Frijters Et Al (2011) Neurocognitive Predictors of Dyslexia
Frijters Et Al (2011) Neurocognitive Predictors of Dyslexia
Frijters Et Al (2011) Neurocognitive Predictors of Dyslexia
LDX
Neurocognitive Predictors of
Reading Outcomes for Children
With Reading Disabilities
Abstract
This study reports on several specific neurocognitive process predictors of reading outcomes for a sample of 278 children
with reading disabilities. Three categories of response (i.e., poor, average, and good) were formed via growth curve
models of six reading outcomes. Two nested discriminant function analyses were conducted to evaluate the predictive
capability of the following models: (a) an intervention and phonological processing model that included intervention group,
phonological awareness, and rapid naming and (b) an additive cognitive neuropsychological model that included measures
of memory, visual processes, and cognitive or intellectual functioning. Over and above the substantial explanatory power
of the base model, the additive model improved classification of poor and good responders. Several of the cognitive
and neuropsychological variables predicted degree of reading outcomes, even after controlling for type of intervention,
phonological awareness, and rapid naming.
Keywords
treatment, dyslexia, response to intervention, reading, neuropsychological assessment
Two specific language processes have been consistently and
convincingly demonstrated to be a keyand some would
say a causalskill set underlying the successful development of reading skill. These processes are phonological
awareness (PA), or the manipulation of individual sounds in
the speech stream (Liberman & Shankweiler, 1985; Wagner
& Torgesen, 1987), and rapid automatized naming (RAN),
or the rapid naming of serial displays of visual symbols
such as letters, numbers, objects, and colors (Wolf & Bowers,
1999; Wolf, Bowers, & Biddle, 2000). A comprehensive
meta-analysis of correlational research linking these processes and reading achievement is provided by Swanson,
Trainin, Necoechea, and Hammill (2003).
Deficiencies in PA and naming speed have been demonstrated to be characteristic of individuals with reading disabilities (RD) and those who struggle to acquire basic
reading skills. Past research has shown that PA and RAN
are distinct constructs but related in their prediction of reading processes. Providing independent predictive power to
explaining reading skills, deficits in both PA and RAN prior
to being able to read have been related to Grade 2 and Grade
3 reading ability (Manis, Seidenberg, & Doi, 1999; Wimmer,
Mayringer, & Landerl, 1998). The two skills may also have
a different developmental course, with PA exerting more
influence on early, decoding-dependent tasks and RAN
exerting more influence on later, word identification and
Corresponding Author:
Jan C. Frijters, Brock University, Departments of Child and Youth
Studies and Psychology, 500 Glenridge Ave., St. Catharines,
Ontario, L2S 3A1, Canada
Email: jan.frijters@brocku.ca
151
Frijters et al.
children at risk for RD and reviewed studies in which the
characteristics of children unresponsive to intervention were
investigated. Nelson, Benner, and Gonzalez (2003) reviewed
many of the same studies, but in a quantitative meta-analytic
framework and with a broader focus on intervention efficacy. Across both reviews, PA and rapid naming were consistently associated with the degree of intervention response.
Al Otaiba and Fuchs reported that poor PA predicted nonresponse in 16 of the 21 studies that included this construct;
similarly, slow naming speed predicted nonresponse in five
of six studies that measured this construct. Nelson et al.
reported in their analyses that the mean effect sizes for the
prediction of intervention efficacy from PA (0.42) and rapid
naming (0.51) were equivalent. In addition, these two skills
yielded the strongest effect sizes, substantially higher than
effects for behavior, orthography, memory, and IQ.
Notably absent, or consistently associated with smaller
effect sizes in the studies covered by these reviews, are several specific neurocognitive processes. Of the 23 studies
reviewed by Al Otaiba and Fuchs (2002), 7 included memory measures and 15 included some measure of IQ. In both
cases, there were equivocal results for each construct, with
some studies reporting associations with nonresponse and
some studies reporting none. Within the studies reviewed
by Nelson et al. (2003), effect sizes for memory (M = 0.30)
and IQ (M = 0.26) were substantially smaller than those for
the phonologically based constructs, which consistently
demonstrated stronger evidence of association with reading. Recent studies have tested IQ as a moderator effect but
have not found evidence of differential growth or change
along these dimensions. For example, Lovett et al. (2008)
and Morris et al. (2010) compared growth curves for average versus low IQ participants and found parallel treatment
curves within intervention conditions. Fuchs and Young
(2006) have recently shown mixed and small effects for IQ
as a predictor of intervention responsiveness, especially for
younger children and for word identification outcomes.
There may be several reasons for these patterns of findings. The first is that such factors as IQ are not important to
remedial outcomethat current best practice multiple component interventions are largely complete instruction.
That is, individual differences in skills and knowledge are
compensated for through interventions designed to provide
explicit instruction, repeated practice, and structured attention to facilitating generalization of learning (Braden &
Shaw, 2009). However, were this the case, studies of sufficient power should show moderation effects for IQ, with
steeper growth rates or greater gains for children low on IQ
and related factors. Another possibility in the case of IQ
may be that the measure of IQ predominately analyzed in
studies to date has been the full-scale IQ measure, obscuring and even averaging out critical profile differences that
may predict responsiveness (Fiorello, Hale, & Snyder, 2006).
Finally, Fuchs and Young (2006) have demonstrated in their
review of 13 studies that the contribution of IQ to
Phonological Coding
The process of encoding or representing linguistic information for later analysis and synthesis is a cognitive skill that
underlies PA, graphemephoneme mapping, and the development of individual word identification. These processes
have been studied at several levels of resolution, including
the individual phoneme and the morpheme. There is strong
evidence that these processes underlie vocabulary development (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; Metsala, Stavrinos, &
Walley, 2009) and spoken language comprehension (Baddeley,
1986). Scarborough (1998) showed that verbal memory
measured in second grade substantially predicted reading
achievement in eighth grade, but only for normally develo
ping readers. Gathercole, Alloway, Willis, and Adams
(2005) reported that phonological memory at the phoneme
and word levels was a significant predictor of reading for
children with RD, though weaker in predictive power than
complex working memory tasks. One form of phonological
coding is nonword repetition, and its relationship with reading on theoretical and empirical levels has been systematically reviewed by Gathercole (2006). The key dynamic in
this relationship is that the ability to repeat nonwords is an
index of the overall quality of the phonological storage system, involved in vocabulary, word learning, and PA.
A recent meta-analysis synthesized the literature comparing children with and without RD on measures of working memory and short-term memory (Swanson, Zheng, &
Jerman, 2009). This review included 43 studies between
1963 and 2006 that involved verbal short-term memory comparisons of these groups of children. Across these studies,
phonological memory measured in tasks such as nonword
repetition was significantly impaired for children with RD
when compared to age-matched children (weighted effect
size = 0.39). The parallel effect size for word-level shortterm memory was 0.55. In the fully partial model that controlled for the influence of working memory and attention,
only phonological memory among measures of short-term
152
Visual Processes
Visual-motor integration (VMI) is the process of integrating visual perception with body movements. Although the
act of reading does not require motor output per se, there
is empirical evidence of a relationship between reading
skill and VMI. Across 161 studies, VMI was shown to have
consistently moderate relationships with reading processes,
with stronger relationships for word reading versus other
reading outcomes and for younger children versus older
children (Kavale, 1982; median r = .39). Younger children
were identified as preschool and older as intermediate,
with an average age of 7.88 years and an average grade
of 3.2 in this review. However, in another review covering
more recent studies, smaller relationships with reading
achiev ement were found (Scarborough, 1998; median
r = .28). This latter finding was confirmed in a recent
multivariate dominance analysis (Schatschneider, Fletcher,
Francis, Carlson, & Foorman, 2004) that yielded correlations ranging from .21 to .29 depending on the particular
reading outcome studied and whether VMI was assessed in
April or October of the kindergarten year. Notably, the
earlier studies reviewed in the meta-analyses focused on
cross-sectional relationships with reading skill, whereas the
Schatschneider et al. (2004) study specifically investigated
kindergarten predictors of Grade 1 and 2 reading outcomes.
OMalley, Francis, Foorman, Fletcher, and Swank (2002)
found that children without reading impairment in kindergarten and Grade 1 showed more robust growth from initial
assessment to Grade 2 in the development of VMI itself.
There has been some debate about the nature and contribution of visual sequential memory (VSM) to explaining
variability in reading skill (see Scheiman & Rouse, 2005).
In a longitudinal study of the kindergarten cognitive predictors of Grade 4 reading, Grogan (1995) found a moderate
contribution of VSM to reading skill (i.e., R2 = .05, after
accounting for age, auditory memory, and motor skills).
Kavales (1982) earlier meta-analysis showed substantial
associations between VSM and word reading (rs = .40 to
.49), with stronger relationships found for the younger children. Differences on tasks of VSM have been found when
the performances of normally developing and children with
RD (Watson & Willows, 1995) have been compared. Diff
erences in VSM were substantial when children with RD
were matched with younger successful readers, a readinglevel match that controlled for reading experience. There is
also evidence contextualizing these findings that children
with RD do not have visual memory problems per se but are
less likely to adopt a verbal labeling strategy on such tasks.
For example, children with RD can improve VSM with
training (Hicks, 1980). In addition, given the substantial
contribution of naming speed to reading skill, the rate of
visual stimuli presentation can have a marked impact on
whether relationships with reading skill are found (Huba,
Vellutino, & Scanlon, 1990). Thus, the deficiencies in VSM
that have been observed may be the result of verbal or rate
issues and not VSM per se.
Intelligence Quotient
Across several studies and meta-analyses, no systematic
differences in the reading achievement of children with RD
or LD that are attributable to IQ have been shown to exist
(Fletcher et al., 2002; Gustafson & Samuelsson, 1999;
Hoskyn & Swanson, 2000; Siegel, 1989, 1992; Stanovich &
Siegel, 1994; Stuebing et al., 2002). The vast majority of
previous studies have examined global intelligence for potential direct relationships to reading achievement. More recently,
perhaps because of reduced interest in IQ discrepancy versus
low achievement definitional debates (see Fletcher et al.,
1994; Scruggs & Mastropieri, 2003), more nuanced investigations into the role of intelligence have emerged. Tiu,
Thompson, and Lewis (2003) tested a structural model of
reading across normally developing and samples with RD
and found that performance IQ (PIQ) was related to reading
comprehension, but only for children with RD and only as
mediated by decoding skill. Consistent with these results,
Johnston and Morrison (2007) recently matched poor readers, reading age controls, and chronological age controls and
divided them into high (>101) and low (<100) IQ subsamples to form six comparison groups. Their results showed
that IQ moderated the relationship between reading outcome
and specific phonological deficits, such that high IQ poor
readers manifested more severe phonological reading
deficits.
153
Frijters et al.
Another way in which the IQreading relationship has
been obscured is that many studies have not considered the
well-defined factors that constitute global intelligence scores.
Vellutino, Scanlon, and Lyon (2000) reported correlations
between reading achievement and Verbal and PIQ factors at
several points from Grade 1 through Grade 4. Their results
indicated that verbal IQ (VIQ) was correlated with word
identification for normal readers from Grade 2 onward, but
for difficult to remediate readers the correlation that was
apparent in Grade 1 was nonsignificant by the winter of
Grade 2. In a recent confirmatory factor analysis of the
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenFourth Edition
(WISC-IV), Glutting, Watkins, Konold, and McDermott
(2006) found that reading achievement on the Wechsler
Individual Achievement Test was related to a g factor but
that the verbal comprehension index also had a direct relationship with reading skill. In an earlier confirmatory factor
analysis of the WISC-III, Oh, Glutting, Watkins, Youngstrom,
and McDermott (2004) found a similar pattern of relationships, with an additional negative relationship between the
perceptual organization factor and reading processes. Hale,
Fiorello, Kavanagh, Holdnack, and Aloe (2007) have noted
that using g alone, or together with subcomponent scores,
is problematic because of the inherent multicollinearity
between full-scale IQ and these components derived from
it. It is important to contextualize the WISC-IV studies
with reference to other studies that have examined the relationship of CattellHornCarroll factors with reading (e.g.,
Evans, Floyd, McGrew, & Leforgee (2001)). These studies
have demonstrated that a focus on g incurs a loss of profile
information that is contained within subcomponents (Hale
et al., 2007). Floyd, Keith, Taub, and McGrew (2007, for
example, found persistent indirect relationships between g
and decoding, whereas within a two stratum structural
model processing speed and crystallized intelligence (i.e.,
specifically listening ability within a four stratum model)
had direct relationships with reading (see also Evans et al.,
2001).
There exists enough research and knowledge about the
development of reading processes to suggest that short-term
memory, visual memory, and IQ are important factors. Little
research is available to suggest whether any of these factors
moderate degree of response to reading intervention among
struggling readers. The present study investigates the contribution of eight specific neurocognitive process predictors
to understanding degree of intervention response among
young children with RD. Each of these predictors has a
theoretical or, at minimum, an empirical association with
reading skill. After estimating intervention response via
multilevel human growth curve models, three categories of
response were formed. Through a discriminant function ana
lysis of these categories the following research questions
were considered:
Method
Participants
The current study reports on 278 participants originally
reported in Morris et al. (in press). Each child participated
in a multisite intervention study that compared four smallgroup interventions (described below) that provided 70
hours of intensive instruction. Children between 78 and 102
months whose first language was English were recruited
through referrals from classroom teachers who considered
them to have significant difficulty acquiring reading skills.
Referral was followed by administration of a screening battery consisting of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT;
Kaufman & Kaufman, 1990), the Woodcock Reading Mastery
TestsRevised (WRMT-R; Woodcock, 1987), the Wide
Range Achievement Test3rd Edition (WRAT-3; Wilkinson,
1993), and a demographic questionnaire completed by
parents. The screening battery was administered by trained
psychometrists at each of three sites (Atlanta, Georgia; Boston,
Massachusetts; Toronto, Ontario).
Two criteria were used to determine whether the child
had a RD. The low achievement criterion was met with a
K-BIT Composite standard score greater than or equal to 70
and a standard score of 85 or lower on at least one of three
reading indices as follows: (a) the average of the WRAT-3
Reading subtest, the WRMT-R Word Identification, Word
Attack, and Passage Comprehension subtests; (b) the
WRMT-R Basic Skills Cluster Score; (c) the WRMT-R
Total Short Scale Score. The abilityachievement regressioncorrected discrepancy criterion was met with a reading
index one standard error of the estimate below the regressionpredicted reading score. An assumption of an average .60
correlation between the reading indices described above
and the K-BIT Composite Index was used to calculate the
discrepancy scores. A child qualified for inclusion having
met either or both criteria.
Morris et al. (in press) described in more detail the number of children screened and the procedures for defining RD
154
SD
93.59
76.57
73.38
78.49
91.54
94.12
90.60
6.07
11.94
10.39
9.85
11.16
10.61
13.46
61.5
75.8
77.3
94.6
status and ensuring representative sampling of socioeconomic levels, IQ, and race. The sampling strategy
resulted in equal numbers and at least 8 children per cell,
with at least 64 per condition and no site contributing
more than 5 participants in any cell. The specific levels of the
three factors were as follows: socioeconomic level, derived
from demographic information provided by parents, as low
or average and above; IQ as low (7090) or average and
above (>90); race as reported by the participants parents,
either Black or Caucasian. Table 1 details the average reading ability, IQ, and age of the 278 children participating in
the present study. Males constituted 61.5% of the sample.
Also contained in Table 1 is the proportion below 1 SD ageexpected performance across the following individual
measures: word reading (WRMT-R Word Identification),
comprehension (WRMT-R Passage Comprehension), and
fluency (Word Reading Efficiency). After confirmation of
RD, the WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991) was administered to all
children along with the cognitive and neuropsychological
measures detailed below.
The study design involved random assignment of small
groups of struggling readers, and their intervention teachers,
to one of four remediation conditions. The factorial design
of the study developed four randomly assigned groups of
struggling readers, such that each group included equal
numbers of Caucasian and Black students, of students with
average and below average family socioeconomic situations, and of students with average and below average IQ.
Groups of four children from each sample were taught one
of four remedial programs (detailed below) by trained
teachers for 70 contact hours during the school year.
155
Frijters et al.
alternative treatment control condition, and PHAB/DI+CSS
provided a phonological-only reading comparison.
Outcome Measures
Six reading and reading-related outcome measures were
included in the present study. These ranged from one phonological skill outcome to several measures of single word
identification, including a speeded measure, and one measure of passage comprehension. The measures for the present analysis were chosen from a much larger set of outcomes
assessed with this sample; the present subset of outcome
measures was selected to include four standardized measures and provide a broad assessment of reading ability.
Outcomes were assessed before intervention began, after
35 hours of intervention, and again after 70 hours of intervention. An overview of each outcome, the construct it
measures, and basic reliability and validity information are
provided below, but see Morris et al. (in press) for more
details on all aspects of the measures, the interventions, and
the overall study design.
Research-based measures. Blending Words from the Com
prehensive Tests of Reading Related Phonological Process
ing (CTRRPP; Torgesen & Wagner, 1996 was the research
version of the test in current publicationthe Comprehen
sive Tests of Phonological Processing (CTOPP; Wagner,
Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). This subtest assessed the ability to combine individual orally presented phonemes into
real words. For the age range in the present study, the measures authors report the internal consistency reliability
(coefficient alpha) to be .92 for a sample of normally developing readers and .87 for a sample of 61 children with RD.
Testretest reliability for this age range was .88.
The Test of Transfer is a computer-administered, timed
word identification test consisting of 40 words that are systematic transformations of high-frequency spelling patterns
taught in the intervention programs (Lovett et al., 2000).
For example, the keyword bake is taught in the intervention,
and the Test of Transfer may contain fake, babe, bike, and
baker. These four words represent onset, rhyme, vowel, and
suffix-based transformations of the keyword. Words were
presented singly and displayed on a computer monitor for
6,000 milliseconds, and children responded orally, speaking into a microphone to trigger the next list word. Cirino
et al. (2002) report the testretest reliability of this measure to
be .94 for the present sample. Raw scores of both Blending
Words and the Test of Transfer were used in the present
analysis.
Standardized tests of reading processes. Three subtests from
the WRMT-R, Form G (Woodcock, 1987) were administered. The Word Identification, Word Attack, and Passage
Comprehension subtests represent respectively context-free
assessments of single-word reading, nonword decoding, and
reading comprehension assessed through a cloze procedure.
156
Par.
Par.
SE
WRMT-R Word
Att.
Par.
SE
Test of
Transfer
Par.
Initial status
Intercept 00 9.76** 0.27 448.62** 0.77 3.76**
Rate of change Intercept 10 2.19** 0.12 10.06** 0.40 4.64**
Variance
components
Level 1
Within
2 4.14** 0.35 69.24** 5.90 10.13**
person
Level 2
In initial
20 16.84** 1.75 105.53** 14.73 32.82**
status
WRMT-R Word
Id.
SE
Par.
SE
WRMT-R P.
Comp.
Par.
SE
TOWRE S.
Word
Par.
SE
0.39 402.84**
0.25 14.83**
1.54 439.92**
0.47
9.30**
1.02 12.25**
0.40 5.01**
0.57
0.19
0.88
3.37
4.18
6.06**
0.51
7.58
1.50
6.92**
0.88
.05
39.63**
42.32**
5.55
.19
49.14**
20.38**
.20
4.36
Note: Par. = Parameter; CTRRPP = Comprehensive Tests of Reading Related Phonological Processing; WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery TestRevised;
TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency.
*p < .05. **p < .001.
Analyses
The overall analytic plan consisted of three steps. First,
response to intervention was estimated via growth curve
models applied to the six reading outcomes. Second, three
categories of intervention response were formed for each
outcome using the individually estimated growth rates generated by the growth models. Third, a series of discriminant
function analyses (DFAs) was performed on these categories, using a fixed set of cognitive and neuropsychological
measures to predict group membership and evaluate the
Results
Estimating Relative Size of
Reading Outcomes
Individual growth curve models were formulated to define
each participants response to intervention. Across the 70 hours
of intervention, three repeated measurements were taken on
each outcome measure, providing sufficient resolution for a
growth model with a random linear slope and random intercept. Visual inspection of individual growth curves did not
provide evidence of curvilinear growth rates. Several quantifications of response to intervention were explored, inc
luding intercept only, intercept and slope together, and
slope alone. In the analysis to follow each led to similar
model results; thus, the slope-only model representing the
number of items gained per 70 hours of intervention was
used to quantify response to intervention. In the case of the
Test of Transfer and the CTRRPP Blending, raw scores were
used, and in the case of the WRMT-R subtests, Rasch-scaled
W scores were used as the scale of measurement. Mean
growth rate per outcome, tests of random slopes and intercepts, and residual within-participant variation are reported
in Table 2.
Three categories representing degree of response to
intervention on each outcome were formed from these data.
Because the current study was designed to investigate factors that contribute to suboptimal and excellent types of res
ponses to intervention, the top (n = 56) and bottom (n = 56)
157
Frijters et al.
Table 3. Gains Over Full Intervention for Three Response Categories
CTRRPP
Blending
(Raw Scores)
Poor
Average
Good
WRMT-R
Word Att. (W
Scores)
Test of
Transfer (Raw
Scores)
WRMT-R
Word Id. (W
Scores)
WRMT-R P.
Comp (W
Scores)
TOWRE S.
Word (Raw
Scores)
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
1.64
4.30
7.48
0.78
0.89
1.02
2.04
8.82
19.24
0.39
3.73
2.84
14.76
20.23
24.97
1.64
1.52
1.78
16.45
28.38
47.90
2.36
5.10
7.82
10.36
18.20
28.62
2.78
2.80
3.47
4.24
9.63
17.48
1.05
2.35
2.93
Note: Par. = Parameter; CTRRPP = Comprehensive Tests of Reading Related Phonological Processing; WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery Test -Revised;
TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Poor and good responders n = 56 per group; average responders n = 166.
quintiles were chosen to define these categories and maximize separation between them. The middle three quintiles
were combined to form a category of average responders
(n = 166). Table 3 details the response categories across the
six outcomes. In general, the poor responders gained some
reading skills across the six outcomes; in contrast, the good
responders made consistently 3 to 4 times the gains of the
poor responders. There was substantial overlap across outcomes on how participants were classified. Across the six
outcomes, significant associations, 2(4) ranging from 6.74
to 120.97; M = 40.03, SD = 34.83, were observed when any
two classifications were paired (evaluated at = .003 to
correct for multiple comparisons), with the exception of the
pairing of the Test of Transfer, 2(4) = 6.74, p = .15, with
CTRRPP Blending. Across all 15 combinations of classifications, the Cramers V average effect size was 0.25 (range =
0.11 to 0.47, SD = 0.11), indicating moderate overlap in the
response categories across the six outcomes.
158
Table 4. Intervention and Phonology Discriminant Function Models, Reporting on Only the First Discriminant Function
Reading Outcome
CTRRPP
Blending
WRMT-R
Word Att.
Test of
Transfer
WRMT-R
Word Id.
WRMT-R P.
Comp.
TOWRE S.
Word
13.94*
.60
.57
2.16*
.26
.07
4.70*
.34
.13
10.00*
.51
.36
11.54*
6.02*
0.48
20.10*
36.90*
8.15*
1.45
0.38
0.15
0.86
8.38*
3.59*
1.42
6.81*
4.78*
11.89*
2.53
1.41
21.51*
17.80*
.61
.25
.92
.29
.73
.47
.68
.28
.60
.66
.52
.50
.42
Note: CTRRPP = Comprehensive Tests of Reading Related Phonological Processing; WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery TestsRevised; TOWRE = Test
of Word Reading Efficiency; RAN = Rapid Automatized Naming. The three orthogonal contrasts were as follows: Contrast 1 = intervention vs. control;
Contrast 2 = multiple component vs. single component intervention; Contrast 3 = Phonological and Strategy Training vs. Retrieval, Automaticity,
Vocabulary Elaboration, Orthography.
*p < .01.
159
Frijters et al.
Table 5. Final Additive Discriminant Model Overall and Univariate Predictor Results
Reading Outcome
CTRRPP
Blending
WRMT-R
Word Att.
Test of
Transfer
WRMT-R
Word Id.
WRMT-R P.
Comp.
4.67*
.54
.41
6.57*
.65
.72
1.71*
.33
.13
7.27*
1.96
7.65*
1.07
2.42
5.82*
1.77
5.89*
6.84*
1.85
3.34
7.38*
10.33*
14.29*
3.91
14.02*
1.77
2.70
1.88
2.67
3.29
3.10
0.34
4.74
5.38
5.09
3.40
3.88
6.01
5.36
10.1*
4.90
.80
.33
.61
.77
.60
.34
.54
.57
3.06*
.43
.23
.32
.36
.40
.30
.33
.25
.61
.42
.34
.60
TOWRE S.
Word
4.93
.55
.44
2.39
4.75
3.52
4.27
8.85*
5.38
5.85*
7.68*
.59
.66
.35
.59
Note: CTRRPP = Comprehensive Tests of Reading Related Phonological Processing; WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery TestsRevised; TOWRE = Test
of Word Reading Efficiency; VMI = visual-motor integration; TVPS-VSM = Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills Visual Sequential Memory subtest; WISC-III =
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenThird Edition; FDI = Freedom from Distractibility Scaled Score; PO = Perceptual Organization Scaled Score;
PS = Processing Speed Scaled Score; VC = Verbal Comprehension Scaled Score. The three orthogonal contrasts were as follows: Contrast
1 = intervention vs. control; Contrast 2 = multiple component vs. single component intervention; Contrast 3 = Phonological and Strategy Training vs.
Retrieval, Automaticity, Vocabulary Elaboration, Orthography.
*p < .004.
160
CTRRPP
Blending
WRMT-R
Word Att.
Test of
Transfer
62
20
21.51*
75
20
31.84*
43
18
10.25*
WRMT-R
Word Id.
33
25
1.10 (ns)
WRMT-R
P. Comp.
TOWRE
S. Word
59
16
24.65*
39
13
13.00*
Poor
Average
Good
0.54
0.19
0.91
0.39
0.39
0.62
0.42
0.23
0.48
0.53
0.21
0.87
0.57
0.18
0.80
0.38
0.18
0.70
Poor
Average
Good
0.33
0.16
0.35
0.20
0.21
0.36
0.27
0.18
0.30
0.87
0.05
0.98
0.34
0.11
0.37
0.28
0.12
0.46
Poor
Average
Good
0.22
0.03
0.57
0.20
0.18
0.26
0.15
0.05
0.17
0.34
0.16
0.11
0.23
0.06
0.43
0.11
0.05
0.24
Note: CTRRPP = Comprehensive Tests of Reading Related Phonological Processing; WRMT-R = Woodcock Reading Mastery TestsRevised; TOWRE = Test of
Word Reading Efficiency. Poor and good responders n = 56 per group; average responders n = 166.
*p < .01.
Discussion
The present study was conducted to establish whether reliable predictors of reading processes are also predictive of
161
Frijters et al.
improve classification, incorporating the cognitive and
neuropsychological variables improved classification of
poor responders on average by 18.2%. Across a broad
range of reading skillsphonological blending, comprehension, and fluencythe cognitive and neuropsychological
predictors improved classification of good responders by
twice the amount for poor responders, an average of 41.3%.
On the three measures of single word reading or nonword
decoding, the improvement to classifying good responders
was equivalent to the improvement in classifying poor
responders.
These results are consistent with Vellutinos (2001) suggestion that cognitive processes may be correlated more
strongly with reading comprehension while less strongly correlated with more basic reading skills, such as nonword reading and word identification. On the surface, the present results
may be viewed as inconsistent with those of Johnston and
Morrison (2007), who found that high-IQ poor readers were
more phonologically impaired than low-IQ poor readers and
had relatively more difficulty decoding new words and nonwords. However, in the context of phonologically intense
instruction, higher IQ versus low IQ was associated with
greater responsethat is, benefit. This finding highlights the
need to consider carefully past correlational results linking
cognitive and neuropsychological factors with reading acquisition. Static predictors of reading ability or the reading process may not demonstrate relationships, whereas the same
predictors may explain reading outcomes when related to
response or rate of growth. Results from the present study
are consistent with this perspective, given that the degree of
response on the two phonologically based outcomes
Blending Words and WRMT-R Word Attackwas uniquely
predicted by the greatest number of neurocognitive process
predictors (see standardized discriminant coefficients reported
in Table 5). The addition of neuropsychological predictors
increased the classification accuracy for poor and good
responders on phonological blending skill by 22 and 57%
respectively, consistent with Torgesen and Davis (1996).
At the univariate level, the pattern of significant predictors was linked to the underlying reading process that each
outcome represents. These relationships were supported by
interpretation of the standardized discriminant coefficients,
which fully partial out the effect of intervention, PA, and
rapid naming skills. For example, growth in phonological
blending skills was explained by VMI and the FDI of the
WISC-III. Vellutino et al. (2000) have shown that there
may be a stronger relationship for VIQ than PIQ with reading outcomes, with the strongest relationship for younger
children in their sample. This is consistent with the present
results, with the WISC-III VC index a statistically significant and notable predictor of degree of response across all
outcomes. As indicated by the standardized discriminant
coefficients in Table 5, VMI, Nonword Repetition, and the
WISC-III indices of FDI, PS, and VC each played a role in
predicting degree of response for these outcomes. The present
sample was a younger sample, with participants ranging from
Limitations
Given the current state of research into the factors that predict responsiveness to intervention, the present analysis was
conducted in an exploratory manner, preserving individual
outcomes and the individual predictors. At the outcome
level, this decision is supported by the tests of association
across classifications derived from the six reading measures. Although the association from measure to measure
on who was a good, average, or poor responder was statistically significant and of moderate effect size, the association
was not perfect. Though some of the disagreement is attributable to the reliability of these outcome measures, all outcome measures demonstrated excellent psychometric properties
as previously reviewed. This finding suggests that each
reading measure had unique ability to separate degree of
response and suggests that studies on intervention responsiveness should take care to incorporate multiple dimensions of reading process.
At the predictor level, it is likely that these predictors are
components of a smaller number of dimensions, which may
or may not relate to intervention response. An additional
argument for retaining the individuality of the predictors is
that those included in the present study are a select group
162
Financial Disclosure/Funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research and/or authorship of this article:
The research reported here was supported by a National
Institutes of Child Health and Human Development Grant
(HD30970) to Georgia State University, Tufts University, and
The Hospital for Sick Children/University of Toronto.
163
Frijters et al.
References
Al Otaiba, S., & Fuchs, D. (2002). Characteristics of children who are unresponsive to early literacy intervention: A
review of the literature. Remedial and Special Education, 23,
300316.
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford
University Press.
Beery, K. E., & Buktenica, N. A. (1989). Developmental Test of
Visual-Motor Integration. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Berninger, V. W., Raskind, W., Richards, T., Abbott, R., & Stock, P.
(2008). A multidisciplinary approach to understanding developmental dyslexia within working-memory architecture:
Genotypes, phenotypes, brain, and instruction. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 33, 707744.
Block, C. C., Parris, S. R., Reed, K. L., Whiteley, C. S., &
Cleveland, M. D. (2009). Instructional approaches that signi
ficantly increase reading comprehension. Journal of Educa
tional Psychology, 101, 262281.
Braden, J. P., & Shaw, S. R. (2009). Intervention validity of cognitive assessment: Knowns, unknowables, and unknowns.
Assessment for Effective Intervention, 34, 106115.
Cirino, P. T., Rashid, F. L., Sevcik, R. A., Lovett, M. W.,
Frijters, J. C., Wolf, M., & Morris, R. (2002). Properties of
measures of beginning decoding and related abilities in poor
readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 526539.
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1974). Rapid automatized naming of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal
children. Cortex, 10, 186202.
Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Naming of objects by
dyslexic and other learning-disabled children. Brain & Lan
guage, 3, 115.
Engelmann, S., & Bruner, E. C. (1988). Reading Mastery I/II fast
cycle: Teachers guide. Chicago, IL: Science Research Associates.
Evans, J. J., Floyd, R. G., McGrew, K. S., & Leforgee, M. H. (2001).
The relations between measures of Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC)
cognitive abilities and reading achievement during childhood
and adolescence. School Psychology Review, 31(2), 246-262.
Fiorello, C. A., Hale, J. B., & Snyder, L. E. (2006). Cognitive
hypothesis testing and response to intervention for children with
reading problems. Psychology in the Schools, 43, 835853.
Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Barnes, M., Stuebing, K. K.,
Francis, D. J., Olson, R. K., . . . Shaywitz, B. A. (2002).
Classification of learning disabilities: An evidenced-based
evaluation. In R. Bradley, L. Danielson, & D. Hallahan (Eds.),
Identification of learning disabilities: Research to practice
(pp. 185239). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D. P., Katz, L.,
Liberman, I. Y., Stuebing, K. K., . . . Shaywitz, B. A. (1994).
Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educa
tional Psychology, 85, 118.
164
Mano, Q. R., & Osmon, D. C. (2008). Visuoperceptualorthographic reading abilities: A confirmatory factor analysis
study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
30, 421434.
Metsala, J. L., Stavrinos, D., & Walley, A. C. (2009). Childrens
spoken word recognition and contributions to phonological
awareness and nonword repetition: A 1-year follow-up. App
lied Psycholinguistics, 30, 101121.
Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., Wolf, M., Sevcik, R. A.,
Steinbach, K. A., Frijters, J. C., & Shapiro, M. (in press).
Multiple-component remediation for developmental reading
disabilities: IQ, SES, and race as factors on remedial outcome.
Journal of Learning Disabilities. Advance online publication.
doi: 10.1177/0022219409355472.
Nelson, J. R., Benner, G. J., & Gonzalez, J. (2003). Learner characteristics that influence the treatment effectiveness of early
literacy interventions: A meta-analytic review. Learning Dis
abilities Research & Practice, 18, 255267.
Oh, H.-J., Glutting, J. J., Watkins, M. W., Youngstrom, E. A., &
McDermott, P. A. (2004). Correct interpretation of latent versus observed abilities: Implications from structural equation
modeling applied to the WISC-III and WIAT linking sample.
Journal of Special Education, 38, 159173.
OMalley, K. J., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., &
Swank, P. R. (2002). Growth in precursor and reading-related
skills: Do low-achieving and IQ-discrepant readers develop
differently? Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 17,
1934.
Reiter, A., Tucha, O., & Lange, K. W. (2005). Executive functions
in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia: An International Journal
of Research and Practice, 11, 116131.
Richards, T. L., & Berninger, V. (2008). Abnormal fMRI connectivity in children with dyslexia during a phoneme task:
Before but not after treatment. Journal of Neurolinguistics,
21, 294304.
Richards, T. L., Berninger, V., Winn, W., Stock, P., Wagner, R.,
Muse, A., & Maravilla, K. (2007). Functional MRI activation
in children with and without dyslexia during pseudoword aural
repeat and visual decode: Before and after treatment. Neuro
psychology, 21, 732741.
Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessment of children (3rd ed.). San Diego,
CA: Jerome M. Sattler.
Scanlon, D. M., & Vellutino, F. R. (1997). A comparison of the
instructional backgrounds and cognitive profiles of poor, average, and good readers who were initially identified as at risk
for reading failure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 191215.
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Predicting the future achievement
of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of
phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ.
Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 115136.
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D.,
& Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading
skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educa
tional Psychology, 96, 265282.
165
Frijters et al.
Scheiman, M., & Rouse, M. W. (2005). Optometric management
of learning-related vision problems. New York, NY: C. V.
Mosby.
Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (2003). Issues in the identification of learning disabilities. In T. E. Scruggs & M. A. Mastropieri
(Eds.), Identification and assessment: Advances in learning and
behavior disabilities (pp. 136). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning
disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 469479.
Siegel, L. S. (1992). An evaluation of the discrepancy definition of
dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 618629.
Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Sarkari, S., Billingsley-Marshall, R.,
Denton, C. A., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2007a). Altering the
brain circuits for reading through intervention: A magnetic
source imaging study. Neuropsychology, 21, 485496.
Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Sarkari, S., Billingsley-Marshall, R.,
Denton, C. A., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2007b). Intensive ins
truction affects brain magnetic activity associated with oral
word reading in children with persistent reading disabilities.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 3748.
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance
profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based
test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Jour
nal of Educational Psychology, 86, 2453.
Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., LeDoux, J. M., Lyon, G. R.,
Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2002). Validity of IQdiscrepancy classifications of reading disabilities: A meta-analysis.
American Educational Research Journal, 39, 469518.
Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with
reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychol
ogy, 96, 249283.
Swanson, H. L., Trainin, G., Necoechea, D. M., & Hammill, D. D.
(2003). Rapid naming, phonological awareness, and reading:
A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. Review of Edu
cational Research, 73, 407444.
Swanson, H. L., Zheng, X., & Jerman, O. (2009). Working memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities: A selective
meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabili
ties, 42, 260287.
Tiu, R. D., Jr., Thompson, L. A., & Lewis, B. A. (2003). The role
of IQ in a component model of reading. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 36, 424436.
Torgesen, J. K. (2005). Recent discoveries on remedial interventions for children with dyslexia. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme
(Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 521537).
Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
Torgesen, J. K., Alexander, A. W., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte,
C. A., Voeller, K. K. S., & Conway, T. (2001). Intensive
remedial instruction for children with severe reading disabilities: Immediate and long-term outcomes from two
instructional approaches. Journal of Learning Disabilities,
34, 3358.
166
Wolf, M., Miller, L., & Donnelly, K. (2000). Retrieval, Automaticity, Vocabulary Elaboration, Orthography (RAVE-O): A
comprehensive, fluency-based reading intervention program.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 375386.
Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests
Revised (WRMT-R). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Service.
Children. Ms. Steinbach has overseen the implementation of several large multi-site grant funded studies. She supervises and trains
psychology staff, and coordinates and directs all assessment and
programming offered in our elementary-level research
classrooms.
Maryanne Wolf is the John DiBiaggio Professor of Citizenship
and Public Service, Director of the Center for Reading and
Language Research, and Professor in the Eliot-Pearson
Department of Child Development at Tufts University. Her recent
research interests include reading intervention, early prediction,
fluency and naming speed, cross-linguistic studies of reading, the
relationship between entrepreneurial talents and dyslexia, and the
uses of brain imaging in understanding dyslexia and treatment
changes.
Rose A. Sevcik is Professor of Psychology at Georgia State
University, Atlanta. Her research has focused on the language,
communication, and reading acquisition of children and youth
with developmental disabilities.
Robin D. Morris is the Vice President for Research and
Regents Professor of Psychology at Georgia State University.
He also holds a joint appointment in the Department of
Educational Psychology and Special Education in the College
of Education. His current research is focused on reading and
language development, reading disabilities and dyslexia, bilingual language and reading development, and neuroimaging of
the developing brain.