Kolam Art
Kolam Art
Kolam Art
Sunita Vatuk
City College of New York, CUNY, USA
svatuk@ccny.cuny.edu
Most visitors to Tamil Nadu are struck by the ubiquity of designs made of white quartz dust and/or rice powder
newly made each morning on the ground outside of homes. They are called kolams, and are put almost
exclusively by Hindu, and some Christian, women. Observant mathematicians and math educators are
immediately struck by links between kolams and various branches of mathematics that include (but are not
limited to) school mathematics or the practical mathematics of everyday life; kolams suggest introductory topics
in discrete math, number theory, abstract algebra, sequences, fractals, and computer science alongside those in
algebra and arithmetic. Early work connecting kolams with research in mathematics and computer science was
conducted by Siromoney, Siromoney, and Krithivasan (1974). In addition, the learning of kolams can be aided
by mathematical knowledge and techniques, suggesting that proficiency in kolams might be accompanied by an
affinity for mathematics.
In contrast with this outsider's view, there is no doubt that for most Tamils, including the vast majority of the
women who put them, kolams are not considered to be mathematical objects. This interview-based study
examines the relationship between the thinking of the women who put kolams and mathematical thinking. Due to
space constraints, a discussion of the cultural context of kolams is not included in this paper. The reader curious
about kolams outside of this paper's narrow focus can find more information elsewhere, for example in Layard
(1937), Nagarajan (1993), Dohmen (2004), Mall (2007), and Laine (2009).
82
Context
In the ethnomathematics and math education literatures, philosophical and practical questions have arisen both
about the view of mathematics as a European creation and about the relationship between research mathematics
as it is conducted in universities, the mathematics taught to undergraduates in technical majors, school
mathematics as it is taught in K-12, and the mathematics that exists outside of academic settings. Gerdes (2006)
and Eglash (1999) highlight the mathematical knowledge that can be gained through exploring traditional design
and the process of making patterns by hand. Researchers and educators such as Selin and DAmbrosio (2000),
Ascher (2004), Frankenstein and Powell (1997), and Joseph (2010) influenced views of the history of
mathematics and led to the wider use of materials from non-European cultures in math classrooms. While some
educators continue to defend the teaching of school mathematics in terms of providing valuable tools for
everyday life, others such as Lave (1988), Nunes, Carraher, and Schliemann (1993) and Dowling (1998) have
highlighted the disconnect between the math of everyday life and school mathematics. Some, such as Evans and
Tsatsaroni (2000) and Lockhart (2009), have also made the case that promoting math as a utilitarian subject hurts
the discipline of math. And many in the mathematics education community, such as Driscoll (2007) have been
promoting teaching mathematics as habits of mind rather than emphasising specific topics.
Methodology
A series of open-ended interviews with kolam experts is underway. Thus far, 49 women have been interviewed.
Eight are high-caste (Brahman), approximately two-thirds grew up in villages, 7 still live in villages, and they
have between zero years of formal schooling to post-graduate degrees. Most are either housewives or domestic
workers, a few are teachers, college students, office workers, and professors. They were all identified as experts
through kolam contests, neighbours, family members, employers, or others in the study. The interviews are
similar to clinical interviews developed as part of research in math education (Ginsburg, 1981), starting with a
simple request to teach the researcher some everyday kolams that the interviewee likes to put, followed up with
questions such as Are any of these kolams alike? Can you make another one like this one that is smaller or
bigger? or Can you finish that kolam in another way? The women were interviewed for between one and
three hours depending on their availability, level of interest and knowledge. The most skilled women could draw
(or put in powder) on the order of 50 different kolams from memory in a single hour. Follow-up interviews with
the most skilled participants are ongoing.
Where possible, the interviews were conducted either with a video camera, or using a pen that links an audio file
to pen-strokes. This allows for the sequence in which different parts of the kolam are drawn, and the facility and
speed with which they are drawn, to be part of the data. The interviews were transcribed in the original Tamil
and English and then translated into English. Notes about which kolam was being drawn at which points in the
interview were added to the transcripts, along with any observations about how it was drawn.
Data Analysis
The initial interviews are being coded to create more focused research questions. A set of structured follow-up
interviews based on this analysis are being piloted. The initial analysis is inspired in part by Lobato (2003),
making a distinction between an actor-oriented analysis and an observer-oriented analysis.
The first approach is an attempt to understand as clearly as possible how the creators think. In this analysis, the
focus is on language the women use, the manner in which they put the kolams, their discussions about kolams
with relatives and neighbours, and so on. This is not without pitfalls: the mere fact of a researcher paying such
close attention to something they are not used to thinking of as important has an impact. Some of the
interviewees say it is the first time they have thought about one or more of the questions.
The second approach is to analyse the kolams from the perspective of a university-trained mathematician. The
researcher has been learning to draw kolams from notebooks, videos and photographs, keeping track of when she
is or is not consciously using mathematics to learn and analyse them. Interviews with math teachers and other
mathematicians will eventually inform this part of the analysis.
Preliminary Observations
During the initial round of data collection and analysis some potentially interesting lines of inquiry have
surfaced. Four of these are discussed below in detail; and some others are briefly noted in the final section. For
the sake of brevity, the issues discussed here are limited to a type of kolam called kambi kolam. An array of dots
(pulli) is placed on the ground, and then one or more curves (kambi) are put around the dots according to fairly
83
strict, but unarticulated, rules. All but a handful of women claimed that these kolams provided the greatest
mental challenge, and mathematicians familiar with kolams find the richest possibilities in them.
84
about how to go onto the next size. At first glance the 4-dot version suggested the next in the sequence should
be a larger square, rather than having the stepping stone array of the other kolams in the sequence. Upon
reflection, the researcher decided that perhaps for the woman who put the 4-dot version, the smallest kolam was
not necessarily the generator of the larger versions; rather she appeared to be following an algorithm for going
down from 24 to 20 and so on down to the 4-dot version.
Since the women do not care about creating a rigorous classification scheme, as a mathematician might, their
classifications are not consistent across types of kolams. There are examples of sequences of kolams where the
smallest exemplar differs from the rest and examples of sequences in which they do not. In some models the
smallest may even have a different symmetry group from the rest of the sequence, even though the symmetry
group is the same for all larger elements of the sequence.
Mathematical Dispositions
Exploring the question of when larger kolams are the same as smaller kolams led to an extended interaction with
a group women in a village. The 9-dot version of the kolam below is fairly common; the 15-dot version much
less so, and to date no woman in the study has drawn one larger than 15. NT's attempt inspired the researcher to
make what she thought ought to be the next three in the sequence, with 21, 27, and 33 dots. To do this, she used a
kind of dual that shows the structure of the kolam and acts as a guide, like that traced in the right hand drawing
on the previous page. After KA tried to draw a 21-dot version that had 17 closed curves instead of 1, and three
other women in the village all tried, unsuccessfully, to draw a 21-dot version with only 1 curve, the researcher
showed her attempts and asked them if they were correct. The women discussed the different versions at
length and ultimately decided that the researcher's version was correct. The discussion was taken very seriously
by all three women, and the decision, once reached, was accepted by all.
Although this was an exceptional sequence of events, several features of the discussion were seen in other
interviews. In particular, the idea that some kolams are correct and others are not, while not universal, is
common. And the persistence with which they approach often difficult tasks is also common, with women often
insisting on fixing difficult kolams, despite sometimes impatient husbands and children waiting for dinner to be
cooked. Many of the women said that they preferred one-curve kolams to the (often easier) multi-curve kolams,
and preferred to put the one-curve kolams all at once rather than in pieces, because of the feeling of satisfaction
gained from succeeding in a challenging task.
85
86
particular interest in these types of kolams. Therefore, it is tempting to say that what she did (putting those
kolams) is a stronger statement than what she said (that she didnt think about them in this way).
Figure 3.
Continuing Research
The initial interviews also shed light on the differences between how experts and novices classify kolams,
awareness the women had about the unarticulated rules of kambi kolams, additional strategies for remembering
kolams, algebraic thinking involved creating sequences, problem-solving strategies used in creating new kolams,
and the disconnect that appears to exist between mathematical thinking displayed in kolam-making and that
learned or taught in school. These, along with some of the more speculative ideas discussed in this paper are the
focus of ongoing data collection, through structured interviews, observations of groups, and conversations with
experts about mathematicians' understanding of kolams. Interviews with math teachers, mathematicians and
math students, whether kolam experts or not, will add to an understanding of what it means to look at kolams
through mathematical eyes.
References
Ascher, M. (2004). Mathematics elsewhere: An exploration of ideas across cultures. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Dohmen, R. (2004). The home in the world: Women, threshold designs and performative relations in contemporary Tamil Nadu, South India. Cultural Geographies 11, 7-25.
Dowling, P. (1998). The sociology of mathematics education: Mathematical myths/pedagogical texts. London:
Falmer Press.
Driscoll, M.J. (2007). Fostering geometric thinking: A guide for teachers, Grades 5-10. Portsmouth, NH.: Heinemann.
Eglash, R. (1999). African fractals: Modern computing and indigenous design. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers
University Press.
Evans, J. & Tsatsaroni, A. (2000). Mathematics and its publics: Texts, contexts and users. Social Epistemology
14, 55-68.
Frankenstein, M. & Powell, A.B. (1997). Ethnomathematics: Challenging Eurocentrism in mathematics education. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Gerdes, P. (2001). Ethnomathematics as a new research field, illustrated by studies of mathematical ideas in
African history. In J.J. Saldana (Ed.) Science and Cultural Diversity. Filling a gap in the history of science,
11-36. Mexico: Cuadernos de Quipu.
Gerdes, P. (2006). Sona Geometry from Angola: Mathematics of an African tradition. Monza, Italy : Polimetrica
International Scientific Publisher.
Ginsburg, H.P. (1981, November). The clinical interview in psychological research on mathematical thinking:
Aims, rationales, techniques. For the Learning of Mathematics 1, 4-11.
87
Joseph, G.G. (2010). The crest of the peacock: Non-European roots of mathematics (Third Edition). Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Laine, A. (2009, Summer). Complementarity between art and anthropology. Experiences among kolam makers
in South India. Suomen Antropologi: Journal of the Finnish Anthropological Society 34.
Lobato, J. (2003, January/February). How design experiments can inform a rethinking of transfer and vice versa.
Educational Researcher, 32, 17-20.
Lockhart, R. (2009). A mathematician's lament: How school cheats us out of our most fascinating and imaginative art form. New York: Bellevue Literacy.
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Layard, J. (1937, June). Labyrinth ritual in south India: Threshold and tattoo designs. Folklore 48, 115-182.
Mall, A. (2007). Structure, innovation and agency in pattern construction: The kolam of southern India. In , E.
Hallam and T. Ingold (Eds.), Creativity and cultural improvisation, 55-78. Oxford and New York: Berg.
Nagarajan, V. R. (1993). Hosting the divine: The kolam in Tamil Nadu. In N. Fisher (Ed.), Mud, mirror and
thread: Folk traditions of rural India. Alberquerque, NM: Museum of New Mexico Press.
Nunes, T., Carraher, D. W., & Schliemann, A. D. (1993). Street mathematics and school mathematics (Learning
in doing: Social, cognitive and computational perspectives). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Selin, H. and D'Ambrosio, U. eds. (2000). Mathematics across cultures: The history of non-western mathematics, New York: Springer.
Siromoney, G., R. Siromoney and K. Krithivasan. (1974). Array grammars and kolam. Computer Graphics and
Image Processing 3, 63-82.