Overview: Functions of The Cerebellum
Overview: Functions of The Cerebellum
Overview: Functions of The Cerebellum
Figure 5.1
Cerebellum
The cerebellum (little brain) is a structure that is located at the back of the brain,
underlying the occipital and temporal lobes of the cerebral cortex (Figure 5.1). Although
the cerebellum accounts for approximately 10% of the brains volume, it contains over
50% of the total number of neurons in the brain. Historically, the cerebellum has been
considered a motor structure, because cerebellar damage leads to impairments in
motor control and posture and because the majority of the cerebellums outputs are to
parts of the motor system. Motor commands are not initiated in the cerebellum; rather,
the cerebellum modifies the motor commands of the descending pathways to make
movements more adaptive and accurate. The cerebellum is involved in the following
functions:
Maintenance of balance and posture. The cerebellum is important for making
postural adjustments in order to maintain balance. Through its input from vestibular
receptors and proprioceptors, it modulates commands to motor neurons to compensate
for shifts in body position or changes in load upon muscles. Patients with cerebellar
damage suffer from balance disorders, and they often develop stereotyped postural
strategies to compensate for this problem (e.g., a wide-based stance).
Coordination of voluntary movements. Most movements are composed of a number
of different muscle groups acting together in a temporally coordinated fashion. One
major function of the cerebellum is to coordinate the timing and force of these different
muscle groups to produce fluid limb or body movements.
Motor learning. The cerebellum is important for motor learning. The cerebellum plays
a major role in adapting and fine-tuning motor programs to make accurate movements
through a trial-and-error process (e.g., learning to hit a baseball).
Figure 5.2
(A) Cerebellar
deep nuclei and
cerebellar cortex
in an idealized
brain section. (B)
External
morphology of the
cerebellum.
Figure 5.3
Midsagittal crosssection of
cerebellum
showing the three
primary lobes of
the cerebellum.
Divisions of the cerebellum. Two major fissures running mediolaterally divide the
cerebellar cortex into three primary subdivisions (Figure 5.2B and Figure 5.3).
The posterolateral fissure separates the flocculonodular lobe from the corpus
cerebelli, and the primary fissure separates the corpus cerebelli into a posterior
lobe and an anterior lobe (Figure 5.4). The cerebellum is also divided sagittally into
three zones that run from medial to lateral (Fig. 5.4). The vermis (from the Latin word
for worm) is located along the midsagittal plane of the cerebellum. Directly lateral to
the vermis is the intermediate zone. Finally, the lateral hemispheres are located
lateral to the intermediate zone (there are no clear morphological borders between the
intermediate zone and the lateral hemisphere that are visible from a gross specimen).
Figure 5.4
Divisions of
cerebellum.
Click PLAY to
see
schematic
unfolding
of
cerebellum.
Cerebellar nuclei. All outputs from the cerebellum originate from the cerebellar
deep nuclei. Thus, a lesion to the cerebellar nuclei has the same effect as a complete
lesion of the entire cerebellum. It is important to know the inputs, outputs, and
anatomical relationships between the different cerebellar nuclei and the subdivisions of
the cerebellum (Figure 5.5).
Figure 5.5
Input and
output
pathways of
the
cerebellum.
Click on the
names of
each
cerebellum
functional
subdivision
(cerebrocere
bellum,
spinocerebel
lum, and
vestibulocer
ebellum) to
view each
pathway in
isolation.
The
cerebellar
deep nuclei
are the sole
outputs of
the
cerebellum.
1. The fastigial nucleus is the most medially located of the cerebellar nuclei. It
receives input from the vermis and from cerebellar afferents that carry
vestibular, proximal somatosensory, auditory, and visual information. It projects
to the vestibular nuclei and the reticular formation.
2. The interposed nuclei comprise the emboliform nucleus and the globose
nucleus. They are situated lateral to the fastigial nucleus. They receive input
from the intermediate zone and from cerebellar afferents that carry spinal,
proximal somatosensory, auditory, and visual information. They project to the
contralateral red nucleus (the origin of the rubrospinal tract).
3. The dentate nucleus is the largest of the cerebellar nuclei, located lateral to the
interposed nuclei. It receives input from the lateral hemisphere and from
cerebellar afferents that carry information from the cerebral cortex (via the
pontine nuclei). It projects to the contralateralred nucleus and
the ventrolateral (VL) thalamic nucleus.
4. The vestibular nuclei are located outside the cerebellum, in the medulla.
Hence, they are not strictly cerebellar nuclei, but they are considered to be
functionally equivalent to the cerebellar nuclei because their connectivity
patterns are identical to the cerebellar nuclei. The vestibular nuclei receive input
from the flocculonodular lobe and from the vestibular labyrinth. They project to
various motor nuclei and originate the vestibulospinal tracts.
In addition to these inputs, all cerebellar nuclei and all regions of cerebellum get special
inputs from the inferior olive of the medulla (discussed below).
It is convenient to remember that the anatomical locations of the cerebellar nuclei
correspond to the cerebellar cortex regions from which they receive input. Thus, the
medially located fastigial nucleus receives input from the medially located vermis; the
slightly lateral interposed nuclei receive input from the slightly lateral intermediate
zone; and the most lateral dentate nucleus receives input from the lateral hemispheres.
Cerebellar peduncles. Three fiber bundles carry the input and output of the
cerebellum.
1. The inferior cerebellar peduncle (also called the restiform body) primarily
contains afferent fibers from the medulla, as well as efferents to the vestibular
nuclei.
2. The middle cerebellar peduncle (also called the brachium pontis) primarily
contains afferents from the pontine nuclei.
3. The superior cerebellar peduncle (also called the brachium conjunctivum)
primarily contains efferent fibers from the cerebellar nuclei, as well as some
afferents from the spinocerebellar tract.
Thus, the inputs to the cerebellum are conveyed primarily through the inferior and
middle cerebellar peduncles, whereas the outputs are conveyed primarily through the
superior cerebellar peduncle. The inputs arise from the ipsilateral side of the body, and
the outputs also go to the ipsilateral side of the body. Note that this is true even for the
outputs to the contralateral red nucleus. Recall from the chapter on descending motor
pathways that the rubrospinal tract immediately crosses the midline after the fibers
leave the red nucleus. Thus, cerebellar output to the red nucleus affects the ipsilateral
side of the body by a double-crossed pathway. Unlike the cerebral cortex, the
cerebellum receives input from, and controls output to, the ipsilateral side of the body,
and damage to the cerebellum therefore results in deficits to the ipsilateral side of the
body.
The cerebellar cortex is divided into three layers (Figure 5.6). The innermost layer, the
granule cell layer, is made of 5 x 1010 small, tightly packed granule cells. The middle
layer, the Purkinje cell layer, is only 1-cell thick. The outer layer, the molecular layer,
is made of the axons of granule cells and the dendrites of Purkinje cells, as well as a
few other cell types. The Purkinje cell layer forms the border between the granule and
molecular layers.
Figure 5.6
Cerebellar
circuitry.
This basic
pattern is
repeated
throughout
all regions
of the
cerebellum.
Granule cells. Granule cells are very small, densely packed neurons that account for
the huge majority of neurons in the cerebellum. Indeed, cerebellar granule cells
account for more than half of the neurons in the entire brain. These cells receive input
from mossy fibers and project to the Purkinje cells.
Figure 5.7
Front view of
Purkinje cell.
Click PLAY to see
side view of the
Purkinje cell.
This view shows
that the cell is
virtually flat in this
dimension. Note
the parallel fibers
of the granule cells
that run
perpendicularly to
the Purkinje cell.
Purkinje cells. The Purkinje cell is one of the most striking cell types in the
mammalian brain. Its apical dendrites form a large fan of finely branched processes
(Figure 5.7). Remarkably, this dendritic tree is almost two-dimensional; looked at from
the side, the dendritic tree is flat (click PLAY on Figure 5.7). Moreover, all Purkinje cells
are oriented in parallel. This arrangement has important functional considerations, as
we shall see below.
Other cell types. In addition to the major cell types (granule cells and Purkinje cells),
the cerebellar cortex also contains various interneuron types, including the Golgi cell,
the basket cell, and the stellate cell.
Connectivity. The cerebellar cortex has a relatively simple, stereotyped connectivity
pattern that is identical throughout the whole structure. Figure 6 illustrates a simplified
diagram of the connectivity of the cerebellum. Cerebellar input can be divided into two
distinct classes.
1. Mossy fibers originate in the pontine nuclei, the spinal cord, the brainstem
reticular formation, and the vestibular nuclei, and they make excitatory
projections onto the cerebellar nuclei and onto granule cells in the cerebellar
cortex. They are called mossy fibers because of the tufted appearance of their
synaptic contacts with granule cells. There is a large degree of divergence in the
mossy fiber-granule cell connection, as each mossy fiber innervates hundreds of
granule cells. The granule cells send axons up toward the cortical surface. Each
axon bifurcates in the molecular layer, sending a collateral in opposite directions.
These fibers, called parallel fibers, run parallel to the folds of the cerebellar
cortex, where they make excitatory synapses with Purkinje cells along the way
(Figure 5.7, rotated view after PLAY). The two-dimensional arbors of the Purkinje
cell dendrites are oriented perpendicular to the parallel fibers. Thus, the
arrangement of Purkinje cells and parallel fibers resembles telephone lines
running between telephone poles. Each parallel fiber makes contact with
hundreds of Purkinje cells; because of the high degree of divergence of the
mossy fiber-granule cell synapses, the firing of each Purkinje cell can be
influenced (disynaptically) by thousands of mossy fibers.
2. Climbing fibers originate exclusively in the inferior olive and make excitatory
projections onto the cerebellar nuclei and onto the Purkinje cells of the
cerebellar cortex. They are called climbing fibers because their axons climb and
wrap around the dendrites of the Purkinje cell like a climbing vine. Each Purkinje
cell receives a single, extremely powerful input from a single climbing fiber. In
contrast to mossy fibers and parallel fibers, each climbing fiber contacts only 10
Purkinje cells on average, making ~300 synapses with each Purkinje cell. Thus,
the climbing fiber is a restricted, but extremely powerful, excitatory input onto
Purkinje cells.
The Purkinje cell is the sole source of output from the cerebellar cortex. It is
important to note that Purkinje cells make inhibitory connections onto the cerebellar
nuclei. (Note the distinction between the Purkinje cells, which constitute the sole output
of the cerebellar cortex, and the cerebellar nuclei, which constitute the sole output of
the entire cerebellum.) Almost all of the spikes generated by the Purkinje cell are
caused by its parallel-fiber inputs. These inputs cause the Purkinje cell to fire at a high
resting rate (~70 spikes/sec), tonically inhibiting its cerebellar nucleus targets. The
powerful inputs from climbing fibers occur less frequently (~1 spike/sec); thus, they
have a minor influence on the overall firing rate of the Purkinje cell. The Purkinje cell
spikes that are generated by climbing fibers are calcium-spikes, however, which allow
the climbing fibers to initiate a number of calcium-dependent changes in the Purkinje
cell. As described below, one important change appears to be a long-lasting change in
the strength of the parallel-fiber inputs to the Purkinje cell.
Figure 5.8
Vestibuloocular reflex (VOR) and cerebellar
learning. Click PLAY to begin demonstration. Under
normal conditions, when a human or animal subject
rotates the head back and forth, the eyes rotate in
an equal and opposite direction in order to keep the
image stable on the retina. The vestibular system
provides the input regarding the head movement,
and the motor system has to learn the precise
output commands in order to keep the image
stable. When magnifying glasses are placed on the
animal, the eyes do not move fast enough to
compensate for the increased speed of movement
of the magnified image, and thus the image moves
along the retina (termed retinal slip) in the
direction opposite to the movement of the head.
Over time, however, the motor system learns to
move the eyes faster (e.g., the gain of the eye
movement command is increased), and the image
becomes stable again. When the goggles are
removed, the eyes now move too quickly, causing
retinal slip in the same direction as head
movement. With time, the system will learn to
calibrate the VOR again. Patients and experimental
animals with damage to the vestibulocerebellum are
not able to adapt their VOR to the addition and
removal of the goggles, demonstrating the role of
the cerebellum in this form of motor learning.
Figure 5.9
A feedback
control
system,
such as the
thermostat
in your
home, is
sufficient for
slow
movements,
such as
posture. The
myotatic
reflex is an
example of a
feedback
control
system in
the spinal
cord.
can instruct the furnace to turn on at a moderate heat. It reads the new room
temperature, and, if it is still too cool, it instructs the furnace to deliver more heat, and
so on. Although this will eventually produce an accurate room temperature at the
desired point, it takes a number of cycles to reach that point. One possible solution for
quicker results would be to turn an enormous furnace on full-blast, such that is heats
the room very quickly. This solution, however, can generate another problem. It will
tend to cause the system to oscillate if the feedback pathways are slow. For example,
assume that the furnace can heat the room at the rate of 5 per second, but that it
takes 2 seconds for the thermometer to adjust to the new temperature, and for the new
error signal to turn the furnace off. In those 2 seconds, the furnace has heated the
room up 10, and now it is too warm. So the error signal turns on the air conditioner,
and it cools the room at 5/sec. Of course, it also takes 2 sec to receive the feedback,
and by the time it is told to shut off, it has cooled the room by 10. You can see what
happens: the system will be sent into an endless oscillation of being 5 too hot and 5
too cold. In order for a feedback system to work well, the transmission time of sensory
information through the comparator to the effector must be rapid compared to the time
of the action.
Feedback control systems work well only when the sensory feedback about the actual
output is fast relative to the actual output. If the actual output is faster than the
sensors ability to provide feedback, then the system will tend to oscillate between
overshooting and undershooting the desired output. Thus, a feedback controller is
useful for slow movements, like postural adjustments. The role of the myotatic reflex in
posture maintenance is an example of a feedback controller in the spinal cord, and the
cerebellum plays a role in coordinating these postural adjustments. Feedback control is
not effective for most of the fast movements we make routinely (such as an eye
movement or reaching out for a cup). For these movements, a feedforward controller is
needed.
Figure 5.10
A
feedforward
control
system is
needed for
fast
movements,
because a
feedback
system is
too slow.
Let us extend the thermostat example to see how a temperature controller operating as
a feedforward system would work to raise the temperature of a room from 70 to 75.
The controller would use diverse sensory information about the environment before
sending its command to the furnace (Figure 5.10). For example, it would read the
current temperature, the current humidity level, the size of the room, the number of
people in the room, and so forth. Based on this information, it would direct the furnace
to turn on for a pre-set period of time, and thats it. There would be no need to
continually compare the current temperature with the desired setting, as the system
has predetermined how long the furnace needs to be working in order to achieve the
desired temperature. How did the controller obtain this information? A feedforward
controller requires a large amount of experience in order to learn the appropriate
actions needed for each set of environmental conditions. If on one trial it turns the
furnace off too soon and the room does not reach the desired temperature, it adjusts its
programming such that the next time it encounters the same environmental conditions,
it turns the furnace on for a longer period of time. Through many such instances of trial
and error learning, the feedforward system creates a look-up table that tells it how
long the furnace needs to be active under the current conditions. The key distinction
between a feedback and feedforward system is that the feedback system uses sensory
information to generate an error signal during the control of a movement, whereas a
feedforward system uses sensory information in advance of a movement. Any error
signal about the final output is used by the feedforward system only to change its
programming of future movements.
representations of the entire sensory context at present and the desired motor output.
When the desired output is not achieved, the climbing fibers signal this error and
trigger a calcium spike in the Purkinje cell. The influx of calcium changes the connection
strengths between parallel fibers and Purkinje cells, such that the next time the same
behavioral context occurs, the motor output will be modified to more closely
approximate the desired output.
Figure 5.11
Figure 5.12
http://neuroscience.uth.tmc.edu/s3/chapter05.html