Financial Literacy and Behavioural Change
Financial Literacy and Behavioural Change
Financial Literacy and Behavioural Change
explaining when and how ASIC will exercise specific powers under
legislation (primarily the Corporations Act)
Disclaimer
This document is a research report and does not contain ASIC policy.
Page 2
Contents
Key findings ...................................................................................................4
A
Page 3
Key findings
What Australians know
While they differ in terms of scope, most of the local studies seeking to
measure financial literacy collect information via surveys. Some rely on
subjective tests, some on objective tests and others on both.
Recently there has been growing recognition of the value of using both
objective and subjective tests because they help us identify gaps
between what people believe they know and what they actually know.
What people do
Knowledge alone is not enough. People dont always act in their own
best interests (e.g. by not engaging at all or by making bad decisions).
Page 4
Less confident/
knowledgeable
Investing
Superannuation
Saving for retirement
More confident/
knowledgeable
Budgeting
Credit
Savings
Debt
Common barriers
to making good
financial decisions
Highlight costs of
competing behaviours
not rational
Promote a tangible
Information and
object or service
choice overload
Consider non-monetary
Complexity and
incentives
uncertainty
Self-control
pledges
Framing (how
information is
presented)
What people
think they know
is greater than
What people
actually know
Page 5
Key points
The primary aim of this report is to take stock of what ASIC has learned
about financial literacy to date to inform the national financial literacy
strategy.
Most of the information in this report was collected in 2008 from Australian
and international sources, including public websites, journal articles and
unpublished research reports. The report was then updated in 2010.
Financial literacy means different things to different people.
Purpose
On 1 July 2008, the functions of the Financial Literacy Foundation moved to
ASIC.
The primary aim of this report is to take stock of what we have learned about
financial literacy to date to inform the national financial literacy strategy. In
particular, we seek to understand:
We then want to use this information to help guide our financial literacy
priorities and program design.
We believe that improving knowledge alone, however, will not be enough to
achieve better financial outcomes for all Australian financial consumers and
retail investors.
As one response in the strategy, we are looking to develop a generic
advice/guidance service for the mass market, which would give Australians
access to free, quality independent financial guidance and back-up aids to
help them implement and stick to the plan.
In assessing the need for such a service, this report also covers:
For a summary of available research about the use of financial planners, see
Appendix 2.
Page 6
Method
Two ASIC researchers compiled this report in 2008. The information
collected was sourced from public websites, journal articles and unpublished
research reports. A third ASIC researcher updated the report in mid-2010.
The scope of the information presented is contained in the following ways:
Those who use the various terms financial literacy, financial education and
financial capability do not always define what they mean by these terms.
Financial education is the terminology used by most jurisdictions when
framing initiatives as solutions to low financial literacy (exceptions include
the United Kingdom, which focuses on financial capability). The
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) (2005)
broadly defines financial education as follows:
Financial education is the process by which financial consumers/investors
improve their understanding of financial products and concepts and,
through information, instruction and/or objective advice, develop the skills
and confidence to become more aware of financial risks and opportunities,
to make informed choices, to know where to go for help, and to take other
effective actions to improve their financial wellbeing. Where:
information involves providing consumers with facts, data, and
specific knowledge to make them aware of financial opportunities,
choices, and consequences;
Page 7
In Australia, the survey of adult financial literacy by the Australia and New
Zealand Banking Group Limited (ANZ)1 has been the first and largest
national survey used to find out the financial literacy levels of all
Australians. The ANZ survey defines financial literacy as the ability to
make informed judgements and to take effective decisions regarding the use
and management of money (2008, p. 6). The study argues that high levels of
financial literacy enable people to:
Make informed and confident decisions on all aspects of their budgeting,
spending, saving and planning for the future as well as on their use of
financial products and services such as everyday banking, borrowing and
investing (ibid.).
Page 8
The definition used for the purposes of the National Consumer and Financial
Literacy Framework, which guides the teaching of financial literacy in
Australias schools, is:
Consumer and financial literacy is the application of knowledge,
understandings, skills and values in consumer and financial contexts and
the related decisions that impact on self, others, the community and the
environment (MCEECDYA, 2009, p. 1).
Page 9
Key points
Australians have differing attitudes to financial matters and varying levels of
financial knowledge.
Overall, people seem to be more knowledgeable and confident about
simple, familiar finance topics such as budgeting, credit, savings and debt,
and less knowledgeable and confident about more complex, unfamiliar
topics such as investing, superannuation and saving for retirement.
While they differ in terms of scope, most of the local studies seeking to
measure financial literacy collect information via surveys. Some rely on
subjective tests, some on objective tests and others on both.
Recently there has been growing recognition of the value of using both
objective and subjective tests because, among other things, they help us
identify gaps between what people believe they know and what they
actually know.
Page 10
For example, ANZ (2008); FLF (2007a); CBF (2004); Mercer Wealth Solutions (MWS) (2006).
For example, Womens Information and Referral Exchange Inc. (WIRE) (2007, 2010).
4
Other methods include focus groups and depth interviews.
3
Page 11
the gaps between what people believe they know and what they actually
know:
Objective tests of financial concepts are a better way of measuring financial
literacy than are surveys which ask respondents to provide a selfassessment of their understanding of financial issues. However, a
comparison of consumers self-assessment with their response to objective
questions that test their financial understanding could indicate to
policymakers where the largest discrepancies are between what consumers
believe they know and what they actually know (OECD, 2005, pp. 4445).
While some people dont rely on any information sources, most people
rely on a wide range of information and advice services when
researching financial decisions and/or trying to gain financial
knowledge, including informal sources (e.g. family and friends,
newspapers) and formal sources (e.g. financial advisers/accountants and
product providers).
Page 12
Table 1:
Financial topic
Peoples self-reported
ability and
understanding
Peoples self-reported
recognition
of the importance of
learning more
Budgeting
90%
57%
Managing debt
89%
61%
Saving
88%
65%
Recognising a scam
88%
69%
85%
68%
85%
83%
49%
Choosing appropriate
insurance
82%
64%
81%
66%
81%
Investing
69%
70%
Understanding financial
language
64%
68%
63%
74%
77%
71%
Page 13
Table 2:
Questions
Percent who
answered
correctly
94%
93%
89%
88%
88%
84%
82%
82%
79%
78%
62%
55%
52%
51%
48%
44%
41%
34%
18%
17%
Page 14
Page 15
Table 3:
Group
Mean
FLS
Total
83.1
Gender:
Group
Mean
FLS
ATSI* background:
Group
Mean
FLS
Language
spoken at home:
Group
Mean
FLS
Geographic place
of residence:
Males
85.9
Yes
63.9
English
84.0
Capital city
83.4
Females
80.5
No
83.4
Other language
77.9
Non-capital city
82.5
Age group:
Gross annual
household income:
Current main
activity:
Current
occupation type:
1824 years
71.5
68.1
Paid work
89.8
Upper white-collar
94.5
2534 years
86.0
$25,000$57,999
77.1
Home duties
76.7
Middle/lower
white-collar
87.1
3544 years
90.9
$58,000$79,999
89.5
Student
73.6
Upper blue-collar
83.6
4559 years
89.1
$80,000$99,999
90.1
Retired
72.6
Lower blue-collar
76.5
6069 years
81.6
$100,000$149,999
92.1
Unemployed
66.7
70 years or over
63.3
$150,000 or more
97.3
Highest level of
education
completed:
Main source of
income:
ARIA**
classification:
SEIFA***
classification:
Year 10 or less
70.7
Salary wages or
business income
88.9
Major cities
83.7
SEIFA group 1
(greatest
disadvantage)
75.5
Year 11/12
80.4
Government
benefit/payment
67.2
Inner regional
83.4
SEIFA group 2
80.7
Trade/TAFE/
diploma
88.2
Retired, government
benefit
66.0
Outer regional
80.8
SEIFA group 3
84.9
University
92.7
Retired, other
source of income
81.8
Remote/very
remote areas
75.6
SEIFA group 4
82.9
SEIFA group 5
(least
disadvantage)
87.6
*
**
***
Source: Adapted from ANZ, 2008, pp. 1011. Note total possible scores could be more than 100.
Page 16
Table 4:
Demographic
Gender
Age
Education
Life stage
Languages
No significant association.
Work status
Income and
savings
Home ownership
Page 17
7.9 million (53%) had low scores (Level 1 or 2) on the numeracy scale10
10.6 million (70%) had low scores (Level 1 or 2) on the problemsolving scale.11
Knowledge by product/activity
The key findings about Australians financial knowledge by financial
products and activities are summarised below. These summaries cite
indicative statistics only and are limited to the key pieces of research about
financial literacy in Australia.
Some of these findings are the result of objective tests, some subjective and
some both. As a result of these different methodologies (and other factors),
some of the findings conflict. For more comprehensive summaries of
available statistics, see Appendix 1.
Document literacy was defined as the knowledge and skills required to locate and use information contained in various
formats including job applications, payroll forms, transportation schedules, maps, tables and charts.
9
Prose literacy was defined as the ability to understand and use information from various kinds of narrative texts, including
texts from newspapers, magazines and brochures.
10
Numeracy was defined as the knowledge and skills required to effectively manage and respond to the mathematical
demands of diverse situations.
11
Problem solving was defined as goal-directed thinking and action in situations for which no routine solution is available.
Page 18
People generally know they should make a budget12 and believe they
understand and have the ability to save (88%) and budget (90%).13 Yet
22% of adults say they dont save and nearly half (48%) do not budget
regularly for their day-to-day finances.
Some people (24%) say they have problems setting money aside for
major financial outlays.14 Those more likely to have problems include
people with household incomes below $25,000 (40%).
Some people also report that they do not keep a close eye on household
and personal expenses (23%).15 Men aged under 35 years (34%) are
more likely to report this.
Between 2002 and 2008, there has been significant growth in the use of
and/or understanding of how to use electronic payment methods. In
particular, internet banking has risen from 52% to 69%, BPAY has risen
from 60% to 72% and direct debits have risen from 78% to 87%.19
Many people (81%) say they take steps to minimise bank fees (19%
either did not know or dont take any steps). 21 The most common steps
people take are only use ATMs from the same bank as my account
(27%), minimise the number of transactions I make per month (17%),
keeping the number of transactions I make to my monthly limit
(14%), make fewer but larger cash withdrawals (12%) and use
telephone/internet banking (12%).
12
Page 19
Most people say they are confident in their ability to deal with and
understand credit cards (83%).22 They also indicate that they dont see
the importance of learning more about dealing with credit cards (49%),
compared to other financial products.23
People generally say they know the best way to reduce credit card
interest (94%).24 They also say they know/take steps to avoid credit card
fees by paying off the monthly credit card balance in full (12%),
ensuring minimum monthly payments are made (9%) and not exceeding
the limit on their credit card (6%).25
Nevertheless, of those with a credit card, 14% said they are sometimes
charged interest and 27% said they are usually charged interest on their
credit card. People in their 30s (35%) and 40s (39%) were most likely
to usually be charged interest.26
People who check their credit or store card transactions (93%) generally
check for fraudulent activity (74%) and to see whether everything is
right (28%).27 The most common reasons people say they dont check
their credit card transactions are someone else is checking the
transactions (23%) and lack of interest (e.g. couldnt be bothered
(23%), dont have the time (20%) and just assuming theyre correct
(17%)).
22
Page 20
People say they are confident in their ability to manage personal debt
(89%).31 Men (81%) are more likely to say they feel comfortable with
debt than women (77%). However, people appear to be less able to deal
with spiralling debt (48%).32
More than forty percent of people (41%) do not know that being more
than 60 days late with the minimum payment on a credit card is the
thing most likely to give someone a bad credit rating.35 Women (47%)
are more likely than men (35%) to not know this, particularly women
aged 18 to 24 years (66%) and those 70 years and over (69%).
People generally dont know what reverse equity loans (also known as
reverse mortgages) are (54%).37 When those who claimed to know what
reverse equity loans are were asked what they would consider when
deciding whether or not to take out a reverse equity loan, the most
common answers were the interest rate (13%), life expectancy (11%),
the conditions of the loan (11%) and the size of the loan that would be
needed (10%).38
31
Page 21
Women have reported that they feel overwhelmed with the amount and
complexity of financial information available and some describe
financial terms/expressions as another language (25%).43
Some investors who receive a prospectus dont read it because they feel
that they wont understand the information in it.48
40
Page 22
People aged under 65 who are not yet retired recognise the importance of
saving for retirement through superannuation (93%).49 Yet some people
believe that retirement is too far away to think about (21%) and that
financial planning is only important for those who have a lot of money
(16%).50 Those with low incomes are more likely to think that financial
planning is only important for those who have a lot of money.51
People are generally confident (81%) in their ability to plan for their
long-term financial future but less confident in their ability to ensure
they have enough money for retirement (63%).52 As few as 11% of
Australians in full-time employment have given retirement a lot of
thought and made preparations.53
Many people do not know how much money they will need to fund a
comfortable retirement and only 27% of those aged under 65 who have
super and are employed have identified an annual figure.54 Those aged
under 30 (27%) are more likely to have no idea how much money they
will need compared to those aged over 40 (23%).55 Two-thirds (66%) of
the people who have thought about retirement, and are able to say how
much money they will need, indicate they will need between $25,000
and $74,999 per year to live on when they retire.56 Based on the
Westpac-ASFA Retirement Standard these estimates appear optimistic.57
Eighty-two percent of people aged under 40 years are aware that they
can contribute a greater percentage of their salary to superannuation.58
Similarly, 79% agree that superannuation is a good way to make me
save and 78% agree that compulsory superannuation is forced savings,
I barely notice it. Women are significantly more likely to agree with
these statements (82%).59
Most people do not believe that the age pension (86%) or employerfunded superannuation alone (73%) will meet their retirement needs.60
The majority of those aged under 65 (91%) believe that they will need
49
Page 23
Superannuation
Those who have super, are employed and under age 65 understand the
basic rules relating to superannuation (i.e. that employers are required
by law to make superannuation payments on their behalf and that they
themselves can make additional contributions to their superannuation)
but are less aware of the tax treatment of super: see Table 5.64 Women
(53%) are among those least likely to say super is taxed at a lower rate
than other investments (compared to 63% of men).
Table 5:
Concept tested
2008
97%
96%
92%
90%
56%
58%
People are also less aware of other super-related benefits, such as the
government co-contribution scheme, their entitlements regarding choice
of fund, and their understanding of the payment of death benefits: see
Table 6.65 Those who are unable to nominate any superannuation
benefits are more likely to be younger (18 to 24 year-olds (35%)).66
61
Page 24
Just over two-thirds (68%) of people are able to correctly identify the
amount of return left after the fees are taken out as the best indication
of how their superannuation fund or managed investments are
performing: see Table 7.70
68
Page 25
Table 6:
Test questions
Percent
answered
correctly
Percent
answered
incorrectly
Percent
dont know
73%
5%
22%
71%
9%
20%
56%
13%
31%
If you were born after 1965, you will need to be aged 70 before
you can access your super. (False)
46%
9%
44%
44%
36%
20%
All Australian workers are eligible for the government cocontribution scheme, regardless of their income or age. (False)
42%
31%
27%
39%
11%
50%
37%
8%
55%
32%
15%
53%
All Australian workers are entitled to choose their own super fund
under Choice of Fund legislation. (False)
15%
77%
9%
In the event of your death, your super fund is required to pay your
death benefit to your nominated beneficiary. (False)
7%
73%
20%
Table 7:
Performance indicator
Percent of
respondents
68%
Return
12%
Per-unit cost
5%
Fees
2%
Cant say
13%
Page 26
Investing
Table 8:
Product owned
75
(ASIC, 2008a)
Australian population
(aged 18+)
Investor population
(aged 18+)
24%
51%
NA
24%
11%
23%
Term deposits
10%
21%
Investment property
10%
19%
4%
12%
5%
11%
6%
9%
76
Direct shares
77
Managed investments
Source: Selected extract from ASIC, 2008a, p. 15.
While many people know that high returns generally mean high risk
(86%), almost half (48%) have trouble applying the risk and return
72
Fear (2008, p. 44). In addition, 42% of people surveyed by MWS (2006, p. 15) rated their level of knowledge of
investment as nothing or minimal (i.e. a score of 03 out of 10). A further 41% rated their knowledge as moderate (46
out of 10).
73
FLF (2007a, p. 79).
74
ASX (2009, p. 25). Base sample: 2008 (n=816); 2006 (n=904) current direct investors.
75
Base for Australian population: n=53,307 and base for investor population: n=1217.
76
A third (33%) of share owners acquired their shares passively, while almost two-thirds (63%) of share owners acquired the
shares actively.
77
The most commonly owned other direct investment was debentures (39%), followed by bonds (21%), short-term securities
(9%) and options (9%).
78
FLF (2007a, p. 79).
Page 27
concept.79 Many (66%) would not consider risk and return when
choosing an investment.80
Table 9:
Investment options
Correct
Incorrect
Dont know
Cash
61%
21%
18%
Cash plus
52%
25%
24%
Fixed interest
44%
41%
15%
Balanced
44%
43%
13%
Socially responsible
51%
24%
25%
Australian shares
32%
53%
15%
International shares
57%
28%
15%
High growth
77%
10%
12%
Insurance
Many people believe they have the ability and understanding to protect
themselves and their assets by choosing appropriate insurance (82%) (e.g.
home and contents insurance, car insurance, life insurance), although
79
Only 52% would consider an investment advertised as having a return well above market rates and no risk as too good to
be true and not invest in it. ANZ (2008, p. 64).
80
FLF (2007a, p. 11).
81
A sample of QSuper superannuation fund members.
82
Gallery et al (2010, p. 23).
83
ANZ (2008, p. 65).
84
FLF (2007a, p. 79).
85
ASIC (2008a, p. 6).
Page 28
women (84%) are more likely than men (79%) to believe in taking out
insurance to be prepared for the unexpected. 86 However, only 37% say
they have enough insurance to ensure that their loved ones wont suffer
financially in the event of their own death, sickness or disability.87
Many people (64%) also believe that its important to learn more about
how to choose appropriate insurance, particularly those aged 18 to 29
years (79%).88
People generally consider the premium (53%), the general level of cover
needed (41%), the brand or reputation of the insurance provider (20%) and
the excess (15%) when they are taking out a new insurance policy.90
People feel that they have the ability to recognise a scam (88%) but it is
likely that they are more vulnerable to scams than they realise.91
People tend to be aware that there are risks associated with internet
banking (78%).93 They believe the main risks are key logging and hacking
(53%), the use of unsecured sites (23%), identity theft (23%) and credit
86
Page 29
card fraud (17%). Sixty-three percent of these people are also aware of
ways to minimise internet banking risk.94 They cite using an up-to-date
anti-virus software (38%), having a firewall (26%) and changing
passwords regularly (22%) as the best ways to reduce internet risks.
The most likely sources of information and advice that people would
consider using are financial advisers (82%), accountants/tax agents
(81%), family members (63%), banks (60%) and friends (55%).99
The most common reasons people seek information and advice are tax
advice/assistance in completing a tax return (28%), investment advice
(27%), general advice (13%) and taking out a mortgage or refinancing
a mortgage (10%).100
One in two people (51%) feel they need further financial education or
information.101 Those most likely to say they need further
education/information are people under 35 years of age (64%). Those
aged 60 years and over (26%) are the least likely to say they need it.
The top five topics that people say they are interested in and want
further education/information about include investing (39%),
94
Page 30
102
Page 31
What people do
Key points
At a time when consumers are being asked to make more financial
decisions than ever before, the environment in which they are making
those decisions is becoming increasingly complex.
Knowledge alone is not enough. People dont always act in their own best
interests, sometimes by not engaging at all and sometimes by making bad
decisions.
Behavioural studies and ASICs own consumer and investor research
identifies a range of related barriers that prevent people from making good
financial decisions, including information and choice overload, complexity
and uncertainty, time factors and pressures, over (and under) confidence,
self-control and framing (i.e. how information is presented).
Research in this area, particularly applied research in real financial
settings, is still developing. More (and better) research is necessary to
understand the drivers of action/inaction.
A key obstacle to saving more is not necessarily lack of awareness, but rather
the ability to take action on the knowledge (Mitchell & Utkus, 2003, p. 6).
While raising peoples level of financial knowledge forms the basis of many
financial literacy initiatives around the world, there is a growing body of
research suggesting that knowledge is only one factor when considering how
to help people make positive financial decisions. This section covers what
people do with the knowledge they have and, more commonly, what people
do in spite of their knowledge and why.111
Common barriers
Consumer and investor research suggests that the ways in which people
approach financial decisions vary widely and depend on a range of shifting
and conflicting factors, including life stage, past experience, emotional
impulses, social networks, personal traits, socio-economic status and the
external environment. Financial decisions, like many life decisions, are
contextual and complex.
111
For detailed statistics about what surveyed Australians say they do (e.g. budgeting), see Appendix 1 Tables A1.1A1.16.
Page 32
Behavioural biases
Behavioural finance, sometimes referred to as behavioural economics
outside Australia, has established a range of theories and a growing body of
research to explain how peoples thought processes affect their ability to
make financial decisions. Behavioural economists argue that the rational
consumer that underpins traditional economic theory does not exist
instead, people are simply normal.
Behavioural economists claim that normal people are affected by
behavioural biases when making everyday decisions and particularly when
making complex decisions, including financial decisions. They believe these
barriers are not limited to certain types or groups of people and in fact can
also affect professional financial service providers such as investment traders
(Gervais & Odean, 2001).
The notion of decision biases is not new or exclusive to behavioural
economics. Essentially, the effect of these shortfalls is that people:
make mistakes.
Page 33
60% of the passers-by approached the table with the extensive-choice option
compared to only 40% for the limited-choice option.
However, purchasing habits were reversed, with 30% of those encountering
the limited option actually purchasing a jam, compared to only 3% of those
offered the extensive choice. Subsequent studies by Iyengar et al (2003) in a
retirement savings context have confirmed these findings. After testing
employee records (including non-participants records) from hundreds of
401(k) retirement plans, they found that plans offering fewer than 10 options
had significantly higher employee participation rates.
Local research too has uncovered the de-motivating impact of too many choices.
For example, Fear (2008) conducted focus groups with Australians about a range
of finance issues and found that many participants, and particularly people over 30,
found the breadth of product choice bewildering:
Even our credit union had seven or eight different types of home loan. And Im
thinking, I just want to buy the bloody house (5070, Adelaide, higher income).
Its just day-to-day things, like mobile phone contracts or your electricity.
What sort of a genius can work out which of these five or six options is the
best one? (5070, Adelaide, higher income) (Fear, 2008, p. 24).
Page 34
Information overload was only one of a range of factors that made the
decision process deeply unpleasant for these retirees: see Figure 2.
Figure 2: Reasons retirement decision was unpleasant (ASIC, 2004)
Degree of concentration
required
Importance of decision (a
lot at stake) combined with
uncertainty that decision
would meet future needs*
Coincidence of decision
with negative life events**
Requirement to consult
with financial advisers,
and their costs
**
The knowledge and experience these retirees gained didnt necessarily lead
to an increased sense of financial competence or comfort. Indeed, at the time
of their interviews, some appeared to be confused about the features and
terminology of the products they had purchased, suggesting that just as we
Page 35
Nature of barrier
Uncertainty/risk (38%)
Uncertainty (general)
Employment
Political/legal/taxation
Financial outcomes
Personal financial situation
Life expectancy
Information/knowledge (32%)
Complexity
Acquiring information
Lack of knowledge/understanding
Ambiguity
Emotion (8%)
Source: Adapted from Clark-Murphy et al, 2002, pp. 1314. See Appendix 1 Table A1.18 for
more detailed information.
Page 36
Age and gender emerged as demographic drivers in this study. Those aged
under 55 and women were more likely to find the decision difficult.112
Gender differences appear regularly in the literature. For example, the ANZ
financial literacy survey (2008) found that women (30%) were significantly
more likely than men (14%) to say their superannuation statements were too
difficult to understand (p. 72).
However, the underlying causes of these differences remain contentious and
inconclusive. For example, Clark-Murphy & Gerrans (2002) later explored
the gender drivers they had discovered in their superannuation study in
closer detail and concluded that the cause might have been that men and
women processed the supporting information differently rather than an
inherent ability/difficulty understanding finances.
112
Those who were over 55 might have found the decision less difficult because of their closer proximity to retirement.
Page 37
Shares and managed funds are also intangible, which leads some investors to
believe they are more risky than more concrete or familiar investments
such as property:
The stock market is more volatile, whereas real estate is less volatile and
long term and the level of risk is lower. You can be scientific about it.
Property vacant for two weeks every year and add another 5% to
maintenance costs (Female, investment property focus group, Melbourne)
(ASIC, 2008b, p. 38).
This may in part be explained by the availability bias, which causes people
to over-value information that is easy to recall or visualise (Tversky &
Kahneman, 1974).
Underestimating product complexity and/or risk was an important factor in
ASICs research about unlisted, unrated debenture (UUD) investors (ASIC,
2008c), in which some investors were motivated by a false perception that
their investment was comparable to a simple, familiar bank deposit product.
This was the case even though the average self-assessed level of
understanding of the prospectus (which described the product) was as high
as 7 out of 10.
Ironically, the disclosure documents designed to help people make financial
choices are themselves considered complex by many people. In ASICs
research into UUD investors, a number of investors (across different
comparison groups) chose not to read the prospectus or ignored the
information in it. Some felt they didnt or wouldnt understand it, some had
already made their decision, some relied on other factors (e.g. assurances
from the salesperson), and others were uncertain about the validity of the
document:
I make a decision before I read the prospectus; its probably naive of me
But even the prospectus can be full of lies anyway, like in this case, its
only a piece of paper with all this information. Even if you read it they can
tell lies as well (Female, ACR,113 in-depth interview, NSW) (ASIC, 2008c,
pp. 2526).
113
Page 38
Superannuation
28%
8%
34%
8%
30%
8%
Insurance
24%
6%
Mortgage
26%
4%
Financial planner/adviser
41%
3%
40%
3%
Recent ASIC (2010b) research into retirees and pre-retirees found that some
people approaching retirement had a life-long pattern of making decisions
on the fly due to a combination of both personality and time pressures.
It is not only complex decisions or actions that are affected by real or
perceived time pressures. For example, time factors featured in an omnibus
survey commissioned by ASIC (2008d) about financial complaints. In this
research, 52% of those surveyed were dissatisfied with their financial
product or service provider in the last two years, but only 29% of those
surveyed had made a complaint in the same period: see Table 12.
Page 39
Percent
16%
3%
3%
Source: ASIC, 2008d (based on Newspoll study conducted in June 2008 by phone among
n=1200 adults aged 18 and over nationally).
Competing time pressures is only one of the time factors that affect peoples
financial decision making. Behavioural economists have also described
peoples tendency towards myopiathat is, the further away an event is, the
harder it is for people to appropriately judge (and respond to) it. As a result,
people tend to overvalue things that are immediate and concrete over things
that are distant and abstract. This has obvious implications for peoples
propensity to save for retirement:
For hyperbolic discounters, rewards are left to accelerate quickly, and then
taper off. Put simply, workers who are hyperbolic discounters place a lower
value on future benefits and overvalue the present. The application to
retirement is clear: they will overconsume today and undersave, as a result
of self-control problems when it comes to saving for retirement (Mitchell &
Utkus, 2003, pp. 56).
It is not surprising then that ASIC and industry surveys regularly find
evidence of poor retirement planning, not only in younger populations
(e.g. FINSIA, 2006) but also those approaching retirement (e.g. ASIC, 2010
and MWS, 2006).
Not only can too much time be just as de-motivating as too much choice, but
the more indefinite the future event is, the more difficult it is to act. For
example Tversky & Shafir (1992, cited by Shafir in Productivity
Commission, 2007) conducted an experiment in which they offered
participants $5 for answering and returning a long questionnaire by a given
date. Three groups of participants were each given different deadlines, and
response rates declined the longer the time given:
This would help explain why less than half (47%) of the 1217 general investors
surveyed by ASIC in 2007 said they had a long-term financial goal and a plan to
reach that goal, and many (37%) had neither a plan nor a goal (2008a).
Page 40
ASICs retiree (2010, 2004) and investor (2008b) research has also found that
people often underestimate the amount of time they need to plan for events
such as retirement, and as a result find themselves faced with sudden, major
financial decisions after unexpected negative life events such as redundancy,
illness, death or divorce. People are not only often bad planners but their
future forecasts are often characterized by widespread overconfidence and
excessive optimism (Mitchell & Utkus, 2003, p. 22). The cold realisation that
they now need to make an important, life-changing financial decision (and the
emotion generated by the negative life event that precipitated the decision)
drives some to panicked, financially unhealthy choices.
Examples
Lack of objectivity
Page 41
Again, gender emerged as a key factor, with men more likely to invest and
ignore the advice of others, and women less likely to invest and more likely
to urge caution.
In 2005, the OECD reported that one of the key commonalities across the
financial literacy surveys conducted by 11 OECD countries at that time was
that consumers often believe they know more about financial matters than is
actually the case (p. 91). Therefore, overconfidence not only hampers
peoples financial decisions but also researchers ability to measure financial
literacy levels. This means that financial literacy tests based on selfassessment are likely to overstate levels of financial literacy.
Nevertheless, even self-assessment based surveys reveal evidence of
overconfidence. For example, 69% of the Australians surveyed in the Financial
Literacy Foundations Understanding Money research (2007a) believed they
understood investing and 88% believed they could recognise a scam or an
investment scheme that seemed too good to be true, but only 34% said they
would consider both risk and return when making an investment decision.
Page 42
ASICs investor research (2008b) also found that low self-confidence can
lead to inertia:
Thereve been many, many times when I meant to do something and I
didnt have quite enough confidence in what I thought (Male, novice
investor: managed investment focus group, Melbourne) (p. 62).
Others were driven by a fear of failure or, more commonly, a fear of risk:
I dont think its procrastination, just having a fear of failure. No one wants
to fail (Female, novice investor: shares focus group, Melbourne) (p. 62).
Ive got to be careful that I keep enough to live off. Just knowing that its
going to be safe and I dont make any wrong decisions (Female, shares
CATI interview, 5559) (p. 58).
The Financial Literacy Foundation (FLF, 2007a) has identified three possible
paths to a poor or suboptimal financial outcome stemming from either a lack of
confidence, confidence or overconfidence: see Appendix 1 Figure A1.1. They
also described the important interplay between confidence and self-efficacy:
In effect, self-efficacy gives a person a boost in confidence to believe that
they can influence the outcome Self-efficacy is a concept that is closely
related to confidence, but is created through the process of taking action.
Self-efficacy is built on the experience of having mastered and overcome
challenges, and can be characterised as learning by doing. It is not
developed through merely thinking about a situation. For each individual,
the action which builds their sense of self-efficacy will be different (p. 53).
Page 43
Self-control
As in other facets of daily life, people sometimes struggle with self-control
when dealing with their finances. This includes knowing what you should do
but not doing it (e.g. budgeting, saving) and knowing what you should not
do but doing it anyway (e.g. credit card overuse and investing in risky offers
that others have advised against, like the cold calling scam described above).
Although other factors such as income, social norms and other behavioural
biases influence peoples spending habits, self-control is undoubtedly one of
the factors behind the Financial Literacy Foundations (FLF, 2007a) finding
that, while ability to budget is the area in which Australians are most
confident (90%):
48% of adults said they do not budget regularly for their day-to-day
finances
27% had difficulty setting money aside for big purchases or spending
17% could not get by for some time in case of a financial emergency.
This not only helps explain why people make poor financial decisions (or
neglect to act at all), but also why some financial literacy programs dont work.
For example, Choi et al (2001) found that two-thirds (68%) of their sample of
401(k) retirement plan participants believed their savings rate was too low but:
only 35% intended to increase the rate in the next few months
four months later, only 14% had actually made changes to their plan.
Page 44
Non-attendees
Planned
change
Actual
change
Actual
change
100%
14%
7%
28%
8%
5%
47%
15%
10%
36%
10%
6%
Planned action
Non-participants
Enroll in 401(k) plan
401(k) participants
The sample is active 401(k)-eligible employees at company locations that offered financial
education seminars from January to June 2000. Actual changes in savings behaviour are
measured over the period from December 31, 1999 to June 30, 2000. Planned changes
are those reported by seminar attendees in an evaluation of the financial education
seminars at the conclusion of the seminar. The planned changes from survey responses
of attendees have been scaled to reflect the 401(k) participation rate of seminar
attendees.
Framing
The concept of framing effects, first explored in an economic context by
Tversky & Kahneman (1981), essentially refers to the fact that people are
susceptible to the way options are presented to them, a bias that heavily
informs legitimate commercial advertising and is widely exploited by those
who market scams and frauds.
The powerful impact of framing was evident in ASICs research into UUD
investors (2008c). ACR/Fincorp investors in particular were strongly
influenced by the product advertising: see Figure 3.
Page 45
100
88.1
Active
General
80
60
45.2
37.0
40
26.6
20
20.7
20.3 19.0
3.9
17.3
3.3
13.8
4.7
0
Advertising ranked as #1
Adviser ranked as #1
Associate ranked as #1
Other ranked as #1
ACR/Fincorp=362,Active=261,General=473
Investors were particularly attracted to the way the messages were framed
around security and certainty.
This framing capitalised on another behavioural bias known as loss
aversion, in which people feel losses more acutely than gains (Tversky &
Kahneman, 1981, 1991). ACR/Fincorp investors were sold safety (i.e. the
prevention of loss) over gain. Indeed, investors often described the fact that
the interest rate offered was modest, and felt this contributed to the
perception that the investment was relatively safe:
I call risk, 10, 12, 14, 16%. I dont call 8% a big risk. Thats why I invested
with them, because I thought it was a steady company and they werent
offering any big money, just a little bit [more than] a bank (Male, ACR,
focus group, NSW) (ASIC, 2008c, p. 27).
The safety theme was also reinforced by the company staff some investors
spoke with:
I think it depends on the salesmanship, because he convinced me the
money was safe...and he gave me a prospectus and he convinced me, thats
it (Female, ACR, in-depth interview, NSW) (p. 23).
Framing was also used to great effect by the marketers described in ASICs
study of Australians approached by international cold calling investment
scams. For example, the language used by the cold callers was carefully
designed to trigger a persons sense of identity and pride:
It is instructive that the cold callers often used the expression capacity
to invest when first making their offers. This wording implies that having
the money is an ability that the person possesses rather than an objective
statement about their finances. This would suggest that the power to invest
had implications about potential investors as people (ASIC, 2002, p. 78).
Framing has also been used to achieve positive financial outcomes. For
example, behavioural economists have attempted to lift savings rates by
Page 46
Negative
114
The maximum matched amount was $1000 at the time the research was conducted. This amount was subsequently
reduced to $500.
115
The relationship managers worked closely with the Saver Plus participants to help them set and achieve their savings
goal. The relationship managers were from community organisations, including the Brotherhood of St Laurence, the
Benevolent Society and the Smith Family.
Page 47
Key points
Although the financial literacy movement has gained momentum over the
past few years, there remains little reliable, conclusive research about
whether financial literacy campaigns and programs work (i.e. whether they
result in sustained changes in behaviour and improved financial outcomes).
Best-practice principles for program and evaluation techniques are still
developing, and it is widely recognised that both are inherently difficult.
In the interim, the established principles of social marketing, frequently
used in health and environmental fields, provide a practical big picture
framework for financial literacy-based behavioural change initiatives.
Evaluation to date
Robust evaluation is essential to draw reliable conclusions about financial literacy
interventions. Without evaluation, an intervention is unproven. Without
appropriate evaluation, an interventions impact might be misinterpreted.
A recent review of financial literacy research by Agarwal et al (2010)
concluded that:
... it is not clear that effective programs improve behaviour through
increased literacy, whether programs are cost-effective, or which types of
programs are most effective. Answering these questions will require a great
deal more research. Fortunately, the recent proliferation of financial
education programs provides ample opportunity to conduct such research.
However, the designs of existing programs are rarely conducive to robust
impact evaluations (Agarwal et al, 2010, p. 25).
Page 48
The types of design shortfalls identified in these and other reports include:
no evaluation at all
116
Page 49
2.
3.
4.
5.
Careful consideration of the sample size, taking into account the analysis
that will be needed to understand the outcomes.
6.
7.
8.
9.
A control group to show the normal changes that take place in the
absence of such an initiative.
10.
Page 50
Their vision was to interweave evaluation into the program itself, making good
programming a part of good measurement and vice versa (p. 65).
Table 17: Suggested framework for the evaluation of the effectiveness
of financial education* (NZ Retirement Commission, 2007)
Tier
Requirements
1. Need
2. Accountability
3. Fine-tuning
4. Micro impact
5. Macro impact
118
For more information about the INFEs initiatives, including its guidelines, see:
www.oecd.org/document/50/0,3343,en_39665975_39666038_39711282_1_1_1_1,00.html.
Page 51
The UK and NZ reviews also recognise that robust evaluation is costly and
time-consuming, a key concern for American financial professionals and
educators surveyed by Lyons et al in 2006: see Table 18. Unfortunately the
cheapest and quickest method of evaluation (i.e. a simple satisfaction
survey) is the least reliable.
Table 18: Barriers and challenges associated with program evaluation
(Lyons et al, 2006)
Barriers and challenges
Percent
59.5%
52.3%
48.3%
38.9%
34.6%
29.9%
8.1%
N/A
Page 52
While not going so far as to abandon its financial capability initiatives, the
FSA endeavoured to take into account these findings when undertaking
costbenefit analysis.
Since then, it has established the Consumer Financial Education Body
(CFEB), a body responsible for helping consumers understand financial
matters and manage their finances better. The CFEB is committed to
examining behavioural change programs and commissioned a study by
Elliott et al (2010) that examined a number of financial capability
interventions. The study concluded that there was enormous scope to alter
the [decision] environment in a way that encourages greater levels of
financial capability and that, while the process was at an early stage,
wellstructured research leading to interventions being implemented and
evaluated to ensure that the desired creation of financial capability is taking
place was an essential element (p. 71).
Page 53
In Australia, much of the literature about social marketing appears within the
health field. Below are the 12 key principles of an effective social marketing
approach devised by Kotler and Lee and advocated by the APSC (2007b,
pp. 2126). These principles are supplemented with finance-based findings
from other relevant sources.
119
The OECD has a website listing some of the available research about
financial literacy, called the International Gateway for Financial
Education (IGFE).119
Page 54
Characteristics
Pre-contemplation
Contemplation
Preparation/action
Page 55
Phase
Characteristics
Maintenance
Termination
Source: Adapted from information in APSC, 2007b; Xiao & Wu, 2006; ONeill et al, 1999.
The TTM was developed by Prochaska et al.
Existing research indicates that Australians generally support the idea of financial
literacy, including government involvement (Fear, 2008), and have some appetite
for financial education across a range of money topics: see Appendix 1 Table
A1.15. However, as demonstrated in Section C, people dont necessarily know
what they dont know and they have trouble carrying out their good intentions.
This also makes it difficult to assess existing findings.
For example the Financial Literacy Foundation (FLF, 2008) found that 68%
of women believe it is important for them to learn more about how to invest
money now, yet ASIC (2008b) research found that only 8% of female
investors have attended paid training courses or seminars. Given the many
possible reasons for these conflicting results (e.g. social desirability bias, low
confidence, competing priorities, a preference against group learning, limited
time, cost restraints), caution must be exercised when assessing which stage
of change particular groups and/or individuals are in.
Page 56
helping raise the quality of (and access to) personal financial advice
improvements to disclosure
We also need to remember that ASIC is only one player in the cause to
remove barriers. The financial wellbeing of Australians is the responsibility
of a wide range of stakeholders, including industry, government,
communities and individuals. Where possible (and appropriate), we should
aim to pool resources and establish a consistent voice.
Interestingly, the work of behavioural economists provides us with an
opportunity to turn barriers into advantages. Based on the argument that
departures from rationality are often, though not always, systematic (Barber &
Odean, 1999), some have designed interventions that use the same behavioural
biases that currently hinder peoples financial decisions to instead improve their
financial outcomes, a strategy referred to as libertarian paternalism (Thaler &
Sunstein, 2008, p. 4).
Specific examples are covered later in this report under the discussion about
programs (see pp. 6269) but they include:
Thaler & Sunstein (2008) argue that people need these nudging devices
for decisions that are difficult and rare, for which they do not get prompt
feedback, and when they have trouble translating aspects of the situation into
terms that they can easily understand (p. 72).120
120
Thaler & Sunstein (2008) define the term nudge as any aspect of the choice architecture that alters peoples behaviour
in a predictable way without forbidding any options or significantly changing their economic incentives (p. 6). The word
nudges also stands for the six principles Thaler & Sunstein recommend for choice architects when designing systems to
improve peoples financial (or other) outcomes:
iNcentives; Understand mappings; Defaults; Give feedback; Expect error; Structure complex choices (p. 100).
Page 57
121
For a summary of some of the ways in which behavioural economics might be applied to policy, see Behavioural
economics: seven principles for policy-makers (Dawnay & Shah, 2005).
Page 58
Percent*
Tax-related advantages:
Lower tax/favourable tax treatment of contributions
30%
8%
6%
Nature of investment:
Money is locked away so you cant get at it
29%
Secure/safe/low-risk
15%
7%
Other advantages:
Government co-contribution
9%
6%
6%
Cant say**
20%
The sample for this question included only superannuation fund members who were
under age 65 (n=766).
**
More common among young people aged 18 to 24 years (35%) and members of
Quintile 1 (46%) and Quintile 2 (34%).
Page 59
These methods are often less expensive than direct monetary incentives and
have the added benefit of social proof, in which the behaviour becomes
desirable among, and spreads within, peer groups.122 They also act as useful
reminders for those trying to avoid relapses.
As well as considering innovative non-monetary incentives to recognise and
reward those who take steps to improve their financial wellbeing, ASIC
could make use of its existing non-monetary resources. While not linked to
actual changes in behaviour, anecdotal evidence suggests that some
Retirement Expo attendees visit the ASIC stall because they are attracted to
ASICs FIDO showbags (e.g. they have seen other expo attendees with the
bags). Anecdotal feedback also suggests that some of the booklets in the
122
For more information about the important role of social networks in changing behaviour see the APSCs (2007b)
discussion about social capital theory (p. 17). See also Brobecks (1999) discussion about the tipping point, which is said to
occur when approximately half the community have changed their behaviour (p. 7).
Page 60
showbags are not only read by the expo attendees but also recommended and
passed on to family members and/or friends.
Page 61
start with small requests (research suggests that people who agree to
small changes are more likely to agree to a subsequent larger change)
Programs
The number of programs dealing with financial literacy has risen steadily in
recent years. While some of these programs have not recorded or reported
Page 62
Topics
Delivery tools
General public
Budgeting/savings
Online resources/training
School children/teenagers
(primary and high school)
Credit/debt management
Group/classroom training
Investment/retirement planning
One-on-one training/coaching
Employees/workplaces
Home ownership
Printed materials
Insurance
TV/film
Tax
123
For more comprehensive information about the scope of the programs in the United States specifically, see Appendix 1
Table A1.19. For a review of programs in the 27 European Union (EU) member states, see Evers & Jung (2007).
124
A local program called Saver Plus, which was developed by the ANZ and the Brotherhood of St Laurence for those on
low incomes, has all three of these elements (Russell et al, 2008a, 2008b).
Page 63
revealed they had no effect on individual savings decisions (Cole & Shastry,
2008). Possibly the benefits of these programs, and in particular schoolbased interventions, will appear in the longer term. Indeed, this was
Atkinsons (2008) conclusion in her review of financial literacy evaluations
for the FSA:
In the shorter term there may be some improvement in confidence but the
evidence that is currently available suggests that only the very youngest
gain financial knowledge through school interventions. Rather, it appears
that the main impact of school financial education is to improve behaviour
in adulthood (p. 73).
For this reason, some financial interventions have incorporated findings from
behavioural economics. While still voluntary, these programs are designed to
help people to who want to save more but lack the willpower to act.
Page 64
Hyperbolic
discounting
Loss aversion
Inertia/status quo
bias
This program has been tested in the United States across three different
firms, each under slightly different conditions (e.g. some gave employees
access to a one-on-one financial consultant and some didnt).
The first firm, a manufacturing company, initially gave eligible employees
the opportunity to speak with an external investment consultant about their
retirement savings levels. Of the 315 eligible employees, 286 agreed to talk
to the consultant. However, only 79 (28%) were willing to accept the
consultants advice. The remainder were given the opportunity to join the
SMarT plan instead, of which 162 (78%) did.
The results for this group were very positive, and have remained positive,
over four subsequent pay increases:
Most participants did not change their mind once the savings increases
took place:
80% remained in the plan through all four pay increases
Page 65
three participants (2%) dropped out of the plan prior to the second pay
increase, 23 (14%) dropped out between the second and third pay
increases and six (4%) between the third and forth pay increases.
Even those who withdrew from the plan saved significantly more than
before the plan (i.e. they did not reduce their contribution rates to the
original levels but rather stopped future increases from taking place).
Before selecting participants for this field experiment, the researchers first
conducted a baseline survey with 1777 existing or former customers of a real
bank. The survey included questions designed to identify peoples time
discounting preferences (i.e. to identify those who might have a preference for
commitment).126 One month later, 710 of the original customers were
randomly selected and offered the commitment product. Of these, 202 (28%)
opened an account. The researchers found that women who exhibited a lower
discount rate for future relative to current tradeoffs, and hence potentially
have a preference for commitment, were indeed significantly more likely to
open the commitment savings account (Ashraf et al, 2005, p. 1).127
Importantly, Ashraf et al used a control group during this field experiment.
Those not offered the SEED product were assigned to either:
125
One withdrawal restriction was based on reaching a predefined date and one on reaching a certain monetary limit. Once
the decision was made, it could not be changed and withdrawals were restricted until the chosen goal was met. Of the 202
opened accounts, 140 opted for the date-based goal and 62 opted for the amount-based goal.
126
More specifically, the researchers were looking for answers indicating impatience in relation to near-term trade-offs but
patience in relation to future trade-offs.
127
A similar trend was observed for men but it was not statistically significant.
Page 66
After a year, the average savings balances of those who accessed the SEED
product increased by 81 percentage points relative to those assigned to the
control group, suggesting not only that the intervention was successful at
raising peoples savings levels, but also that the result was less likely to be a
short-term response to a new product (Ashraf et al, 2005, p. 1).
Saver Plus
Saver Plus was developed to help people on low incomes establish a longterm savings habit. The program works by matching peoples savings dollarfor-dollar once they reach their savings goal, to a maximum amount.128 ANZ
(2009) research conducted by Chant Link and Associates identified sustained
behavioural change in some participants as a result of the Saver Plus
program, and analysed these outcomes within a behavioural economics
framework. The three levels of change the researchers identified were:
Moderate change: For these Saver Plus participants, the program had
some positive impact (e.g. new skills or financial benefits) but did not
significantly change their longer-term financial situation.
The research suggested that certain program elements increased the likelihood
of behavioural change, including the involvement of a relationship manager,
financial education workshops, matching funds, and the program rules in
general.129 These factors influenced participants at different stages of the
program (e.g. the financial incentive tended to be most successful in
encouraging participation at the start). Although all program elements were
found to be important, overall the most critical were, in order of importance:
128
The maximum matched amount was $1000 at the time the research was conducted. This amount was subsequently
reduced to $500.
129
The relationship managers worked closely with the Saver Plus participants to help them set and achieve their savings
goal. The relationship managers were from community organisations, including the Brotherhood of St Laurence, the
Benevolent Society and the Smith Family.
Page 67
Page 68
Ego: We act in ways that make us feel better about ourselves (p. 11).
Conclusion
In closing, while there appears to be little certainty about the ultimate direction
of financial literacy, there does seem to be reasonable consensus that:
Regardless of the debates about the best way to change peoples behaviour
for the better, most agree that there is a genuine need to do so, and that the
government plays an important role:
Ultimately we have to realise that people have weaknesses and limitations.
People realise that themselvesthey want help and are willing to ask for it,
including from government (Shafir in Productivity Commission, 2007, p. 145).
Page 69
Research title
Methodology/sample
Method(s) used
to assess
behaviour
and/or financial
130
literacy
ABS (2009)
Retirement and
retirement
intentions
Subjective
assessment
Personal fraud
Subjective
assessment
Retirement and
retirement
intentions
Subjective
assessment
130
Page 70
Research
item
(source)
Research title
Methodology/sample
Method(s) used
to assess
behaviour
and/or financial
130
literacy
ANZ (2009)
Understanding the
success of Saver
Plus
Combination
assessments
ANZ (2008)
Combination
assessments
Australian investors:
At a glance
Combination
assessments
Understanding
investors in the
unlisted, unrated
debenture (UUD)
market
Subjective
assessment
ASIC
(2008d)
Complaint
resolution in relation
to financial
institutions study
Subjective
assessment
ASIC
(2007a)
Subjective
assessment
CBA (2007)
The Commonwealth
Banks 2007
E-Money survey
highlights
Subjective
assessment
Page 71
Research
item
(source)
Research title
Methodology/sample
Method(s) used
to assess
behaviour
and/or financial
130
literacy
CBF (2006)
Australian financial
literacy assessment
Objective
assessment
Improving financial
literacy in Australia:
Benefits for the
individual and the
nation
Objective
assessment
Subjective
assessment
Citibank
(2008)
Subjective
assessment
Fear (2008)
Choice overload:
Australians coping
with financial
decisions
Subjective
assessment
Generational wealth
divide research
FINSIA
(2009)
Page 72
Research
item
(source)
Research title
Methodology/sample
Method(s) used
to assess
behaviour
and/or financial
130
literacy
FINSIA
(2006)
Subjective
assessment
FLF
(2007a)
Financial literacy:
Australians
understanding
money
Subjective
assessment
FPA (2007)
Consumer attitudes
to financial planning
Subjective
assessment
Report of findings of
qualitative research
into effective
disclosure (stage II)
Subjective
assessment
IFSA (2007)
MWS
(2010)
Super decisions:
Communicating with
customers and
effective disclosure
Superannuation
sentiment index
Subjective
assessment
Page 73
Research
item
(source)
Research title
Methodology/sample
Method(s) used
to assess
behaviour
and/or financial
130
literacy
MWS
(2009)
Superannuation
sentiment index
Subjective
assessment
Mercer 2006
financial literacy
and retirement
readiness study
Combination
assessments
Page 74
Table A1.2: What Australians know and do: Banking and payment/transacting methods
What people know
Percent of respondents
Research item
2005
2008
ATM
92%
94%
EFTPOS
90%
91%
Credit card
92%
92%
Direct debit
83%
87%
BPAY
68%
72%
Cheques
90%
88%
Internet banking
62%
69%
Telephone banking
71%
72%
Lay-bys
83%
83%
Money orders
82%
83%
Store cards
71%
69%
NA
15%
Payday loans
What people do
Percent of respondents
Research item
Use:
2005
2008
ATM
78%
80%
EFTPOS
74%
76%
Credit card
68%
65%
Direct debit
60%
64%
BPAY
46%
52%
Cheques
44%
39%
Internet banking
40%
51%
Telephone banking
36%
32%
Lay-bys
27%
25%
Money orders
21%
20%
Store cards
16%
13%
NA
2%
97%
97%
NA
46%
22%
20%
21%
19%
2008
2010
100%
100%
Payday loans
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 75
Research item
88%
FLF (2007a)
65%
FLF (2007a)
34%
FLF (2007a)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
Dont save
22%
FLF (2007a)
62%
FLF (2007a)
15%
FLF (2007a)
4%
FLF (2007a)
72%
FLF (2007a)
What people do
2008
2010
50%
45%
22%
18%
28%
36%
15%
13%
2005
2008
69%
72%
11%
10%
NA
46%
22%
20%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 76
Research item
90%
FLF (2007a)
57%
FLF (2007a)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
48%
FLF (2007a)
27%
FLF (2007a)
18%
FLF (2007a)
17%
FLF (2007a)
What people do
2008
2010
19%
21%
Despite their best intentions they make a budget but dont always
stick to it
60%
59%
21%
20%
44%
NA
2005
2008
24%
24%
Spend all of their income as soon as they get it and dont really
plan for the future
14%
14%
24%
23%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 77
Research item
89%
FLF (2007a)
61%
FLF (2007a)
88%
CBF (2004)
Able to choose the best option when they cannot pay a bill
55%
CBF (2004)
48%
CBF (2004)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
21%
FLF (2007a)
17%
FLF (2007a)
32%
FLF (2007a)
Pay more than the minimum and make extra payments where
they can
31%
FLF (2007a)
Dont regularly pay the total balance owing on their credit card
when its due
20%
FLF (2007a)
Pay only the minimum balance owing on their credit card when
its due
13%
FLF (2007a)
What people do
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 78
Research item
83%
FLF (2007a)
49%
FLF (2007a)
2005
2008
Able to correctly identify the most likely cause of bad credit rating
61%
59%
29%
NA
72%
77%
94%
CBF (2004)
62%
CBF (2004)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
What people do
2008
2010
69%
73%
2005
2008
68%
65%
16%
13%
5%
8%
12%
12%
5%
9%
NA
93%
NA
74%
71%
28%
21%
21%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 79
Table A1.7: What Australians know and do: Loans and mortgages
Percent of
respondents
Research item
93%
CBF (2004)
51%
CBF (2004)
2005
2008
61%
59%
11%
11%
9%
11%
7%
6%
Unsure
12%
13%
Both persons are responsible for the repayment of the entire loan
80%
83%
15%
12%
3%
3%
1%
<1%
Unsure
1%
2%
What people do
Percent of
respondents
If two people take out a loan, which one of the following most
accurately describes the responsibility for repayment of the loan?
Research item
62%
FLF (2007a)
2008
2010
Have a mortgage
38%
46%
30%
NA
2005
2008
29%
34%
11%
11%
14%
17%
12%
14%
9%
9%
9%
7%
2%
2%
NA
2%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 80
Table A1.7 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Loans and mortgages
What people do (cont.)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
27%
24%
Family or friends
14%
14%
11%
10%
Finance company
6%
7%
1%
1%
Pawnbroker
1%
2%
Payday lender
1%
1%
NA
<1%
Other
1%
2%
None of these
55%
55%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 81
Table A1.8: What Australians know and do: Equity release product (reverse mortgages)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
46%
ANZ (2008)
51%
ANZ (2008)
Unsure
3%
ANZ (2008)
14%
ANZ (2008)
Interest rate
13%
ANZ (2008)
11%
ANZ (2008)
10%
ANZ (2008)
My ongoing expenses
9%
ANZ (2008)
9%
ANZ (2008)
7%
ANZ (2008)
4%
ANZ (2008)
8%
ANZ (2008)
Dont know
33%
ANZ (2008)
Borrowers who did not know how much the loan was likely to cost
them over time
14 out of 29
(ASIC 2007a)
6 out of 29
(ASIC 2007a)
Borrowers who did not know what would happen if they breached
a loan condition
17 out of 29
(ASIC 2007a)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2%
ANZ (2008)
What people do
Have an equity release product
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 82
Table A1.9: What Australians know and do: Financial documents (financial statements, PDSs,
prospectuses)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
21%
FLF (2007a)
79%
FLF (2007a)
48%
CBF (2006)
25%
CBF (2006)
2005
2008
Very difficult
6%
4%
Difficult
28%
27%
Easy
52%
53%
Very easy
12%
12%
Cant say
3%
4%
Prospectuses:
Type 1 investor rated their understanding as medium to high
6.7 out of 10
ASIC (2008c)
7.0 out of 10
ASIC (2008c)
PDSs:
People are confused about the role of PDSs. They view them either as a selling tool
and/or an education tool, and/or a reference document.
Year 9 and Year 10 students able to understand the term product
disclosure statement
What people do
IFSA (2008c)
43%
CBF (2006)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
76%
75%
15%
17%
9%
8%
Couldnt be bothered
39%
36%
8%
14%
7%
13%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 83
Table A1.9 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Financial documents (financial statements,
PDSs, prospectuses)
What people do (cont.)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
10%
4%
29%
23%
8%
8%
11%
12%
NA
4%
Unsure/cant recall
Prospectuses:
Investors who said they received the prospectus
71.6%
ASIC (2008c)
14%
IFSA (2007)
PDSs:
People who reviewed the PDS before making the decision to
switch their superannuation
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 84
Research item
42%
MWS (2006)
70%
FLF (2007a)
46%
Fear (2008)
89%
CBF (2004)
88%
CBF (2004)
84%
CBF (2004)
82%
CBF (2004)
79%
CBF (2004)
78%
CBF (2004)
2005
2008
Believe that investments with high returns are likely to have higher
than average risks
87%
86%
47%
52%
64%
67%
79%
78%
34%
FLF (2007a)
5%
FLF (2007a)
6%
FLF (2007a)
6%
FLF (2007a)
51%
ASIC (2008a)
7%
ASIC (2008a)
36%
ASIC (2008a)
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 85
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
NA
46%
NA
4%
22%
20%
74%
76%
Hold shares
40%
38%
27%
20%
24%
20%
19%
19%
18%
FLF (2007a)
46%
FLF (2007a)
62%
FLF (2007a)
46%
FLF (2007a)
72%
FLF (2007a)
49%
ASIC (2008a)
46%
ASIC (2008a)
16%
ASIC (2008a)
24%
ASIC (2008a)
46%
ASIC (2008a)
14%
ASIC (2008a)
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 86
Table A1.11: What Australians know and do: Planning for retirement
Percent of
respondents
Research item
21%
FLF (2007a)
16%
FLF (2007a)
81%
FLF (2007a)
63%
FLF (2007a)
77%
FLF (2007a)
71%
FLF (2007a)
2008
2010
51%
44%
13%
20%
2005
2008
93%
NA
Have not identified how much money they would need each year
to live on when they retire
64%
73%
94%
91%
44%
50%
NA
1%
$15,000$24,999
NA
4%
$25,000$49,999
NA
34%
$50,000$74,999
NA
32%
$75,000$99,999
NA
10%
$100,000 or more
NA
15%
Unsure
NA
4%
2008
2009
22%
27%
Do not know
12%
7%
Do not believe that the age pension will be sufficient for retirement
86%
FLF (2007a)
73%
FLF (2007a)
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 87
Table A1.11 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Planning for retirement
What people do
Percent of
respondents
Research item
76%
FLF (2007a)
2009
2010
Have not given much thought to retirement and have not made
any preparations for it
15%
19%
29%
34%
41%
36%
2008
2010
12%
14%
83%
80%
2005
2008
24%
20%
45%
24%
Allocated pension
16%
37%
12%
15%
Market-linked pension/annuity
8%
15%
Fixed-term pension/annuity
10%
10%
Other type
14%
8%
Unsure
5%
7%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 88
Research item
44%
Fear (2008)
39%
FPA (2007)
29%
MWS (2009)
82%
FINSIA (2006)
82%
CBF (2004)
2005
2008
97%
96%
92%
90%
22%
27%
Did not know about the potential problems with having multiple
superannuation funds
15%
16%
34%
31%
77%
68%
Did not know the types of things people commonly do that often
result in the reduction of the final value of superannuation or
managed investments
42%
42%
2009
2010
30%
27%
47%
47%
24%
22%
18%
21%
11%
13%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 89
Percent of
respondents
Research item
33%
FINSIA (2006)
2005
2008
74%
76%
59%
54%
26%
28%
8%
9%
6%
6%
Unsure
2%
3%
27%
19%
Industry fund
21%
33%
15%
18%
14%
14%
7%
7%
5%
5%
1%
1%
Other
1%
2%
Unsure
21%
20%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 90
Research item
82%
FLF (2007a)
44%
CBF (2004)
2005
2008
Not aware that a claim can be refused if the policyholder did not
meet duty of accurate disclosure
51%
54%
Not aware of the cooling-off period when taking out new insurance
policy
65%
68%
Premium
54%
53%
40%
47%
17%
20%
16%
14%
11%
15%
What people do
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2008
2010
48%
47%
18%
16%
34%
37%
2005
2008
82%
88%
79%
87%
60%
55%
35%
34%
12%
30%
NA
10%
74%
75%
24%
24%
86%
FLF (2007a)
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 91
Table A1.14: What Australians know and do: Protecting moneyscams and fraud
Percent of
respondents
Research item
88%
FLF (2007a)
69%
FLF (2007a)
93%
CBA (2007)
92%
CBA (2007)
55%
CBA (2007)
47%
CBA (2007)
2005
2008
Yes
78%
78%
No
20%
19%
59%
53%
Unsecured sites
27%
23%
19%
17%
11%
11%
Email scams
8%
9%
Phishing
4%
8%
Viruses
3%
2%
Identity theft
NA
23%
Cant say/unsure
4%
4%
Other
3%
12%
66%
63%
Use a firewall
32%
26%
27%
38%
23%
22%
19%
14%
11%
12%
9%
12%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 92
Table A1.14 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Protecting moneyscams and fraud
What people know (cont.)
What consumers know about how to minimise internet banking
risks (cont.):
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
9%
7%
7%
8%
2%
6%
Cant say/unsure
7%
5%
Other
10%
9%
Percent of
respondents
Research item
36% (5,809,100)
ABS (2008a)
2% (329,000)
ABS (2008a)
Lotteries
0.5% (84,100)
ABS (2008a)
Pyramid schemes
0.4% (70,900)
ABS (2008a)
0.4% (57,800)
ABS (2008a)
Financial advice
0.2% (28,500)
ABS (2008a)
Chain letters
0.2% (26,700)
ABS (2008a)
0.1% (16,000)
ABS (2008a)
Other scams
0.4% (69,100)
ABS (2008a)
5% (806,000)
ABS (2008a)
Victim of ID fraud
3.1% (499,500)
ABS (2008a)
2.4% (383,300)
ABS (2008a)
Identity theft
0.8% (124,000)
ABS (2008a)
18%
ASIC (2008a)
Do lots of research/homework
18%
ASIC (2008a)
Check out what you are investing in/who you are dealing with
17%
ASIC (2008a)
14%
ASIC (2008b)
11%
ASIC (2008b)
What people do
Scams:
Exposed to a scam
Victim of a scam
Victim of selected scams:
Fraud:
Victim of personal fraud
Victim of ID fraud:
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 93
Table A1.15: What Australians know and do: Information, advice and further education
Percent of
respondents
Research item
40%
ASIC (2008a)
24%
ASIC (2008a)
28%
FLF (2007a)
Investment advice
27%
FLF (2007a)
General advice
13%
FLF (2007a)
10%
FLF (2007a)
9%
FLF (2007a)
8%
FLF (2007a)
4%
FLF (2007a)
Financial adviser
82%
FLF (2007a)
Accountant/tax agent
81%
FLF (2007a)
Family
63%
FLF (2007a)
Bank
60%
FLF (2007a)
Friends
55%
FLF (2007a)
Seminars/educational institutions
52%
FLF (2007a)
Government website
49%
FLF (2007a)
Other website
48%
FLF (2007a)
Business/money-related magazines
48%
FLF (2007a)
Newspapers
47%
FLF (2007a)
Community services
42%
FLF (2007a)
Work
37%
FLF (2007a)
TV/radio
31%
FLF (2007a)
Centrelink
29%
FLF (2007a)
Preferred sources:
2005
2008
Investing
33%
39%
Superannuation
31%
25%
Budgeting
12%
14%
Taxation
7%
13%
9%
9%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
For more information about financial advisers please see Appendix 2, Statistics on access to advice.
Page 94
Table A1.15 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Information, advice and further education
What people know (cont.)
Topics for further education/information (cont.):
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
NA
9%
12%
7%
10%
7%
Business finance
5%
5%
Retirement planning
3%
3%
Insurance
3%
3%
5%
2%
2%
2%
Interest rates
NA
2%
Other responses
2%
5%
Cant say
16%
16%
Managing debt
What people do
Percent of
respondents
Research item
2005
2008
37%
42%
27%
26%
Finance websites
22%
27%
30%
27%
24%
23%
11%
12%
Seminars
17%
15%
62%
64%
Accountant
44%
45%
Tax specialist
23%
26%
Financial planner/adviser
34%
34%
Mortgage broker
13%
18%
Insurance broker
12%
12%
Stockbroker
10%
10%
Bank manager/employee
35%
39%
Family/friends
45%
47%
Financial counsellor
13%
13%
NA
11%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
For more information about financial advisers please see Appendix 2, Statistics on access to advice.
Page 95
Table A1.15 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Information, advice and further education
Percent of
respondents
Research item
29%
Citibank (2008)
18%
Citibank (2008)
Internet
13%
Citibank (2008)
8%
ASIC (2008a)
6%
ASIC (2008a)
3%
ASIC (2008a)
Accountant/tax agent
68%
FLF (2007a)
Financial adviser
54%
FLF (2007a)
Bank
57%
FLF (2007a)
131
2006
2009
33%
37%
Roy Morgan
Research (2006 [12
months to March
2006], 2010b [12
months to
December 2009])
investors:
18%
ASIC (2008a)
17%
ASIC (2008a)
15%
ASIC (2008a)
Daily newspapers
14%
ASIC (2008a)
11%
ASIC (2008a)
28%
ASIC (2008a)
21%
ASIC (2008a)
Daily newspapers
19%
ASIC (2008a)
Internet
14%
ASIC (2008a)
Financial magazines
9%
ASIC (2008a)
Internet
132
investors:
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
For more information about financial advisers please see Appendix 2, Statistics on access to advice.
131
Actual investors were people who were surveyed about the last investment decision they had actually made (as opposed
to hypothetical investors).
132
Hypothetical investors were people who were surveyed about a hypothetical investment scenario in which they had
inherited $100,000 (as opposed to actual investors).
Page 96
Table A1.15 (cont.): What Australians know and do: Information, advice and further education
Percent of
respondents
Research item
22%
FPA (2007)
29%
brandmanagement
(2008)
34%
brandmanagement
(2008)
investors:
2005
2008
NA
6%
NA
19%
Either of these
NA
23%
Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
For more information about financial advisers please see Appendix 2, Statistics on access to advice.
133
Mass affluent investors are defined as those with greater than $500,000 investable assets, excluding their home.
Page 97
Table A1.16: What Australians know and do: Consumer rights and responsibilities
Percent of
respondents
Research item
85%
FLF (2007a)
74%
FLF (2007a)
People who agreed they were very clear about their rights if they
had a problem with a financial institution
2005
2008
68%
67%
57%
52%
Who Australians would contact if they experienced difficulties with a financial product/ service:
Industry ombudsman/Ombudsman
36%
ANZ (2008)
26%
ANZ (2008)
14%
ANZ (2008)
13%
ANZ (2008)
Family or friends
13%
ANZ (2008)
Industry/professional association
5%
ANZ (2008)
Bank manager
5%
ANZ (2008)
1%
ANZ (2008)
Provider
1%
ANZ (2008)
Local MP
1%
ANZ (2008)
Unsure
17%
ANZ (2008)
Percent of
respondents
Research item
52%
ASIC (2008d)
29%
ASIC (2008d)
36%
ASIC (2008d)
Waste of time / dont have time / time involved / not worth the
time/effort/bother / too much hassle/trouble
16%
ASIC (2008d)
11%
ASIC (2008d)
11%
ASIC (2008d)
28%
ASIC (2008a)
What people do
Consumer complaints:
* Some of the information contained in these tables is sourced directly and some has been paraphrased for ease of reference.
Page 98
Table A1.17: Taxonomy of cognitive biases likely to be relevant for financial decision making
(de Meza et al for the FSA, 2008)
Adjustment
Naive statistics
Memory
Bias
Hindsight [3]
Procrastination [3]
Base-rate neglect,
stereotyping [1, 3, 4]
Conjunction [1, 4]
Correlation [1, 3, 4]
Disjunction [1, 4]
Anchoring [4]
Default [3, 5]
Disposition [2]
Endowment [2, 3, 5]
Page 99
Table A1.17 (cont.): Taxonomy of cognitive biases likely to be relevant for financial decision
making (de Meza et al for the FSA, 2008)
Confidence
Adjustment (cont.)
Bias
People exaggerate the degree to which their future tastes will resemble
their current tastes.
Regression to mean
Consider two variables X and Y which have the same distribution. If one
selects individuals whose average X score is higher than the mean of X
by k units, then the average of their Y scores will usually deviate from the
mean of Y by less than k units. Often people do not take this into account
in their judgements. For example, investments that have been
extraordinarily profitable yesterday are likely to regress back to their
mean today.
Omission [3]
People like things to stay the same. An alternative may be chosen only
because it was used before (habit).
Belief [3, 4, 5]
Difficulty evaluating conclusions that conflict with what one thinks one
knows about the world.
Completeness [3, 4, 5]
Confirmation, Myside
[3, 4, 5]
Unrealistic optimism,
desire, wishful thinking [3]
Overconfidence [3]
The ability to solve difficult or novel problems and the accuracy of our
own judgements is often overestimated.
Success
Often failure is associated with poor luck and success with the abilities of
the decision maker.
Page 100
Table A1.17 (cont.): Taxonomy of cognitive biases likely to be relevant for financial decision
making* (de Meza et al for the FSA, 2008)
Situation
Presentation
Bias
Framing [3, 4, 5]
Linear
Information order,
recency [4]
Scale [4]
The perceived variability of data can be affected by the scale of the data.
This might cause that small extra purchases are perceived as minor
expenditures when they follow larger purchases.
Attribution
Complexity [4, 5]
Extract only. For full table, see de Meza et al, 2008, pp. 94103.
Numbers in brackets [ ] indicate (most) likely relevance for different characteristics of financial capability: [1] being able to
manage money; [2] keeping track of finances; [3] planning ahead; [4] making informed decision about financial product; [5]
staying up-to-date about financial matters.
Source: Selected extract from de Meza et al for the FSA, 2008, pp. 94103.
Page 101
Table A1.18: What makes superannuation decisions difficult? (Clark-Murphy et al, 2002)
Information/knowledge
Uncertainty/risk
Classification
Description
Representative comment
Uncertainty
Employment
Unpredictability of future
economic conditions
Political/legal/taxation
Financial outcomes
Personal financial
situation
Life expectancy
Complexity
Acquiring information
Lack of knowledge/
understanding
Ambiguity
Importance of the
decision
Once-only decision
Deciding
Page 102
Description
Representative comment
Timing
Closeness to retirement.
Time constraint
Anxiety
Distrust
Avoidance
Superannuation is a pain
Emotion
Classification
Nature of the decision
/time (cont.)
Table A1.18 (cont.): What makes superannuation decisions difficult? (Clark-Murphy et al, 2002)
Page 103
Figure A1.1: Role of confidence in financial outcomes (Financial Literacy Foundation, 2007a)
Page 104
Table A1.19: Financial education and program delivery in the United States (Lyons et al, 2006)
Details of program
Percent*
93.4%
Credit/debt management
91.6%
88.6%
Consumer protection
63.5%
Home ownership
60.2%
53.6%
Risk management/insurance
46.4%
Taxation
37.1%
49.2%
Low-to-moderate income
60.6%
Moderate-to-upper income
18.8%
Young adults
46.3%
29.5%
Elderly
20.9%
Baby boomers
19.1%
Military
19.4%
Other
13.1%
55.4%
36.7%
Workshops/seminars
77.0%
Multi-session courses
69.7%
Printed materials
84.0%
66.4%
Internet delivery
35.2%
25.1%
Distance education
15.9%
Page 105
Page 106
Key points
It is difficult to pinpoint the percentage of Australians who use financial
planners because:
some survey respondents have a loose understanding of what a
financial planner/adviser is (e.g. some view knowledgeable
friends/colleagues as financial advisers)
the result varies depending on the survey at hand, including the way the
question is asked, when the survey was conducted and who is included
in the survey (e.g. the proportion of Australians who have ever used a
financial adviser is higher than those who currently use or have recently
used one).
However, a significant number of Australians appear to have never used a
financial planner, based on the information at hand (as many as 63% of
Australians aged 14 and over).
Overall, friends and family are the most common source of financial advice,
and a considerable number of Australians do not seek any advice.
The estimated percentage of Australians aged 14 and over who had ever
met an adviser (either from a bank, building society, credit union or
another institution) was 37%, including:
34% who had met an adviser from a bank, building society or credit
union, and
16% who had met an adviser from another financial institution.
The estimated percentage of Australians aged 14 and over who had met
an adviser in the last four weeks (either from a bank, building society,
credit union or another institution) was only 5%, including:
5% who had met an adviser from a bank, building society or credit union,
and
1% who had met an adviser from another financial institution.135
134
135
Base sample: 1085 Australians sourced via an online survey. Investment Trends (2010).
Base sample: 51,874 Australians aged 14 and over. Roy Morgan Research (2010b).
Page 107
22% of Australians aged 16 and over were at that time using the
services of a financial planner
half (49%) of those who had ever used a financial planner had not used
one in the last 12 months.141
136
Base sample: 1400 Australians (data weighted to represent the Australian population). FaHCSIA (2009a, 2009c).
Base sample: 1201 Australians aged 18 and over. Question: When was the last time you personally consulted a financial
planner or financial adviser? ISN (2009).
138
Estimates only. See source report for parameters. FPA (2008).
139
Base sample: 3500 Australians aged 18 and over. Question: And have you consulted any of the following people
regarding your finances? ANZ (2008).
140
Base sample: 400 Australians aged 18 and over. Citibank (2008).
141
Base sample: 1100 Australians aged 16 and over. FPA (2007).
137
Page 108
142
Page 109
Key points
It is difficult to pinpoint the percentage of investors who use financial
planners (for the same reasons as for Australians in general) and also
because there is no consistent definition for the category investor.
Notwithstanding the difficulties measuring the percentage of investors who
use financial planners, it appears that a considerable proportion of
investors, even relatively active or affluent investors, do not use a
financial planner:
At best, IFSA research found 62% of active investors would use a
planner if they were looking for information about managed investments,
including superannuation.
At worst, ASIC research found that only 15% of general investors
surveyed used a financial planner as a main source of information when
they made their last investment decision.
Again, friends and family were a common source of financial advice and a
considerable number of investors do not seek any advice.
33.6% mass affluent investors said they have ever used a professional
financial adviser.149
145
Base sample: 683 working Australians aged 18 to 64 living in capital cities. IFSA (2008a).
Base sample: 789 active investors. active investors included: a) those with investments $10,000 plus (not including
super) for managed funds, property, direct shares, cash management accounts, fixed-term deposits, savings accounts; and b)
those with investments less than $10,000 but with super $25,000 plus making additional contributions. IFSA (2008b).
147
Mass affluent investors are defined as those with greater than $500,000 investable assets, excluding their home.
brandmanagement (2008).
148
Question sample: 4271 mass affluent investors.
149
Question sample: 3058 mass affluent investors.
146
Page 110
Newspaper articles
TV Advertising
Print advertising
Professional financial adviser
Company provided material
Family/friends/work colleagues
Radio advertising
Investment magazines
Other finance professionals (e.g. accountant)
Magazines
Internet sites (excluding ASIC/FIDO websites)
TV shows
Radio shows
ACR/Fincorp
ASIC website
Active
General
FIDO website
0%
ACR/Fincorp=368, Active=280, General=494
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
150
Page 111
70%
100
88.1
Active
General
80
60
45.2
37.0
40
26.6
20
20.7
20.3 19.0
3.9
17.3
3.3
13.8
4.7
0
Advertising ranked as #1
Adviser ranked as #1
Associate ranked as #1
Other ranked as #1
ACR/Fincorp=362,Active=261,General=473
152
Other included a wide range of influences on investment decisions (e.g. bank manager, own research, internet and direct mail).
Base sample: 1217 investors. Hypothetical investors were told to imagine they had received $100,000 from an inheritance
and were asked to describe how they would invest this money. They were not permitted to pay off debts or give the money to
charity, family and friends. ASIC (2008b).
154
Hypothetical investors were those who had not made an investment in the past year (or more than five years in the case of
property).
153
Page 112
Overall, the most common sources of advice for these investors were a
financial institution/bank/building society/credit union (18%) and family and
friends (17%). Echoing the trends for Australians in general, 16% of
investors in this survey did not rely on any advice sources.
According to a survey of self-managed superannuation fund (SMSF) trustees
released by IFSA in 2006, 28% of SMSF trustees used a financial planner to
help with their funds.155
155
Page 113
ASICs research into UUD investors identified two key types of investors,
Type 1 and Type 2. Type 1 investors were less likely to seek advice about
their investment decision (from either formal or informal networks). Type 1
investors tended to be older than Type 2 investors: see Table A2.1 below.
Table A2.1:
Key
investment
characteristics
Key
demographic
characteristics
Mean age of 64
Mean age of 41
156
156
ACR investors were particularly likely to be female (61.8% of ACR investors were female).
Page 114
were more likely to own their own home outright (just over 50%,
compared to just under 10%).158
157
Page 115
90%
80%
70%
60%
53%
45%
50%
40%
37%
35%
30%
30%
24%
20%
10%
8%
5%
7%
5%
4%
3%
0%
Total
AB Quintile
C Quintile
D Quintile
E Quintile
FG Quintile
Socio-economic status
Estimated percentage of Australians aged 14+ who have ever met an adviser (either from a bank, building society,
credit union or other financial institution)
Estimated percentage of Australians aged 14+ who have met an adviser (either from a bank, building society, credit
union or other financial institution) in the last 4 weeks
older (34% of those aged 50 to 64 and 30% of those aged 65 and over,
compared to 7% of those aged 18 to 24). 161
Research commissioned by ISN in early 2009 indicated that people who had
at some point consulted a financial planner/adviser were more likely to be:
older (66% of people aged 50 and over had ever consulted a planner,
while 29% of those aged 18 to 24 had)
161
The preceding question in the survey was about the hypothetical use of various information sources (including financial
planners) in the event of financial stress or hardship, which may have influenced respondents conception of the term
financial management issues, potentially resulting in the lower reported usage of financial planners. Base sample: 1400
Australians (data weighted to represent the Australian population). FaHCSIA (2009a, 2009c).
162
Base sample: 1201 Australians aged 18 and over. Question: When was the last time you personally consulted a financial
planner or financial adviser? ISN (2009).
Page 116
According to the results of the Citibank Citi Fin-Q survey released in 2008,
women and over-40s were more likely to report using finance professionals
such as accountants, financial planners or bank officials when looking for
information on money matters than men and those aged under 40:
16 to 24 years (7%)
25 to 34 years (21%)
35 to 49 years (23%)
18 to 29 years (30%)
30 to 44 years (57%)
45 to 54 years (63%)
55 to 64 years (69%)
163
Citibank (2008).
FPA (2007).
165
FLF (2007a).
164
Page 117
Table A2.2:
Shares
Highinterest
savings
account
Term
deposits
Other direct
invest.
Total
Managed
invest.
SMSFs
Super
(extra
contrib.)
569
53
79
44
133
127
47
32
54
Financial
institution/
bank/building
society/ credit
union
18%
6%
6%
2%
4%
0%
53%
27%
13%
Friend/family
member
17%
15%
18%
24%
20%
15%
16%
8%
21%
Professional
financial adviser
15%
63%
34%
14%
9%
15%
6%
8%
15%
Daily newspapers
14%
6%
11%
4%
18%
33%
4%
9%
22%
Internet
11%
15%
10%
5%
12%
21%
10%
0%
12%
Financial
magazines
8%
11%
14%
0%
6%
18%
2%
9%
5%
Accountant
5%
6%
11%
7%
11%
5%
0%
0%
1%
5%
0%
0%
0%
23%
0%
2%
0%
0%
Stockbroker
3%
0%
6%
0%
0%
14%
0%
0%
9%
TV investment
programs
3%
0%
0%
0%
1%
7%
3%
9%
9%
Workplace
colleagues
3%
4%
0%
14%
0%
1%
3%
0%
2%
Managed fund,
prospectus
3%
4%
6%
3%
1%
2%
1%
2%
14%
16%
0%
14%
14%
27%
17%
10%
15%
16%
Actual investors, n=
Invest.
property
166
Page 118
167
ANZ (2005).
Page 119
Key points
Overall, the most common reasons for not using a planner are:
a perception that professional advice is not necessary (e.g. because the
decision is simple or people prefer to do it themselves)
lack of trust in planners, including not knowing how to find a good one
cost issues, including concerns about:
planner fees
168
Base sample: 12 focus group discussions and 28 one-on-one in-depth interviews conducted with low to middle income
participants. ASIC (2010b).
169
Base sample: 1218 Australians aged 40 and over sourced via an online survey. Investment Trends (2009).
170
Base sample: 12 focus group discussions and 28 one-on-one in-depth interviews conducted with low to middle income
participants. FaHCSIA (2009a, 2009b).
171
Base sample: 1400 Australians (data weighted to represent the Australian population). FaHCSIA (2009a, 2009c).
Page 120
In 2008, ING Australia released the results of research suggesting that 59%
of surveyed advisers believe a client needs at least $100,000 of investable
assets to receive financial advice (down from 63% in 2007).173
In 2008, The Australia Institute released the results of six focus groups
conducted with Australians in which people were asked if they had ever sought
financial advice and how helpful the advice was. The results showed that:
In the omnibus survey commissioned by the FPA in 2007, those who said
they do not use a financial planner said it was because:
172
Base sample: 1201 Australians aged 18 and over. Question: When was the last time you personally consulted a financial
planner or financial adviser? ISN (2009).
173
ING (2008).
174
Fear (2008).
175
FPA (2007).
Page 121
In the national investor survey Roy Morgan Research conducted for ASIC in
2007, investors who indicated that they did not/would not use a financial
adviser as a main source of information had a wide range of reasons for not
doing so. Table A2.3 below summarises these reasons by investment type
but overall the top reasons were that they:
Table A2.3:
Shares
Highinterest
savings
account
Term
deposits
Other
direct
invest.
Total
Managed
invest.
SMSFs
Super
(extra
contrib.)
875
75
67
43
171
364
45
45
65
17%
19%
19%
11%
23%
18%
8%
15%
17%
12%
6%
9%
9%
12%
11%
10%
18%
28%
11%
7%
12%
12%
14%
7%
21%
6%
6%
10%
4%
8%
9%
7%
8%
15%
13%
22%
8%
6%
4%
14%
2%
5%
15%
22%
2%
7%
5%
3%
4%
11%
8%
8%
4%
11%
6%
7%
1%
24%
5%
3%
8%
5%
3%
5%
9%
10%
0%
7%
6%
2%
5%
3%
5%
13%
12%
2%
8%
4%
1%
2%
4%
4%
3%
0%
7%
1%
2%
14%
3%
5%
3%
2%
3%
3%
3%
3%
3%
5%
3%
3%
7%
7%
0%
2%
3%
2%
2%
1%
97%
92%
99%
100%
99%
93%
100%
99%
99%
Actual/hypothetical investors, n=
Total
*
Invest.
property
Yellow shadow indicates percentages that are at least 2% higher than the total investor result (i.e. these investors appear
to be more likely than total investors to nominate this reason). Figures in this table are vertical percentages.
176
ASIC (2008b).
Page 122
177
178
some consumers had the impression that the planner was not interested
in them as a client, possibly because the planner was targeting a
different market segment (e.g. one volunteer of modest means was told
by all three planners they approached that there was nothing the planner
could offerthis was despite the consumer being very anxious to plan
for her retirement in five years time and having some spare cash flow)
some planners were too busy with other clients at the time they were
approached for advice
some planners gave verbal advice but would not do a formal plan.178
ASIC (2006).
ASIC & ACA (2003).
Page 123
Key terms
Term
ABS
ACFT
ACR
APSC
ARIA
ASFA
ASIC
ASX
ATSI
CBF
CFEB
CPA
DBP
DCP
FaHCSIA
FIDO
Fincorp
FINSIA
FLF
FLS
FPA
FSA
HECS
Page 124
Term
IFSA
IGFE
INFE
LFS
MAS
MCEECDYA
MCEETYA
MPHS
MWS
OECD
PDS
SEIFA
Single Source
database
SMSF
Understanding Money
UUD
WIRE
Page 125
References
Source code
Reference
ABS (2009)
Australian Bureau of Statistics, Retirement and retirement intentions, cat. no. 6238.0,
ABS, Canberra
ABS (2008a)
Australian Bureau of Statistics, Personal fraud, cat. no. 4528.0, ABS, Canberra
ABS (2008b)
Australian Bureau of Statistics, Retirement and retirement intentions, cat. no. 6238.0,
ABS, Canberra
ABS (2006)
Australian Bureau of Statistics, Adult literacy and life skills survey results, cat. no. 4228.0,
ABS, Canberra
ACFT (2008)
Agarwal et al
(2010)
ANZ (2009)
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited, Understanding the success of Saver
Plus, (Chant Link & Associates) ANZ Banking Group, Melbourne
ANZ (2008)
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited, ANZ survey of adult financial literacy
in Australia, (The Social Research Centre) ANZ Banking Group, Melbourne
ANZ (2005)
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited, ANZ survey of adult financial literacy
in Australia, (AC Nielsen) ANZ Banking Group, Melbourne
ANZ (2004a)
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited, Community development finance in
Australia: ANZ response to consultation, ANZ Banking Group, Melbourne
ANZ (2004b)
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited, A report on financial inclusion in
Australia, (Chant Link & Associates) ANZ Banking Group, Melbourne
ANZ (2003)
Australia and New Zealand Banking Group Limited, ANZ survey of adult financial literacy
in Australia, (Roy Morgan Research) ANZ Banking Group, Melbourne
APSC (2007a)
APSC (2007b)
Ashraf et al (2005)
Ahraf, N, Karlan, DS & Yin, W, Tying Odysseus to the mast: Evidence from a commitment
savings product in the Philippines, Economic Growth Center Discussion Paper no. 917,
Economic Growth Center, Yale University, New Haven
ASIC (2010a)
Page 126
Source code
Reference
ASIC (2010b)
Australian Securities and Investments Commission, How Australians plan for and make
decisions about their retirement (Susan Bell Research) Australian Securities and
Investments Commission, Sydney (unpublished)
ASIC (2008a)
ASIC (2008b)
Australian Securities and Investments Commission, ASIC investor research, Report 122,
(Roy Morgan Research) Australian Securities and Investments Commission, Sydney
(unpublished)
ASIC (2008c)
ASIC (2008d)
ASIC (2008e)
ASIC (2007a)
Australian Securities and Investments Commission, All we have is this house: Consumer
experiences with reverse mortgages, Report 109, Australian Securities and Investments
Commission, Sydney
ASIC (2007b)
ASIC (2006)
ASIC (2005)
Australian Securities and Investments Commission, Getting home insurance right, a report
on home building underinsurance, Report 54, Australian Securities and Investments
Commission, Sydney
ASIC (2004)
ASIC (2003)
ASIC (2002)
Australian Securities and Investments Commission, Hook, line & sinker: Who takes the
bait in cold calling scams? Report 15, Australian Securities and Investments Commission,
Sydney
Page 127
Source code
Reference
ASX (2009)
Atkinson (2008)
AXA (2007)
AXA, Avenue fourth quarter review: Learnings and recommendations, AXA, London
Barber, BM & Odean, T, Boys will be boys: Gender, overconfidence, and common stock
investment, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, vol. 116, no. 1, February, pp. 261292
brandmanagement
(2008)
Brobeck (1999)
CBA (2007)
CBA (2006)
Commonwealth Bank of Australia, New survey reveals baby boomers are planning to live,
not retire, Media Release, 17 May 2006
CBF (2006)
CBF (2004)
Commonwealth Bank Foundation, Improving financial literacy in Australia: Benefits for the
individual and the nation, CommFoundation, Sydney
Choi et al (2001)
Choi, JJ, Laibson, D, Madrian, BC & Metrick, A, Defined contribution pensions: Plan rules,
participant decisions, and the path of least resistance, Working Paper no. 8655, National
Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge (Mass.)
Citibank (2010)
Citibank, Results and insights from the Citi Fin-Q Survey, Citigroup, Sydney (media
release and private correspondence)
Citibank (2008)
Citibank, Results and insights from the Citi Fin-Q Survey, Citigroup, Sydney
Clark-Murphy &
Gerrans (2002)
Clark-Murphy et al
(2002)
Cole, S & Shastry, GK, If you are so smart, why arent you rich? The effects of education,
financial literacy and cognitive ability on financial market participation, Working Paper no.
09-071, Harvard Business School, Boston (Mass.)
Consumer Affairs
Victoria (2006)
Page 128
Source code
Reference
Criddle (2006)
Criddle E, Financial literacy: Goals and values, not just numbers, In Alliance, vol. 34, p. 4
Dawnay, E & Shah, H, Behavioural economics: seven principles for policy-makers, new
economics foundation, London
Decision
Technology (2007)
Decision Technology, Turning good intentions into actions: challenges for the provision of
financial advice, Decision Technology Group, University of Warwick, Coventry (online)
de Meza et al
(2008)
Elliott et al (2010)
Habschick M, Siedl B & Evers J, Survey of Financial Literacy Schemes in the EU27: VT
Markt/2006/26HFinal report, Financial Services Evers & Jung Research and Consulting,
Hamburg
FaHCSIA (2009a)
FaHCSIA (2009b)
FaHCSIA (2009c)
Fear (2008)
Fear, J, Choice overload: Australians coping with financial decisions, Discussion Paper
no. 99, The Australia Institute, Canberra
FINSIA (2009)
FINSIA (2006)
Financial Services Institute of Australasia, Saving the future: Can the under-40s afford to
grow old? Public Opinion Research Paper, Financial Services Institute of Australasia,
Sydney
FLF (2008)
FLF (2007a)
FLF (2007b)
FLF (2006)
Page 129
Source code
Reference
Flowers et al
(2001)
Fox, J & Bartholomae, S, Financial education and program evaluation, in JJ Xiao (ed.)
Handbook of Consumer Finance Research, Springer, New York, pp. 4768
Fox et al (2005)
Fox, J, Bartholomae, S & Lee, J, Building the case for financial education, Journal of
Consumer Affairs, vol. 39, pp. 195213
FPA (2008)
FPA (2007)
Gallery et al (2010)
Gallery, N, Gallery, G, Brown, K, Furneaux, C & Palm, C, Financial literacy and pension
investment decisions, Financial Accountability and Management (forthcoming)
Headey, B & Warren, D, Families, incomes and jobs, volume 3: a statistical report on
waves 1 to 5 of the HILDA Survey, Melbourne Institute of Applied Economics and Social
Research, The University of Melbourne
IFSA (2008a)
Investment and Financial Services Association, Newspoll Survey: Half of Australians are
concerned about financial markets but most holding nerve on super, (Newspoll Market
Research) Investment and Financial Services Association, Media Release, 30 October 2008
IFSA (2008b)
IFSA (2008c)
IFSA (2007)
IFSA (2006)
ING (2008)
ISN (2009)
Industry Super Network, Financial Planners, (Newspoll) Industry Super Network, Sydney
(unpublished)
Investment Trends
(2010)
Investment Trends, Advice and Limited Advice Report: December 2009, Investment
Trends, Sydney (unpublished)
Page 130
Source code
Reference
Investment Trends
(2009)
Iyengar et al (2003)
Iyengar, SS, Jiang, W & Huberman, G, How much choice is too much? Contributions to
401(k) retirement plans, Pension Research Council Working Paper, Pension Research
Council, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia
Lyons et al (2006)
Lyons, A, Palmer, L, Jayarantne, KSU & Scherpf, E, Are we making the grade? A national
overview of financial education and program evaluation, Journal of Consumer Affairs, vol.
40, no. 2, pp. 208235
Mandell (2006)
MCEECDYA
(2009)
Ministerial Council for Education, Early Childhood Development and Youth Affairs,
National Consumer and Financial Literacy Framework (Revised 2009), Ministerial Council
for Education, Early Childhood Development and Youth Affairs, Canberra
Mitchell, O & Utkus, S, Lessons from behavioral finance for retirement plan design,
Pension Research Council Working Paper, Pension Research Council, The Wharton
School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia
MWS (2010)
MWS (2009)
MWS (2006)
Mercer Wealth Solutions, Mercer 2006 financial literacy and retirement readiness study,
Mercer Human Resource Consulting, Melbourne
OConnell (2007)
OECD (2005)
ONeill et al (1999)
ONeill, B, Bristow, B & Brennan, P, Changing financial behavior: Implications for family
and consumer science professionals, Journal of Family and Consumer Sciences: From
Research to Practice, vol. 91, no. 4, pp. 4348
Productivity
Commission (2007)
Read (2007)
Read D, Behavioural economics: Implications for consumer decision making in the market
for advice, University of Durham (unpublished)
Roy Morgan
Research (2010a)
Roy Morgan Research, 2010 Single Source database, Roy Morgan Research, Melbourne
(12 months to March 2010) (unpublished)
Roy Morgan
Research (2010b)
Roy Morgan Research, 2010 Single Source database, Roy Morgan Research, Melbourne
(12 months to December 2009) (unpublished)
Page 131
Source code
Reference
Roy Morgan
Research (2006)
Roy Morgan Research, 2006 Single Source database, Roy Morgan Research, Melbourne
(12 months to March 2006) (unpublished)
Russell et al
(2008a)
Russell, R, Harlim, J & Brooks, R, Saver Plus 2008 follow-up survey results, (ANZ &
Brotherhood of St Laurence) RMIT University, Melbourne
Russell et al
(2008b)
Russell, R, Doan, MP, Harlim, J & Brooks, R, Saver Plus 2008interim report, (ANZ &
Brotherhood of St Laurence) RMIT University, Melbourne
Sandlant (2007)
Sandlant, R, Behavioural change and public policy implications, internal working paper,
Financial Literacy Foundation, Canberra (unpublished)
Saunders &
Sampson (1998)
Saunders, S & Sampson, J, Desirable calculators: Women, money and power, Literacy &
Numeracy Studies, vol. 8, no. 1, pp. 2336
Thaler, RH & Benartzi, S, Save more tomorrow: Using behavioral economics to increase
employee saving, Journal of Political Economy, vol. 112, no. 1, pt. 2, pp. 164187
Thaler, RH & Sunstein, CR, Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth and
happiness, Yale University Press, New Haven
Thoresen (2008)
Tversky &
Kahneman (1991)
Tversky &
Kahneman (1981)
Tversky, A & Kahneman D, The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice,
Science, new series, vol. 211, no. 4481, pp. 453458
Tversky &
Kahneman (1974)
Tversky, A & Kahneman, D, Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases, Science,
new series, vol. 185, no. 4157, pp. 11241131
Willis (2008)
Willis, L, Against financial literacy education, Iowa Law Review, vol. 94, pp. 197285
WIRE (2010)
WIRE Womens Information, Womens financial literacy research report, WIRE Womens
Information and Referral Exchange Inc, Melbourne
WIRE (2007)
WIRE Womens Information, Womens financial literacy research report, WIRE Womens
Information and Referral Exchange Inc, Melbourne
Worthington (2006)
Xiao, JJ & Wu, J, Encouraging behavior change in credit counseling: An application of the
transtheoretical model of change (TTM), TCAI Working Paper, Take Charge America
Institute for Consumer Financial Education and Research, University of Arizona, Tucson
Page 132