P6 PDF
P6 PDF
P6 PDF
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning
By Anne Watson, University of Oxford
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Contents
Paper 1: Overview
Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical
representation
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation
in mathematics
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating
concepts
Paper 8: Methodological appendix
References
Summary of Paper 6
Algebraic reasoning
37
Summary of paper 6:
Algebraic reasoning
Headlines
Understanding symbolisation
97 49 + 49
as structures based on relationships between
numbers, avoiding calculation, identifying variation, and
having a sense of limits of variability, are able to reason
with relationships more securely and at a younger age
than those who have focused only on calculation. An
expression such as 3x + 4 is both the answer to a
question, an object in itself, and also an algorithm or
process for calculating a particular value. This has
parallels in arithmetic: the answer to 3 5 is 3/5.
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Meanings of letters
and signs
Large studies of students interpretation and use of
letters have shown a well-defined set of possible
actions. Learners may, according to the task and
context:
try to evaluate them using irrelevant information
ignore them
used as shorthand for objects, e.g. a = apple
treat them as objects
use a letter as a specific unknown
use a letter as a generalised number
use a letter as a variable.
Teachers have to understand that students may use
any one of these approaches and students need to
learn when these are appropriate or inappropriate.
There are conventions and uses of letters
throughout mathematics that have to be understood
in context, and the statement letters stand for
numbers is too simplistic and can lead to confusion.
For example:
it is not always true that different letters have
different values
a letter can have different values in the same
problem if it stands for a variable
the same letter does not have to have the same
value in different problems.
A critical shift is from seeing a letter as representing
an unknown, or hidden, number defined within a
number sentence such as:
3+x=8
to seeing it as a variable, as in y = 3 + x, or 3 = y x.
Understanding x as some kind of generalized
number which can take a range of values is seen by
some researchers to provide a bridge from the idea
of unknown to that of variables. The use of boxes
to indicate unknown numbers in simple missing
number statements is sometimes helpful, but can
also lead to confusion when used for variables, or
for more than one hidden number in a statement.
Misuse of rules
Students who rely only on remembered rules often
misapply them, or misremember them, or do not
think about the meaning of the situations in which
they might be successfully applied. Many students
will use guess-and-check as a first resort when
solving equations, particularly when numbers are
small enough to reason about hidden numbers
instead of undoing within the algebraic structure.
Although this is sometimes a successful strategy,
particularly when used in conjunction with graphs,
or reasoning about spatial structures, or practical
situations, over-reliance can obstruct the
development of algebraic understanding and
more universally applicable techniques.
Large-scale studies of U.K. school children show
that, despite being taught the BIDMAS rule and its
equivalents, most do not know how to decide on
the order of operations represented in an algebraic
expression. Some researchers believe this to be due
to not fully understanding the underlying operations,
others that it may be due to misinterpretation of
expressions. There is evidence from Australia and
the United Kingdom that students who are taught
to use flow diagrams, and inverse flow diagrams, to
construct and reorganise expressions are better
able to decide on the order implied by expressions
involving combinations of operations. However, it is
not known whether students taught this way can
successfully apply their knowledge of order in
situations in which flow diagrams are inappropriate,
such as with polynomial equations, those involving
the unknown on both sides, and those with more
than one variable. To use algebra effectively,
decisions about order have to be fluent
and accurate.
Misapplying arithmetical
meanings to algebraic
expressions
Analysis of childrens algebra in clinical studies with
12- to 13-year-olds found that the main problems in
moving from arithmetic to algebra arose because:
the focus of algebra is on relations rather than
calculations; the relation a + b = c represents three
unknown quantities in an additive relationship
students have to understand inverses as well as
operations, so that a hidden value can be found
even if the answer is not obvious from knowing
number bonds or multiplication facts; 7 + b = 4 can
be solved using knowledge of addition, but c + 63
= 197 is more easily solved if subtraction is used as
the inverse of addition
some situations have to be expressed algebraically
first in order to solve them. My brother is two
years older than me, my sister is five years younger
than me; she is 12, how old will my brother be in
three years time? requires an analysis and
representation of the relationships before solution.
Algebra in this situation means constructing a
method for keeping track of the unknown as
various operations act upon it.
letters and numbers are used together, so that
numbers may have to be treated as symbols in a
structure, and not evaluated. For example, the
structure 2(3+b) is different from the structure of 6
+ 2b although they are equivalent in computational
terms. Learners have to understand that sometimes
it is best to leave number as an element in an
algebraic structure rather than work it out.
the equals sign has an expanded meaning; in
arithmetic it is often taken to mean calculate but in
algebra it usually means is equal to or is equivalent
to. It takes many experiences to recognise that an
algebraic equation or equivalence is a statement
about relations between quantities, or between
combinations of operations on quantities. Students
tend to want closure by compressing algebraic
expressions into one term instead of understanding
what is being expressed.
Expressing generalisations
In several studies it has been found that students
understand how to use algebra if they have
focused on generalizing with numerical and spatial
representations in which counting is not an option.
Attempts to introduce symbols to very young
students as tools to be used when they have a need
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Recommendations
Research about mathematical
learning
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Algebraic reasoning
10
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
11
Summary
Algebra is not just generalised arithmetic; there are
significant differences between arithmetical and
algebraic approaches.
12
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Meaning of letters
Students understanding of the meaning of letters
in algebra, and how they use letters to express
mathematical relationships, are at the root of
algebraic development. Kuchemann (1981) identified
several different ways adolescent students used
letters in the Chelsea diagnostic test instrument
(Hart, 1981). His research is based on test papers of
2900 students between 12 and 16 (see Appendix 1).
Letters were:
evaluated in some way, e.g. a = 1
ignored, e.g. 3a taken to be 3
used as shorthand for objects, e.g. a = apple
treated as objects
used as a specific unknown
used as a generalised number
used as a variable.
Within his categorisation there were correct and
incorrect uses, such as students who ascribed a value
to a letter based on idiosyncratic decisions or past
experience, e.g. x = 4 because it was 4 in the
previous question. These interpretations appear to
be task dependent, so learners had developed a
sense of what sorts of question were treated in
what kinds of ways, i.e. generalising (sometimes
idiosyncratically) about question-types through
familiarity and prior experience.
13
3+x=8
to seeing it as a variable, as in y = 3 + x, or 3 = y x.
While there is research to show how quasi-variables
such as boxes can help students understand the use
of letters in relational statements (see Carpenter and
Levi, 2000) the shift from unknown to variable when
similar letters are used to have different functions is
not well-researched. Understanding x as some kind of
generalised number which can take a range of values
is seen by some researchers to provide a bridge from
the idea of unknown to that of variables (Bednarz,
Kieran and Lee, 1996).
The algebra of unknowns is about using solution
methods to find mystery numbers; the algebra of
variables is about expressing and transforming relations
between numbers. These different lines of thought
develop throughout school algebra. The variable view
depends on the idea that the expressions linked by the
equals sign might be not just numerically equal, but
also equivalent, yet students need to retain the
unknown concept when setting up and solving
equations which have finite solutions. For example, 10x
Summary
Letters standing for numbers can have many
meanings.
The ways in which operations and relationships are
written in arithmetic and algebra differ.
Learners tend to fall into well-known habits and
assumptions about the use of letters.
A particular difficulty is the difference between
unknowns, variables, parameters and constants,
unless these have meaning.
Difficulties in algebra are not merely about using
letters, but about understanding the underlying
operations and structures.
Students need to learn that there are different uses
for different letters in mathematical conventions; for
example, a, b and c are often used as parameters,
or generalised lengths in geometry, and x, y and z
are often used as variables.
14
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Recognising operations
In several intervention studies and textbooks
students are expected to use algebraic methods for
problems for which an answer is required, and for
which ad hoc methods work perfectly well. This
arises when solving equations with one unknown on
one side where the answer is a positive integer (such
as 3x + 2 = 14); in word problems which can be
enacted or represented diagrammatically (such as I
have 15 fence posts and 42 metres of wire; how far
apart must the fence posts be to use all the wire
and all the posts to make a straight fence?); and in
these and other situations in which trial-andadjustment work easily. Students choice to use
non-algebraic methods in these contexts cannot
be taken as evidence of problems with algebra.
In a teaching experiment with 135 students age 12
to 13, Bednarz and Janvier (1996) found that a
mathematical analysis of the operations required for
solution accurately predicted what students would find
difficult, and they concluded that problems where one
could start from what is known and work towards
what is not known, as one does in arithmetical
calculations, were significantly easier than problems in
which there was no obvious bridge between knowns
and the unknown, and the relationship had to be
worked out and expressed before any calculations
could be made. Many students tried to work
arithmetically with these latter kinds of problem,
starting with a fictional number and working forwards,
generating a structure by trial and error rather than
identifying what would be appropriate. This study is
one of many which indicate that understanding the
meaning of arithmetical operations, rather than merely
being able to carry them out, is an essential precursor
not only to deciding what operation is the right one to
do, but also to expressing and understanding structures
of relations among operations (e.g. Booth, 1984). The
impact of weak arithmetical understanding is also
observed at a higher level, when students can confuse
the kinds of proportionality expressed in y = k/x and y
= kx, thinking the former must be linear because it
involves a ratio (Baker, Hemenway and Trigueros,2001).
The ratio of k to x in the first case is specific for each
value of x, but the ratio of y to x in the second case is
invariant and this indicates a proportional relationship.
Booth (1984) selected 50 students from four schools
to identify their most common errors and to
interview those who made certain kinds of error. This
led her to identify more closely how their weakness
with arithmetic limited their progress with algebra. The
15
Summary
Learners use number facts and guess-and-check
rather than algebraic methods if possible.
Doing calculations, such as in substitution and
guess-and-check methods, distracts from the
development of algebraic understanding.
Substitution can be useful in exploring equivalence
of expressions.
Word problems do not, on their own, scaffold a
shift to algebraic reasoning.
Learners have to understand operations and their
inverses.
Methods of recording arithmetic can scaffold a shift
to understanding operations.
16
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
17
Understanding expressions
An expression such as 3x + 4 is both the answer to
a question, an object in itself, and also an algorithm
or process for calculating a particular number. This is
not a new way of thinking in mathematics that only
appears with algebra: it is also true that the answer to
3 5 is 3/5, something that students are expected to
understand when they learn about intensive
quantities and fractions. Awareness of this kind of
dual meaning has been called proceptual thinking
(Gray and Tall, 1994), combining the process with its
outcome in the same way as a multiple is a number
in itself and also the outcome of multiplication. The
notions of procept and proceptual understanding
signify that there is a need for flexibility in how we
act towards mathematical expressions.
Operational understanding
Many young students understand, at least under
some circumstances, the inverse relation between
addition and subtraction but it takes students
longer to understand the inverse relation
between multiplication and division5. This may
be particularly difficult when the division is not
symbolized by the division sign but by means of
a fraction, as in 1/3. Understanding division when it
is symbolically indicated as a fraction would require
students to realise that a symbol such as 1/3
represents not only a quantity (e.g. the amount of
pizza someone ate when the pizza was cut into
three parts) but also as an operation. Kerslake
(1986) has shown that older primary and younger
secondary students in the United Kingdom rarely
understand fractions as indicating a division. A
further difficulty is that multiplication, seen as
repeated addition, does not provide a ready image
on which to build an understanding of the inverse
operation. An array can be split up vertically or
18
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Summary
Learners tend to persist in additive methods rather
than using multiplicative and exponential where
appropriate.
It is hard for students to learn the nature of
multiplication and division both as inverse of
multiplication and as the structure of fractions and
rational numbers.
Students have to learn that subtraction and division
are non-commutative, and that their inverses are
not necessarily addition and multiplication.
Students have to learn that algebraic terms can
have equivalent forms, and are not instructions to
calculate. Matching terms to structures, rather than
using them to practice substitution, might be useful.
Relational reasoning
Students may make shifts between arithmetic and
algebra, and between operations and relations,
naturally with enough experience, but research
suggests that teaching can make a difference to the
timing and robustness of the shift. Carpenter and
Levi (2000) have worked substantially over decades
to develop an approach to early algebra based on
understanding equality, making generalisations
explicit, representing generalisations in various ways
including symbolically, and talking about justification
and proof to validate generalities. Following this
work, Stephens and others have demonstrated that
students can be taught to see expressions such as:
97 49 + 49
as structures, in Kierans second sense of
relationships among operations (see also the
paper on natural numbers). In international studies,
students in upper primary in Japan generally tackled
these relationally, that is they did not calculate all the
operations but instead combined operations and
inverses, at a younger age than Australian students
made this shift. Chinese students generally appeared
to be able to choose between rapid computation
and relational thinking as appropriate, while 14-yearold English students varied between teachers in their
treatment of these tasks (Fujii and Stephens, 2001,
2008; Jacobs, Franke, Carpenter, Levi and Battey,
2007). This seeing relationally seems to depend on
the ability to discern details (Piaget, 1969 p. xxv)
and application of an intelligent sense of structure
Summary
Learners naturally generalise, they look for patterns
and habits, and familiar objects.
Inductive reasoning from several cases is a natural
way to generalise, but it is often more important to
look at expressions as a whole.
Learners can shift from seeing number expressions
as instructions to calculate to seeing them as
relationships.
This shift can be scaffolded by teaching which
encourages students not to calculate but to identify
and use relations between numbers.
Learners who are fluent in both ways of seeing
expressions, as structures or as instructions to
calculate, can choose which to use.
Combining operations
Problems arise when an expression contains more
than one operation, as can be seen in our paper on
functional relations where young children cannot
understand the notion of relations between
relations, such as differences of differences. In
arithmetical and algebraic expressions, some relations
between relations appear as combinations of
operations, and learners have to decide what has
to be done first and how this is indicated in the
notation. Carpenter and Levi (2000), Fujii and
Stephens (2001, 2008), Jacobs et al. (2007), draw
attention to this in their work on how students read
number sentences. Linchevski and Herscovics (1996)
19
Summary
Understanding operations and their inverses is a
greater problem than understanding the use of
symbols.
Learners tend to use their rules for reading and
other false priorities when combining operations,
i.e. interpreting left to right, doing addition first,
using language to construct expressions, etc. They
need to develop new priorities.
New rules, such as BODMAS (which can be
misused), do not effectively and quickly replace old
rules which are based on familiarity, habit, and
arithmetic.
Equals sign
A significant body of research reports on difficulties
about the meaning of the equals sign Sfard and
Linchevski (1994) find that students who can do 7x
+ 157 = 248 cannot do 112 = 12x + 247, but
these questions include two issues: the position and
meaning of the equals sign and that algorithmic
approaches lead to the temptation to subtract
smaller from larger, erroneously, in the second
example. They argue that the root problem is the
failure to understand the inverse relation between
addition and subtraction, but this research shows
how conceptual difficulties, incomplete
understandings and notations can combine to make
multiple difficulties. If students are taught to make
changes to both sides of an equation in order to
solve it (i.e. transform the equation y 5 = 8 into y
5 + 5 = 8 + 5) and they do not see the need to
maintain equivalence between the values in the two
sides of the equation, then the method that they
are being taught is mysterious to them, particularly
as many of the cases they are offered at first can be
easily solved by arithmetical methods. Booth (1984)
shows that these errors combine problems with
understanding operations and inverses and
problems understanding equivalence.
20
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Summary
Learners persist in using = to mean calculate
because this is familiar and meaningful for them.
The equals sign has different uses within
mathematics; sometimes it indicates equivalence
and sometimes equality; learners have to learn
these differences.
Different uses of the equals sign carry different
implications for the meaning of letters: they can
stand for hidden numbers, or variables, or
parameters.
Equivalence is seen when graphs coincide, and can
be understood either structurally or as generation
of equal outputs for every input; equality is seen
when graphs intercept.
21
22
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Summary
Once students understand the equals sign, they are
likely to use intuitive number-rules as a first resort.
The appearance of the negative sign creates need
for a major shift to abstract meanings of operations
and relations, as concrete models no longer
operate.
The appearance of the unknown on both sides of
an equation creates the need for a major shift
towards understanding equality and variables.
Students appear to use procedural manipulations
when solving equations and inequalities without a
mental image or understanding strong enough to
prevent errors.
Students appear to develop action-based rules
when faced with situations which do not have
obvious concrete manifestations.
23
Manipulatives
It is not only arithmetical habits that can cause
obstacles to algebra. There are other algebraic
activities in which too strong a memory for process
might create obstacles for future learning. For
example, a popular approach to teaching algebra
is the provision of materials and diagrams which
ascribe unknown numerical (dimensional) meaning
to letters while facilitating their manipulation to
model relationships such as commutativity and
distributivity. These appear to have some success in
the short term, but shifts from physical appearance
to mental abstraction, and then to symbolism, are
not made automatically by learners (Boulton-Lewis,
Cooper, Atweh, Pillay, Wilss and Mutch,1997). These
manipulatives provide persistent images and
metaphors that may be obstructions in future
work. On the other hand, the original approach to
dealing with variables was to represent them as
spatial dimensions, so there are strong historical
precedents for such methods. There are reported
instances of success in teaching this, relating to
Bruners three perspectives, enactive-iconicsymbolic (1966), where detachment from the
model has been understood and scaffolded by
teaching (Filloy and Sutherland, 1996; Simmt and
Kieren, 1999). Detachment from the model has to
be made when values are negative and can no
longer be represented concretely, and also with
fractional values and division operations. Spatial
representations have been used with success
where the image is used persistently in a range
of algebraic contexts, such as expressions and
equations and equivalence, and where teachers
use language to scaffold shifts between concrete,
numerical and relational perspectives.
Use of rod or bar diagrams as in Singapore (NMAP,
2008; Greenes and Rubenstein, 2007) to represent
part/whole comparisons, reasoning, and equations,
appears to scaffold thinking from actual numbers to
structural relationships, so long as they only involve
addition and/or repeated addition. Statements in the
problem are translated into equalities between
lengths. These equal lengths are constructed from
rods which represent both the actual and the
unknown numbers. The rod arrangements or values
can then be manipulated to find the value of the
Summary
Manipulatives can be useful for modelling algebraic
relationships and structures.
Learners might see manipulatives as just something
else to learn.
Teachers can help learners connect the use of
objects, the development of imagery and the use of
symbols through language.
Students have to appreciate the limitations of
concrete materials and shift to mental imagery
and abstract understandings.
24
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
knowledge)
base their reasoning on incorrect method
have a sound strategy that may not match formal
method
may be taught methods leading up to formal, but
not matching the formal method
may retain other methods, which may have limited
application
may retain formalisation but lose meaning, then
misapply a formal method in
future
pre-formal enactive or iconic experiences may have
been forgotten
might be able to use materials to explain formal
method
may interpret formal notation inadequately.
The research described above, taken as a whole,
suggests that the problems students have with using
formulae in subjects other than mathematics are
due to: not being fluent with the notation; not
understanding the underlying operations; experience
of using such formulae in mathematics lessons being
limited to abstract or confusing situations, or even to
situations in which an algebraic formula is not
necessary. In addition, of course, they may not
understand the intended context.
Summary
Learners are able to construct formulae for
themselves, at least in words if not symbols, if they
have sufficient understanding of the relationships
and operations.
Learners problems using formulae have several
possible root causes.
1 Underlying knowledge of the situation or
associated concepts may be weak.
2 Existing working strategies may not match the
formal method.
3 Notational problems with understanding how
to interpret and use the formula.
Expressing generalisations
from patterns
One approach to address inherent difficulties in
algebra is to draw on our natural propensity to
observe patterns, and to impose patterns on
disparate experiences (Reed 1972). In this
approach, sequences of patterns are presented
and students asked to deduce formulae to
describe quantitative aspects of a general term
in the sequence. The expectation is that this
generates a need for algebraic symbolisation, which
is then used to state what the student can already
express in other ways, numerical, recursive,
diagrammatically or enactively.
This approach is prevalent in the United Kingdom,
Australia and parts of North America. The NMAP
(2008) review finds no evidence that expressing
generality contributes to algebraic understanding, yet
others would say that this depends on the definition
25
..
11
...
....
...
...
Moss, Beatty and Macnab (2006) worked with nineyear-old students in a longitudinal study and found
that developing expressions for pattern sequences
was an effective introduction to understanding the
nature of rules in guess the rule problems. Nearly
all of the 34 students were then able to articulate
general descriptions of functions in the classic
handshake problem7 which is known to be hard for
students in early secondary years. By contrast, Ryan
and Williams (2007) found in large-scale testing that
the most prevalent error in such tasks for 12- and
14-year-olds was giving the term-to-term formula
rather than the functional formula, and giving an
actual value for the nth term. Cooper and Warren
(2007, and Warren and Cooper, 2008), worked for
three years in five elementary classrooms, using
patterning and expressing patterns, to teach students
to express generalisations to use various
representations, and to compare expressions and
structures. Their students learnt to use algebraic
conventions and notations, and also understood that
expressions had underlying operational meanings.
Clearly, students are capable of learning these aspects
26
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
27
critical-thinking strategies
the use of organizing heuristics such as a table
the use of simplifying heuristics such as trying out
simpler cases
task familiarity
use of technology to do the arithmetic so that large
numbers can be handled efficiently.
As with all mathematics teaching, limited experience
is unhelpful. Some students only know one way to
construct cases, one way to accomplish generalisation
(table of values and pattern spotting), and have only
ever seen simple cases used to start sequence
generation, rather than deliberate choices to aid
observations. Students in this situation may be
unaware of the necessity for critical, reflective thinking
and the value of simplifying and organising data.
Furthermore, this collection of studies on expressing
generality shows that construction, design, choice and
comparison of various representational means does
not happen spontaneously for students who are
capable of using them. Choosing when and why to
switch representations has long been known to be a
mark of successful mathematics students (Krutetskii,
1976) and therefore this is a strategy which needs to
be deliberately taught. Evidence from Blanton and
Kaputs intervention study with 20 teachers (2005) is
that many primary children were able to invent and
solve missing number sentences using letters as
placeholders, symbolize quantities in patterns, devise
and use graphical representations for single variables,
and some could write simple relations using letters,
codes, secret messages or symbols. The intervention
was supportive professional development which
helped teachers understand what algebraic reasoning
entails, and gave them resources, feedback, and other
support over five years. Ainley (1996) showed that
supportive technology can display the purpose of
formal representations, and also remove the technical
difficulties of producing new representations. Ten-yearold students in her study had worked for a few years
in a computer-rich environment and used
spreadsheets to collect data from purposeful
experiments. They then generated graphs from the
data and studied these, in relation to the data, to
make conjectures and test them. One task was
designed to lead to a problematic situation so that
students would have to look for a shortcut, and she
observed that the need to teach the computer how
to perform a calculation led to spontaneous formal
representation of a variable.
So, if it is possible for students to learn to make
these generalizations only with a great deal of
...
...
28
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Summary
Learners naturally make generalisations based on
what is most obviously related; this depends on the
visual impact of symbols and diagrams.
Seeing functional, abstract, relationships is hard and
has to be supported by teaching.
Deconstruction of diagrams, relationships, situations
is more helpful in identifying functional relationships
than pattern-generation.
Development of heuristics to support seeing
structural relationships is helpful.
There is a further shift from seeing to expressing
functional relationships.
Learners who can express relationships correctly
and algebraically can also exemplify relationships
with number pairs, and express the relationships
within the pairs; but not all those who can express
relationships within number pairs can express the
relationship algebraically.
Learners who have combined patterngeneralisation with function machines and other
ways to see relationships can become more fluent
in expressing generalities in unfamiliar situations.
Conflicting research results suggest that the nature
of tasks and pedagogy make a difference to success.
Summary
As with all algebraic expressions, learners may react
to the visual appearance without thinking about the
meaning.
29
Spreadsheets
Learners have to know how to recognise structures
(based on understanding arithmetical operations
and what they do), express structures in symbols,
and calculate particular cases (to stimulate inductive
understanding of concepts) in order to use algebra
effectively in other subjects and in higher
mathematics. Several researchers have used
spreadsheets as a medium in which to explore
what students might be able to learn
(e.g. Schwartz and Yerushalmy, 1992; Sutherland and
Rojano, 1993; Friedlander and Tabach, 2001). The
advantages of using spreadsheets are as follows.
In order to use spreadsheets you have to know the
difference between parameters (letters and numbers
that structure the relationship) and variables, and the
spreadsheet environment is low-risk since mistakes
are private and can easily be corrected.
The physical act of pointing the cursor provides an
enactive aspect to building abstract structures.
Graphical, tabular and symbolic representations are
just a click away from each other and are updated
together.
Correspondences that are not easy to see in other
media can be aligned and compared on a
30
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Summary
Use of spreadsheets to build formulae:
allows large data sets to be used
provides physical enactment of formula
construction
allows learners to distinguish between variables and
parameters
gives instant feedback
does not always lock learners into arithmetical and
empirical viewpoints.
Multiple representations
A widespread attempt to overcome the obstacles of
learning algebra has been to offer learners multiple
representations of functions because:
different representations express different aspects
more clearly
different representations constrain interpretations
these have to be checked out against each other
relating representations involves identifying and
understanding isomorphic structures (Goldin 2002).
Functional approach
Authors vary in their use of the word function.
Technically, a function is a relationship of
dependency between variables, the independent
variables (input) which vary by some external
means, and the dependent variables (output) which
vary in accordance with the relationship. It is the
relationship that is the function, not a particular
representation of it, however in practice authors
and teachers refer to the function when indicating
a graph or equation. An equivalence such as
temperature conversion is not a function, because
these are just different ways to express the same
thing, e.g. t = 9/5 C + 32 where t is temperature in
degrees Fahrenheit and C temperature in degrees
Celsius (Janvier 1996). Thus a teaching approach
which focuses on comparing different expressions of
the same generality is concerned with structure and
would afford manipulation, while an approach which
focuses on functions, such as using function
machines or multiple representations, is concerned
with relationships and change and would afford
thinking about pairs of values, critical inputs and
outputs, and rates of change.
Function machines
Some researchers report that students find it hard
to use inverses in the right order when solving
equations. However, in Booths work (1984) with
function machines she found that lower secondary
students were capable of instructing the machine by
writing operations in order, using proper algebraic
syntax where necessary, and could make the shift to
understanding the whole expression. They could
then reverse the flow diagram, maintaining order,
to undo the function.
31
32
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Summary
Learners can compare representations of a
relationship in graphical, numerical, symbolic and
data form.
Conflicting research results suggest that the nature
of tasks and pedagogy make a difference to success.
The hardest of these representations for learners is
the symbolic form.
Previous experience of comparing multiple
representations, and the situation being modelled,
helps students understand symbolic forms.
Learners who see unknowns as special cases of
equality of two expressions are able to distinguish
between unknowns and variables.
Teachers can scaffold the shifts between
representations, and perceptions beyond surface
features, through language.
Some researchers claim that learners have to
understand the nature of the representations in
order to use them to understand functions, while
others claim that if learners understand the
situations, then they will understand the
representations and how to use them.
33
Summary
With teaching:
Young children can engage with missing number
problems, use of letters to represent unknown
numbers, and use of letters to represent
generalities that they have already understood.
Young children can appreciate operations as
objects, and their inverses.
34
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Part 3: Conclusions
and recommendations
Conclusions
Error research about elementary algebra and
pre-algebra is uncontentious and the findings are
summarised above. However, it is possible for young
learners to do more than is normally expected in
the curriculum, e.g. they will accept the use of letters
to express generalities and relationships which they
already understand. Research about secondary
algebra is less coherent and more patchy, but broadly
can be summarised as follows.
Teaching algebra by offering situations in which
symbolic expressions make mathematical sense,
and what learners have to find is mathematically
meaningful (e.g. through multiple representations,
expressing generality, and equating functions) is more
effective in leading to algebraic thinking and skill use
than the teaching of technical manipulation and
solution methods as isolated skills. However, these
methods need to be combined through complex
pedagogy and do not in themselves bring about all
the necessary learning. Technology can play a big part
in this. There is a difference between using ICT in
the learners control and using ICT in the teachers
control. In the learners control the physical actions
of moving around the screen and choosing between
representations can be easily connected to the
effects of such moves, and feedback is personalised
and instant.
There is a tenuous relationship between what it
means to understand and use the affordances of
algebra, as described in the previous paragraph, and
understanding and using the symbolic forms of
algebra. Fluency in understanding symbolic
expressions seems to develop through use, and
also contributes to effective use this is a two-way
process. However, this statement ignores the
messages from research which is purely about
procedural fluency, and which supports repetitive
practice of procedures in carefully constructed
varying forms. Procedural research focuses on
obstacles such as dealing with negative signs and
fractions, multiple operations, task complexity and
cognitive load but not on meaning, use, relationships,
and dealing with unfamiliar situations.
Recommendations
For teaching
These recommendations require a change from a
fragmented, test-driven, system that encourages an
emphasis on fluent procedure followed by
application.
Algebra is the mathematical tool for working
with generalities, and hence should permeate
lessons so that it is used wherever mathematical
meaning is expressed. Its use should be
commonplace in lessons.
Teachers and writers must know about the
research about learning algebra and take it into
account, particularly research about common
errors in understanding algebraic symbolisation and
how they arise.
Teachers should avoid using published and webbased materials which exacerbate the difficulties by
over-simplifying the transition from arithmetic to
algebraic expression, mechanising algebraic
transformation, and focusing on algebra as
arithmetic with letters.
The curriculum, advisory schemes of work, and
teaching need to take into account how shifts from
arithmetical to algebraic understanding take time,
multiple experiences, and clarity of purpose.
Students at key stage 3 need support in shifting
to representations of generality, understanding
relationships, and expressing these in
conventional forms.
Students have to change focus from calculation,
quantities, and answers to structures of operations
and relations between quantities as variables. This
shift takes time and multiple experiences.
Students should have multiple experiences of
constructing algebraic expressions for structural
relations, so that algebra has the purpose of
expressing generality.
The role of guess-the-sequence-rule tasks in the
algebra curriculum should be reviewed: it is
mathematically incorrect to state that a finite
number of numerical terms indicates a unique
underlying generator.
35
For policy
The requirements listed above signal a training
need on a national scale, focusing solely on algebra
as a key component in the drive to increase
mathematical competence and power.
There are resource implications about the use of
ICT. The focus on providing interactive whiteboards
may have drawn attention away from the need for
students to be in control of switching between
representations and comparisons of symbolic
expression in order to understand the syntax and
the concept of functions. The United Kingdom may
be lagging behind the developed world in exploring
the use of CAS, spreadsheets and other software
to support new kinds of algebraic thinking.
In several other countries, researchers have been
able to develop differently-sequenced curricula in
which students have been able to use algebra as a
way of expressing general and abstract notions as
these arise. Manipulation, solution of equations,
and other technical matters to do with symbols
develop as well as with formal teaching, but are
For research
Little is known about school learning of algebra in
the following areas.
The experiences that an average learner needs, in
educational environments conducive to change, to
shift from arithmetical to algebraic thinking.
The relationship between understanding the nature
of the representations in order to use them to
understand functions, and understanding the
situations as an aid to understanding the
representations and how to use them.
Whether teaching experiments using functional,
multi-representational, equation or generalisation
approaches have an impact on students typical
notation-related difficulties. In other words, we do
not know if and how semantic-focused approaches
to algebra have any impact on persistent and wellknown syntactic problems.
How learners synthesise their knowledge to
understand quadratic and other polynomials, their
factorisation and roots, simultaneous equations,
36
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
Endnotes
1 The importance of inverses was discussed in the paper on
natural numbers
2 In the paper on rational numbers we talk more about the
relationship between fractions and rational numbers, and we
often use these words interchangeably.
Acknowledgements
This chapter was produced with the help of Nichola
Clarke who did much of the technical work.
37
References
38
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
39
40
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
41
42
PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers
www.nuffieldfoundation.org