Research Digest
Research Digest
Research Digest
Examine.com
Research Digest
Issue 4
February 2015
Table of Contents
05
14
17
25
33
39
45
51
59
66
69
tionnaires and took some blood draws, and out of all those
Foreshadowing? Yes.
but are instead doing health research (hey, it should pay off
diabetic mice.
You dont buy the alpha lipoic acid. You have a little bit of
Contributors
Researchers
Trevor Kashey
Ph.D(c)
Alex Leaf
M.S(c)
Anders Nedergaard
Ph.D.
Jeff Rothschild
M.Sc., RD
Katherine Rizzone
M.D.
Spencer Nadolsky
D.O.
Editors
Gregory Lopez
Pharm.D.
Reviewers
Arya Sharma
Ph.D., M.D.
Natalie Muth
M.D., M.P.H., RD
Mark Kern
Ph.D., RD
Gillian Mandich
Ph.D(c)
Stephan Guyenet
Ph.D.
Sarah Ballantyne
Ph.D.
Introduction
mass during energy restriction in sedentary and athletic populations. These benefits are important for dietary adherence
and long-term success, but they are only a piece of the puzzle.
Dieting can be psychologically complex. The brain and nervous system communicate through small chemicals called
neurotransmitters. Collectively, these neurotransmitters are
what allow us to be aware, have emotion, remember things,
move our body, regulate body temperature, sleep, and do or
feel anything that our brain allows for. In fact, many diseases like Parkinson's, Alzheimers, depression, insomnia,
ADHD, and anxiety have been linked to neurotransmitter
imbalances.
acids (BCAAs) have been shown to alter brain neurochemistry through basic competition. That is, these amino
acids all share the same transporters that allow access into
the brain, and thus they all compete for entry. Since the
transporters are not specific to any one of the amino acids,
the largest determinant of which enters the brain is their
concentrations. Thus, if the plasma level of the BCAAs
increases, then brain concentrations of tryptophan, tyrosine, and their respective neurotransmitters is reduced.
Theoretically, this may have negative consequences for
mood, sleep, hunger, and overall liveliness.
On the other hand, carbohydrate intake has been observed
to increase serotonin production secondary to insulin promoting tryptophan uptake in the brain. Theoretically, this
may benefit mood. > The study under review aimed to compare the effects of different dietary protein-to-carbohydrate
ratios on cognitive performance, mood, and sleep quality
during short-term energy restriction.
The outcomes presented in this study were actually secondThere are many ways to classify neurotransmitters, but for
our purposes its especially important to understand the
role that amino acids play in different neurotransmitters.
Some amino acids, such as glycine, taurine, and glutamate,
serve directly as neurotransmitters, whereas other amino
acids like tyrosine and tryptophan serve as precursors for
neurotransmitter synthesis. Tyrosine is the precursor for
the synthesis of the catecholamines: dopamine, adrenaline
(epinephrine), and noradrenaline (norepinephrine). These
neurotransmitters play a central role in attention, learning,
motivation, and alertness. Tryptophan serves as the precursor for serotonin, which can have an indirect effect on
well-being and happiness, and plays a variety of other roles
as well.
Stratified block
randomization
Randomization methods are very
important for avoiding potential
sources of bias, yet often are not
reported adequately in journal articles.
When it comes to designing clinical trial groups, balance is key. If two groups
are tested, and one of the groups is
a good deal healthier than the other
one, the results are likely to be biased.
Randomization solves much of this
problem, by giving each subject an
equal likelihood of being in different
study groups. But what happens when
the sample size is small (as it often is
in trials, as huge intervention studies
require huge amounts of money)?
The smaller the sample size, the more
likely it is that groups will differ in characteristics that affect the outcome.
You can statistically control for known
factors, but you cant control for factors that arent measurable or that you
dont know about.
As opposed to simple randomization,
randomization through a stratified
block design allows the researchers to
statistically remove variables of the
study subjects that could potentially
influence the study results but arent
being studied. When randomly allocating the subjects into the experimental
and control groups, the researchers
first stratify them into blocks based
on traits they wish to hold constant
In this study, 39 people were kept under observation for a month while progressively consuming
fewer calories and engaging in more physical
activity as the study went on. Researchers administered cognitive tests and measured plasma amino
acid levels.
The original trial that provided data for this study assessed
the impact of protein intake on body composition. All three
groups lost weight during the energy deficit, but the total fat
loss as a percentage of weight loss was greater and the total
fat-free mass loss smaller in the moderate- and high-protein
Total mood
disturbances,
anger, and
tension [...] were
not significantly
different between
any of the groups,
suggesting that
diet composition
had no effect on
mood changes.
Similar things can be said for cognition, with all but one
feeling less alert upon waking during the first ten days of
would not explain why the sleep effects were not seen in the
other groups.
Another limitation of the study is that it was originally
So overall, what does this study tells us? It shows that a 40%
caveats in mind. This study does not tell us how the dietary
already extremely lean. Nor does this study tell us how the
compare.
Energy deficits have also been explored in several studies.
One found that two days of near 100% caloric deprivation
had no effect on mood in healthy young men, and a study
in physically active soldiers found a 30-day energy deficit
(40%) to not significantly affect mood-state in comparison
with a calorie-adequate diet.
In overweight and obese men and women, a six month long
This study is one of many that examine differences in cognition and mood between low- and high-carbohydrate
weight-loss diets that use different durations, study populations, and magnitude of energy deficit.
For instance, in an eight week study of overweight or obese
subjects, nearly all cognitive outcomes showed no difference
between low or high carbohydrate groups. In a separate
year-long study, obese subjects had similar mood improvements from both low-carb and low-fat diets in the first two
[...] overweight
and obese people
experience significant
improvements on a
range of subjective
symptoms after weight
loss, regardless of diet
composition.
high-protein group ended up with significantly higher plasma concentrations of the BCAAs, which the authors suggest
may have been the cause of the improved reaction time
Collectively, the above suggests that changes in mood are
reliant on a variety of factors, such as the length of the diet,
magnitude of caloric restriction, and occasionally composition of the diet. Although the evidence is somewhat
ed the administration
of a BCAA, essential
amino acids (EAA), or
a milk EAA mixture on
brain amino acid and
neurotransmitter concentrations. It found that
the BCAAs in an amount
similar to many low
dose BCAA supplements
that are currently being
marketed does indeed
12
Jumping into a 40% energy deficit may result in mood disturbances for the first ten or so days, which return to pre-dieting
levels by the third week, regardless of the protein-to-carbohydrate ratio. This matches up quite nicely with what many refer to
as the low-carb flu. The lack of differences between the groups
of the current study suggests that perhaps it should be called
the dieting flu, as it appears that these effects are not isolated to
low-carbohydrate diets. Similarly, cognition and subjective sleep
quality showed minimal changes, some of which were transient.
Any mood
or cognitive
disturbances
that do occur
appear to be
transient and
may be owed
to the caloric
deficit itself
rather than the
macronutrient
ratios.
negative ones.
If Americans have indeed
shifted their diets to align
more closely with the
DGA, the results are not
what was originally intended. On the other hand,
if the U.S. government
has been unable, for the
past 35 years, to convince
Americans to follow its
dietary advice, maybe its
time to quit trying.
Would
Americans
starting eating
dirtor chairs?
I dont think so.
Maybe, they
might just start
eatingbetter.
Introduction
Within the last few decades there have been massive developments in portable technology. High-powered devices have now become lightweight, convenient, and affordable.
Many activities, like book reading, have been digitized. With the development of this
technology, what once had been a common pastime to help get us to sleep may now
actually be doing the opposite by causing a shift in our circadian rhythm for sleep.
A circadian rhythm is essentially an organism's daily internal clock. In humans it
accounts for many of our physiological fluctuations throughout the day. A major molecule that affects our sleep biorhythms is melatonin. Many have heard of melatonin
used as a sleep aid, and for good reason. Melatonin is a hormone that is released by the
pineal gland in the brain and is involved with sleepiness and sleep regulation.
Melatonin production is heavily influenced by sunlight interacting with retinal pigments. When light hits the retina, arylalkylamine N-acetyltransferase production is
17
The
International
Agency for
Research on
Cancer, an
agency
directly
related the
World Health
Organization,
has classified
shift-working
as a probable
carcinogen.
18
out the duration of the study. The iPad was at a fixed angle
different light reflections with colorful comics) and technical reading (possibly sleep inducing). Participants remained
suppression, caused by iPad use before sleep, on sleep quality and feelings of lingering sleepiness after waking up.
19
Alzheimers disease.
Keeping in mind the suppressed melatonin levels and decreases in REM sleep, iPad users reported lower levels of sleepiness
in the evening. EEG readings confirmed this by showing less
power in the delta/theta frequency ranges (associated with
iPad use consistently suppressed night-time levels of melatonin, which were significantly lower than when paper
books were used. In fact, reading paper books resulted in no
suppression. Once the lights were dimmed, the melatonin
onset peak was shifted to a later time period with iPad use
sleep and drowsiness) when compared with print book readers. The following morning, iPad users were much sleepier
than print readers. It took iPad users on the order of hours to
fully recover from their sleep episode and gain full alertness
after waking in the morning. Chronic use of these devices
before bed may impair wakefulness, to the point where iPad
users might not feel truly awake until late morning.
Data for the print book readers was essentially the opposite, with greater feelings of tiredness in the evening and
increased wakefulness in the morning. This increase of
tiredness in the morning experienced by iPad users is likely
related to decrease in REM sleep, and might be explained
by the circadian phase shift. Since the wake times were set
between the groups, and the phase was shifted to a later
time, the study participants who used iPads essentially woke
up when their body thought they should still be sleeping. If
Effectively, the
iPad artificially
inflated the length
of the day
perceived by the
brain, by about an
hour and a half!
20
sleep and wake times were not regulated, its possible that this may not have been
observed. However, since most people must wake at a particular time (but not go
to bed at a particular time) its easy to see how these issues quickly compound.
The iPads peak light output is in the blue range of the visible spectrum at 452 nm.
The physiological effects from this blue light makes sense given the light output of
humans original light source: sunlight also peaks under 500 nm, and melanopsin
pigment in the human eye absorbs in a peak range of about 480 nm. Its not unrealistic to posit an artificial sun response when exposed to these higher energy
photons. Even though this wavelength may not seem as bright to humans when
exposed to an equal power of white light (which peaks at 612 nm), what mostly
matters to the brain is the excitation of the correct eye pigments to depress the
production of the arylalkylamine N-acetyltransferase, which in turn decreases
circulating melatonin.
In the short term, a person may get acute sleep onset insomnia due to a shift in
circadian rhythm. However, using electronic devices that emit blue light may
have biological ramifications in the long term, especially since chronic melatonin
suppression had been linked to an increased risk of various types of cancers.
Unlike in this study, though, most people self-select their bedtimes (and to a
lesser extent wake times). If a self-selected bedtime and wake time are not aligned
with the bodys natural circadian rhythm, this, combined with blue-light exposure, may further exacerbate the phase-shift phenomenon and lead to chronic
issues associated with sleep deprivation.
Lastly, a four-hour read time was established in this study, when many people
may spend even longer times in front of a screen (either TV, computer, tablet,
or phone) from afternoon to bedtime. The effects of longer exposure times are
unknown, while tablet and phone displays continue to get brighter each year.
Study participants using iPads before bed took about ten minutes
longer to fall asleep, experienced less REM sleep, and found it much
harder to feel fully awake in the morning than people that read print
books before bed.
The findings are somewhat scary for the average person who loves the Internet,
which houses a vast collection of research on nutrition and health issues (and cat
pictures) that can easily keep you up for hours. Back-lit tablets suppress and delay
21
ing humans?
understated. This study only had people in a phase shifted state for five days, where a person may be chronically
ditions. But this is not the case. On the one hand, study
tent rate. The use of these devices near bed time is likely to
inactive adult.
If you have to
use a tablet/
laptop/phone at
night, it may be
helpful to dim
the screen as low
as possible, while
still being able
to read.
make you look quite silly (but also smart and innovative
even mood. If you cant or wont use these glasses, but still
more red as the sun sets. Some apps can even lower screen
use a bit earlier each week) can make the transition easier.
rhythm?
If melatonin is used as a sleep aid in order to fall asleep
The study authors noted that the relatively easier time print-
effective and side effect free treatment option for those with
sleep issues.
23
ble amount.
Dont you dare read this past 10pm. But if you choose to
Introduction
Red meat is one of the foods the media loves to hate, perhaps while still secretly loving it. Any time new research
is published that deals with red meat consumption and X
(where X might be cancer, heart disease, diabetes, etc.) the
major news outlets pick it up, running scary headlines to
attract clicks. The science, however, is typically a bit more
nuanced than the bold claims of news headlines would lead
the average reader to believe. So is there actually anything
to worry about when eating red meat?
26
Though the media often links red meat consumption to an increased risk of cancer, scientists are still
trying to determine what aspect of red meat results
in cancer, or if this link exists at all. Researchers in
this study tried to determine if red meat consumption results in systemic inflammation due to the
presence of foreign, meat-derived molecules.
phospho-N-acetylneuraminic acid hydroxylase (CMAH)
els in milk and cheeses, bison, lamb, and pork. In beef, the
Neu5Gc content was approximately 2-30 times higher than
in other red meats and ranged from 0.025-.231mg/g, the
highest of any of the tested foods other than caviar. The
Neu5Gc content of beef varied greatly however, and the
lower range was more in line with other foods. By contrast,
poultry, dairy, and seafood were all high in Neu5Ac.
The researchers used a genetically modified mouse model for their experiments, as is often done in initial in vivo
studies. This particular mouse had the Cmah gene removed,
meaning that they could not produce the cytidine mono-
Neu5Gc.
meat. After being fed this diet for several weeks, the levels
28
Scientists crack
why red meat is
linked with cancer
- and SUGAR may
be to blame.
- Headline on the Daily Mail, 12/30/14
In the second experiment, the test mice were first exposed
to Neu5Gc immunogens (which are able to induce an
immune response in addition to an antibody response)
Mice fed diets high in a cell surface sugar molecule found in red meat had higher levels of
inflammation markers. The inflammatory
response was specific to Neu5Gc and not the similar Neu5Ac precursor molecule. The combination
of consuming that sugar molecule and manipulating the mouse immune system to make antibodies
against that sugar molecule caused mice to develop more liver tumors than control groups.
12/30/14
know why red meat causes cancer, this study actually had
some limitations. First off - there is no human data present-
Because the mice lacked the Cmah gene, they were unable
nistic question, but this may just be the first baby step on
29
large scale human study that includes a wide variety of population factors (age,
sex, race, weight) would. Also, the PSM-enhanced food intervention was meant
to simulate only red meat consumption, not to simulate a typical human diet that
is usually full of varied food products other than red meat. For example, a recent
small study in humans suggested that consumption of a certain type of starch
may counteract some of the cancer risk effects of red meat consumption. And the
immune exposure to the Neu5Gc immunogen in the final experiment introduced
an external factor at a single point in time, instead of a lifetime of immune system
exposure that likely has varied and currently unknown effects. The immune system is incredibly complex, and there are examples of immune system responses
having both pro- and anti-cancer effects.
As the study authors note, there is currently no published research that examines
the correlation of anti-Neu5Gc antibodies in humans with cancer risk. So it will
be important for future research to examine the mechanism found in this study
in a human sample. Finally, there is the question of organ system relevance. This
particular mouse model was used because there is a low level background incidence of spontaneous tumor formation in the liver over time, which more closely
resembles the spontaneous formation of tumors in older humans rather than
tumors that are artificially implanted in mouse organs for research purposes.
However, red meat consumption has not been correlated with increased liver
cancers. Some of the primary non-genetic risk factors for that disease are certain
viral infections and alcohol consumption, both of which may also involve some
aspect of chronic inflammation, but have never been linked to Neu5Gc. Cancer is
a complex disorder, so its difficult to draw direct comparisons between a tumor
that forms in the liver in one species versus one that forms in the colon in another species. The tumors in the mouse did show incorporation of Neu5Gc into the
tissue, but Neu5Gc has been found in a number of different malignant tissues in
humans (as well as in healthy tissue), so there may be non-specific incorporation
of the molecule into tumors in general.
The results of this study suggest a possibly novel explanation for the correlation
between red meat consumption and cancer, and might be able to explain some of
the contradictions with previously proposed explanations. The jury is still out on
whether this is relevant to humans. The research team believes in the importance
of this study, however. The last two authors listed on the paper have licensed
anti-Neu5Gc antibody technology from their institution and co-founded a company that is investigating targeted antibodies as a potential cancer therapeutic. Its
possible that regardless of any mechanistic explanations, Neu5Gc still might be
an interesting new target for cancer drugs, or perhaps a biomarker that can be
used in additional research.
30
This study provides evidence to support a molecular link between meat consumption and cancer
caused by inflammation, but since no human
evidence was gathered in this trial, much more
research is needed before these results can be
applied to people.
The fact that the genetically modified mice lacked the gene
that is also non-functioning in humans did make this
mouse model more human-like for the purposes of this
studys aims. The quantity of the food exposure is at least
a slight but consistent correlation between red meat consumption and elevated cancer risk in humans, but rather
attempting to tease out a possible molecular cause for the
effects seen, given that previous theories such as mutagens
created by grilling and generation of free radicals may not
explain the whole story.
Even one of the study authors commented in an interview
that dietary changes are probably not necessary for everybody, and if this line of research does bear out it may be
more useful for people with either a previous personal history of cancer, or a family history of cancer.
31
32
Sodium phosphate:
a potentially underutilized
ergogenic aid
Effect of sodium phosphate supplementation
on repeated high-intensity cycling efforts
Introduction
33
research setting that's a more realistic reflection of real-life cycling conditions (repeated maximal sprint and short time-trial efforts), after six days
of sodium phosphate supplementation, as well as four days after stopping
supplementation, to determine any lasting effects.
this study. As seen in Figure 1, these are elite level athletes who ride about
200 miles per week, with an average of 5.4 years of racing experience and
VO2 max of 71 ml/kg/min.
After an initial familiarization session and assessment, baseline performance was measured, followed by six days of supplementation with either
sodium phosphate or a placebo. The performance testing was then repeated
on day one and day four after stopping the supplementation.
Why do study
results differ for a
given ergogenic
supplement?
One reason that sports supplements
can show benefit in some studies
but not others is because there are
many different testing procedures
that researchers can use to determine if there is an effect from the
supplement trial.
For example, studies can test
participants using a time-trial (covering a pre-set distance as quickly
as possible), time to exhaustion
(maintaining a pre-set pace for as
long as possible), or repeated sprint
tests (generating as much power as
possible each time). Within these
different protocols there is a lot of
potential variability, like the distance of the time trials, the intensity
for the time to exhaustion tests, or
the number of repeated sprints to
be measured, as well as rest time
between efforts, whether the participants are recreational athletes or
trained professionals, etc.
This was a double-blind placebo-controlled study. Participants were given either 50 milligrams per kilogram of fat-free mass per day (about 3.3
grams) of sodium phosphate or placebo (mix of glucose and table salt),
divided into four doses and taken with meals (every four to five hours) for
six days.
Great care was taken to replicate the potential confounding variables before
each test. Participants did not exercise for 24 hours before each trial, and
testing took place at the same time of day (within an hour) to control for
34
of all food and drink intake prior to the initial test and were
each set.
Another important aspect of this study is that no differences
Seventeen competitive male cyclists supplemented sodium phosphate or a placebo for six days.
Performance tests were done on the first and
fourth day after supplementation stopped.
in power output were found between day one and day four
in the sodium phosphate group, meaning the effects of the
supplementation were still present four days after supplementation had ceased. No changes in power output were
observed in the placebo group throughout the testing.
No differences in serum phosphate concentrations were
reported between the groups at baseline, day one, or day
four. However, compared with baseline, serum phosphate
output (but not time trial power output) would likely still
placebo group.
is reasonable to think that sodium phosphate would benefit endurance athletes competing in events lasting not only
Cyclists supplementing sodium phosphate experienced a 5% boost to power output during the
performance test. Most of the benefits were
observed later in the tests, suggesting sodium
phosphate can reduce fatigue.
36
dosing period is, how long the effects will last upon stop-
sodium phosphate. Previous research using dicalcium phosphate or calcium phosphate has failed to find performance
improvements. It is possible that the lack of performance
enhancements were due to using a different loading protocol (in some cases only giving a single dose), or possibly
lower bioavailability. However, further research is needed to
confirm this hypothesis.
How much sodium phosphate should I supplement?
Studies have generally used three to five grams or 50 milligrams per kilogram of fat-free mass, per day. However,
different dosages havent been compared much against each
other. And as mentioned previously, studies tend to be in
males only. Along with the limited sample size of many studies, and their inclusion of highly-trained endurance athletes,
the results may not apply as well to a typical active individual.
Will sodium phosphate supplementation cause GI distress?
It is possible that high doses of sodium phosphate can cause
gastrointestinal distress. To minimize this side-effect, sodium phosphate is best ingested in smaller doses spread in
three to four doses throughout the day, with food.
What about the association between serum phosphorous
and cardiovascular disease?
There is an association between serum phosphate and
cardiovascular disease, however this study showed that supplementation does not raise serum levels and paradoxically
may even decrease them.
Cyclists, endurance athletes, and sodium phosphate aficionados (do they exist? maybe they should) can check out the
private Facebook ERD forum for more discussion on this
paper.
38
On the whey to
getting lean: one
more round of
whey vs. soy
protein feeding affects protein synthesis at the whole-body level, as well as specifically in the muscle tissue (obtained from muscle biopsy specimens).
Measuring protein synthesis (or degradation) in muscle is the best biomarker of
ongoing muscle anabolism or catabolism that exists. Protein synthesis during
weight loss is important since keeping muscle on helps regulate blood
sugar and keeps metabolism higher. Obtaining information about
fat or protein turnover at a particular point in time can tell us
how well the body is conserving lean mass during weight loss,
which is what this study intended to do.
39
Essential
Amino Acid
Whey Protein
Isolate
Soy Protein
Isolate
Isoleucine
Leucine
Lysine
Methionine
Phenylalanine
Threonine
Tryptophan
Valine
Total BCAAs
Total EAAs
6.1
12.2
10.2
3.3
3.0
6.8
1.8
5.9
24.2
49.2
4.9
8.2
6.3
1.3
5.2
3.8
1.3
5.0
18.1
36.0
area under the curve [AUC]) from whey was almost three
times higher than that for soy, whereas the essential and
Overweight and obese participants were randomized to receive either soy or whey protein,
or a carbohydrate control, and put on a hypocaloric diet. Body composition measurements and
muscle protein synthesis were measured and compared between groups.
The results regarding amino acid, glucose, insulin, and glycerol concentrations were in line with numerous previous
observations.
calls for more subjects. The study duration was short (14
days) and therefore the changes in lean and fat mass are
small compared to the margin of error using a DXA scanner, meaning that if any difference did exist between groups,
synthesis measurement
What the study did show, however, was that whey stimu-
Introduction
For example, it was once believed that monosodium glutamate (MSG) could cause headaches. This belief was largely
the result of a doctor who wrote a letter to the New England
Journal of Medicine on Chinese restaurant syndrome, his
term for the physical effects of his own overindulgences.
The link between MSG and headache has not been proven
in rigorous studies, and the evidence is covered on Examine.
com. Yet that single letter was enough to make MSG the
most researched dietary factor related to headaches.
45
factors too. One of the most common factors is hypertension. Because hypertension is associated with headaches,
site, five days per week. All of their other food was provid-
Study participants were split into two groups: eating a diet designed
to reduce hypertension, or a typical American diet. Then, each
participant spent a month eating at each of three different sodium
levels: low, medium, or high. Researchers measured blood pressure
and body weight, as well as tracked when headaches occurred.
Figure 1: Incidence of
severe headaches by
diet and sodium intake
The high sodium diets (whether typical or DASH) had a significantly greater
incidence of headaches compared to the low sodium diets.
Going from low to intermediate to high sodium levels steadily increased headache
odds in both the DASH diet (36% to 38% to 43%) and control diets (39% to 41% to
47%). However, the difference in headache risk between the intermediate sodium
intake group and the lowest and highest sodium intake groups was not statistically significant. This may be related to the fact that this is a secondary analysis of a
study not specifically designed to analyze headache occurrence. Headaches were
were only measured once in each study period, and the study didnt collect data on
previous headache prevalence, among other design limitations.
Interestingly, the association between headache occurrence and sodium intake
persisted regardless of hypertension status or blood pressure. As one might
expect in a trial of hypertensive individuals who were fed the DASH diet, some
participants experienced a decrease in blood pressure below hypertensive levels.
However, even in these individuals, the correlation between headache risk and
sodium intake remained.
Perhaps most surprisingly, there was no overall difference in headache occurrence between the healthy DASH diet and the typical American diet. There
was however a difference between DASH dieters eating low sodium (36% had
headaches) and control dieters eating high sodium (47% had headaches).
The study participants also classified their headaches as mild, moderate, or severe.
Most headaches were classified as mild, and higher sodium intakes generally were
associated with a bit higher (although not statistically significant) mild-to-moderate headache rates. Despite the small number of severe headaches, Figure 1 shows
that high sodium seemed to have an association with severe headache incidence.
This association trended toward significance in both diet groups.
47
refute anecdotal evidence linking headache to MSG conThis is supported by the data that show that hyperten-
are a panacea of sorts, and can curb pretty much any malady
are probably correlated, but its still not known how. The
happens. Regardless, this study still makes the case for lower
which may be one of the reasons that this studys findings are
only significant for the high and low sodium intake groups.
Its relatively
certain that
sodium intake
(or intake of
sodium-rich
foods) affects
headache
occurrence, but
theres no good
explanation
as to how that
happens.
quite sparse.
Lastly, those with low blood pressure can also find salt to be
useful when facing hypotensive episodes.
50
Many people say that dieting is hard, and that keeping the lost weight off is even harder. But
we dont need to take their word for it, the science of weight loss backs up their claims. One
review, containing evidence gathered during most of the 20th century, found that only about
15% of obese people were successful in keeping a significant amount of weight off long-term
through dieting!
Why is dieting so hard? Maybe its because the way we go about dieting isnt quite right.
Government agencies in the U.S., U.K., and Australia all recommend an initial, gradual weight
loss strategy of one to two pounds a week. Gradual weight loss is recommended for several
reasons. It allows more time for people to form the habit of eating fewer calories. Also, crash
diets with very low daily caloric intake can sometimes lead to nutritional deficiencies. Plus, its
claimed that crash diets lead to larger rebounds after the diet is over, which makes sense considering that very low calorie diets have been seen to lower basal metabolic rate.
51
their diet with the goal of 1800 calories per day succeeded
the literature suggests that faster initial weight loss was associated with better long-term weight maintenance, as long
as proper support is provided. A recent trial, which encouraged older, obese women to decrease daily caloric intake
to 1200 while increasing physical activity, found that the
people who achieved more rapid weight loss both lost more
weight and kept more weight off. However, this trial was not
randomized, so its hard to say whether the rapid weight loss
caused the increased success in weight maintenance or not.
Sustained rapid weight loss is a bit more common in clinical
settings. Protein-sparing modified fasts (PSMFs) are sometimes prescribed to very obese patients, and have calorie
intakes of 500-600 kcal per day. For these patients, PSMFs
can have better long-term weight loss than other approaches. While there is a
rebound, as with any
diet, adherence is fairly
high and people remain
motivated because they
can see the scale moving. These patients, as
they tend to be very
heavy, also tend to face
substantial and often
immediate health risks
if weight is not lost.
The purpose of this
study was to address
Melbourne, Australia. To be included in the trial, the participants had to be obese (defined as a BMI between 30 and
45) but otherwise healthy adults between 18 and 70 years
old. People who smoked, were on medications that could
affect weight, had any clinically significant disease (such as
diabetes), or who used weight-loss drugs or adhered to a
very low calorie diet three months prior to recruitment were
all excluded.
The 204 participants were studied in two different phases.
The goal of the first phase was to have participants lose 15%
Random samples,
random assignment
This study was a randomized trial. But what
exactly was randomized, and why does randomness matter in the first place? There are two
types of randomness that pertain to trials like
this: random sampling and random assignment.
Random sampling is when each member of a
population has an equal chance of being selected for a study. It allows you generalize the result
of a study from the subset included in the study
to the entire population. Why cant you do this
without a random sample? Because there may
be bias otherwise. For instance, if we put a poll
up on Examine.com to ask people Do you care
about the evidence behind abstracts? wed
probably get a large number of yes responses.
You Examine.com readers exceptionally curious,
and definitely not a random and representative
sample.
This study did not use a random sample of people, but instead enrolled people who answered
an ad or heard about the study through word of
mouth. Since this sample could be biased (for
instance, perhaps those who answered the ad
were very motivated to lose weight), it would be
hard to generalize the results of this study to all
people.
However, this study used another kind of randomness that is also important: they randomly
assigned participants to groups. This helps to
infer causality in a study because it helps to get
rid of confounding factors.
One of the
biggest findings
from this study
is that the rate of
weight loss does
not affect weight
regain in the tested
population
(one from the slow group and five from the fast group)
cholecystitis, a condition known to occur with rapid weight changes, where the bile
duct of the gallbladder is blocked, which leads to inflammation. The authors of the
study considered this to likely be caused by the rapid weight loss. Two other serious
adverse events occurred in the rapid weight loss group (cancer) that the authors do
not think was caused by the rapid weight loss. One person in the rapid weight loss
group also withdrew due to worsening depression, on which the authors did not
comment. There were no adverse events in the slower weight loss group.
Its quite
apparent
that the
body fights
losing
weight and
keeping it
off.
The fast weight loss group had an easier time sticking to the diet
than the slow weight loss group. The fast weight loss group was also
more successful in losing at least 12.5% of their weight in phase 1 of
the study. Both groups gained back about 30 pounds during phase 2
while trying to maintain their weight loss, with no significant difference between the two groups. Hormonal and subjective measures of
hunger rose throughout phase 1, and only dipped slightly in phase 2.
One of the biggest findings from this study is that the rate of weight loss does not
affect weight regain in the tested population: obese nonsmokers, who dont take
any weight loss drugs and who dont have diabetes. This puts a dent in the idea
that rapid weight loss leads to greater weight gain down the road. It also tells us
that rapid weight loss can be even more successful than gradual weight loss, since
more people were able to stick to the rapid weight loss plan than the gradual one,
while also meeting their weight loss goal.
It also tells us something many of us already know: dieting is hard. The total
weight loss for both groups ended up being about 5%. Most of the weight lost
during the dieting phase of the study was regained during the maintenance phase,
although that phase was an incredible 144 weeks long, so its not like the weight
was regained right away. Its also important to note that results reflect averages for
the sample -- some may have been wildly successful, while for others life events
may have gotten in the way, as they often do. In other words, there can be quite a
bit of human variation when trying rapid vs gradual weight loss.
Its quite apparent that the body fights losing weight and keeping it off. Hunger
was observed to increase and persist in both groups throughout both phases,
boosted by a rise in ghrelin (a hunger hormone) that persisted for at least three
years (the length of this study), and which doesnt level off much, even as weight
is regained. Leptin levels (a satiety hormone) also remained low in people who
56
Making more
choices drains
willpower, which
has been shown
to affect food
choice.
meal replacements that use high-quality protein and are fortified with nutrients have a better safety record when used
under medical supervision. Early PSMF diets employed for
rapid weight loss used fairly incomplete proteins like collagen, along with very few micronutrients, and thus do not
apply to modern PSMFs.
There are some aspects of the rapid weight loss plan that
may have helped the participants of this study be more
successful. First, participants in the fast group in this study
didnt have to measure portions, as they completely relied
on meal replacements to lose weight, whereas people in the
slow group still ate some conventional meals. Measuring
portions to count calories isnt always accurate and can be
hard to learn, which may explain some of the fast groups
success. Also, the use of meal replacements eliminates
Despite the fact that some government agencies and common sense dictates that losing weight more slowly is more
sustainable, this study showed that rapid weight loss can be
a more effective method of losing weight, and doesnt necessarily lead to more weight gain. Very low calorie diets have
had a history of safety concerns, primarily caused by poor
nutrition. At such a low calorie intake, youre unlikely to
get the nutrients you need to be healthy. However, modern
variety in the diet, which helps satiety and leads to less food
eaten, and has been seen to increase diet compliance and
weight loss in another study. The psychological phenomenon of decision fatigue may also play a role here. Making
more choices drains willpower, which has been shown to
affect food choice.
While caution in extrapolating the results of this study to
other populations is warranted, this study is an important
57
obesity.
over three years. After one year, they had still retained more
ward than slower weight loss. This method also had a higher
the road.
Is the glycemic
index actually
useful for making
food choices?
Foods that contain similar amounts of carbohydrates may differ in the amount and
speed at which they raise blood glucose levels. This quality is referred to as the
glycemic index (GI), and is based upon the blood glucose response (total
area under the glucose curve) over two hours to 50 grams of its carbohydrate content, as compared with 50 grams of glucose.
The higher a GI value is, the more the food raises blood sugar in a
way similar to pure glucose. Lower GI carbs are those that digest
more slowly and dont raise raise blood glucose to the same degree.
However, GI can also be a function of insulin response, and not
just a function of carbohydrate digestion or meal composition.
It has become common practice among healthcare professionals to
suggest an individual consume low glycemic index carbohydrates.
However the effects of GI on diabetes and cardiovascular disease (CVD)
risk factors have been equivocal in trials, as well as meta-analysis of
observational studies.
59
been shown to reduce blood pressure, lower risk of coronary heart disease, and improve bone mineral status. The
cessed food.
meals used in the study for high and low glycemic index
Examples
Whole wheat bread,
rice, quinoa
Apples, bananas, oranges
Spinach, tomatoes, carrots
Milk, yogurt, cheese
Beef, chicken, salmon
Almonds, cashews, beans
Avocado, olive oil
Number of Servings/day
6-12
4-6
4-6
2-4
1.5-2.5
3-6/week
2-4
60
Low carb,
high GI
Low carb,
low GI
High carb,
high GI
High carb,
low GI
Kcal
2011
1993
2011
1998
Protein (%)
23
23
16
16
Carbs (%)
41
40
58
57
Fat (%)
37
37
27
27
Fiber (g)
29
33
32
37
Potassium
(mg)
3949
4026
39663
4103
Glycemic
Load
112
64
172
104
Area under the glucose curve (AUC) this is a way to measure how much and for how long a persons blood sugar is
high-carb diets.
Low-GI,
low-carb diet
vs
High-GI,
high-carb diet
Triglyceride levels
decreased by 23%
Low-GI,
high-carb diet
vs
High-GI,
high-carb diet
Insulin sensitivity
decreased by 20%
LDL-cholesterol
increased by 6%
Low-GI,
low-carb diet
vs
High-GI,
low-carb diet
Triglyceride levels
decreased by 5%
In line with the prevailing opinion in the medical community, the authors of this study expected that consuming
foods with a lower GI would lead to improvements in insulin sensitivity and CVD risk factors. Unexpectedly, the low
GI, high-carb diet compared with the high GI, high-carb
diet actually decreased insulin sensitivity and increased
LDL-cholesterol and LDL apo-B levels.
It has been well established that a DASH-type diet can successfully lower blood pressure, and the same authors have
previously shown that a lower-carb DASH diet can reduce
plasma triglycerides, VLDL levels, and diastolic blood pressure. Varying the glycemic index of the carbohydrates in the
DASH-type of diet had yet to be studied. This study suggests that in the context of a DASH diet, low GI foods do
not improve CVD risk factors and may actually have some
detrimental effects compared with higher GI foods.
On a high-carb diet (58% of daily energy intake), choosing
foods with a lower GI had no effect on levels of HDL cholesterol, triglycerides, or blood pressure, and led to small
but unfavorable changes in LDL-cholesterol and insulin
sensitivity. On a low-carb diet (40% of daily energy intake),
choosing foods with a lower GI had no effect on levels of
HDL-cholesterol, LDL-cholesterol, insulin sensitivity or
62
well as refined bakery products, may cause a disproportionately high insulin response.
Focusing dietary
recommendations
simply on the
glycemic index
appears to be an
oversimplification
of the multifactorial
process involved
in healthy blood
sugar control.
Additional research has suggested that glycemic index values may be related as much to clearance of glucose as they
are to entry of glucose into the bloodstream. Focusing
dietary recommendations simply on the glycemic index
appears to be an oversimplification of the multifactorial
process involved in healthy blood sugar control.
Although LDL-cholesterol was higher in the high-carb, low
GI group, the researchers did not measure LDL-particle
63
In the context of
a diet rich in fruit
and vegetables
and low in
processed foods,
glycemic index
may not matter a
whole lot.
size or LDL-particle number. It is possible that these changes in LDL-cholesterol could be benign or even favorable.
Additionally, the low carb diets contained higher amounts
of protein than the high carb diets (23 vs 16% daily kcals),
and the high carb, low GI diet had the greatest amount of
fiber (37 g/day vs 29-33 g/day for the others). It is certainly
plausible that these factors could affect results, though one
might expect the effect of high fiber to be the opposite of
what was observed.
have consumed and any of the food given to them that they
64
Ive [got a history of diabetes in my family] [am prediabetic] [am diabetic]. Should I eat low glycemic? Should I try
the DASH diet?
There are arguments on both sides. On the one hand, the
DASH diet has been shown in meta-analysis to improve
certain markers of blood sugar control. While evidence is
mixed, long term observational data suggests that a lower
GI diet can reduce the risk of some chronic diseases, with
trials also showing a reduction in inflammatory markers.
However, the randomized trial under review suggests that
theorized benefits may be overblown. Observational data
may not mean much, as people who eat lower GI could
have a variety of other characteristics that help them with
disease prevention.
Also, micromanaging every meal based on glycemic index
of specific foods is likely to be more annoying than helpful.
Fat and fiber are typically present in meals consisting of
minimally processed foods, and can blunt high blood sugar
responses. The glycemic load of a mixed meal can be hard
to estimate. If you are diabetic and on medication, listen
to your physician and other health professionals, and dont
rely solely on your own research. But following a named
diet isnt a prerequisite for controlling your blood sugar.
micromanaging
every meal based
on glycemic index
of specific foods is
likely to be more
annoying than
helpful.
65
INTERVIEW:
Ivan Oransky
Ivan Oransky, MD, is the vice president and global editorial director of MedPage Today,
co-founder of the MacArthur Foundation-funded Retraction Watch, and founder of
Embargo Watch. He previously was executive editor of Reuters Health and held editorial positions at Scientific American and The Scientist. A 2012 TEDMED speaker, he was
awarded the 2015 John P. McGovern Award for excellence in biomedical communication from the Southwest Chapter of the American Medical Writers Association. He has
authored or co-authored four books and written for numerous publications, including
Nature, The New Republic, and The New York Times.
I believe Retraction Watch is turning five later on this year. Considering the massive number of retractions
you've reported on, what are a couple of your favorite ones? Like ones that might make for a good made-forTV movie (with "good" loosely defined).
Retraction Watch does indeed turn five in August. Good memory! My favorite stories often change over
time, but right now those that involve fake peer review often reviewed by the very authors who wrote the
papers are still at the top of my list. It might be difficult to turn those stories into a made-for-TV movie, but weve turned them into a longer feature for Nature. Theyre a perfect mix of pressure to publish and
ingenuity, in the same way someone might construct a perfect crime but then get caught. For a more
typical made-for-TV movie script, though, Id go with the story of Karel Bezouska, who broke into a lab one
night to tamper with an investigation into his work, or Jatinder Ahluwalia, who sabotaged his colleagues
and later turned out to have been dismissed from an earlier PhD program for the same reasons he eventually had to leave a second university.
Why do you think the number of retractions has been going up over time?
Youd expect to see more retractions as there are more papers published, but as Nature pointed out in 2011,
the rise in the rate of retractions from 2001 to 2010 far outstripped the growth in papers. Part of that was
definitely due to the advent of plagiarism detection software, and the fact that there are more eyeballs on
66
papers now that journals are all online. Theres also at least
of how that issue is dealt with? After all, there isn't a duty
for meta-analysis authors to follow-up on included studies,
tions like?
add, or give us a few details that flesh out the story. Most
then pretend they dont exist and then have to defend sci-
of papers that are published online but not in print yet. The
INTERVIEW:
Jessica Richman
Jessica Richman started and sold her first company after high school. At Stanford
University she earned degrees in Economics and Science, Technology and Society
(with a computer science focus). Along the way, she worked for Google, McKinsey,
Lehman Brothers, the Grameen Bank, and top-tier Silicon Valley venture firms.
Jessica arrived at Oxford University as a Clarendon Scholar and completed an MSc at
the Oxford Internet Institute. She is currently a DPhil student at Oxford with a focus
on innovation, social networks, and collective intelligence.
In layman's terms, how does uBiome go about testing for bacteria? What can you detect and what
can't you detect? And also...is the testing technology changing from year to year?
Great question, and a warm hello to all inquiring Examine.com readers. uBiome offers kits for people to sample the bacterial populations anywhere on the body and do personal experiments to learn
about themselves.
Sampling is done from the comfort and privacy of your own home, with a simple and non-intrusive
procedure. For the gut sample, all we need is a swab of your toilet paper; for the skin sample, its just a
swab behind your ear. We also sell kits for nose, genitals, and mouth.
Each site has a unique microbiome that is home to a specific balance of flora. With the samples from
each of these body sites we can tell you about the bacteria that inhabit that microbiome. You also get
access to our ever-expanding database of bacteria, to see the role those bacteria play in keeping your
microbiome balanced.
You can track the results of probiotic usage, diet and supplement experiments, and antibiotic treatments,
so you can see how they affect your microbiome over time. You can also compare your results to other
participants, to see how you compare to users who have made similar dietary or lifestyle choices.
69
For people who are new to this area, we want to help them
health and lifestyle choices. What you eat impacts not only
What you
eat impacts not
only your gut
flora but your
microbiome at
other sites too.
health? What about people who are already somewhat educated on the subject?
microbiome can help fight off the bacteria that cause cavi-
writing to us at studies@ubiome.com.
study approval from E&I Review Services. To be completely sure that we had our participants privacy, security and
Most people focus on the gut, but uBiome samples five sites
71
[...] question
to your readers:
what are the
most burning
microbiomerelated questions
on your mind, or
whats the number
one thing youd
want to learn from
looking at your
microbiome?
72
ERD
Until Next Issue...
Join the
Discussion Online
73
Credits
Copy Editor: Dmitri Barvinok
iStock.com/ilustro
iStock.com/RKaulitzki
iStock.com/Andrii_Oliinyk
iStock.com/Dacian_G
iStock.com/MackoFlower
iStock.com/barol16
iStock.com/Mari
iStock.com/duckycards
iStock.com/jophil
iStock.com/DebbiSmirnoff
iStock.com/Lisovskaya
iStock.com/masaltof
iStock.com/wsfurlan
iStock.com/Nikada
iStock.com/mel-nik
iStock.com/Dole08
iStock.com/carlosdelacalle
iStock.com/lleerogers
iStock.com/lzf
iStock.com/lvenks
iStock.com/denisgo
iStock.com/dtimiraos
74