of motivation. This
analysis will be carried out in the context of and based on the results of this analysis,
recommendations for coaching behaviors in the given context will be attempted in order to
promote the desired types of more self-determined motivation and beneficial outcomes such
as better performance, lower drop-out rates or more enjoyment during sport participation.
To the knowledge of the author no study on motivation which compromises the basic
tenets of self-determination theory was carried out with senior, male handball players of
different playing levels before. As the majority of research examining motivation from a self-
determination theory point of view focuses on youth and university sports participants
1 Introduction
4
(Treasure, Lemyre, Kuczka, & Standage, 2007), it was decided to restrict the participants to
those who are playing and training in organized club structures.
1.2 Review of literature for the study
Self-determination theory (SDT) is a theory on motivation which focuses on the degree to
which individuals perceive themselves to be the origin of their own actions and choices (Deci
& Ryan, 1985a, 2000). The basic assumption of SDT is that people are innately and
proactively motivated to master their social environment (Mallett et al., 2007, p. 601). In
order to master ones social environment, three basic needs which function as nutriments
essential to growth, integrity, and well-being (Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2007, p. 13; Ryan, 1995)
have to be fulfilled. They comprise the need for competence, the need for autonomy, and the
need for relatedness. These three needs are at the center of cognitive evaluation theory (Deci,
1975; Deci & Ryan, 1980, 1985b, 1991, 2000), which is one of the subtheories of the meta-
theory SDT. The need for competence can be understood as a human desire to feel
responsible and competent in producing desired outcomes and preventing undesired ones.
(Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Deci, 1975; Deci & Ryan, 1985b; Harter, 1978; Vallerand, 2007;
White, 1959). The need for autonomy describes the desire of feeling to be the origin of ones
action (deCharms, 1968; Deci, 1975, 1980; Deci & Ryan, 1985a; Vallerand, 2007). The need
for relatedness is the desire to be part of a social group when engaging in an activity
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Deci & Ryan, 2000; Richer & Vallerand, 1998: Ryan, 1993;
Vallerand, 2007).
Deci and Ryan (2000) suggest that these needs are universal and the desire to fulfill
them is inherent in every individual. However, one of the fundamental differences of self-
determination theory in contrast to other motivational theories such as regulatory focus theory
(Higgins, 1997) or expectancy theory (Vroom, 1964) is the fact that it does not only
differentiate between intrinsic, extrinsic and amotivation but also offers a more complex
model of different degrees of autonomy in extrinsic motivation.
Intrinsic motivation describes a state where an activity is performed for its own sake,
because it is perceived as interesting and satisfying in itself without the help of any external
forces (Deci, 1971). A state of motivation produced by external forces would be described as
extrinsic motivation. (Deci & Ryan, 2002) Amotivation is characterized by the absence of any
extrinsic and intrinsic forces and thus not having either intention or energy directed toward
action (Deci & Ryan, 2007, p. 6).
1 Introduction
5
In line with its two subtheories, cognitive evaluation theory (Deci & Ryan, 2002) and
organismic integration theory (Ryan & Connell, 1989), self-determination theory also
differentiates the degree to which extrinsic motives are perceived to be autonomous and
volitional (Deci & Ryan, 2002). For this reason, extrinsic motivation is subdivided into four
different degrees of perceived autonomy, where integrated regulation represents the most
autonomous form of extrinsic motivation on a continuum where each of the following
regulations, i.e. identified, introjected and external regulation all represent decreasing degrees
of perceived autonomy, with external regulation representing the type of extrinsic motivation
with the lowest degree of perceived autonomy (ibid.).
Consequently, the probably most important tenet of self-determination theory states
that the higher the fulfillment of the three fundamental needs for competence, autonomy, and
relatedness, the more the reasons for an activity are perceived as self-determined with an
increased likelihood of intrinsic or integrated extrinsic motivation (Deci & Ryan, 1980).
Ryan and Connell (1989) additionally state that an interplay between intrinsic and
extrinsic motives exists, which makes an analysis of these different kinds of motivation
necessary when aiming to analyze the overall quality of motivation and which also makes it
important to analyze the influence of coaching behavior on all different motivational concepts
and not only one in particular.
1.2.1 Validity of instruments and findings of related studies
The self-determination taxonomy of motives, the continuum of relative autonomy, and the
predicted consequences have been validated in different cross-cultural settings in the field of
sports (Matsumoto, Takenaka, & Takaya, 2003; Mullan, Markland, & Ingledew, 1997;
Wilson, Rodgers, Fraser, & Murray, 2004). Several studies have reported the positive
consequences associated with intrinsic motivation on behavioral outcomes such as persistence
(Markland & Ingledew, 2007, p. 29) and intrinsically motivated individuals are more likely
to choose to participate and work hard when extrinsic rewards or reinforcements are not
available, experience lower levels of performance-related anxiety, and exhibit greater levels
of skill learning relative to those with a more extrinsic motivational orientation (Amorose,
2007, p. 210; compare also Vallerand, 1997; Vallerand & Losier, 1999; Weiss & Ferrer-Caja,
2002). Additionally, autonomous motivation which can be considered synonymous with self-
determined motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2007; Pelletier & Sarrazin, 2007) was found to be
associated with positive attitudes towards sporting behavior (Vallerand & Losier, 1994),
positive emotions (Frederick, Morrison, & Manning, 1996; Li, 1999), flow (Kowal & Fortier,
1 Introduction
6
1999, 2000), and physical activity intentions (Kowal & Fortier, 2005; Standage, Duda, &
Ntoumanis, 2003; Wilson & Rodgers, 2004) as well as with behavioral outcomes like sport
persistence (Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, & Briere, 2001; Sarrazin, Vallerand, Guillet,
Pelletier, & Cury, 2002), leisure-time physical activity (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Culverhouse,
& Biddle, 2003), exercise adherence (Fortier & Grenier, 1999; Ryan, Frederick, Lepes, Rubio,
& Sheldon, 1997), stage of physical activity behavior change (Fortier et al., 2006; Ingledew,
Markland, & Medley, 1998; Mullan & Markland, 1997), and physical fitness (Wilson,
Rodgers, Blanchard, & Gessell, 2003). However, research with Bulgarian athletes also
indicated positive effects on performance of less self-determined forms of motivation (i.e.
introjected regulation and external regulation) in certain conditions (Chantal, Guay, Dobreva-
Martinova, & Vallerand, 1996) as can be found in highly competitive and controlling
structures which emphasize external incentives and pressures to compete (Treasure et al.,
2007).
1.2.2 Potential influences on motivation on different levels
The influence of the social context, such as the political system, as opposed to a particular
coaching behavior, such as praise in response to a players positively judged performance,
shows that different levels of social factors exist. Vallerand (2001) proposes a model with
three different social levels of motivation in order to have a more refined understanding of
motivational processes involved in human behavior (Vallerand, 2007, p. 260). This
hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation (HMIEM) (Vallerand, 1997, 2001)
implies (1) global factors which are present even in different contexts such as a personality
trait which is considered to be stable over time (Vallerand, 2007). (2) Contextual factors
comprise all factors present in one particular life domain. The three typical life domains on
the contextual level are education, leisure and interpersonal relationships (Blais, Vallerand,
Gagnon, Briere, & Pelletier, 1990). The third social factor level besides the global and the
contextual level is the situational level which refers to the motivation individuals experience
when engaging in a specific activity at a given moment in time (Vallerand, 2007, p. 260).
This situational level refers to specific actions, for example a particular task such as shooting
on the goal during a training session. Consequently motivation has to be considered on all
three levels, as these levels are proposed to be interconnected, when analyzing the impact of a
social factor such as the coaching behavior on perceived motivation. All three different levels
described before are also related to different amounts of motivation for each individual. For
1 Introduction
7
example a highly self-determined individual on the contextual level of handball has a very
high likelihood of also being highly self-determined motivated on the situational level, for
example during one particular exercise. Additionally an individual whose personal trait it is to
be mainly motivated by less self-determined extrinsic types of motivation on the global level,
also has a high likelihood of displaying this kind of motivation on the contextual level, for
example in school or during sports. (Appendix 14)
Therefore it becomes evident that the coaches role which has been identified as being
particularly important in sports (Horn, 2002; Weiss & Ferrer-Caja, 2002) has a direct
influence both on the contextual level through his position (i.e. leadership style etc.) in a
given context as well as on the situational level through particular behaviors displayed at a
given point in time (i.e. through praise, punishment, encouragement, non-verbal
communication etc.). However, although the coach is in a position which is restricted to the
actual context of sport and has no direct effect on global motivation, bottom up effects of
motivation were found to be present (Vallerand, 2007) in that contextual motivation has a
bottom-up effect on the global motivation.
The overwhelming amount of reported benefits of more intrinsically or autonomously
perceived motivation in different contexts such as psychological need satisfaction, leisure-
time physical activity behavior, teacher ratings of motivated behavior, concentration, changes
in subjective vitality and obviation of negative affects (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Barkoukis,
Wang, & Baranowski, 2005; Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Culverhouse, & Biddle, 2003;
Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2003, 2006; Gagn, Ryan, & Bargmann,
2003; Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, & Briere, 2001; Reinboth & Duda, 2006; Reinboth, Duda,
& Ntoumanis, 2004) make clear that an analysis of the coaching behaviors is of paramount
importance when trying to analyze its influence the diverging different types of self-
determined motivation.
Based on these findings, the present study will not focus on the behavioral
consequences but rather on the perceptions of self-determination of the athletes which may be
increased or decreased through the coaching behaviors and on measurement issues of how to
assess the various underlying concepts of motivation.
1.3 Coaching assessment tools
As mentioned before, the coach in his unique influential position for decisions on tactics,
training session structuring, verbal and non-verbal feedback to players actions (Appendix 15;
1 Introduction
8
16) has been identified as a powerful social factor influencing players motivation (Amorose,
2007). For this reason, different assessment tools have been developed in order to measure
different coaching behaviors.
The Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS) (Smith, Smoll, & Hunt, 1977)
(see Appendix 22 for German Version) is a behavioral observation approach which allows
trained observers to code verbal and non-verbal feedback of the coach in practice and game
situations. The CBAS consists of twelve behavioral categories which are (a) reinforcement (a
positive, rewarding action, verbal or non-verbal, to a good play or good effort), (b) non-
reinforcement (failure to respond to a good performance), (c) mistake-contingent
encouragement (encouragement given to an athlete following a mistake), (d) mistake-
contingent technical instruction (instruction or demonstration given to an athlete to explain
how to correct a mistake), (e) punishment (negative reaction, verbal or non-verbal, following
a mistake), (f) punitive technical instruction (technical instruction following a mistake given
in a punitive or hostile manner), (g) ignoring mistakes (failure to respond to an athletes
mistake), (h) keeping control (reactions intended to restore or maintain order among team
members), (i) general technical instruction (spontaneous instruction in the techniques and
strategies of the sport, not following a mistake), (j) general encouragement (spontaneous
encouragement that does not follow a mistake), (k) organization (administrative behavior that
sets the stage for play by assigning duties or responsibilities), and (i) general communication
(interactions with players unrelated to the game) (Barnes, 2003).
These coaching behaviors can be classified into (a) reactive and (b) spontaneous
behaviors. The reactive behaviors are those which immediately follow a players action which
is either a desired or undesired behavior. These reactive behaviors include reinforcement,
non-reinforcement, mistake-contingent encouragement, mistake-contingent technical
instruction, punishment, punitive technical instruction, ignoring mistakes, and keeping control
(Smith et al., 1977). The spontaneous behaviors are general technical instruction, general
encouragement, organization, and general communication. These behaviors are not associated
to and therefore not reactive with preceding events (Smith et al., 1977).
However, in the previous years different or adapted versions of the CBAS have been
developed, with some versions (Millard, 1996; Smith, Zane, Smoll, & Coppel, 1983; Smith,
& Smoll, 1990; Pappas, 2004) using additional categories such as humor or uncodable and
some versions not even using all categories from the original version (e.g., general
communication and keeping control). According to Conroy and Coatsworth, little is known
1 Introduction
9
about the psychometric properties of scores from this measure and single-item measures tend
to have limited reliability (2007b, p. 675). This suggests some items within the CBAS were
found to have lower reliabilities while other items have higher reliabilities. Furthermore,
reliability scores of the CBAS (test-retest, internal consistency) vary between studies (Barnes,
2003; Conroy & Coatsworth, 2007b; Smith, Smoll, & Hunt, 1977), suggesting that CBAS
scores are influenced by irrelevant context and sample characteristics, suggesting that results
of the CBAS should be interpreted cautiously.
The Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (CFQ) (Horn & Glenn, 1988) (Appendix 17;
21), which is a reliable and valid scale (Nicaise, Cogerino, Bois, & Amorose, 2006) contains
sixteen items, representing different feedback patterns all of which are measured twice.
Following desired behaviors the three response patterns are a) praise/reinforcement, b) non-
reinforcement, c) reinforcement plus technical instruction which is a category not present in
the CBAS. The five response patterns following unsuccessful outcomes/behaviors are d)
mistake-contingent encouragement, e) ignoring mistakes, f) corrective instruction, g)
punishment and h) corrective instruction plus punishment. For the statistical analysis, it was
decided to count the two categories, corrective instruction and corrective instruction plus
punishment together, leading to 4 items for this category. The items (Appendix 21) have to be
indicated on a 5-point scale (very typical to not typical at all) in order to assess how typical
the particular behaviors were displayed by the teams coach during games and training
sessions. Alpha coefficients for individual subscales of this questionnaire have ranged from
.62 to .91 (Horn & Glenn, 1988)
Based on the complex study design suggested, and although the CFQ seems to be
more reliable, both the CBAS and the CFQ will be used while keeping the reliability scores in
mind when interpreting the results.
1.4 Tools to assess sport participation motivation
The sport motivation scale (SMS) (Pelletier et al., 1995) (Appendix 19; 20) is probably the
scale most widely used to measure the various regulatory styles proposed by self-
determination theory in the context of sport (Pelletier & Sarrazin, 2007). The sport
motivation scale (SMS) is a questionnaire with 28 items which assesses external regulation,
introjected regulation, and identified regulation, which are all extrinsic motivational concepts
that differ on their degree of perceived autonomy and self-determination (Deci & Ryan,
1985a, 2000). Additionally, it assesses amotivation and three types of intrinsic motivation (to
1 Introduction
10
know, to accomplish, and to experience stimulation). French and English versions of the
questionnaire were validated in studies with Canadian athletes from different individual and
team sports (Pelletier & Sarrazin, 2007). The results revealed satisfactory internal
consistency, a seven-factor structure that corresponds to the forms of motivation targeted by
the scale, adequate construct validity, and moderate-to-high indices of temporal stability
(Pelletier & Sarrazin, 2007, p. 144). A simplex pattern which implies that scales which are
theoretically closer have a higher correlation was proven in a meta-analysis of twenty-one
studies (Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang, 2003; Li & Harmer, 1996).
Furthermore, an adequate test-retest reliability of .58 to .84 of the seven items was found by
the developers of the questionnaire (Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, Tuson, & Briere, 1995).
However, the sport motivation scale (SMS) in its current form, distinguishing three
different intrinsic motivation scales (to know, to experience stimulation, to accomplish, has
been criticized for not incorporating integrated motivation (Mallett & Hanrahan, 2004), a lack
of factorial validity (Martens & Webber, 2002; Riemer, Fink, & Fitzgerald, 2002), and low
reliability/unacceptable internal consistency (Raedeke & Smith, 2001; Martin & Cutler, 2002;
Vlachopoulos, Karageorghis, & Terry, 2000). Still Pelletier, Vallerand and Sarrazin (2007)
have outlined that the structure, the reliability and the construct validity of the SMS has been
proven for several populations from team-sports as well as individual sports. (Pelletier,
Vallerand, & Sarrazin, 2007, p.617)
Here I put forward the hypothesis that the observed and perceived coaching behaviors
by the coaches have a powerful impact on their athletes motivation mainly through the
facilitation of the basic needs for competence and relatedness. Based on the reviewed
literature, coaches using more punitive and non-reinforcing behaviors are expected to have
athletes with less intrinsic and self-determined motivation than coaches who act in a more
encouraging and reinforcing way. Additionally, I expect that coaches from teams which play
in lower leagues will be found to have less influence on their players motivation, as the need
for competence might be of less importance in leagues with a lower playing level.
The primary purpose of the study was to determine if observed coaching behaviors are
related to athletes self-reported motivation. Several sources suggest that it is the perception
of coaching behaviors by the athletes rather than the actually displayed and observed coaching
behaviors which have an effect on motivation (Ommundsen & Bar-Eli, 1999; Allen & Howe,
1998; Amorose & Horn, 2000; Amorose & Smith, 2003; Amorose & Weiss, 1998; Black &
Weiss, 1992). A secondary purpose was to determine the extent to which the coaching
1 Introduction
11
behaviors which were categorized and observed match with the athletes perception of the
coaching behaviors.
Additionally, and acknowledging the importance of perceptions, the relation between
perceived coaching behaviors and athletes self-reported motivation will be assessed as this
link might be of highest importance in determining athletes motivation. Additional
comparisons will be made between the quantity of measured behaviors between these two to
assess if significant differences exist between the observed coaching behaviors during games
and training sessions. The primary goal of this work is to analyze the influence of different
coaching behaviors on different types of motivation of handball players. For this purpose the
coaching behaviors will be differentiated between observed and perceived coaching behaviors
which both will be put separately in relation to the different types of motivation. The
secondary goal is to analyze which factors (different coaching behaviors and assumed
confounding factors) might have an influence on the types of motivation and the extent of the
interplay of these different factors.
2 Methods
12
2 Methods
2.1 Overview of study designs
Acknowledging the fact that the sport motivation scale is probably the most frequently used
scale to assess contextual sports motivation on the grounds of self-determination theory and
the numerously existing studies confirming the construct validity and reliability of the SMS
(Jackson, Kimiecik, Ford, & Marsh, 1998; Hamer, Karageorghis & Vlachopoulos, 2002;
Ntoumanis, 2001; Sarrazin, Vallerand, Guillet, Pelletier, & Curry, 2001; Chantal, Guay, &
Dobreva Martinova, 1996; Alexandris, Tsorbatzoudis, & Grouios, 2002; Doganis, 2000;
Georgiadis, Biddle, & Chatzisarantis, 2001) in different cultural backgrounds, it was decided
to use the SMS as the measurement instrument of motivation in the design of this study as it
may give valuable insights into the different motivational constructs effected by coaching
behaviors.
Although different other coaching assessment systems like the leadership scale for
sports (LSS) (Chelladurai & Saleh, 1978), the coaching behavior recording form (Darst,
Zakrajsek, & Mancini, 1989) or the Arizona State University Observation Instrument
(ASUOI) (Lacy & Darst, 1989) do exist, the author decided to restrict the instruments to the
three discussed before as these were found to be especially useful in assessing motivation and
coaching behaviors using the theoretical framework of self-determination theory.
Different ways of gathering data were used. An observational design was chosen to
categorize the displayed coaching behaviors during one training session and one game. The
athletes motivation was measured by a questionnaire (SMS) assessing why the athletes are
active in their sport. Perceived coaching behaviors were assessed through the Coaching
Feedback Questionnaire. The research performed for this study was correlational in nature
and no intervention was initiated.
2.2 Participants
The participant sample was composed of handball players actively involved in competitive
structures characterized by regular games and training sessions. A total of seventeen teams
and their male coaches were observed, leading to 177 players (mean 10.41 players per team)
in total who filled out the questionnaires. All teams were male teams and consequently the
study was conducted exclusively with male participants. Participating players had a mean age
of 25.09 years (SD 8.83). The playing experience of the players was 15.83 years (SD 8.18)
2 Methods
13
(i.e., passed years since issuing the first playing license and therefore being eligible to play)
(Appendix 8 and 9). One condition of including teams was their scheduled training days. In
order to achieve a good comparability it was decided only to include teams which train on
Mondays or Fridays in order to have a time span between the games at the weekend and the
training sessions observed of either one or two days after or one or two days before a game.
The sample size calculation was made with some presuppositions. The slope
coefficient between observed coaching behaviors and perceived coaching behaviors was
estimated to be approximately 0.4 %. The highest difference of one observed coaching
behavior in relation to another coach was expected to be 20 %. The variance on the 5-point
Likert scale was expected to be approximately 1. Values on the Likert scale of more than 5 or
less than 1 were counted as 5 or 1 accordingly. The probability of having a type 1 error was
set at 5 %. Taking these assumptions for granted, the sample size calculation revealed that
eighteen teams were necessary to achieve a statistical power of over 80 %.
It was decided to include teams from different playing leagues in order to achieve a
cross-sectional design with different playing levels. For practical reasons it was decided to
only include teams from the Lower Rhine region in order to be able to have distances that
allow more than one observation per day. For male handball, there are ten different playing
leagues in that region. As the focus of the study is on coaching behaviors and their effects it
was decided to exclude professional and semi-professional handball, which eliminates the
first, the second, and the third league as the author hypothesizes that financial incentives
influence players motivation in a way that cannot be controlled for adequately in this study.
The tenth league was excluded as one inclusion criteria was that all teams had regular training
sessions and an identifiable coach, and this condition was not given for this lowest league.
Additionally, four youth teams (A-Juniors) were included, who are at the age of 17 to 18
years in order to also include young adult participants. Younger participants were excluded
because the instruments used were only validated for an adult population.
In order to minimize the possible influence of the observed teams placement in their
leagues tables, it was attempted to have at least two teams from each league, with one being
from the bottom half of each leagues respective table and one team being from the upper half
of this league. However, as the set time frame for observations was nine weeks, all
observations had to fit into this time frame, and training observations necessarily took place
before the game observations some leagues had more participating teams because their games
and training sessions fit better into this time frame.
2 Methods
14
Eventually, two teams from the fourth league (Oberliga), one team from the fifth
league (Verbandsliga), two teams from the sixth league (Landesliga), five teams from the
seventh league (Bezirksliga), three teams from the eight league (Kreisliga), one team from the
ninth league (1. Kreisklasse) and four youth teams participated. However, one team from the
seventh league, whose game observation took place before the training session observation
cancelled its training session, and as this was the last week of their season and no training
took place afterwards within the set time frame, this team had to be excluded from the study
(as the questionnaires were given out during training sessions and this was not possible
anymore with this team). Additionally, for one team of the lowest observed league (ninth
league 1. Kreisklasse) the coaching behavior could not be observed as the coach was only
present during games. The observers were not told this before this training session; however,
the questionnaires were given out and collected. In this case only the coaching behavior
during the game was put into relation with the self-reported motivation of the athletes. Two of
the four observed youth teams were observed during their qualification round which is a
preliminary round in which, based on the results and places available in the different playing
leagues, the participating teams are grouped into the different leagues according to their
results during that tournament. Consequently, for these two teams it could not be determined
before these games which league they were belonging to as this was only decided afterwards
on grounds of these results.
Due to these aforementioned recruitment conditions, it was not possible to achieve an
equal distribution of teams within each league. Additionally, it was necessary to include some
teams (n=4) which either trained on Tuesdays or Thursdays. This was regarded as
undesirable, but unproblematic as having one day less or more before a game was not
considered to have a significant impact on the coaching behavior and else it would not have
been possible to include this number (n=17) of teams within the aforementioned time frame.
Therefore it was decided to change the inclusion criteria slightly rather than to drop a high
number of participating teams which would not have fit these criteria exactly.
Recruitment of participants began with finding out which teams of the relevant
leagues had their training sessions on the aforementioned days and additionally had their
games on the weekend at compatible times, so that as many teams as possible could be
observed each weekend. After finding out their contact numbers, the coaches of the teams
which fit these criteria were contacted by telephone. After being told the rationale and the
procedure of the study, the coaches were asked for consent and an agreement was made for
2 Methods
15
the date of the first observation. Although the coaches were told that their verbal and non-
verbal feedback to players actions were recorded and categorized, they were not being told
what categories and what method of categorizing and recording would be used. This was done
because it was expected that there would be less reactivity in coaches behavior if the coaches
did not know exactly what the observers were looking for. Of all teams contacted (n=19) only
one team refused allowance without being willing to give reasons.
2.3 Recording procedure
As an observational method the coaching behavior assessment system (Smith et al., 1977)
was used in a slightly modified version as proposed by Pappas (2004). Two of the twelve
categories suggested by Smith et al. (1977) were dropped as it was found that they were
problematic in being distinguished from the other categories. These two categories were
general communication and keeping control.
For the present study it was decided to use the ten category version as this would fit
best to typical handball coaching behaviors. It was decided to have two observers who are
familiar with and licensed in handball coaching in order to be able to interpret specific
handball terms into the appropriate categories. Additionally, it was found desirable to have
two different recordings for every game and training session in order to achieve an inter-rater
reliability that may help to evaluate in how far the different categories are distinguishable and
the extent to which both observers differ in interpreting the same behaviors.
The observations were always carried out by the same two observers, one of who
being the author of this work and the other one being a licensed coach of childrens handball.
Both observers were trained by the Training Manual for the Coaching Behavior Assessment
System as this manual was designed as part of an instructional program for training
researchers in the use of the Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS) (Smith et al.,
1977, p. 1). This training included fitting different example coaching behaviors into categories
and discussing the results as well as comparing them to the solutions as proposed in the
training manual.
Every team was informed by their coaches about the rationale of the study and the
recording procedure immediately before the training session. The coaches also informed their
teams that the observations would take place at this particular training session and at the game
on the weekend of the same week. In most cases, the two observers were also given the
chance to introduce themselves shortly and give information about the method of the study.
2 Methods
16
2.3.1 Recording procedure and categorization
All training sessions took place in the evenings in the sports halls of the chosen teams. Both
observers were visually present for all players during the whole of all training sessions.
Recording of the coaches behavior began with all players assembling on the court and the
coaches initial speech about organizational issues (such as todays training focus, mistakes
from the last games etc.). If the coaches decided to introduce the two observers and inform the
players about the rationale of the study, this behavior was not counted and recorded as the
author considered this behavior to be irrelevant to the actual coaching process. The recording
ended when the coach verbally ended the training session; if a final meeting of players on the
court initiated by the coach took place immediately before leaving the sports hall, these
tactical or organizational issues were still counted as being part of the training session and
therefore they were recorded. Importantly, it was made clear beforehand who the first coach
was if more than one coach was present, as only his behavior was counted. However, there
was never really a problem to differentiate the coach from his assistant whose behaviors were
not counted.
For all training sessions the two observers placed themselves at a position where they
could observe and understand the coaches feedback in an unobtrusive manner while still
being able to hear statements given in a calm way. For this reason, the two observers
frequently had to change location when the coach chose new positions for his feedback. The
two observers positioned themselves in such a distance from each other that neither of them
could see the notes of the other observer, however, close enough for being able to ask the
other observer what the coach said, if the words were hard to understand.
It turned out that with this method nearly all of the feedback the coach was giving to
the players could be easily understood. In the few cases in which behaviors were not clearly
understood by both observers they compared their respective understanding. If then there
was still no certainty about what had been said, the observers guessed what might have been
said based on the non-verbal behavior and the action which preceded the feedback.
After one pilot observation which was carried out for training purposes and which was
not included into the study results both observers discussed the potential issues of the
categorization system. Especially the categorizations of non-reinforcement and ignoring
mistakes were found to be controversial.
As reinforcement or non-reinforcement are defined as reactive behaviors (to a players
successful performance in the case of reinforcement and to a players unsuccessful
2 Methods
17
performance in the case of non-reinforcement) the definitions of successful and unsuccessful
performances had to be made clear. Strictly speaking, every caught pass is a successful
performance. Accordingly, every ball that is not passed properly or not caught immediately
is a mistake. Therefore, every time when the coach does not comment on such a successful
or unsuccessful action it needs to be counted. However, it was decided only to include
significant actions like a missed fast-break, a missed penalty shot, and repetitive mistakes
like not catching the ball or failing to give a pass. Both observers agreed on what these
significant actions were and tried to be consistent in the counting of these actions and the
according feedback of the coach.
2.3.2 Distinction between different behaviors and categories
Every coaching behavior was counted with a dash in the check-list. Every coaching category
which was displayed was counted separately. One single utterance or sentence could therefore
include more than one category and consequently was counted accordingly. A statement like
That was a good pass, but you have to keep your arm extended while shooting could
therefore be counted as reinforcement and mistake-contingent technical instruction. Non-
verbal feedback was counted according to the category it was assigned to.
Additionally, a separation between behaviors was made when the same behavior took
longer than ten seconds. As long as the same behavioral performance took ten seconds or less,
even if more than one sentence was devoted to the same behavior, it was counted as one dash
in the check list. As soon as the same category was displayed for longer than ten seconds, the
behavior was counted again every ten seconds. This separation and re-counting was
especially useful for the two categories of organization and general technical instruction as
these could sometimes last longer than the ten seconds chosen to delimit one unit.
This ten-seconds separation of the same behavior was also suggested by other authors
(Pappas, 2004; Cushion, 2001). However, as with the different categories used in different
studies, the intervals in separating the different behaviors of the same category have not been
consistent. Cushion (2001) for example suggested intervals of five seconds. For this study,
after the pilot observation, it was decided to use ten seconds as the appropriate interval.
Every training session lasted approximately 90 minutes. The game observations
started with the beginning of the games as indicated by the initial whistle by the referees. No
tactical or organizational commands were recorded before that time. However, the two
observers also recorded the coaches instructions during half-time in the cabin where they also
placed themselves in a visible but unobtrusive manner. As a handball game quite frequently
2 Methods
18
has time-outs during which the coaching behaviors were still recorded and a break of ten
minutes, the recorded time in total, until the final whistle of the match which was the end of
the recording, was also approximately 90 minutes.
2.3.3 Measurement of self-reported motivation and perceived coaching behavior
Immediately following the training session, the players were given a questionnaire which
consisted of two parts. The first part was the Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (CFQ) (Horn
& Glenn, 1988) which is the questionnaire form assessing the perceptions of athletes based
on the coaching categories of the CBAS (Smith et al., 1977) as described in the literature
section. Indices of coaching behaviors were calculated in order to aggregate the compositional
score of the two items which are supposed to measure the same coaching category.
Additionally, the sport motivation scale (SMS) (Pelletier, et al., 1995), which is also discussed
in the section on research about coaching effectiveness, was included into the questionnaires
given out. The coaching feedback questionnaire includes 16 items assessing 8 different types
of motivation such as: That was a really stupid play!. The sport motivation scale includes
28 items with 4 items testing each of the 7 different types of motivation.
All participants were asked to fill out the questionnaires immediately after the training
session in order to assure high return rates of the questionnaires. Only players who actively
participated in the training sessions were allowed to fill out a questionnaire. Players who were
injured or just watched the training session as a spectator were not asked to fill out the
questionnaire as it was hypothesized that only those who participated actively and over the
whole training session were influenced by the coaching behavior in the way which was tried
to be assessed by the study instruments.
Also players who attended and participated in the training session as guests or who
were not nominated for the game at the weekend were asked to fill out the questionnaire
based on their perceptions and experiences they had made with the particular coach so far.
Questionnaires were only given out after the one observed training session and all (n=177) but
one questionnaire were returned immediately after being given out and being filled out on the
same day. The one person who did not return the questionnaire just felt not like filling out a
questionnaire.
The questionnaires also included a column which asked the participants to indicate
their age and their years of playing experience (since the first issuing of their playing license).
Approximately half of the teams (n=9) had an additional question in their questionnaire which
asked whether they felt that their coach was structuring and leading the training session as
2 Methods
19
usual in spite of the observation. Only nine of the teams had questionnaires with this
additional question because the idea of having this reactivity measurement only came up
half way during the study when one participant told the observers that the coach was
unusually friendly during that training session.
All questionnaires were collected after the training session and gathered in a separate
folder for every team.
2.4 Translation procedure of the questionnaires
As all three instruments, the sport motivation scale (SMS) (Pelletier, et al., 1995), the CBAS
(Smith et al. 1977) and the Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (CFQ) (Horn & Glenn, 1988)
were developed and validated in their English version, in order to use the instruments in a
German field setting they had to be translated into German. The translation procedure was
done by two bilingual speakers, as proposed by Sobhonslidsuk (2004), who both are native
speakers of German and have a high proficiency in English as one is of half American descent
and the other one being in possession of an academic degree in the English language. All
items of both the CFQ and the SMS as well as all categories of the CBAS and all additional
information such as introductory words on the questionnaires were independently translated
by both translators from English into German. Then the results of both translations were
compared and the few differences were discussed. As only few differences existed, both
translators easily reached agreement on the few divergent items and no third refereeing
translator had to be contacted.
2.5 Statistical Analysis
To analyze if there are significant differences between the observed coaching behaviors
during training sessions and the observed coaching behaviors during games, a paired
independent t-test will be conducted. The interobserver reliability of the two observers
categorization of the observed coaching behavior was performed separately for training
sessions and the game situations for all of the 10 coaching behaviors.
The reliability of the items of the Sports Motivation Scale questionnaire was
calculated using Cronbachs alpha for every scale. The observed coaching behaviors which
were put in relation to (1) the different types of motivation and (2) the coaching behaviors as
perceived by the players, were always taken as the mean of the two observers. The observed
and the perceived coaching behaviors cannot be matched on a one-to-one basis. The
2 Methods
20
equivalences can be found in Appendix 8. This means that not all observed coaching
behaviors can be matched with a perceived coaching behavior. The observed coaching
behaviors were taken as the mean of the training and the game observation. Values of the
different types of motivation and the perceived coaching behaviors were both assessed on the
individual level. (n=160-177)
All results lie on an interval and therefore the Pearson correlation coefficient was used
to test whether significant differences between observed coaching behaviors and perceived
coaching behaviors as assessed by the Coaching Feedback Questionnaire existed.
The mean values of the observed coaching behaviors were put in relation to the
different types of motivation as assessed by the Sports Motivation Scale. The Pearson
correlation coefficient was again used to check for significant relationships as well as for the
potential relationships between perceived coaching behaviors and the different types of
motivation. For all analyses, the different kinds of motivation were taken as scales using the
aggregated score of the four items assessing the seven different types of motivation.
Hierarchical regressions were conducted to analyze the potential impact of different
variables taken together on the handball players motivation. The different types of motivation
were taken as the dependent variable and all of the seven types of motivation as assessed
through the SMS (Appendix 20) were checked separately for the observed coaching behaviors
and the hypothised confounding variables. The second type of regression models was
calculated taking the seven types of motivation (Appendix 20) again as the dependent variable
in relation to the perceived coaching behaviors and the hypothised confounding variables.
The characteristics of each team such as participating players per team, playing
league, juniors/seniors, result of the observed game (lost, draw, won), result of the last game
(lost, draw, won), and placement in the leagues table (upper half or bottom half) can be found
in Appendix 1. The games result will be a dichotomous variable as the game result will be
differentiated between won or not won, meaning that the one case of a draw will be
counted as not won. The placement in the table will also be dichotomous as the
differentiation will be between upper half of the table and bottom half of the table.
All of these characteristics together with the players age and playing experience were
checked as potential confounders in the hierarchical regression analysis.
3 Results
21
3 Results
In total, 177 participants filled out the questionnaire (mean 10.41 players per team).
Participating players had a mean age of 25.09 years (SD 8.83) (Appendix 8). The playing
experience of the players had a mean of 15.83 years (SD 8.18) (Appendix 9).
The inter-reliability of the ten observational categories (Appendix 22) for the game
observation showed very strong correlations (>0.8) in six categories (Appendix 10). Two
additional items had a strong correlation (>0.7), which could still be considered satisfactory
for an observational instrument.
Only the two items non-reinforcement and organization showed coefficients between
0.6 and 0.7 which were not considered satisfactory, indicating that there is quite a big
difference between the two observers in recording this item. Consequently, all results which
are put in relation to these items in the following have to be judged cautiously.
For the training observations (Appendix 11), nine of the ten observational categories
showed very strong correlations (>0.8) between both observers, with eight of these nine
categories even having correlations of higher than (>0.9). However, similar to the game
observation results, the category non-reinforcement showed no satisfying reliability between
both observers with a correlation of only (>0.5). Therefore, it has to be outlined again that all
results which are in correlation with the item non-reinforcement must be considered as having
a low reliability. The item organization, however, had a different reliability in game situations
(>0.7) compared to training sessions. (<0.9)
3.1 Differences in coaching behavior between games and training sessions
Of the ten pairs analyzed only three proved to be significantly different (Appendix 12 and 13
for descriptive statistics and t-tests).
Paired T-Test
Paired differences
T df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Standard deviation
Non-Reinforcement (Game)
Non-Reinforcement (Training)
2.5 1.87 5.35 15 .000
Mistake-contingent technical instruction (Game)
Mistake-contingent technical instruction (Training)
-11.0 17.2 -2.56 15 .02
Organization (Game) - Organization (Training) -30.16 29.48 -4.09 15 .001
Table 1: Significant correlational differences between observed coaching behaviors (CBAS) recorded
during training sessions and games
3 Results
22
The coaching behavior non-reinforcement was displayed significantly more often in game
situations than in training sessions (p=.000, T=5.35, df:15). Mistake-contingent technical
instruction was significantly more often displayed in training sessions (p=.02, T=2.56, df:15).
Organization was displayed nearly twice as often in training sessions (p=.001, T=4.09, df:15).
Punishment misses significance with a p=value of .07. All other pairs of coaching behavior
differences between games and training sessions are clearly not significant.
3.2 Reliability of Sports Motivation Scale items
The SMS scales had acceptable reliability; Cronbachs alphas were between .64 (amotivation)
and .78 (intrinsic motivation to know). The other scales had Cronbachs alpha values of .73
(intrinsic motivation to accomplish), .68 (intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation),
.69 (extrinsic motivation identified), .72 (extrinsic motivation introjected) and .70
(extrinsic motivation external regulation).
3.3 Correlations among Sports motivation scales (SMS)
Intrinsic
motivation
to know
Intrinsic
motivation
to
accomplish
Intrinsic
motivation
to
experience
stimulation
Extrinsic
motivation
- identified
Extrinsic
motivation
- introjected
Extrinsic
motivation
external
regulation
Amotivation
Intrinsic
motivation
to know
- .60** .54** .23** .38** .27** -.11
Intrinsic
motivation
to
accomplish
- .68** .41** .40** .36** -.18*
Intrinsic
motivation
to
experience
stimulation
- .43** .54** .32** -.21**
Extrinsic
motivation -
identified
- .30** .44** -.10
Extrinsic
motivation -
introjected
- .25** -.14
Extrinsic
motivation
external
regulation
- .12
Amotivation
-
*. Correlation significant on a 0.05 (2-tailed) level.
**. Correlation significant on a 0.01 (2-tailed) level.
Table 2: Correlations among Sports Motivation Scales (SMS)
3 Results
23
The results confirmed the simplex pattern (Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang,
2003; Li & Harmer, 1996) of the SMS. As the different types of motivation are assumed to lie
on a continuum, categories further away from each other must have a lower correlation
whereas categories nearer each other must have a higher correlation. This pattern can be
found in the present sample, however, the motivation type Extrinsic motivation - introjected
is the exception as only one of its correlations fits this pattern. All correlations were
significant with the exception of amotivation which was insignificant in four correlations.
3.4 Relationships between perceived coaching behaviors and observed coaching
behaviors
Observed coaching behavior
Perceived coaching behavior Correlation N
Positive reinforcement .20** 176
Non-reinforcement -.08 176
Mistake-contingent encouragement .20** 176
Mistake-contingent technical instruction .03 176
Punishment .31** 176
Punitive technical instruction .09 175
Ignoring mistakes .02 176
**. Correlation significant on a 0.01 (2-tailed) level.
Table 3: Significant correlations between observed coaching behaviors and perceived coaching behaviors
For positive reinforcement a positive correlation was present between this observed coaching
category and the perceptions of the players for this category. (r = .20, r = 4%, p=.008)
Mistake-contingent encouragement showed a positive correlation (r = .20, r = 4%, p=.007)
between the observations and the perceptions of this coaching category. Punishment had a
positive correlation between the observations and the perceptions for this coaching category.
(r = .31, r = 9.4%, p=0.00) All other correlations were non-significant.
3.5 Relationships between observed coaching behaviors and different types of
motivation
Of the 70 possible correlations six are significant. Non-reinforcement positively correlated
with Intrinsic motivation - to know (r = .19, r = 3.0%, p=.01), Intrinsic motivation - to
accomplish (r = .16, r = 2.6%, p=.04), Extrinsic motivation - external regulation (r = .16, r =
2.6%, p=.03). Positive reinforcement negatively correlated with Extrinsic motivation:
Introjected (r = -.16, r = 2.6%, p=.03) and Extrinsic motivation - External regulation (r = -
3 Results
24
.19, r = 3.0%, p=.01). Ignoring mistakes positively correlated with Amotivation (r = .15, r =
2.3%, p=.04).
Intrinsic
motivation
- to know
Intrinsic
motivation
- to
accomplish
Intrinsic
motivation
to
experience
stimulation
Extrinsic
motivation
- identified
Extrinsic
motivation
-
introjected
Extrinsic
motivation
- external
regulation
Amotivation
Positive
reinforcement
-.07
-.04
-.12
-.07
-.16
*
-.19
*
-.15
Non-reinforcement
.19
*
.16
*
.13
.05
.09
.16
*
-.03
Mistake-contingent
encouragement
-.01
.05
-.05
-.07
-.04
-.11
-.03
Mistake-contingent
technical
instruction
-.05
.06
-.07
-.11
-.05
-.07
-.04
Punishment
.06
.04
.06
-.08
-.00
-.05
.03
Punitive technical
instruction
.01
.00
-.01
-.09
.03
-.07
.06
Ignoring mistakes
-.01
-.10
-.01
-.05
.09
-.03
.15
*
General technical
instruction
-.02
-.03
-.08
-.02
-.07
-.11
-.12
General
encouragement
-.00
.03
-.08
-.04
-.05
-.09
-.08
Organization
.02
-.09
.01
-.14
-.02
-.12
-.06
*. Significant on a 0.05 (2-tailed) level.
Table 4: Correlations between observed coaching behaviors and different types of motivation
3.6 Relationships between perceived coaching behaviors and different types of
motivation
Positive reinforcement correlated positively with Intrinsic motivation - to accomplish (r = .15,
r = 2.3%, p=.05) and Intrinsic motivation - to experience stimulation (r = .20, r = 4.9%,
p=.01). Ignoring mistakes negatively correlated with Intrinsic motivation - to accomplish (r= -
.17, r = 2.9%, p=.035). Amotivation positively correlated with 4 different coaching
behaviors. Non-reinforcement (r = .23, r = 5.2%, p=.001), punishment (r = .16, r = 2.5%,
3 Results
25
p=.04), punitive technical instructions (r = .26, r = 6.6%, p=.001) and ignoring mistakes (r =
.16, r = 2.6%, p=.04). All other correlations were insignificant.
Intrinsic
motivation
- to know
Intrinsic
motivation
- to
accomplish
Intrinsic
motivation
to
experience
stimulation
Extrinsic
motivation
- identified
Extrinsic
motivation -
introjected
Extrinsic
motivation
- external
regulation
Amotivation
Positive
reinforcement
.06
.15
*
.20
**
.12
.12
-.04
-.13
Non-reinforcement
-.00
-.06
-.10
-.10
-.13
.06
.23
**
Mistake-contingent
encouragement
.09
.10
.05
.02
.11
-.08
-.04
Mistake-contingent
technical
instruction
-.01
.08
.10
-.01
.08
-.09
.02
Punishment
.09
.04
.08
.06
.08
.10
.16
*
Punitive technical
instruction
-.00
-.05
.00
-.07
-.02
-.01
.26
**
Ignoring mistakes
-.13
-.17
*
-.11
-.05
-.10
-.05
.16
*
Table 5: Correlations between perceived coaching behaviors and different types of motivation
3.7 Regression analysis
3.7.1 Regression analyses of observed coaching behaviors, potential confounding
variables and different types of motivation
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.008) of Intrinsic motivation to know is
6.9%, leaving over 90% to influences of variables outside the model. Playing experience ( =
-.19, p=.02) is one of the two significant variables in the model indicating that the higher the
playing experience, the lower this type of motivation in the participants. Punishment ( = .25,
p=.03) is the second significant variable in this model. A higher presence of this category lead
to a higher value in Intrinsic motivation - to know.
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.04) of Intrinsic motivation to
accomplish is 4.2%. The influence of the placement in the table ( = .42, p=.01) indicates that
3 Results
26
if a team is placed in the upper half of their table, this kind of behavior is significantly more
often displayed by the coaches. More positive reinforcement ( = .-32, p=.03) leads to less
motivation of this kind. General encouragement ( = .27, p=.05) leads to more Intrinsic
motivation - to accomplish, the more often it is displayed. More frequently displayed
coaching behavior of Organization ( = .-21, p=.01) lowers this kind of motivation.
The regression model of Intrinsic motivation to experience stimulation is non-significant
whereas the explained variance of the regression model (p=.002) of Extrinsic motivation -
Intrinsic
motivation -
to know
Intrinsic
motivation -
to
accomplish
Intrinsic
motivation
to
experience
stimulation
Extrinsic
motivation -
identified
Extrinsic
motivation -
introjected
Extrinsic
motivation -
external
regulation
Amotivation
Positive
reinforcement
-.32*
-.17*
Non-
reinforcement
Mistake-
contingent
encouragement
-.88**
Mistake-
contingent
technical
instruction
Punishment
.25*
Punitive
technical
instruction
-.35*
Ignoring
mistakes
.17*
General
technical
instruction
.43*
General
encouragement
.27*
Organization
-.21*
-.36**
Playing league
Last games
result
-.33*
-.21*
Observed
games result
Placement in
the table
.42**
Age
Playing
experience
-.19*
.24**
R
6.9%**
4.2%*
0.9%
9.7%**
2.7%*
2.1%*
2.4%*
* Significant on a 0.05 (2-tailed) level.
3 Results
27
** Significant on a 0.01 (2-tailed) level.
Table 6: Regression analyses of the ten observed coaching behaviors, the six potential confounding variables
and the seven different types of motivation
identified is 9.7%. Playing experience in years ( = .24, p=.002) has an increasing influence
on this kind of motivation indicating that the higher the experience, the lower this kind of
motivation. The last games result ( = -.33, p=.02) indicates that if the last game was won
this Extrinsic motivation - identified was lower than for those players who did not win. The
coaching behavior, Mistake-contingent encouragement, ( = -.88, p=.003) lead to a
significantly lower motivation of this type, the more often it was displayed which was also the
case for Punitive technical instruction ( = -.35, p=.03) and Organization ( = -.36, p=.001).
General technical instruction ( = .43, p=.02) lead, the more often it was displayed to more
Extrinsic motivation - identified.
The explained variance of the model (p=.04) of Extrinsic motivation external
regulation is 2.1%. The only variable left in the model is the observed coaching behavior,
Positive reinforcement ( = -.17, p=.04), which lead to higher Extrinsic motivation - external
regulation, the more often it was displayed.
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.03) of Amotivation is 2.4%. The
only significant variable left in the model is the observed coaching behavior, Ignoring
mistakes ( = .17, p=.03), which means that the more the coach ignores mistakes the more
amotivated the players are.
3.7.2 Regression analyses of perceived coaching behaviors, potential confounding
variables and different types of motivation
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.004) of Intrinsic motivation to know is
5.5%. The only significant variable, Playing experience in years, ( = .23, p=.004) indicates
that the higher the playing experience, the lower this kind of motivation.
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.02) of Intrinsic motivation to
accomplish is 3.5%. The only significant influence of any variable is the influence of the
perceived coaching behavior: Ignoring mistakes ( = -.17, p=.04) which indicates that the
higher this kind of behavior the lower Intrinsic motivation - to accomplish.
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.01) of Intrinsic motivation to
experience stimulation is 4.1%. The perceived coaching behavior, positive reinforcement, (
= -.18, p=.03) leads, the more often it is displayed, to more Intrinsic motivation - to
experience stimulation and is the only variable with a significant influence.
3 Results
28
The explained variance of the regression model (p=.002) of Extrinsic motivation -
identified is 8.0%. Players age in years ( = .26, p=.001) had an increasing influence for this
kind of motivation, the higher the age of the players the lower this kind of motivation. If the
observed game ( = .23, p=.05) was won, Extrinsic motivation - identified was higher. The
more Non-reinforcement ( = .-16, p=.04) was displayed by the coach, the lower was this
kind of motivation. The explained variance of the model (p=.01) of Extrinsic motivation
introjected is 4.2%. If the last game ( = -.18, p=.02) was won this kind of motivation was
Intrinsic
motivation
- to know
Intrinsic
motivation
- to
accomplish
Intrinsic
motivation
to
experience
stimulation
Extrinsic
motivation
- identified
Extrinsic
motivation
-
introjected
Extrinsic
motivation
- external
regulation
Amotivation
Positive
reinforcement
.18*
Non-
reinforcement
-.16*
Mistake-
contingent
encouragement
Mistake-
contingent
technical
instruction
Punishment
Punitive
technical
instruction
.23**
Ignoring
mistakes
-.17*
.15*
Playing league
Last games
result
-.18*
-.16*
Observed
games result
.23*
Placement in
the table
Age
.26**
Playing
experience
-.23**
R
5.5%**
3.5%*
4.1%*
8.0%**
4.2%*
3.4%
9.9%**
* Significant on a 0.05 (2-tailed) level.
** Significant on a 0.01 (2-tailed) level.
Table 7: Regression analyses of the seven perceived coaching behaviors, the six potential confounding variables
and seven different types of motivation
lower. The regression model of Extrinsic motivation external regulation was non-
significant. The explained variance of the model (p=.001) of Amotivation is 9.9%. Punitive
technical instruction ( = .23, p=.003) and Ignoring mistakes ( = .15, p=.05) lead the more
3 Results
29
often they were displayed by the coaches to more amotivation in the players. The last games
result ( = -.16, p=.04) had the statistically significant influence, that if the last game was
won, Amotivation was lower in the players.
4 Discussion
30
4 Discussion
4.1 Findings of the study and limitations of the CBAS results
Altogether, the results based on the study designs and samples used do not support the
hypotheses which were formulated before the study was carried out. The main hypothesis that
the different observed coaching behaviors have a strong influence on different motivational
concepts of handball players could not be verified on the basis of the obtained results.
Of the seventy possible correlations of the ten coaching behaviors with the seven
motivational categories only six were significant. Three of these significant correlations were
related to the item non-reinforcement which was found to be not reliable in the test-retest
procedure. Therefore, analyzing these results seems to be not useful as the validity and
reliability of the results obtained cannot be trusted and have to be seen very critical. However,
the findings that a higher amount of displayed non-reinforcement by the coach leads to more
intrinsic motivation - to accomplish and more intrinsic motivation - to know is contradictory
to findings stating that it is in fact reinforcement which increases players intrinsic motivation
(Vallerand, 1983; Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Cameron & Pierce, 1994).
In line with cognitive evaluation theory (Deci & Ryan, 1980), this may be because of
the increased perceived competence which derives from reinforcement following desired
outcomes. However, this relationship could not be proven based on the findings in this study.
Therefore it is especially surprising based on the tenets of self-determination theory that
positive reinforcement decreased Extrinsic motivation - Introjected and Extrinsic motivation -
External regulation, but it does not increase intrinsic motivation. Ignoring mistakes positively
correlated with Amotivation indicating that a player which is not given feedback after
mistakes may feel less competent and also the need for relatedness may be decreased leading
to lower self-determination.
The relationship of the eight perceived coaching categories and the seven types of
motivation showed seven significant correlations which do all support the tenets of self-
determination theory. Positive reinforcement increased Intrinsic motivation - to accomplish
and Intrinsic motivation - to experience stimulation which may be because of the increased
perceived competence. Ignoring mistakes may have lead to a lower competence leading to a
decreased Intrinsic motivation - to accomplish. Ignorance of undesired behaviors may also
contradict the universal need for relatedness as the athlete may feel neglected and also may
wish to have clear instructional feedback on how to perform better next time to increase his
4 Discussion
31
competence. This decrease of competence may also very well be the reason for increase in
amotivation following the four different negatively oriented coaching behaviors: Non-
reinforcement, punishment, punitive technical instructions and ignoring mistakes. It has to be
outlined that the explained variance of all significant relationships is small.
Before conducting this correlation study different potential confounding factors have
been considered. As the influence of the observed coaching behaviors and the perceived
coaching behaviors were found to be marginal for most correlations it might be especially
interesting to look at other factors that might have an influence on the players motivation, as
these may represent parts of the large unexplained variance which has an influence on
motivation. However, the separate regression analyses for all seven motivational categories as
proposed by self-determination theory also showed not one single model with an explained
variance of more than 10%. The model with the highest explained variance (9.9%) was the
amotivation model in relation to the potential confounding variables and the perceived
coaching behaviors. All three significant variables may have had an influence on amotivation
through the basic need of perceived competence which may be increased through Punitive
technical instruction and Ignoring mistakes and decreased through the positive feedback on
competence after winning the last game.
The explained variance of the regression model of the observed coaching behaviors,
the potential confounding variables and Extrinsic motivation - identified was 9.7%. Playing
experience in years may have an increasing influence on this kind of motivation as with a
higher age and a higher experience it is less the destiny for achievements which leads the
athletes but rather the conviction of its usefulness. The last games result may at the same
time decrease a wish to win the next game if the last one was already won. This finding goes
in line with achievement goal theory (Nicholls, 1984, 1989) which differentiates between
judging competence in a more self-referenced manner where the self-improvement is in the
center of interest (task-involvement) or putting emphasis on a normative fashion (such as the
playing result in the study) which is concerned with ego-involvement. Ego-involved people
were found to be driven less by intrinsic kinds of motivation but more driven by extrinsic
kinds of motivation, which supports the results obtained in this study (Dweck, 1986; Nicholls,
1984, 1989; Balaguer, Duda, Atienza, & Mayo, 2002). The position of the coach has also
been outlined as an important influence on whether individuals have more task-involvement
or ego-involvement in the context of sport (Ames, 1992b; Nicholls, 1989).
4 Discussion
32
Of the impact of the three coaching behaviors, Mistake-contingent encouragement,
Punitive technical instruction and Organization, only the impact of punitive technical
instruction goes in line with the tenets of self-determination theory as it may again decrease
perceived competence leading to non-identified values and goals. The fact that general
technical instruction the more often it was displayed, lead to more Extrinsic motivation -
identified may be attributed to the players need to get valuable instructions to increase their
competence and to feel related through this kind of feedback. The influence of the perceived
coaching behaviors and the potential confounders on Extrinsic motivation - identified
explained 8% of the variance in this regression model. In this model it was again the case that
higher players age lead to a higher kind of identification with extrinsic motivation. The
influence of a win in the observed game may lead to more identification through the
perception of competence, whereas the frequent display of Non-reinforcement may again
decrease this feeling of competence. It has to be outlined that the explained variance of all
regression models is lower than 10% leaving over 90% unexplained in every model.
An additional finding is that the observed coaching behaviors and the coaching
behaviors as perceived by the players correlated significantly in only three of the seven
possible correlations with consequently four coaching behavior categories where the
perceptions of the players and the observations of the coaching behaviors did not correlate
significantly indicating quite a big difference in observations and perceptions. It is especially
noteworthy that the most significant correlation was related to punishment, with the other
category, mistake-contingent technical instruction, also focusing on an undesired behavior
which means that the focus is on an undesired negative behavior in two of the three cases.
This finding indicates that negative feedback may be especially present in the perception of
players. At the same time, this finding also suggests a possible presence of reactivity in the
coaching behaviors during observations (Landsberger, 1958). However, this can only be
partially confirmed. The questionnaires administered to the last eight teams included the
additional question if the players were of the opinion that their coach was behaving and giving
feedback in the same way as usual. This idea came up when a few players stated that their
coach behaved more friendly during the observation. Yet of the ninety-six players who
answered this question only nine (9.4%) players stated that their coach behaved unusual
which indicates a rather low, however present reactivity.
The three differences in coaching behaviors between games and training sessions all
seem to make sense as for example the coach in the case of the already controversially
4 Discussion
33
discussed non-reinforcement category might feel that potential desired behaviors need to be
reinforced more in training sessions where the voice and feedback of the coach is more
present than in game situations. Additionally, he might feel that the result is already a
reinforcement or non-reinforcement during games and additional reinforcement might not be
needed to the extent present in training sessions. The category organization is much more
present in training situations, in fact nearly twice as often, which can be easily explained with
the coaches most important duties of organizing training sessions. Giving tactical and
technical instructions is assumed to be of higher importance in games (Cassidy, Jones, &
Potrac, 2009).
Mistake-contingent technical instruction was significantly more often displayed
during training sessions which also makes sense as technical instruction is a coaching
behavior which is typically done on an individual basis during training sessions were the
coach can take his time to focus more on behaviors which improve players techniques or
performances in the long term. In comparison, coaching behaviors during games typically
focus on achieving a good result in the particular game by giving technical and tactical
instructions which directly should have an influence on the teams performance on the team
level (Martens, 2004).
The findings (Vangucci, Potrac, & Jones, 1997; Lacy & Darst, 1989; Sherman &
Hassan, 1986) that a higher expertise in top-level coaches leads to more specific feedback,
such as technical and tactical feedback, whereas coaches of lower leagues and qualification
tend to be more general in their feedback (Wandzilak, Ansorge, & Potter, 1988) including a
higher amount of punishment related behaviors (Conroy, & Coatsworth, 2007a) could also not
be verified based on the results of the study as no significant differences in coaching were
detected across leagues.
The reliability of the Sports Motivation Scale (SMS) for the present sample was
acceptable for all items supporting the validity and reliability reported in other studies.
(Chatzisarantis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang, 2003; Li & Harmer, 1996) It has to be stated
that the inter-rater reliability of the CBAS (Smith, Smoll, & Hunt, 1977) was found to be high
in eight of the ten categories. These results support the high test-retest reliability found by
other authors (Smith, Smoll, & Hunt, 1977; Barnes, 2003) as the independent application and
comparison of two different observers which was the case in this study can be regarded as a
test-retest procedure. The coaching behavior organization was only found to be not reliable
during game situations. The reason for this might be that although the observers always had a
4 Discussion
34
very practical placement during the games to listen to the coaches behaviors and statements,
the acoustics in games are much more disturbing when trying to filter out the organizational
statements by the coaches than in the training situations where only the team is present.
Additionally, organizational coaching behaviors during games seem to be more on an
individual level as the coaches know about the acoustic problems, i.e. that not all players may
be able to hear them. When, for example, a coach tells a player on which position he has to
play, this is done on a calm acoustic level with which the game specific noises stated before
interfere. In contrast to this, the organizational statements of the coach during training
sessions in general are more on a team level, addressing big parts of the team as they typically
contain explanations of exercises and drills. These differences between game and training
situations may lead to the very high agreement between the two observers for the category
organization during training sessions and the much lower agreement during games (Appendix
10 and 11).
However, the category non-reinforcement as stated in the literature review was found
to be especially hard to distinguish in the filed setting of team handball. As non-reinforcement
is defined as the answer to a desired behavior, the ultimate question which arises is: What is a
desired behavior in handball? The two observers decided to include only significant
positive actions, like scoring a goal, saving a penalty or giving a crucial pass which leads to a
good chance for the own team. However, it has to be admitted that following this procedure,
desired behavior is not clearly defined which may have lead to low reliability of the
category non-reinforcement in both training and game situations.
4.2 General limitations of the study
As the correlations between coaching behaviors and different motivational concepts which
were obtained are fewer and those few which were found are much weaker than expected the
question evolves what may have lead to these rather unexpected results.
A possible explanation could be found in the data gathering procedure of this work.
The correlation study cannot give any prove for causality of the influence of certain coaching
behaviors. Taking the Hierarchical Model of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation (Vallerand,
1997) as the basis, it has to be made clear that the different instruments do not all measure the
same levels of motivation. The Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS) (Smith,
Smoll, & Hunt, 1977) measures different coaching behaviors in a particular situation, in the
case of the study in one training session and one game, whereas the Coaching Feedback
4 Discussion
35
Questionnaire (CFQ) (Horn & Glenn, 1988) tries to assess the coaching behaviors as they are
perceived by the players on a contextual level. The CFQ focuses on the question how typical
certain coaching behaviors are for their coach, which means in general and in more than this
particular training session. The sport motivation scale (Pelletier et al., 1995) also measures
motivation on a contextual rather than on a situational level. So two of the analyzed variables
are located on the contextual level of handball (SMS + CFQ) whereas the observational
variable (CBAS) is located on the situational level, which might have had an influence on the
results. Although top-down effects from the contextual level to the situational level have been
reported (Vallerand, 2007), situational factors concern transient variables encountered in a
specific activity, at a specific time, that may not remain constant (Vallerand, 2007, p. 263)
this is a crucial finding as this points to the difference between the more stable contextual
factors and the less constant situational factors, i.e. the coaching behavior in one particular
training session or game.
Additionally, the assessment of motivation through the SMS can only point to the
motivation stated by the players at one particular point in time. Further research might address
this issue by assessing a development of motivation and its relation to different coaching
behaviors displayed over a longer period of time. This was not possible in the present study as
this study was rather describing the status quo of motivation in relation to different coaching
behaviors and no baseline results were present. It might very well be the case that for example
reinforcing coaching behaviors increased the intrinsic motivation of a particular team with a
lower baseline intrinsic motivation although this increase could not be detected by the present
design as this design just compared the obtained values in relation to the other teams.
Therefore, to assess whether such effects exist for one particular team repeated measure
designs are necessary.
The study also implicitly assumes that the different coaching behaviors with their
influence on motivation are a question of quantity, however what might be of bigger impact is
the quality of the feedback given by the coach (Muraven, Gagn, & Rosman, 2008) and its
perception by the players. Additionally, the study solely focuses on the influence of the coach
and therefore neglects possible influences of family, friends, referees or other team members
which might also have an influence on the motivation of each individual player. So of all ten
possible sources of competence information as proposed by Horn, Glenn, and Wentzell
(1993) the coach is only one. However, a second social factor source which was tested in the
study was the result of the game.
4 Discussion
36
The Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS) (Smith, Smoll, & Hunt, 1977)
measures verbal and non-verbal feedback given by the coach. Although the Coaching
Feedback Questionnaire (CFQ) developed on the basis of the CBAS was created as a
questionnaire form, it focuses on verbal feedback (Appendix 22). The only two non-verbal
categories which are tested in the CFQ are related to ignoring desired or undesired behaviors
by the athletes. Body language is not assessed in the CFQ, however, it is measured by the
CBAS when, for example, the coach shakes his head. These differences between the two
measurement instruments may have also lead to differences in the assessment.
Additionally, the recorded coaching behaviors by the CBAS are recorded on a general
level which means that they were directed to the team as a whole. No differentiation was
made on whether one player was addressed directly or not. In contrast to this procedure the
CFQ can be interpreted as if the behaviors had been directed at the player who fills out the
questionnaire (e.g. Great play! Now youre keeping your eyes on the ball). This difference
in the CBAS assessment procedure and the CFQ assessment procedure may explain the
different results for players of the same coach in the perceptions of his coaching behavior.
Based on the quantitative assessment of the observations, a dose-response of
autonomy support is assumed as more reinforcement and may, for example, lead to higher
perceived competence by the athletes. However, this solely quantitative approach of assessing
coaching behaviors neglects the individual context of each coaching behavior displayed.
Consistent reinforcements given for an activity that is already perceived as intrinsically
motivating may lead to an overjustification effect (Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973; Enzle &
Ross, 1978; Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999) and so reinforcing behaviors by a coach have to
be considered context-specific, which cannot be done in a simple quantitative observational
assessment. Further research therefore should try to assess the individual context in which
certain coaching behaviors are displayed in order to gain a more profound insight into their
influence.
The study design also assesses the influence of the coaching behaviors solely with
regard to their effect on motivation. Therefore, based on the study design no conclusions can
be drawn as to whether these coaching behaviors are effective for skill learning or
performance improvement.
Any kind of motivation, be it extrinsic or intrinsic, be it more autonomous or less
autonomous cannot be considered as being of higher value per se. Individual predispositions
towards achievement as being characterized in the achievement goal framework of Elliot and
4 Discussion
37
McGregor (2001), which differentiates between mastery-approach and -avoidance goals as
well as performance-approach and -avoidance goals, and also in the Regulatory Focus Theory
(Higgins, 1997), which differentiates between promotion and prevention focus, may be of
high importance in the case of extrinsic motivation. Further research might rather focus on the
question if the individual achievement motivation direction (avoidance or performance focus)
is in line with the individual predispositions which might even have a stronger positive impact
than more self-determined motivation which is not in line with the individuals achievement
focus.
Besides quantifying the coaching behaviors, the additional knowledge of athletes
chronic regulatory orientation is of importance for the selection of athletes, the adjustment of
tactics and strategies, and coaches framing of instructions (Plessner, Unkelbach, Memmert,
Baltes, & Kolb, 2009, p. 108).
4.3 Further research recommendations
Further research which may aim at giving practical coaching recommendations may
need to assess the individual regulatory orientation of the players. Addressing the weaknesses
and limitations of the correlation design of this study, further research should also address the
aforementioned regulatory achievement orientations. This may be conducted in experimental
designs to assess whether these hypothesized relationships between motivation and individual
predispositions can be manipulated by different coaching behaviors. Research assessing the
long-term effects of the influence of coaching behaviors on motivation may also assess the
question whether the positive consequences of intrinsic or more self-determined motivation,
which have been mentioned by other authors for samples different than handball, may also be
present in handball specific samples like the one used in this study and whether the results
obtained can also be generalized to female handball players and other sport contexts.
The present study is conducted from two different perspectives, i.e. an observers view
on the coaching behaviors, and the players view on the coaching behaviors and on their own
motivation. It would be interesting to have the coaches view on both, i.e. his perceptions of
his own coaching behavior and of his athletes motivation, as this would give further insight
on possible differences in perceptions of external observers, players and the coach.
Additionally, the coaching experience of the coach and the years of responsibility for the
tested teams would be of interest in this context.
4 Discussion
38
The design of the present study analyses the coach-established climate as an
antecedent of the athletes motivation and thereby completely neglects the possibility that
coaching and the motivational climate may be an interactive process where athletes
motivation might have an influence on the coaching behaviors as well as the coaching
behaviors on the athletes motivation (Sarrazin, Boiche, & Pelletier, 2007).
Even in repeated measurement designs, the quantity of measurements typically does
not exceed more than two measurements (in the case of the study, the measurements have
been restricted to two, one training and one game situation). However, to assess typical
coaching behaviors over a longer period of time or to assess development in motivation, it
may be necessary to rely on measurement procedures which do not make a presence of the
observers or raters necessary. One such method which has been proposed is the use of diary
studies (Gagn & Blanchard, 2007) which requires athletes to recall what they felt during a
number of training sessions and average these feelings over a period of time (e.g. in the past
month) (Gagn & Blanchard, 2007, p. 249).
Based on the obtained results no recommendation for coaching behaviors of handball
can be given, as the expected relations of encouraging and reinforcing behaviors and
motivation could not be proven in the present sample. The present study may give a valuable
insight into the relationships between observed coaching behaviors, perceived coaching
behaviors and motivation for a particular sample of handball players on the grounds of self-
determination theory. Based on the hypotheses made before the study and the review of
literature, the results have to be described as rather surprising as nearly all assumed
correlations were not present and those significant correlations which were present were only
of marginal effects for this specific sample. However, these results should not be interpreted
as a denial of the coaches influence on athletes motivation. Whether and in how far the
results of the chosen sample can be generalized for handball players of all playing levels,
possibly also including professional handball should be analyzed in further research. More
sophisticated study designs than the present correlation design which do not only examine a
specific point in time will have to be developed to understand the very complex interplay
through which coaches influence their athletes motivation.
39
Bibliography
Alexandris, K., Tsorbatzoudis, C., & Grouios, G. (2002). Perceived constraints on
recreational sport participation: Investigating their relationship with intrinsic motivation,
extrinsic motivation and amotivation. Journal of Leisure Research, 34, 233-252.
Allen, J.B., & Howe, B.L. (1998). Player ability, coach feedback, and female adolescent
athletes perceived competence and satisfaction. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
20, 280-299.
Ames, C. (1992a). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and motivational processes. In
G.C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 161-176). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Ames, C. (1992b). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 84, 261-271.
Amorose, A.J., & Horn, T.S. (2000). Intrinsic motivation: Relationships with collegiate
athletes gender, scholarship status, and perceptions of their coaches behavior. Journal of
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 22, 63-84.
Amorose, A.J., & Smith, P. (2003). Feedback as a source of physical competence
information: Effects of age, experience and type of feedback. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 25, 341-359.
Amorose, A.J., & Weiss, M.R. (1998). Coaching feedback as a source of information about
perceptions of ability: A developmental examination. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 20, 395-420.
Amorose, A.J. (2007). Coaching Effectiveness. Exploring the Relationship Between Coaching
Behavior and Self-Determined Motivation. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.),
40
Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 209-227). Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.
Balaguer, I., Duda, J.L., Atienza, F.L., & Mayo, C. (2002). Situational and dispositional goals
as predictors of perceptions of individual and team improvement, satisfaction and coach
ratings among elite female handball teams. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3, 293-308.
Barnes, K.A. (2003). NCAA Division I Athletes Coaching Behavior Preferences.
Unpublished masters thesis. University of North Texas, Denton, Texas, USA.
Barnett, N., Smith, R.L., & Smoll, F. (1992). Effects of enhancing coach-athlete relationships
on youth sport attrition. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 111-127.
Baumeister, R.F., & Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 925-945.
Biddle, S.J.H., & Mutrie, N. (2001). Psychology of physical activity: Determinants, well-
being and interventions. London: Routledge.
Black, S.J., & Weiss, M.R. (1992). The relationship among perceived coaching behaviors,
perceptions of ability, and motivation in competitive age-group swimmers. Journal of Sport
and Exercise Psychology, 14, 309-325.
Blais, M.R., Vallerand, R.J., Gagnon, A., Briere, N.M., & Pelletier, L.G. (1990). Significance,
structure, and gender differences in life domains of college students. Sex Roles, 22, 199-212.
Bredemeier, B.J.L., & Shields, D.L.L. (1993). Moral psychology in the context of sport. In R.
Singer, M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology
(pp. 587-599). New York: Macmillan.
Cassidy, T., Jones, R., & Potrac, P. (2009). Understanding Sports Coaching: The Social,
Cultural and Pedagogical Foundations of Coaching Practice. Abingdon: Routledge.
41
Chantal, Y., Guay, F., Dobreva-Martinova, T., & Vallerand, R.J. (1996). Motivation and elite
performance: An exploratory investigation with Bulgarian athletes. International Journal of
Sport Psychology, 27, 173-182.
Chatzisarantis, N.L.D., Hagger, M.S., Biddle, S.J.H., Smith, B., & Wang, J.C.K. (2003). A
meta-analysis of perceived locus of causality in exercise, sport, and physical education
contexts. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 284-306.
Chelladurai, P., & Saleh, S.D. (1978). Preferred leadership in sports. Canadian Journal of
Applied Sciences, 3, 85-92.
Chelladurai, P. (1993). Leadership. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphy, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.),
Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 647-671). New York: Macmillan.
Clark, D.G., & Blair, S.N. (1988). Physical activity and the prevention of obesity in
childhood. In N.A. Krasneger, G.D. Grane, & N. Kretchnar (Eds.), Childhood obesity: A
biobehavioral perspective (pp. 121-142). Caldwell, NJ: Telford.
Connell, J.P., & Wellborn, J.G. (1991). Competence, autonomy, and relatedness: A
motivational analysis of self-system processes. In M.R. Gunnar & L.A. Sroufe (Eds.),
Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, Vol. 23 (pp. 43-77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Conroy, D.E., & Coatsworth, J.D. (2007a). Assessing Autonomy-Supportive Coaching
Strategies in Youth Sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 671-684.
Conroy, D.E., & Coatsworth, J.D. (2007b). Coaching Behaviors Associated With Changes in
Fear of Failure: Changes in Self-Talk and Need Satisfaction as Potential Mechanisms.
Journal of Personality, 75, 383-419.
Cushion, C.J., & Jones, R.L. (2001). A systematic observation of professional toplevel youth
soccer coaches. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24, 354-376.
42
Darst, P.W., Zakrajsek, D.B., & Mancini, V.H. (Eds.) (1989). Analyzing physical education
and sport instruction (2nd edition), Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Deci, E.L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105-115.
Deci, E.L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1980). The empirical exploration of intrinsic motivational
processes. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 13,
pp. 39-80). New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985a). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (1985b). The general causality orientations scale: Self-
determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109-134.
Deci, E.L. & Ryan, R.M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality.
In R.A. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on motivation: Perspectives on motivation
(Vol. 38, pp. 237-288). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Deci, E.L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R.M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments
examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
125, 627-68.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs and
self-determination of behavior. Psychology Inquiry, 11, 227-268.
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2002). Handbook of Self-Determination Research: Theoretical and
Applied Issues. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
43
Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2007). Active Human Nature. Self-Determination Theory and the
Promotion and Maintenance of Sport, Exercise, and Health. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D.
Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport
(pp. 1-19). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Doganis, G. (2000). Development of a Greek version of the Sport Motivation Scale.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 505-512.
Dweck, C.S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41,
1040-1048.
Elliot, A.J., & McGregor, H.A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501-519.
Enzle, M.A., & Ross, J.M. (1978). Increasing and decreasing intrinsic interest with contingent
rewards: A test of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
14, 588-597.
Fortier, M., & Grenier, M. (1999). Determinants personnels et situationnels de ladherence a
un programme dexercise: Un etude prospective. Revue STAPS, 49, 25-38.
Fortier, M., Sweet, S., Tulloch, H., Blanchard, C., Sigal, R., & Kenny, G. (2006). Self-
determination and exercise stage progression: A longitudinal study. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Frederick, C., Morrison, C., & Manning, T. (1996). Motivation to participate, exercise affect,
and outcome behaviors toward physical activity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 691-701.
Gagn, M., Blanchard, C. (2007). Self-Determination Theory and Well-Being in Athletes: Its
the Situation That Counts. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic
44
Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 243-254). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Gagn, M., Ryan, R.M. & Bargmann, K. (2003). Autonomy support and need satisfaction in
the motivation and well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 372-
390.
Georgiadis, M.M., Biddle, S.J.H., & Chatzisarantis, N.L.D. (2001). The mediating role of
self-determination in the relationship between goal orientations and physical worth in Greek
exercisers. European Journal of Sport Sciences, 1, 1-9.
Hagger, M.S., Chatzisarantis, N.L.D., Barkoukis, V., Wang, C.K.J., & Baranowski, J. (2005).
Perceived autonomy support in physical education and leisure-time physical activity: A cross-
cultural evaluation of the trans-contextual model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97,
376-390.
Hagger, M.S., Chatzisarantis, N.L.D., Culverhouse, T., & Biddle, S.J.H. (2003). The
processes by which perceived autonomy support in physical education promotes leisure-time
physical activity intentions and behavior: A trans-contextual model. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 95, 784-795.
Hamer, M., Karageorghis, C.I., & Vlachopoulos, S.P. (2002). Motives for exercise
participation as predictors of exercise dependence among endurance athletes. Journal of
Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 42, 233-238.
Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a development model. Human
Development, 21, 34-64.
Higgins, E.T. (1997). Beyond Pleasure and Pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280-1300.
45
Horn, T.S. (1987). The influence of teacher-coach behavior on the psychological development
of children. In D. Gould & M.R. Weiss (Eds.), Advances in pediatric sport sciences. Vol. 2:
Behavioral issues (pp. 121-142). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Horn, T.S. (1988). The relationship between athletes psychological characteristics and their
preference for particular coaching behaviors. Paper presented at the meeting of the North
American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity, Knoxville, TN.
Horn, T.S., Glenn, S.D., & Wentzell, A.B. (1993). Sources of information underlying
personal ability judgements in high school athletes. Pediatric Exercise Science, 5, 263-274.
Horn, T.S., & Glenn, S. (1988). The relationship between athletes psychological
characteristics and their preference for particular coaching behaviors. Paper presented at the
meeting of the North American Society for the Psychology of Sport and Physical Activity,
Knoxville, TN.
Horn, T.S. (2002). Coaching effectiveness in the sport domain. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances
in sport psychology (pp. 309-354). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ingledew, D.K., Markland, D., & Medley, A.R. (1998). Exercise motives and stages of
change. Journal of Health Psychology, 3, 477-489.
Jackson, S.A., Kimiecik, J.C., Ford, S.K., & Marsh, L.W. (1998). Psychological correlates of
flow in sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 20, 358-378.
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives.
Psychological Review, 82, 1-25.
Kowal, J., & Fortier, M.S. (1999). Motivational determinants of flow: Contributions from
self-determination theory. Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 355-368.
46
Kowal, J. & Fortier, M.S. (2000). Testing relationships from the hierarchical model of
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation using flow as a motivational consequence. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 171-181.
Kowal, J. & Fortier, M.S. (2005). Determinants of physical activity behavior change in
middle-aged women: Integrating concepts of self-determination theory and theory of planned
behavior. Unpublished manuscript, University of Ottawa.
Lacy, A.C., & Darst, P.W. (1985). Systematic observation of behaviors of winning high
school head football coaches. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 4, 256-270.
Lacy, A.C., & Darst, P.W. (1989). The Arizona State University Observation Instrument
(ASUOI). In P.W. Darst, D.B. Zakrajsek, & V.H. Mancini (Eds.), Analyzing physical
education and sport instruction: Second edition, (pp. 369-378).
Landsberger, H. (1958). Hawthorne revisited. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press.
Lepper, M.R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R.E. (1973). Undermining childrens interest with
extrinsic reward: A test of the overjustification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 28, 129-137.
Li, F., & Harmer, P. (1996). Testing the simplex assumption underlying the sport motivation
scale: A structure equation modeling analysis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67,
396-405.
Li, F. (1999). The Exercise Motivation Scale: Its multifaceted structure and construct validity.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 11, 97-115.
Mageau G.A., & R.J. Vallerand (2003). The coach-athlete relationship: a motivational model.
Journal of Sport Sciences, 21, 883-904.
47
Mallett, C., Kawabata, M., Newcombe, P., Otero-Forero, A., & Jackson, S. (2007). Sport
motivation scale-6 (SMS-6): A revised six-factor sport motivation scale. Psychology of Sport
and Exercise, 8, 600-614.
Markland, D., & Ingledew, D.K. (2007). Exercise Participation Motives. A Self-
Determination Theory Perspective. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic
Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 23-34). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Martens, M.P., & Webber, S.N. (2002). Psychometric properties of the sport motivation scale:
An evaluation with college varsity athletes from the U.S. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 24, 254-270.
Martens, R. (2004). Successful Coaching, 3rd Edition. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Martin, J.J., & Cutler, K. (2002). An exploratory study of flow and motivation in theatre
actors. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 344-352.
Matsumoto, H., Takenaka, K., & Tayaka, N. (2003). Development of the exercise motivation
scale for exercise adherence based on self-determination theory: Reliability and validity.
Japanese Journal of Health Promotion, 5, 120-129.
Millard, L. (1996). Differences in Coaching Behaviors of Male and Female High School
Soccer Coaches. Journal of Sport Behavior, 19, 19-31.
Mullan, E., Markland, D. (1997). Variations in self-determination across the stages of change
for exercise in adults. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 342-362.
Mullan, E., Markland, D., & Ingledew, D.K. (1997). A graded conceptualization of self-
determination in the regulation of exercise behaviour: Development of a measure using
confirmatory factor analytic procedures. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 745-752.
48
Muraven, M., Gagn, M., & Rosman, H. (2008). Helpful self-control: Autonomy support,
vitality, and depletion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 573-585.
Murray, M.C., & Mann, B.L. (2001). Leadership effectiveness. In J.M. Williams (Ed.),
Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak performance (4th ed., pp. 82-106). Palo
Alto, CA: Mayfield.
Nicaise, V., Cogerino, G., Bois, J., & Amorose, A.J. (2006). Students Perceptions of Teacher
Feedback and Physical Competence in Physical Education Classes: Gender Effects. Journal
of Teaching in Physical Education, 25, 36-57.
Nicholls, J.G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Ntoumanis, N. (2001). Empirical links between achievement goal theory and self-
determination theory in sport. Journal of Sport Sciences, 19, 397-409.
Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A prospective study of participation in optional school physical
education using a self-determination framework. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 444-
453.
Ommundsen, Y., & Bar-Eli, M. (1999). Psychological outcomes: Theories, research, and
recommendations for practice. In Y.V. Auweele, F.B. Bakker, S. Biddle, M. Durand, & R.
Seiler (Eds.), Psychology for Physical Education (pp. 13-114). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Pelletier, L.G., Fortier, M.S., Vallerand, R.J., Tuson, K.M., & Briere, N.M. (1995). Toward a
New Measure of Intrinsic Motivation, Extrinsic Motivation, and Amotivation in Sports: The
Sport Motivation Scale (SMS). Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 35-53.
49
Pelletier, L.G., Fortier, M.S., Vallerand, R.J., Briere, N.M. (2001). Associations among
perceived autonomy support, forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A prospective study.
Motivation and Emotion, 25, 279-306.
Pelletier, L.G., & Sarrazin, P. (2007). Measurement Issues in Self-Determination Theory and
Sport. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation and Self-
Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 143-152). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Pelletier, L.G., Vallerand, R.J., & Sarrazin, P. (2007). The revised six-factor Sport Motivation
Scale (Mallett, Kawabata, Newcombe, Otero-Forero, & Jackson, 2007): Something old,
something new, and something borrowed. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 615-621.
Plessner, H., Unkelbach, C., Memmert, D., Baltes, A., & Kolb, A. (2009). Regulatory fit as a
determinant of sport performance: How to succeed in a soccer penalty shooting. Psychology
of Sport and Exercise, 10, 108-115.
Raedeke, T.D., & Smith, A.L. (2001). Development and preliminary validation of an athlete
burnout measure. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 23, 281-306.
Richer, S., & Vallerand, R.J. (1998). Construction et validation de lEchelle du sentiment
dappartenance sociale. [Construction and validation of the Relatedness Feeling Scale.] Revue
europenne de psychologie applique/European Journal of Applied Psychology, 48, 129-137.
Riemer, H., Fink, J.S., & Fitzgerald, M.P. (2002). External validity of the sport motivation
scale. Avante, 8 (2), 57-66.
Ryan, R.M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motivation, autonomy and the self in
psychological development. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation
(Vol. 40, pp. 1-56). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Ryan, R.M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal
of Personality, 63, 397-427.
50
Ryan, R.M., & Connell, J.P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization:
Examining reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
57, 749-761.
Ryan, R.M. & Deci, E.L. (2007). Active Human Nature: Self-Determination Theory and the
Promotion and Maintenance of Sport, Exercise and Health. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D.
Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport
(pp. 1-19). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ryan, R.M., Frederick, C.M., Lepes, D., Rubio, N., & Sheldon, K. (1997). Intrinsic
motivation and exercise adherence. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 335-354.
Sarrazin, P., Boiche, C.S., & Pelletier, L.G. (2007). A Self-Determination Theory Approach
to Dropout in Athletes. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation
and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 229-241). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Sarrazin, P., Vallerand, R.J., Guillet, E., Pelletier, L.G., & Cury, F. (2002). Motivation and
drop-out in female handballers: A 21-month prospective study. European Journal of Social
Psychology, 32, 395-418.
Scanlan, T.K. (1986). Competitive stress in children. In M.R. Weiss, & D. Gould (Eds.),
Sport for children and youth (pp. 113-118). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Sherman, M.A., & Hassan, J.S. (1986). Behavioral studies of youth sport coaches. In M.
Pieron, & G. Graham (Eds.), The 1984 Olympic scientific congress proceedings (Vol. 6,
pp. 103-108). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Simons, J., Dewitte, S., & Lens, W. (2003). Dont do it for me. Do it for yourself! Stressing
the personal relevance enhances motivation in physical education. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 25, 145-160.
51
Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Hunt, E.B. (1977). A system for the behavioral assessment of
athletic coaches. Research Quarterly, 48, 401-407.
Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Hunt, E.B. (1977). Training Manual for the Coaching Behavior
Assessment System (CBAS). JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 7 (2).
Smith, R.E., & Smoll, F.L. (1990). Self-esteem and childrens reactions to youth sport
coaching: A field study of self-enhancement processes. Developmental Psychology, 26, 987-
993.
Smith, R.E., Zane, N.W.S., Smoll, F.L., & Coppel, D.B. (1983). Behavioral assessment in
youth sports: Coaching behaviors and childrens attitudes. Medicine and Science in Sports
and Exercise, 15, 208-214.
Smoll, F.L., & Smith, R.E. (2002). Coaching behavior research and intervention in youth
sport. In F.L. Smoll & R.E. Smith (Eds.), Children and youth in sport (pp. 211-231).
Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.
Standage, M., Duda, J.L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003). A model of contextual motivation in
physical education: Using constructs from self-determination and achievement goal theories
to predict physical activity intentions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 97-110.
Standage, M., Duda, J.L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2006). Students motivational processes and their
relationship to teacher ratings in school physical education: A self-determination theory
approach. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 77, 100-110.
Treasure, D.C., Lemyre, P.-N., Kuczka, K.K., & Standage, M. (2007). In M.S. Hagger &
N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and
Sport (pp. 153-165). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
52
Vallerand, R.J. (1983). The Effect of Differential Amounts of Verbal Feedback on the
Intrinsic Motivation of Male Hockey Players. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 100-107.
Vallerand, R.J. & Losier, G.F. (1994). Self-determined motivation and sportsmanship
orientations: An assessment of their temporal relationship. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 16, 229-245.
Vallerand, R.J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. In
M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 271-360). New
York: Academic Press.
Vallerand, R.J., & Losier, G.F. (1999). An integrative analysis of intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation in sport. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 11, 142-169.
Vallerand, R.J. (2001). A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and
exercise. In G. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (2nd ed., pp. 263-
319). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Vallerand, R.J. (2007). A Hierarchical Model of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation for Sport
and Physical Activity. In M.S. Hagger & N.L.D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic Motivation
and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport (pp. 255-279). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
Vangucci, M., Potrac, P., & Jones, R.L. (1997). A systematic observation of elite womens
soccer coaches. Journal of Interdisciplinary Research in Physical Education, 1, 1-17.
Vlachopoulos, S.P., Karageorghis, C.I., & Terry, P. (2000). Motivation profiles in sport: A
self-determination theory perspective. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 387-
397.
Vroom, V. (1964). Work and Motivation. New York: Wiley.
53
Vuori, I.M. (1995). Exercise and physical health: Musculoskeletal health and functional
capabilities. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 66, 276-85.
Wandzilak, T., Ansorge, C.J., & Potter, G. (1988). Comparison between selected practice and
game behaviors of youth sport soccer coaches. Journal of Sport Behavior, 11 (2), 78-88.
Weiss, M.R., & Ferrer-Caja, E. (2002). Motivational orientations and sport behavior. In T.S.
Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 101-184). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
White, R.W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological
Review, 66, 297-333.
Wilson, P.M., Rodgers, W.M. (2004). The relationship between perceived autonomy support,
exercise regulations and behavioral intentions in women. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 5,
229-242.
Wilson, P.M., Rodgers, W.M., Blanchard, C.M., & Gessell, J. (2003). The relationship
between psychological needs, self-determined motivation, exercise attitudes, and physical
fitness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 2373-2392.
Wilson, P.M., Rodgers, W.M., Fraser, S.N., & Murray, T.C. (2004). Relationships between
exercise regulations and motivational consequences in university students. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 75, 81-91.
Wing, R.R. (1999). Physical activity in the treatment of adulthood overweight and obesity:
Current evidence and research issues. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 31, 547-552.
54
Table of Appendices
Appendix 1: Participating teams characteristics ..................................................................... 55
Appendix 2: Mean values Game observations (Observer 1+2) ............................................. 56
Appendix 3: Mean values Training observations (Observer 1+2) ........................................ 56
Appendix 4: Mean, Median, Standard deviation of coaching behavior observed
during games (Observer 1+2) ..................................................................................... 57
Appendix 5: Mean, Median, Standard deviation of coaching behavior observed
during training sessions (Observer 1+2) .................................................................... 57
Appendix 6: Motivation scales as assessed by the Sports motivation Scale (SMS)
questionnaire (Total of all participants) ..................................................................... 57
55
APPENDICES A:
Team
Number of
participating
players
Playing
League
Juniors (J)
Seniors (S)
Observed Game:
Lost (L)
Draw (D)
Won (W)
Last Game:
Lost (L)
Draw (D)
Won (W)
Bottom
half leagues
table (B) /
Upper half
leagues table (U)
SV Wersten 7 8 S W W U
TUS Bommern 13 4 S L W B
HSG Dsseldorf - A1
Juniors
10 1 J W W U
HSG Dsseldorf - A2
Juniors
9 4 J W W U
HSG Gerresheim 1 15 7 S W W U
TSG Benrath 9 6 S W W B
HSG Jahn / West 1 13 6 S W W U
LTV Wuppertal - A-Juniors 13 2 J L L B
Neusser HV 3 8 7 S D W B
TUS Lintorf 3 6 9 S W W U
JSG Solingen Nord - A-
Juniors
11 2 J W W U
Tura Bderich 8 8 S L W B
Garather SV 11 7 S L L B
HSG Gerresheim 2 10 8 S L L B
TUS Erkrath 7 5 S L L B
TUS Opladen 14 4 S W W U
HSG Jahn/West 2 13 7 S L L U
Total:
177
J = 4
S = 13
L = 7
D = 1
W = 9
L = 5
W = 12
U = 9
B = 8
Appendix 1: Participating teams characteristics
P
o
s
i
t
i
v
e
r
e
i
n
f
o
r
c
e
m
e
n
t
N
o
n
-
r
e
i
n
f
o
r
c
e
m
e
n
t
M
i
s
t
a
k
e
-
c
o
n
t
i
n
g
e
n
t
E
n
c
o
u
r
a
g
e
m
e
n
t
M
i
s
t
a
k
e
-
c
o
n
t
i
n
g
e
n
t
t
e
c
h
n
i
c
a
l
i
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
P
u
n
i
s
h
m
e
n
t
P
u
n
i
t
i
v
e
t
e
c
h
n
i
c
a
l
i
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
I
g
n
o
r
i
n
g
m
i
s
t
a
k
e
s
G
e
n
e
r
a
l
t
e
c
h
n
i
c
a
l
i
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
G
e
n
e
r
a
l
e
n
c
o
u
r
a
g
e
m
e
n
t
O
r
g
a
n
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
Team Mean Mean Mean Mean Mean Mean Mean Mean Mean Mean
SV Wersten 22.5 6.0 5.0 22.0 18.5 4.0 .0 85.0 35.5 52.0
TUS Bommern 35.5 1.0 3.0 17.0 1.5 .5 .5 59.0 24.0 66.5
HSG Dsseldorf -
A1 Juniors
56.0 5.5 14.0 20.5 2.0 .5 .0 43.0 30.0 21.5
HSG Dsseldorf -
A2 Juniors
42.0 3.0 2.5 26.5 11.5 2.5 .0 76.0 22.5 17.0
HSG Gerresheim 1 65.5 2.5 4.5 15.0 13.5 5.5 .0 75.0 39.5 30.0
TSG Benrath 22.5 4.0 2.5 4.0 9.5 6.5 1.5 37.5 47.5 32.5
56
HSG Jahn / West 1 69.0 3.0 5.5 32.0 8.5 4.5 .0 56.0 23.5 45.0
LTV Wuppertal A
Juniors
36.0 3.5 7.5 26.0 18.0 4.0 .0 66.0 43.0 35.0
Neusser HV 3 28.5 1.0 3.0 12.0 11.0 2.5 3.5 34.0 26.5 20.5
TUS Lintorf 3 40.5 .0 3.0 19.5 7.0 7.5 .0 74.0 38.0 33.0
JSG Solingen Nord
A Juniors
5.0 2.5 1.5 22.0 6.0 6.0 1.0 28.0 16.0 32.5
Tura Bderich 38.5 2.5 7.0 7.0 29.5 2.5 1.5 43.5 10.5 27.0
Garather SV 29.0 .5 10.0 11.0 6.0 .0 6.0 86.5 73.0 35.0
HSG Gerresheim 2 10.0 3.0 2.0 20.0 19.5 7.0 .0 30.5 27.5 33.5
TUS Erkrath 29.5 6.5 2.0 13.0 13.0 4.5 .5 53.0 11.5 17.0
TUS Opladen 98.5 1.0 18.5 24.5 4.5 5.5 .0 121.0 127.0 44.5
HSG Jahn/West 2 26.0 2.0 .0 21.0 28.0 11.5 .0 43.5 4.0 29.0
Total (Mean) 38.5 2.79 5.38 18.41 12.21 4.41 .85 59.5 35.26 33.62
Appendix 2: Mean values Game observations (Observer 1+2)
P
o
s
i
t
i
v
e
r
e
i
n
f
o
r
c
e
m
e
n
t
N
o
n
-
r
e
i
n
f
o
r
c
e
m
e
n
t
M
i
s
t
a
k
e
-
c
o
n
t
i
n
g
e
n
t
e
n
c
o
u
r
a
g
e
m
e
n
t
M
i
s
t
a
k
e
-
c
o
n
t
i
n
g
e
n
t
t
e
c
h
n
i
c
a
l
i
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
P
u
n
i
s
h
m
e
n
t
P
u
n
i
t
i
v
e
t
e
c
h
n
i
c
a
l
i
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
I
g
n
o
r
i
n
g
m
i
s
t
a
k
e
s
G
e
n
e
r
a
l
t
e
c
h
n
i
c
a
l
i
n
s
t
r
u
c
t
i
o
n
G
e
n
e
r
a
l
e
n
c
o
u
r
a
g
e
m
e
n
t
O
r
g
a
n
i
z
a
t
i
o
n
Team
SV Wersten 37.0 .0 1.5 38.5 3.5 .5 .0 33.5 12.0 65.0
TUS Bommern 21.0 .0 1.0 23.0 7.5 4.5 1.0 44.5 12.0 74.5
HSG Dsseldorf -
A1 Juniors
49.5 2.0 .0 16.0 6.5 2.0 2.0 105.0 108.0 135.5
HSG Dsseldorf -
A2 Juniors
13.5 .0 8.5 2.0 5.0 .0 1.0 8.5 49.0 37.0
HSG Gerresheim 1 37.5 .0 5.0 20.5 5.0 .5 .0 54.5 26.0 56.0
TSG Benrath 33.0 1.0 4.5 32.5 8.5 2.0 4.5 27.5 15.5 38.5
HSG Jahn / West 1 54.0 .0 9.5 35.5 13.0 2.5 1.0 59.5 71.5 57.0
LTV Wuppertal A
Juniors
16.5 2.0 .0 41.0 19.0 21.5 .0 26.0 14.5 55.5
Neusser HV 3 33.0 .0 5.5 48.5 8.5 1.0 .0 44.0 28.5 65.5
TUS Lintorf 3 - - - - - - - - - -
JSG Solingen Nord
A Juniors
33.5 .0 8.0 49.5 10.0 1.5 .0 68.5 40.0 81.5
Tura Bderich 49.0 .0 3.5 18.0 16.0 1.5 .0 55.5 46.5 50.0
Garather SV 30.0 1.5 10.5 8.0 .0 2.0 1.5 33.5 35.5 83.5
HSG Gerresheim 2 3.0 .0 .0 6.5 1.0 .0 1.0 17.0 11.5 38.0
TUS Erkrath 24.5 .0 13.0 35.0 14.5 3.0 .0 35.5 50.0 29.5
TUS Opladen 77.0 .0 14.5 38.0 3.0 1.0 .0 47.5 67.0 53.5
HSG Jahn/West 2 65.0 1.0 4.5 57.0 22.0 12.0 3.5 28.0 10.5 100.5
Total (Mean) 36.06 .47 5.59 29.34 8.94 3.47 .97 43.03 37.38 63.81
Appendix 3: Mean values Training observations (Observer 1+2)
57
Mean Median Standard-deviation Valid N
Positive reinforcement (Observer 1+2) 38.5 35.5 23.01 17
Non-reinforcement (Observer 1+2) 2.79 2.5 1.89 17
Mistake-contingent encouragement 5.38 3.0 4.85 17
Mistake-contingent technical instruction (Observer 1+2) 18.41 20.0 7.34 17
Punishment (Observer 1+2) 12.21 11.0 8.23 17
Punitive technical instruction (Observer 1+2) 4.41 4.5 2.93 17
Ignoring mistakes (Observer 1+2) .85 .0 1.62 17
General technical instruction (Observer 1+2) 59.5 56.0 24.66 17
General encouragement (Observer 1+2) 35.26 27.5 28.72 17
Organization (Observer 1+2) 33.62 32.5 12.8 17
Appendix 4: Mean, Median, Standard deviation of coaching behavior observed during games
(Observer 1+2)
Mean Median Standard-deviation Valid N
Positive reinforcement (Observer 1+2) 36.06 33.25 19.31 16
Non-reinforcement (Observer 1+2) .47 .0 .76 16
Mistake-contingent encouragement 5.59 4.75 4.67 16
Mistake-contingent technical instruction (Observer 1+2) 29.34 33.75 16.46 16
Punishment (Observer 1+2) 8.94 8.0 6.42 16
Punitive technical instruction (Observer 1+2) 3.47 1.75 5.59 16
Ignoring mistakes (Observer 1+2) .97 .5 1.36 16
General technical instruction (Observer 1+2) 43.03 39.75 23.0 16
General encouragement (Observer 1+2) 37.38 32.0 27.46 16
Organization (Observer 1+2) 63.81 56.5 27.06 16
Appendix 5: Mean, Median, Standard deviation of coaching behavior observed during training
sessions (Observer 1+2)
Mean Median
Standard-
deviation Valid N
Intrinsic motivation: to know 4.59 4.75 1.2 176
Intrinsic motivation: to accomplish 4.94 5.0 1.02 176
Intrinsic motivation: to experience stimulation 5.26 5.38 .97 176
Extrinsic motivation: identified 4.67 4.75 1.12 176
Extrinsic motivation: introjected 5.59 5.75 1.14 176
Extrinsic motivation: external regulation 3.33 3.25 1.16 176
Amotivation 2.02 1.75 1.04 176
Appendix 6: Motivation scales as assessed by the Sports motivation Scale (SMS) questionnaire
(Total of all participants)
Item in the Coaching Feedback
Questionnaire (CFQ)
Corresponding Item in the Coaching
Behavior Assessment System (CBAS)
"Good play!" Positive reinforcement
The coach ignores your good performance. Non-reinforcement
58
"Way to go! You really extended your
elbow that time.
Positive reinforcement
"Great play. Now youre keeping your
eyes on the ball.
Positive reinforcement
"Excellent work in practice today. Positive reinforcement
Coach doesnt say anything to you about
your good performance.
Non-reinforcement
"Thats O.K. Keep working at it! Mistake-contingent encouragement
Coach ignores your error or good
performance.
Ignoring mistakes
"That was a really stupid play! Punishment
"You dropped your elbow. Next time keep
it up.
Mistake-contingent technical instruction
"How many times have I told you to
extend your elbow?
Punitive technical instruction
"Hang in there! You will do better next
time.
Mistake-contingent encouragement
Coach doesnt say anything to you about
your error or poor performance.
Ignoring mistakes
"Your technique looks lousy! Keep your
head up.
Punitive technical instruction
"That play sucked! Punishment
"No, thats not right. You need to work on
a faster release.
Mistake-contingent technical instruction
Appendix 7: Correspondence between CFQ Items and CBAS Items
Participating players age (in years)
N Valid 170
Missing 7
Mean 25.09
Median 22.0
Standard deviation 8.83
Appendix 8: Participants age Appendix 9: Participants playing experience
Correlation Games (Observer 1+2)
Observed Category
Positive reinforcement Pearson Correlation .99**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Non-reinforcement Pearson Correlation .62**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Mistake-contingent encouragement Pearson Correlation .85**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Mistake-contingent technical instruction Pearson Correlation .79**
Participants playing experience (in years)
N Valid 175
Missing 2
Mean 15.83
Median 14.0
Standard deviation 8.18
59
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Punishment Pearson Correlation .97**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Punitive technical instruction Pearson Correlation .78**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Ignoring mistakes Pearson Correlation .94**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
General technical instruction Pearson Correlation .80**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
General encouragement Pearson Correlation .96**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
Organization Pearson Correlation .64**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 17
**. Correlation significant on a 0.01 (2-tailed) level.
*. Correlation significant on a 0.05 (2-tailed) level.
Appendix 10: Correlation for each item observed through the CBAS between both observers for
the games
Correlation Training sessions (Observer 1+2)
Observed category
Positive reinforcement Pearson Correlation .96**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
Non-reinforcement Pearson Correlation .47
Significance (2-tailed) .066
N 16
Mistake-contingent encouragement Pearson Correlation .81**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
Mistake-contingent technical instruction Pearson Correlation .96**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
Punishment Pearson Correlation .92**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
Punitive technical instruction Pearson Correlation .98**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
Ignoring mistakes Pearson Correlation .95**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
General technical instruction Pearson Correlation .95**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
60
N 16
General encouragement Pearson Correlation .98**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
Organization Pearson Correlation .96**
Significance (2-tailed) < .001
N 16
**. Correlation significant on a 0.01 (2-tailed) level.
*. Correlation significant on a 0.05 (2-tailed) level.
Appendix 11: Correlation for each item observed through the CBAS between both observers for
the training sessions
Descriptive statistics for paired coaching behavior categories (Games/Training sessions)
Mean N Standard deviation
Pair 1 Positive reinforcement (Game l, Observer 1+2) 38.38 16 23.76
Positive reinforcement (Training, Observer 1+2) 36.06 16 19.31
Pair 2 Non-reinforcement (Game, Observer 1+2) 2.97 16 1.80
Non-reinforcement (Training, Observer 1+2) .47 16 .76
Pair 3 Mistake-contingent encouragement (Game, Observer 1+2) 5.53 16 4.97
Mistake-contingent encouragement (Training, Observer 1+2) 5.59 16 4.67
Pair 4 Mistake-contingent technical instruction (Game, Observer 1+2) 18.34 16 7.58
Mistake-Contingent technical instruction (Training, Observer 1+2) 29.34 16 16.46
Pair 5 Punishment (Game, Observer 1+2) 12.53 16 8.38
Punishment (Training, Observer 1+2) 8.94 16 6.42
Pair 6 Punitive technical instruction (Game, Observer 1+2) 4.22 16 2.91
Punitive technical instruction (Training, Observer 1+2) 3.47 16 5.59
Pair 7 Ignoring mistakes (Game, Observer 1+2) .91 16 1.66
Ignoring mistakes (Training, Observer 1+2) .97 16 1.36
Pair 8 General technical instruction (Game, Observer 1+2) 58.59 16 25.18
General technical instruction (Training, Observer 1+2) 43.03 16 23.0
Pair 9 General encouragement (Game, Observer 1+2) 35.09 16 29.65
General encouragement (Training, Observer 1+2) 37.38 16 27.46
Pair 10 Organization (Game, Observer 1+2) 33.66 16 13.22
Organization (Training, Observer 1+2) 63.81 16 27.06
Appendix 12: Differences between coaching categories (CBAS) for training and game observations
Paired T-Test
Paired differences
T df
Sig.
(2-
tailed)
Mean Standard deviation
Pair
1
Positive reinforcement (Game, Observer 1+2) Positive
reinforcement (Training, Observer 1+2)
2.31 19.48 .48 15 .64
Pair
2
Non-reinforcement (Game, Observer 1+2)
Non-reinforcement (Training, Observer 1+2)
2.5 1.87 5.3 15 .000
Pair
3
Mistake-contingent encouragement (Game, Observer
1+2) Mistake-contingent encouragement (Training,
Observer 1+2)
-.06 6.04 -.04 15 .97
Pair
4
Mistake-contingent technical instruction (Game,
Observer 1+2) Mistake-contingent technical
instruction (Training, Observer 1+2)
-11.0 17.2 -2.56 15 .02
Pair
5
Punishment (Game, Observer 1+2 - Punishment
(Training, Observer 1+2)
3.59 7.46 1.93 15 .07
61
Pair
6
Punitive technical instruction (Game, Observer 1+2)
Punitive technical instruction (Training, Observer 1+2)
.75 5.67 .53 15 .61
Pair
7
Ignoring mistakes (Game, Observer 1+2) Ignoring
mistakes (Training, Observer 1+2)
-.06 2.07 -.12 15 .91
Pair
8
General technical instruction (Game, Observer 1+2) -
General technical instruction (Training, Observer 1+2)
15.56 36.83 1.69 15 .11
Pair
9
General encouragement (Game, Observer 1+2)
General encouragement (Training, Observer 1+2)
-2.28 36.86 -.25 15 .81
Pair
10
Organization (Game, Observer 1+2) - Organization
(Training, Observer 1+2)
-30.16 29.48 -4.09 15 .001
Appendix 13: T-test analysis for differences of coaching behaviors between game and training
situations
APPENDIX B:
Appendix 14: A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and exercise.
Vallerand, 2001
62
Appendix 15: Horns (2002) Model of Coaching Effectiveness
In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (pp. 309-354). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Appendix 16: Smolls and Smiths Cognitive-Mediational Model
63
Appendix C:
64
Appendix 17: Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (German Version)
65
Appendix 18: Informed Consent (in German)
66
67
Appendix 19: Questionnaire 1: Sports Motivation Scale (Translated, German Version)
68
69
70
Appendix 20: Questionnaire 2: Sports Motivation Scale (English Version)
71
Coaching Feedback Questionnaire
As you perhaps already know, coaches really differ from each other in the type of feedback they give in response to their athletes
performances.
This questionnaire is designed to find out what type of coaching feedback your coach gives you in practices and games.
Coaching Reponses to Players Successes
Listed below are six examples of feedback your coach might give you after you have had a successful performance in a game or
practice. PLEASE RATE EACH STATEMENT IN TERMS OF HOW TYPICAL YOUR COACH GIVES YOU THIS KIND OF
FEEDBACK AFTER YOU HAVE HAD A SUCCESSFUL PERFORMANCE.
Not Typical
At All
Very
Typical
1. Good play!
1 2 3 4 5
2. The coach ignores your good performance.
1 2 3 4 5
3. Way to go! You really extended your elbow that time.
1 2 3 4 5
4. Great play. Now youre keeping you eyes on the ball.
1 2 3 4 5
5. Excellent work in practice today.
1 2 3 4 5
6. Coach doesnt say anything to you about your good
performance.
1 2 3 4 5
72
Coaching Reponses to Players Errors
Listed below are ten examples of feedback your coach might give you after you have had made a mistake or committed an error in a
game or practice. PLEASE RATE EACH STATEMENT IN TERMS OF HOW TYPICAL YOUR COACH GIVES YOU THIS
KIND OF FEEDBACK AFTER YOU HAVE HAD A PERFORMANCE ERROR OR POOR PLAY.
Not Typical
At All
Very
Typical
1. Thats O.K. Keep working at it!
1 2 3 4 5
2. Coach ignores your error or poor performance.
1 2 3 4 5
3. That was a really stupid play!
1 2 3 4 5
4. You dropped your elbow. Next time keep it up.
1 2 3 4 5
5. How many times have I told you to extend your elbow?
1 2 3 4 5
6. Hang in there! You will do better next time.
1 2 3 4 5
7. Coach doesnt say anything to you about your error or
poor performance.
1 2 3 4 5
8. Your technique looks lousy! Keep you head up.
1 2 3 4 5
9. That play sucked!
1 2 3 4 5
10. No, thats not right. You need to work on a faster
release.
1 2 3 4 5
Appendix 21: Questionnaire 3: Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (English Version)
73
Beobachtungsbogen
Datum:
Liga:
Beobachter:
Mannschaft:
Reaktive Verhaltensweisen des Trainers Verbal und Non-
Verbal:
Rckmeldungen auf gewnschte Ausfhrungen
1. Positive Bestrkung/Verstrkung:
2. Nicht-Bestrkung/Verstrkung:
Rckmeldungen auf Fehler/fehlerhaftes Verhalten:
3. Fehlerbedingte Aufmunterung/Ermunterung:
4. Fehlerbedingte technische Anweisungen:
5. Bestrafung:
6. Bestrafende technische Instruktionen:
7. Fehler-Ignorierung:
Spontane Verhaltensweisen des Trainers Verbal und
Non-Verbal:
8. Generelle technische Anweisungen:
9. Generelle Ermutigung/Aufmunterung:
10. Organisation:
Kommentare:
Appendix 22: German Version of the coaching behavior assessment system (CBAS) as used during the
observations