This document summarizes six commonly researched treatments for problem behavior maintained by escape from instructional activities: activity choice, curricular revision, demand fading, differential reinforcement, extinction, and noncontingent escape. It describes the key aspects of each treatment and provides contexts for when each treatment would be most appropriate. The document aims to provide behavior analysts with a framework for selecting the most suitable treatment based on client characteristics and environmental constraints.
This document summarizes six commonly researched treatments for problem behavior maintained by escape from instructional activities: activity choice, curricular revision, demand fading, differential reinforcement, extinction, and noncontingent escape. It describes the key aspects of each treatment and provides contexts for when each treatment would be most appropriate. The document aims to provide behavior analysts with a framework for selecting the most suitable treatment based on client characteristics and environmental constraints.
Original Description:
escape mentained behavior problems
Original Title
Function Based Treatment for Escape Mentained Behavior Problems
This document summarizes six commonly researched treatments for problem behavior maintained by escape from instructional activities: activity choice, curricular revision, demand fading, differential reinforcement, extinction, and noncontingent escape. It describes the key aspects of each treatment and provides contexts for when each treatment would be most appropriate. The document aims to provide behavior analysts with a framework for selecting the most suitable treatment based on client characteristics and environmental constraints.
This document summarizes six commonly researched treatments for problem behavior maintained by escape from instructional activities: activity choice, curricular revision, demand fading, differential reinforcement, extinction, and noncontingent escape. It describes the key aspects of each treatment and provides contexts for when each treatment would be most appropriate. The document aims to provide behavior analysts with a framework for selecting the most suitable treatment based on client characteristics and environmental constraints.
Escape from instructional activities is a common maintaining variable for problem behavior. There are several effective function-based treatments that can be used to address escape-maintained behavior, including differential reinforcement and noncontingent reinforcement.
Escape from instruction and tasks, attention, and automatic reinforcement are common maintaining variables. Escape specifically is very prevalent and instructional factors like task difficulty can contribute.
Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior, differential reinforcement of other behavior, noncontingent reinforcement, and stimulus fading are described as common function-based treatments for escape-maintained behavior.
22 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL
ne of the most common rein-
forcement functions of problem behavior is escape from instruc- tional stimuli. Escape, or the social-neg- ative reinforcement function, has been shown to be at least as prevalent as and sometimes more prevalent than attention (i.e., social-positive reinforcement) and automatic reinforcement functions. For example, in an analysis of the functions of self-injurious behavior (SIB) of 152 individuals with developmental disabili- ties, Iwata, Pace, Dorsey, et al. (1994) demonstrated that 35% of the individu- als displayed SIB maintained by escape from instruction, compared to 23% and 26% of individuals whose SIB was maintained by attention and automatic reinforcement, respectively. Similarly, Asmus et al. (2004) demonstrated that social-negative reinforcement was the most common maintaining variable for problem behavior either solely or in combination with social positive reinforcement (i.e., multiple control) for 138 individuals with and without developmental disabilities. Finally, Love, Carr, and LeBlanc (2009) found that escape was the second most common function of problem behavior, identied for 50% of 32 children with autism spectrum disorders. Individuals with disabilities are frequently exposed to learning situa- tions that target important habilitative skills such as pre-academics, activities of daily living, communication, social behavior, among others. Unfortunately, a number of aspects of the instructional environment might become aversive and establish escape from them as a negative reinforcer. For example, task difculty, rate of instruction delivery, and particu- lar prompting strategies could all have aversive properties for some learners. If problem behavior occurs in response to the aversive situation, a common and understandable reaction of many instructors might be to allow the client time away from the task to calm down. Frequent instruction, impaired reper- toires associated with disabilities, and natural reactions to problem behavior from caregivers likely combine to make escape functions quite common. Practicing behavior analysts who work with individuals with disabilities in any type of instructional setting should be prepared to treat escape-maintained problem behavior. If a functional assessment indicates that problem be- havior is maintained by escape from in- structional activities, there are a number of treatments that might be employed as part of a behavioral intervention plan. The current standard for reductive treat- ments is to base them on the results of a functional assessment. These function- based treatments directly address some aspect of the behaviors maintaining contingency (e.g., establishing operation, reinforcer) by, for example, eliminating the contingency through extinction, weakening the establishing operation by making a task less aversive, or teaching the individual a more appropriate way to access the reinforcer (i.e., escape). The remainder of this article will focus exclusively on selecting treatments that directly address a problem behaviors negative reinforcement function. Selecting an intervention that is likely to be successful for a given client and therapeutic environment can be challenging unless the behavior analyst is well-versed in the characteristics of each treatment and has a framework for choosing between multiple appropriate treatments. Thus, the rst purpose of this article is to describe six categories of Function-Based Treatments for Escape-Maintained Problem Behavior: A Treatment-Selection Model for Practicing Behavior Analysts Kaneen B. Geiger, M.S., James E. Carr, Ph.D., BCBA-D, and Linda A. LeBlanc, Ph.D., BCBA-D Auburn University Escape from instructional activities is a common maintaining variable for problem behavior and a number of effective treatments have been devel- oped for this function. Each of these treatments has characteristics that make them optimal for certain environments and clients, but less optimal for others. We summarize the most commonly researched function-based treatments for escape-maintained behavior, describe the contexts for which they are most appropriate, and provide a clinical model for selecting treat- ments based on client characteristics and the constraints of the therapeutic environment. Keywords: Activity choice, clinical decision making, curricular revision, de- mand fading, differential reinforcement, escape, extinction, function-based treatment, noncontingent reinforcement ABSTRACT O Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3(1), 22-32 BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 22 4/18/10 11:04:11 PM 23 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL commonly researched, function-based treatments for escape- maintained problem behavior: (a) activity choice, (b) curricular and instructional revision, (c) demand fading, (d) differential reinforcement, (e) extinction, and (f ) noncontingent escape (see Table 1). This summary is followed by a clinical decision- making model for selecting the most appropriate treatment based on characteristics of the client and therapeutic environ- ment and their match with the treatments specic advantages and disadvantages. The focus of the model is escape-maintained behavioral excesses (e.g., self-injury, aggression, property de- struction) rather than noncompliance (e.g., non-responding, verbal refusal), but we refer the interested reader to Houlihan, Sloane, Jones, and Patton (1992) and Cipani (1998) for reviews of treatments for noncompliance. In addition, punishment procedures are not included in the present model and the prac- titioner might view this model as a guide for exploring the full range of function-based treatments before considering explicit punishment procedures. Function-Based Treatments for Escape-Maintained Problem Behavior Activity Choice Activity choice involves providing the learner with an opportunity to select either the order in which, or time at which, tasks are completed (Dyer, Dunlap, & Winterling, 1990). Activity choice is considered a function-based interven- tion because the individual can presumably avoid the aversive aspects of one task by selecting another. For example, Dyer et al. used activity choice to reduce escape-maintained disruptive behavior (e.g., aggression, SIB, tantrums) of three children with developmental disabilities. Each participant was provided with a choice between 3 to 4 academic tasks (e.g., completing a puzzle, labeling picture cards, sorting). When the rst task was complete, the participant chose from the remaining tasks, and so on. For all three participants, activity choice produced substantial reductions in disruptive behaviors. Activity choice is an easily implemented intervention that has been shown to increase compliance and reduce problem be- havior without the loss of instructional time (Kern et al., 1998). It also includes choice-making opportunities for the consumer, which is often a habilitative goal with high social validity (Kern et al., 1998). There are a number of variables the practitioner must consider before selecting an activity choice intervention. First, activity choice may require up-front preparation of multiple sets of task materials from which the consumer can choose. Second, it is critical to ensure that the curricular ac- tivities are appropriate to the consumers existing skill repertoire before presenting choices. Third, activity choice is only effective with consumers with existing choice-making skills who can tolerate instruction. Finally, because giving a consumer a choice of activi- ties is an antecedent intervention, there is no explicit plan for how to respond to problem behavior, should it occur. Therefore, combining activity choice with a consequence-based procedure such as differential reinforcement or extinction might further reduce problem behavior. We refer the reader to the Kern et al. (1998) literature review for additional information on implementing activity-choice interventions. Curricular and Instructional Revision Curricular and instructional revision involves assessing aspects of the curricular targets or instructional procedures that might be aversive for the learner and making alterations to attenuate or eliminate these features to abolish escape from instruction as an effective reinforcer. To maintain consistency with the research literature, curricular and instructional revision will henceforth be referred to as curricular revision. Some of the curricular variables that might establish escape from work as a negative reinforcer are tasks that are too difcult or too easy in relation to the learners current repertoire (Dunlap, Kern- Dunlap, Clarke, & Robbins, 1991; Ferro, Foster-Johnson, & Dunlap, 1996; Kern, Childs, Dunlap, Clarke, & Falk, 1994; Roberts, Marshall, Nelson, & Albers, 2001), are non-preferred (Clarke et al., 1995), are novel (Mace, Browder, & Lin, 1987; Smith, Iwata, Goh, & Shore, 1995), or do not produce skills that are functional in the learners environment (Dunlap, Foster-Johnson, Clarke, Kern, & Childs, 1995). Dunlap et al. (1991) describe the use of curricular revi- sion to reduce the problem behavior of a girl with mental retardation. The authors assessed the effects of four curricular variables on problem behavior: 1) ne- vs. gross-motor tasks, 2) short- vs. long-duration tasks, 3) arbitrary vs. functional tasks, and 4) activity choice vs. no choice. The assessment revealed that the participant exhibited higher rates of problem behavior and lower rates of on-task behavior when presented with ne-motor tasks, long-duration tasks, arbitrary tasks, and no activity choice. Curricular (e.g., increased functional tasks) and instructional revisions (e.g., short teaching durations) were then implemented and produced increases in on-task behavior and elimination of problem behavior. Some of the instructional variables that might establish escape from work as a negative reinforcer include lengthy sessions (Dunlap et al., 1991; Kern et al., 1994; Smith et al., 1995), massed trials (McCurdy, Skinner, Grantham, Watson, & Hindman, 2001), certain prompting strategies (Munk & One of the most common reinforcement functions of problem behavior is escape from instructional stimuli BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 23 4/18/10 11:04:11 PM 24 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL Repp, 1994), high rates of trial presentation (Smith et al.), and low rates of positive reinforcement (Smith & Iwata, 1997). Several studies have addressed the nal concern (low reinforce- ment during instruction). For example, Lalli et al. (1999) showed that delivering positive reinforcers for compliance was more effective in reducing escape-maintained problem behavior than delivering breaks contingent on compliance, even when the problem behavior still produced escape from the task. Similarly, Ingvarsson, Hanley, and Welter (2009) showed that the delivery of contingent and noncontingent positive reinforcers were each effective in reducing escape-maintained problem behavior. The fact that increased positive reinforcement during tasks minimizes escape-maintained problem behavior, even when contingent escape is still available (as in Lalli et al. and Ingvarsson et al.), suggests that this procedure might work to abolish the aversive properties of the tasks. Curricular revision could result in improve- ments in teaching procedures or curriculum assessment that not only benet the target consumer, but could have benecial effects on other consumers served in the environment. Additionally, improvements in teaching strate- gies and curricula create a more effective learning environment which can produce more efcient and effective skill acquisition while reducing and potentially preventing problem behavior. Furthermore, it is a behavior analysts ethical responsibility to promote effective learning environments rather than teach individuals with disabilities to tolerate ineffective ones (Winett & Winkler, 1972). However, curricular revision requires someone with expertise to assess and change aspects of the cur- riculum or instructional strategy. Additionally, the time and effort required to assess and make changes can be of concern if it is important to eliminate problem behavior immediately. We refer the reader to a literature review by Dunlap and Kern (1996) for additional information on curricular revision. Demand Fading Demand fading (instructional or stimulus fading) involves the removal of all instructions, followed by their gradual reintroduction (Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree, & McIntyre, 1993). Such demand removal eliminates the aversive tasks, which remain absent until they are systematically and gradually faded back in. For example, Pace et al. faded the frequency of tasks to decrease escape-maintained SIB of three individuals with developmental disabilities. The initial elimination of all tasks substantially reduced levels of SIB and they remained low as tasks were gradually reintroduced. It is important to note that demand fading works best when implemented with escape extinction (i.e., withholding the negative reinforcer when problem behavior reemerges during fading; Zarcone, Iwata, Smith, Mazaleski, & Lerman, 1994). Because the rst step of demand fading is the elimination of all instructions, there should be an immediate decrease in problem behavior, which is a benecial outcome for consumers who exhibit severe problem behavior or who are too large to physically prompt to comply with a task. In addition, because instructions are gradually reintroduced over time, demand fading might increase a consumers tolerance of instructional activities. However, demand fading involves a loss of instruc- tional time, which could be impractical due to the disruption of classroom activities or inadequate stafng to supervise the consumer while away from instruction. Also, fading in the instructions is often logistically difcult and requires the su- pervision of someone with expertise to oversee the process. We refer the reader to the empirical article by Zarcone et al. (1994) for additional information on demand fading. Differential Reinforcement Differential negative reinforcement of alternative behavior. Perhaps the most common procedural form of differential nega- tive reinforcement of alternative behavior (DNRA) involves providing escape from instruction contingent on an alternative prosocial response (e.g., compliance) while placing problem behavior on extinction (Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). Differential negative reinforcement of alternative behavior can also be ar- ranged by providing escape for an alternative response while punishing the problem behavior or by providing more valu- able breaks (e.g., longer duration) for the alternative response and less valuable breaks for the problem behavior (Athens & Vollmer, in press). Vollmer, Roane, Ringdahl, and Marcus (1999) imple- mented differential negative reinforcement of compliance (with problem behavior placed on extinction) to reduce the escape-maintained SIB and aggression of two children with mental retardation. For both children, DNRA reduced problem behavior and increased compliance. In addition, when DNRA was implemented with lower integrity (i.e., problem behavior occasionally produced escape), problem behavior remained low and compliance remained high as long as compliance was reinforced on a denser schedule of reinforcement than problem behavior. Differential negative reinforcement of alternative behavior decreases problem behavior while actively targeting more adap- tive skills and providing continued access to the functional reinforcer, escape. Further, there is evidence that DNRA can It is a behavior analysts ethical responsibility to promote effective learning environments rather than teach individuals with disabilities to tolerate ineffective ones BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 24 4/18/10 11:04:11 PM 25 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL still be effective at reduced procedural integrity (Vollmer et al., 1999). However, DNRA requires the delivery of breaks im- mediately after the alternative response criterion is met, which might be disruptive to classroom activities or be impractical if there is inadequate stafng to supervise the consumer dur- ing the break. In addition, DNRA requires the supervision of someone with expertise to supervise schedule thinning for the alternative behavior. We refer the reader to the literature review by Vollmer and Iwata (1992) for additional information on DNRA. Functional communication training. Functional communi- cation training (FCT) is a form of DNRA that involves provid- ing escape from instruction contingent on a communicative re- sponse (e.g., vocal, sign) as the specic prosocial behavior while problem behavior is placed on extinction (Durand & Merges, 2001) or is punished (Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, & Maglieri, 2005). In order to effectively reduce problem behavior, the communi- cative response, or mand for escape, should ideally require less response effort, have a denser schedule of reinforcement, and have a shorter delay to reinforcement than the problem behav- ior (Horner & Day, 1991). Researchers have reduced problem behavior by teaching mands for escape (Horner & Day) and for assistance (Carr & Durand, 1985). For example, Carr and Durand used FCT to reduce escape-maintained disruptive behavior (e.g., aggression, tantrums, screaming, SIB) of three children with developmental disabilities. The authors taught the participants the vocal response, I dont understand, which was immediately followed by assistance on the task. Occurrences of disruptive behavior were followed by continued task presentation without assistance. For all three participants, FCT reduced disruptive behavior to near-zero levels. Functional communication training decreases problem behavior while actively targeting a communication skill and providing continued access to escape. Moreover, research has demonstrated that some individuals prefer FCT over noncontingent reinforcement and extinction (Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, Contrucci, & Maglieri, 1997). However, FCT may result in high rates of the communicative response, which must immediately be followed by breaks from instruction. As with DNRA, delivering breaks contingent on the consumers communicative responses can be logistically difcult to manage in some environments. Also as with DNRA, FCT sometimes requires the supervision of someone with expertise to oversee additional interventions to reduce high rates of the communi- cative response. We refer the reader to a literature review by Tiger, Hanley, and Bruzek (2008) for additional information on FCT. Differential negative reinforcement of zero rates of responding. Differential negative reinforcement of zero rates of responding (DNRO) involves delivering escape when the problem behav- ior has not occurred for a specic period of time (Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). The general suggestion is to use initial intervals that are shorter than the mean inter-response time of the problem behavior during baseline, giving the individual a high probability of contacting the programmed contingency (Deitz & Repp, 1983). A com- mon feature of DNRO is interval resetting, by which occurrences of the problem behavior immediately reset the timer to zero seconds and a new interval begins (Vollmer & Iwata). Buckley and Newchok (2006) used DNRO to reduce problem behavior maintained by escape from music of a 7-year-old boy with pervasive developmental disorder. The DNRO procedure decreased disruptive behavior to near-zero levels that were maintained as the interval duration was successfully increased to 5 min. For problem behavior maintained by escape from in- structional activities, DNRA (including FCT) is generally more preferred than DNRO because the former procedure includes a skill acquisition component (Vollmer & Iwata, 1992). Furthermore, DNRA has shown to be more effective than DNRO in reducing escape-maintained problem behavior (Roberts, Mace, & Daggett, 1995). Alternatively, DNRO may be more appropriate for increasing tolerance to an aversive activity (e.g., an invasive medical procedure) because breaks may not be permitted to be under the clients control, as they are in DNRA (Vollmer & Iwata). One benet of DNRO is that it provides continued access to breaks while increasing tolerance to aversive situations that are necessary, such as medical procedures. However, DNRO is labor intensive because it requires constant monitoring of the consumer for occurrences of problem behavior. In addition, providing breaks on dense schedules at treatment outset can be logistically difcult if they disrupt ongoing activities. Finally, DNRO requires the supervision of someone with expertise in establishing the DNRO intervals and monitoring the schedule thinning process. We refer the reader to the literature review by Vollmer and Iwata (1992) for additional information on DNRO. Escape Extinction Escape extinction involves the continued presentation of an aversive activity (e.g., instructional tasks) while eliminating the possibility of escape from the activity contingent on problem In order to effectively reduce problem behav ior, the communicative response, or mand for escape, should ideally require less response effort, have a denser schedule of reinforcement, and have a shorter delay to reinforcement than the problem behavior BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 25 4/18/10 11:04:11 PM 26 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL behavior (Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, 1990). Iwata et al. implemented escape extinction with physical guid- ance to reduce the escape-maintained SIB of six children with mental retardation 1 . For 5 of 6 participants, escape extinction with physical guidance reduced SIB to low levels and resulted in increased task compliance. For the sixth participant, escape extinction did not reduce SIB until response blocking was added. Further, Iwata, Pace, Cowdery, and Miltenberger (1994) showed that escape extinction was an effective treatment for escape-maintained SIB, but had no effect on SIB maintained by attention or automatic reinforcement. This nding under- scores the importance of matching reductive treatments to the function of problem behavior. The main benet of escape extinction is that it can be combined with other treatments (e.g., activity choice, demand fading, DNRO) to enhance their effectiveness. However, escape extinction may not immediately decrease problem behavior and there is often a high degree of effort associated with implementing the procedure. Instructors are likely to implement escape extinction with lower treatment integrity than other procedures, which might make problem behavior more resistant to extinction in the future (McConnachie & Carr, 1997). Furthermore, extinction might result in a burst of responding that is at least as high as pretreatment rates and might evoke aggressive behavior, although these outcomes are not guaranteed (Lerman, Iwata, & Wallace, 1999). We refer the reader to literature reviews by Ducharme and Van Houten (1994) and Lerman and Iwata (1996) for additional informa- tion on escape extinction. Noncontingent Escape Noncontingent escape (NCE) involves the delivery of escape from instructional activities on a time-based schedule (e.g., xed-time, variable-time), regardless of the individuals problem behavior (Kodak, Miltenberger, & Romanuik, 2003; Vollmer, Marcus, & Ringdahl, 1995). At the beginning of the procedure, escape is typically provided on a denser schedule than what the problem behavior typically produces (Carr & LeBlanc, 2006). After NCE is successful in reducing problem behavior, the reinforcement schedule is generally thinned to a more manageable value. For example, Vollmer et al. used NCE to reduce the escape-maintained SIB of two males with developmental disabilities and were able to thin the schedule of noncontingent (xed time) breaks from 10 s to 2.5 min for one participant and to 10 min for the other. In addition, NCE and DNRO were compared with one of the participants and NCE resulted in quicker reductions in problem behavior. 1 Although Iwata et al. (1990) suggested that the physical guidance necessary to keep an individual from escaping an instructional situation may constitute a form of punishment, the response reductions that typically occur under such procedures share characteristics of those associated with extinction (e.g., response bursts, gradual reductions; Iwata, Pace, Cowdery et al., 1994). One of the main benets of NCE is that it immediately reduces problem behavior while continuing to provide the functional reinforcer (Vollmer et al., 1995). Additionally, because NCE is an antecedent intervention, it does not require the occurrence of problem behavior to be effective, and might even prevent problem behavior from occurring. Another potential benet is that noncontingent reinforcement has been shown to be effective without extinction, which would make the intervention an option for when extinction is impractical (Lalli, Casey, & Kates, 1997). A potential concern with NCE is that the schedule of noncontingent breaks is quite dense at the beginning of intervention, which could be impractical or disruptive to the consumers environment. Like other inter- ventions that require schedule thinning, NCE requires the involvement of someone who can adequately calculate schedule values and oversee the thinning process. Another potential, but probably unlikely, concern is that noncontingent reinforcement has sometimes been shown to accidentally reinforce problem behavior (Vollmer, Ringdahl, Roane, & Marcus, 1997). In such an event, skipping or briey delaying scheduled breaks that occur just after problem behavior should eliminate the problem. We refer the reader to the book chapters by Carr and LeBlanc (2006) and Vollmer and Wright (2003) for additional information on noncontingent escape. Clinical Considerations and Decision Making The clinical decision-making model described here is in- tended for use by individuals with experience with functional assessment and function-based treatment of problem behavior, instructional curricula, and effective teaching procedures. Seasoned practitioners who have strong inuence over their clinical environments most likely have their own guides for selecting treatments. However, behavior analysts who have less control over clinical environments, such as those who consult or are newly in charge of the settings may nd these recom- mendations useful. Each of the treatments described in the previous section is empirically supported for the treatment of escape-maintained problem behavior. However, not every treatment is equally well-suited to a particular client or therapeutic environment. Identifying the function of problem behavior is a necessary precondition before selecting each of these interventions. Fortunately, there are numerous helpful resources for conduct- ing a functional assessment (e.g., Carr, LeBlanc, & Love, 2008; Iwata & Dozier, 2008; Vollmer, Marcus, Ringdahl, & Roane, 1995). The next step in the process is equally important and involves consideration of specic characteristics of the client and therapeutic environment. Some important client charac- teristics to consider include the current skill repertoire, level of compliance, and severity and dangerousness of the problem behavior. Some important environmental factors to consider include the appropriateness of the curriculum and instruction, tolerance for disruption to others in the environment, stafng ratios, and the amount of available technical expertise. BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 26 4/24/10 11:38:54 AM 27 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL Treatment Description Strengths Potential Limitations Activity Choice Offer a choice among selected tasks Might prevent problem behavior No lost instruction time Provides choice-making opportunities Increased compliance No programmed consequence for problem behavior Requires preparation of additional instructional materials Requires choice-making skills Requires an appropriate curriculum be in place Requires learners who can tolerate some instruction Curricular and Instructional Revision Change curricular targets or instructional procedures Results in improvements in teaching Might benet other learners in the environment Might produce more efcient and ef- fective skill acquisition Might prevent problem behavior Requires time, effort, and expertise to change curriculum/instruction No programmed consequence for problem behavior Demand Fading Remove all demands, then gradually reintroduce them over time; include escape extinction Immediately reduces problem behavior The rst step of the intervention (de- mand removal) is often already done Might prevent problem behavior Might increase tolerance of instruction A good match for dangerous behavior and large clients Gradually fading in demands might be logistically difcult Requires expertise to establish and oversee the fading process Periods of non-instruction could be disruptive to classroom activities Differential Negative Reinforcement of Alternative Behavior Provide a break from work after a new, alternative behavior and place the problem be- havior on extinction (see text for other variations) Actively targets new skills or increases existing ones Provides continued access to escape throughout the intervention May be used without extinction Periods of non-instruction could be disruptive to classroom activities Requires expertise to establish and oversee schedule thinning process Differential Negative Reinforcement of Zero Rates of Behavior Provide a break from work if the problem be- havior has not occurred for a specied amount of time and place the problem behavior on extinction Provides continued access to escape throughout the intervention Useful for increasing tolerance of neces- sary, but aversive, stimuli Requires constant monitoring for occurrences of problem behavior Periods of non-instruction could be disruptive to classroom activities Requires expertise to establish and oversee schedule thinning process Extinction Do not provide a break from work contingent on problem behavior; continue presenting the task regardless of problem behavior Provides a contingency for problem behavior Compatible with other treatments to enhance their effectiveness High response effort of implementation Might produce a response burst or aggression Might make behavior more resistant to extinction without strong treatment integrity Does not result in immediate response suppression Functional Communication Training Provide a break from work for a new, com- municative response and place the problem behavior on extinction (or punishment) Actively targets new skills or increases existing ones Provides continued access to escape throughout the intervention Preferred by some individuals over NCE and extinction May be used without extinction May result in high rates of the communicative response Does not result in immediate response suppression Periods of non-instruction could be disruptive to classroom activities Requires expertise to establish and oversee schedule thinning process Noncontingent Escape Provide breaks from work on a time-based schedule, irrespective of problem behavior Provides continued access to escape throughout the intervention Immediately reduces problem behavior Might prevent problem behavior May be used without extinction May produce adventitious reinforcement of problem behavior Periods of non-instruction could be disruptive to classroom activities Requires expertise to establish and oversee schedule thinning process Table. Strengths and potential limitations of treatments for escape-maintained problem behavior. BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 27 4/18/10 11:04:12 PM 28 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL The Table summarizes the important strengths and po- tential limitations of each treatment. Consider the example of NCE. For clients with very dangerous behavior, NCE is a promising option because it frequently produces immediate re- ductions in problem behavior, particularly when the schedule is nearly continuous. In addition, NCE can be implemented with or without extinction, making this treatment a good match for environments that are unable or unwilling to implement escape extinction. However, minimal instruction occurs during NCE, particularly at the onset of the treatment, and expertise is required to effectively guide the schedule thinning process. In addition, no new skill is explicitly targeted, which may be a concern for clients who need to develop functional commu- nication repertoires but may not be a concern for clients who already have those repertoires. A Clinical Model for Optimal Treatment Selection It may be challenging for practitioners to simultaneously consider all of the relevant client and environment variables that should impact treatment selection. One solution to this challenge is to prioritize the clinical considerations and follow a specic model in decision-making. The Figure illustrates a clinical model for sequentially asking and answering questions that will lead to differential treatment selection. The ordering of the questions is based on ethical responsibilities, safety and practical considerations, and organizational issues. Each time a question is answered negatively, 1 or 2 treatments become the optimal options. The earlier in the framework a question is answered afrmatively, the more possible treatments there are from which to choose. Refer to the Table for a comparison of the strengths and potential limitations of each treatment when deciding between multiple options. Extinction in isolation is presented as an optimal alternative in one area of the model; however, several of the procedures (noted with an asterisk in the Figure) can be implemented with or without extinction depending on the constraints of the clinical situation. If clinical progress turns a no response into a yes response, but some degree of problem behavior remains, return to the clinical model. For example, if implementation of NCE or demand fading after negatively answering question 3 produces some level of compliance, you may have the option of continuing with questions 4 and 5 in the model. The rst question about the appropriateness of the cur- riculum and instructional procedures is important for two reasons. First, it speaks to the behavior analysts ethical respon- sibility to promote effective learning environments rather than teaching individuals with disabilities to tolerate ineffective ones (Winett & Winkler, 1972). Second, improving curricular and instructional procedures is a practical way to directly address the escape contingency by abolishing the reinforcing value of escape. When clients are presented with tasks that are far above their current capabilities or are exposed to ineffective prompt- ing strategies, learning environments are typically aversive. The most direct way to address the problem would be to teach the relevant pre-requisite skills before advanced skills and to use more effective instructional strategies, thus abolishing escape as a negative reinforcer. Such changes should enhance learning in addition to decreasing problem behavior. The practitioner will only need to progress to the next step in the model if the curriculum and instruction are appropriate, if curriculum/in- structional revision fails to produce adequate treatment effects, or if inuence over these variables is not currently possible. The second question about behavioral severity and en- vironmental tolerance for the behavior speaks to the need to determine if there must be an immediate suppression of prob- lem behavior during treatment. Several scenarios may make it imperative that no or few problem behaviors occur at the onset of treatment. Those in the therapeutic environment may be unwilling or unable to allow a single instance of problem behavior or may insist on termination of services if even one more instance of problem behavior occurs. This situation is most likely to occur when the client is considered difcult to physically manage (e.g., a large, aggressive client), the behavior would produce unacceptable danger to the client (e.g., severe SIB, elopement), or if the behavior is socially offensive (e.g., public disrobing, sexual misbehavior). In these circumstances, providers often have already eliminated all demands in an effort to avoid problem behavior. The aforementioned circumstances drastically limit the number of optimal treatments because certain treatments that might eventually prove effective often do not produce immedi- ate suppression of problem behavior (e.g., extinction, FCT) and might represent a sudden reintroduction of demands into the environment (e.g., FCT, activity choice). On the other hand, demand fading and NCE immediately abolish the reinforcing value of escape and, thus, are typically associated with rapid reductions in problem behavior. In addition, these two treat- ments can be implemented without extinction, and stakehold- ers with a low tolerance for problem behavior are likely to also have little tolerance for implementation of escape extinction procedures. Consider demand fading and NCE as the optimal starting point under these circumstances. As treatment gains are made, it may become possible to consider other treatment options as an alternative or supplement and the next questions in the model can guide your selection at that time. Third, the practitioner should consider the clients current rate of compliance with instructions. If virtually no instructions are met with compliance, demand fading and NCE are still at- tractive options because their early phases include few instruc- tions with the client gradually encountering more as treatment progresses. Both of these procedures could be implemented with or without extinction depending on the environments tolerance of escape extinction. Because the curriculum and in- structional practices have already been deemed appropriate or have been revised to be appropriate, the client will presumably come into contact with sufcient reinforcement and effective prompting strategies for any newly occurring instances of compliance. Without the prior curriculum and instructional revision (i.e., question 1 in the model), it is unreasonable to expect sustained improvements in compliance and problem BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 28 4/18/10 11:04:12 PM 29 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL Functional Assessment Indicates Problem Behavior is Maintained by Escape from Demands 1. Is the curriculum appropriate and is instruction optimal? Yes No 2. Can the environment tolerate any level of problem behavior? Yes No 3. Are there any demands to which the client already complies? Yes No 4. Is time away from instruction tolerable? Yes No Curricular and Instructional Revision Demand Fading NCE Demand Fading* NCE* Activity Choice Extinction 5. Which is the most important clinical/educational goal? Communication Other Curricular Targets Tolerance of an Aversive Event DNRO* DNRA* FCT* Figure. A model for selecting function-based treatments for escape-maintained problem behavior. Note: *Consider including extinction if viable; DNRA = differential negative reinforcement of alternative behavior, DNRO = differential negative reinforcement of zero rates of behavior, FCT = functional communication training, NCE = noncontingent escape. BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 29 4/18/10 11:04:12 PM 30 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL behavior as instructional periods are increased. Other excellent treatments that involve a direct instructional component (e.g., FCT) or a requirement of compliance from the outset (e.g., DRA) are less optimal initially but might become viable when compliance becomes more reliable. Next, the practitioner should consider whether time away from instruction is tolerable. Some of the common concerns with breaks from instruction include logistical difculties asso- ciated with supervision of a learner away from the main learner group, stigma or fairness problems associated with one person getting a break while others do not, and loss of instructional opportunities when break schedules are dense. It may be pos- sible to discuss alternatives that would mitigate implementer concerns while increasing the number of potential treatment options. For example, in an inclusive or general education environment, the rst two concerns could be mitigated if the student were to remain at his/her desk while having a brief break from instruction (e.g., brief use of headphones during lecture). If you are unable to mitigate the concerns, the optimal treatments are activity choice, extinction, and DNRA with ex- tinction. When the option of choosing seems highly preferred by the learner, activity choice is a good option and it may be combined with other treatments such as DNRA or NCE. However, it has the drawback of requiring preparation of ad- ditional materials for the learners selection. Extinction has the advantage of directly addressing the contingency for problem behavior but the drawbacks of potential extinction-related side effects and high response effort of implementation. The nal question prompts the practitioner to choose the most pressing clinical or educational goal for the client and to select an optimal treatment accordingly. When a client does not have a meaningful communication repertoire, the optimal treatment is FCT because this treatment establishes a mand response that allows the client to synchronize breaks with his or her own motivating operations. If the client already has communication skills that would allow him to request a break, then consider targeting other important curricular areas (e.g., language, mathematics). When establishing the criterion to earn a break, remember that you can target one of many important dimensions of responding such as compliance or accuracy by providing breaks contingent on performance (i.e., DNRA). When the presenting problem involves an aversive event that has to occur for the clients well-being (e.g., medical procedures) rather than skill acquisition, DNRO presents an appealing option for producing tolerance to these events and should be initially implemented with the breaks occurring based on very brief intervals. Conclusion A number of effective treatments for escape-maintained behaviors have been developed and each has characteristics that make it optimal for certain environments and clients and less optimal for others. The present article summarizes the most commonly researched function-based treatments for escape- maintained behavior and the clinical contexts for which they are most appropriate. In addition, we provide a clinical model for selecting function-based treatments based on client charac- teristics and the constraints of the therapeutic environment. Our model is based on selecting a single optimal interven- tion at a time, which is advisable when the behavior analyst needs to train providers to prociency and ensure high treat- ment delity. However, one treatment may sometimes enhance the effects of another. For example, providing activity choice or adding an extinction contingency may enhance the effects of any of the other treatments (e.g., DNRA, FCT, NCE). However, the behavior analyst should consider whether the response effort associated with implementing additional treatment components is likely to produce fatigue or poor treatment integrity. One particular treatment combination to avoid is NCE combined with FCT because research indicates that NCE interferes with acquisition of the communication response, at least when the NCE schedule is rich (Goh, Iwata, & DeLeon, 2000). However, these two treatments might be implemented sequentially. In our model, a practitioner might initially select NCE as an optimal treatment (at questions 2 and 3) and elect to target a functional communication response after NCE has been successfully discontinued or the schedule has been thinned. We have attempted to integrate the ndings from a large experimental literature on treatments for escape-maintained problem behavior into a decision-making framework for prac- ticing behavior analysts. Although the model is based on the empirical literature, our clinical experience guided the ordering of the questions and the determination of the appropriateness and usefulness of treatments at different decision points. We have no experimental evidence that this particular model is more effective than any alternative, but it is a logical framework for the practitioner seeking guidance in treatment selection that could be experimentally validated in future research. References Asmus, J. M., Ringdahl, J. E., Sellers, J. A., Call, N. A., Andelman, M. C., & Wacker, D. P. (2004). Use of a short-term inpatient model to evaluate aberrant behavior: Outcome data summaries from 1996 to 2001. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 283-304. Athens, E. S., & Vollmer, T. R. (in press). An investigation of differential reinforcement of alternative behavior without extinction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. Buckley, S. D., & Newchok, D. K. (2006). Analysis and treatment of problem behavior evoked by music. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 141-144. Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior problems through functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111-126. Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2006). Noncontingent reinforcement as antecedent behavior support. In J. K. Luiselli (Ed.), Antecedent assessment & intervention: Supporting children & adults with developmental disabilities in community settings (pp. 147-164). Baltimore, MD: Brookes. BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 30 4/18/10 11:04:12 PM 31 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL Carr, J. E., LeBlanc, L. A., & Love, J. R. (2008). Experimental functional analysis of problem behavior. In W. T. ODonohue, J. E. Fisher, & S. C. Hayes (Eds.), Cognitive behavior therapy: Applying empirically supported techniques in your practice (2nd ed.) (pp. 211-221). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Cipani, E. (1998). Three behavioral functions of classroom noncompliance: Diagnostic and treatment implications. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disorders, 13, 66-72. Clarke, S., Dunlap, G., Foster-Johnson, L., Childs, K. E., Wilson, D., White, R., et al. (1995). Improving the conduct of students with behavioral disorders by incorporating student interests into curricular activities. Behavioral Disorders, 20, 221-237. Deitz, D. E. D., & Repp, A. C. (1983). Reducing behavior through reinforcement. Exceptional Education Quarterly, 3, 34-46. Ducharme, J. M., & Van Houten, R. (1994). Operant extinction in the treatment of severe maladaptive behavior: Adapting research to practice. Behavior Modication, 18, 139-170. Dunlap, G., Foster-Johnson, L., Clarke, S., Kern, L., & Childs, K. E. (1995). Modifying activities to produce functional outcomes: Effects on problem behaviors of students with disabilities. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 20, 248-258. Dunlap, G., & Kern, L. (1996). Modifying instructional activities to promote desirable behavior: A conceptual and practical framework. School Psychology Quarterly, 11, 297-312. Dunlap, G., Kern-Dunlap, L., Clarke, S., & Robbins, F. R. (1991). Functional assessment, curricular revisions, and severe behavior problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 387-397. Durand, V. M., & Merges, E. (2001). Functional communication training: A contemporary behavior analytic intervention for problem behaviors. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 16, 110-119. Dyer, K., Dunlap, G., & Winterling, V. (1990). Choice making on the serious problem behaviors of students with severe handicaps. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 515-524. Ferro, J., Foster-Johnson, L., & Dunlap, G. (1996). Relation between curricular activities and problem behaviors of students with mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 101, 184-194. Goh, H., Iwata, B. A., & DeLeon, I. G. (2000). Competition between noncontingent and contingent reinforcement schedules during response acquisition. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 195-205. Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Contrucci, S. A., & Maglieri, K. A. (1997). Evaluation of client preferences for function-based treatment packages. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 459-473. Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Maglieri, K. A. (2005). On the effectiveness of and preference for punishment and extinction components of function-based interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 51-65. Horner, R. H., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of response efciency on functionally equivalent competing behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 719-732. Houlihan, D. D., Sloane, H. N., Jones, R. N., & Patten, C. (1992). A review of behavioral conceptualizations and treatments of child noncompliance. Education and Treatment of Children, 15, 56-77. Ingvarsson, E. T., Hanley, G. P., & Welter, K. M. (2009). Treatment of escape-maintained behavior with positive reinforcement: The role of reinforcement contingency and density. Education and Treatment of Children, 32, 371-401. Iwata, B. A., & Dozier, C. L. (2008). Clinical application of functional analysis methodology. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 3-9. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An analysis of procedural form and function. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 131-144. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994). The functions of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215-240. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery, G. E., & Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental analysis and extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 11-27. Kern, L., Childs, K. E., Dunlap, G., Clarke, S., & Falk, G. D. (1994). Using assessment-based curricular intervention to improve the classroom behavior of a student with emotional and behavioral challenges. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 7-19. Kern, L., Vorndran, C. M., Hilt, A., Ringdahl, J. E., Adelman, B. E., & Dunlap, G. (1998). Choice as an intervention to improve behavior: A review of the literature. Journal of Behavioral Education, 8, 151-169. Kodak, T., Miltenberger, R. G., & Romanuik, C. (2003). Comparison of differential reinforcement and noncontingent reinforcement for the treatment of a childs multiply controlled problem behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 267-278. Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for severe problem behavior: Some procedural variations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 127-137. Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C., Borrero, J., Daniel, D., et al. (1999). Competition between positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 285-296. Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Developing a technology for the use of operant extinction in clinical settings: An examination of basic and applied research. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 345-382. Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1999). Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting and aggression during the treatment of self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 1-8. Love, J. R., Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2009). Functional assessment of problem behavior in children with autism spectrum disorders: A summary of 32 outpatient cases. Journal BAIP-Vol3No1.indb 31 4/18/10 11:04:12 PM 32 TREATMENT-SELECTION MODEL of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 363-372. Mace, F. C., Browder, D. M., & Lin, Y. (1987). Analysis of demand conditions associated with stereotypy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychology, 18, 25-31. McConnachie, G., & Carr, E. G. (1997). The effects of child behavior problems on the maintenance of intervention delity. Behavior Modication, 21, 123-158. McCurdy, M., Skinner, C. H., Grantham, K., Watson, T. S., & Hindman, P. (2001). Increasing on-task behavior in an elementary school student during mathematics seatwork by interspersing additional brief problems. School Psychology Review, 30, 2332. Munk, D. D., & Repp, A. C. (1994). The relationship between instructional variables and problem behavior: A review. Exceptional Children, 60, 390-402. Pace, G. M., Iwata, B. A., Cowdery, G. E., Andree, P. J., & McIntyre, T. (1993). Stimulus (instructional) fading during extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 205-212. Roberts, M. L., Mace, F. C., & Daggett, J. A. (1995). Preliminary comparison of two negative reinforcement schedules to reduce self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 579-580. Roberts, L. M., Marshall, J., Nelson, R., & Albers, C. A. (2001). Curriculum-based assessment procedures embedded within functional behavioral assessments: Identifying escape- motivated behaviors in a general education classroom. School Psychology Review, 30, 264272. Smith, R. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1997). Antecedent inuences on behavioral disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 343375. Smith, R. G., Iwata, B. A., Goh, H., & Shore, B. A. (1995). Analysis of establishing operations for self-injury maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 515-535. Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional communication training: A review and practical guide. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16-23. Vollmer, T. R., & Iwata, B. A. (1992). Differential reinforcement as treatment for behavior disorders: Procedural and functional variations. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 13, 393- 417. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Ringdahl, J. E. (1995). Noncontingent escape as treatment for self-injurious behavior maintained by negative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 15-26. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane, H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 561-576. Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., Roane, H. S., & Marcus, B. A. (1997). Negative side effects of noncontingent reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 161-164. Vollmer, T. R., Roane, H. S., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A. (1999). Evaluating treatment challenges with differential reinforcement of alternative behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 9-23. Vollmer, T. R., & Wright, C. S. (2003). Noncontingent reinforcement as treatment for problem behavior. In W. ODonohue, J. E. Fisher, & S. C. Hayes (Eds.), Cognitive behavior therapy: Applying empirically supported techniques in your practice (pp. 266-272). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Winett, R. A., & Winkler, R. C. (1972). Current behavior modication in the classroom: Be still, be quiet, be docile. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 499-504. Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., Mazaleski, J. L., & Lerman, D. C. (1994). Reemergence and extinction of self- injurious escape behavior during stimulus (instructional) fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 307-316. Author Notes We thank Amy Polick, Megan Heinicke, Laura Grow, Jessa Love, Anna Petursdottir, and Dorothea Lerman for their com- ments on an earlier version of the model. Address correspondence to James E. Carr, Department of Psychology, 226 Thach Hall, Auburn University, AL 36849- 5214 (e-mail: carr@auburn.edu).
PDF A Complete ABA Curriculum for Individuals on the Autism Spectrum with a Developmental Age of 1 4 Years A Step by Step Treatment Manual Including Skill A Journey of Development Using ABA Julie Knapp download