Howea & Leiserowitz 2013
Howea & Leiserowitz 2013
Howea & Leiserowitz 2013
Department of Environment and Society, Quinney College of Natural Resources, Utah State University, 5215 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322, USA
School of Forestry and Environmental Studies, Yale University, 195 Prospect Street, New Haven, CT 06405, USA
A R T I C L E I N F O
A B S T R A C T
Article history:
Received 2 April 2013
Received in revised form 19 September 2013
Accepted 25 September 2013
This paper explores the phenomenon of local climate perception and the extent to which public
perceptions match climate conditions as recorded in instrumental climate data. We further examine
whether perceptions of changes in local climates are inuenced by prior beliefs about global warming,
through the process of motivated reasoning. Using national survey data collected in the United States in
2011, we nd that subjective experiences of seasonal average temperature and precipitation during the
previous winter and summer were related to recorded conditions during each season. Beliefs about
global warming also had signicant effects on subjective experiences with above-normal temperatures,
particularly among those who believed that global warming is not happening. When asked about the
summer of 2010, those who believed that global warming is not happening were signicantly less likely
to report that they had experienced a warmer-than-normal summer, even when controlling for
demographics and local climate conditions. These results suggest that the subjective experience of local
climate change is dependent not only on external climate conditions, but also on individual beliefs, with
perceptions apparently biased by prior beliefs about global warming.
2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Keywords:
Climate change
Global warming
Risk perception
Experience
Motivated reasoning
Spatial analysis
Survey research
1. Introduction
Thus far, global warming has manifested gradually over many
decades and at spatial scales well beyond the direct perceptual
capabilities of any individual human being (e.g. the global or
continental scale). Local weather conditions, on the other hand, are
a readily available source of information that, when aggregated
over time, may enable people to detect long-term climate trends at
the local scale (Howe et al., 2013; Orlove et al., 2010). The
translation of personal experience of changes in local weather
conditions to perceptions of climate variability and change is an
important component of individual and community adaptation
(Adger et al., 2007). Research on local climate knowledge has found
that people are able to detect and respond to changes in climate
(Strauss and Orlove, 2003), but the characteristics of local
manifestations of climate change that are perceived have been
hypothesized to be dependent on a variety of individual and
contextual factors. These factors include the importance of specic
those who have strongly held beliefs about global warming (Myers
et al., 2012). Motivated reasoning is the tendency to interpret
information to t pre-existing beliefs (Kunda, 1990). In this paper,
we extend previous ndings by exploring the effect of motivated
reasoning on perceptions of local seasonal climate while controlling for actual local climate conditions. Drawing from a nationally
representative survey of the U.S. population, we rst characterize
the relationship between instrumental climate data and perceptions of local seasonal climate. We subsequently examine the
relationship between sets of beliefs about global warming and
perceptions of local climate conditions.
The relationship between personal experience and beliefs about
global warming is of considerable interest as changes in local
weather and climate conditions continue to be consistent with
scientic projections of global warming. For example, between
January 2000 and September 2009 maximum temperature records
were broken more than twice as frequently as minimum temperature records in the contiguous U.S. (Meehl et al., 2009), and extreme
events such as the 2011 Texas heat wave and drought have become
much more likely (Peterson et al., 2012). While direct attribution of
any single weather event to long-term processes like global
warming is not possible, the accumulation of weather events that
fall outside the range of previous experience does provide evidence
that the climate is changing, since local extreme events become
more likely as the world warms (Hansen et al., 2012; Meehl and
Tebaldi, 2004; Rahmstorf and Coumou, 2011). But can individuals,
drawing upon their personal experience, accurately detect the
extent to which recent conditions have changed relative to the past?
It is therefore important to understand how people subjectively
experience their local climate, and what factors inuence their
judgments about whether local climates are changing. Previous
broad-scale survey research suggests that changes in local climate
conditions can inuence public perceptions of local warming trends
(Howe et al., 2013). While there is some evidence that recent
experience with short-term ambient temperatures may inuence
global warming beliefs (Akerlof et al., 2013; Borick and Rabe, 2010;
Egan and Mullin, 2012; Goebbert et al., 2012; Hamilton and
Stampone, 2013; Joireman et al., 2010; Li et al., 2011; Risen and
Critcher, 2011), there has been little attention to the possibility that
subjective experiences of local climate may also be inuenced by
pre-existing beliefs and attitudes about global warming, which
could affect the ability to recognize local climate change.
2. Background
Research in communities around the world has documented
many cases of people using personal experience to detect changes
in their local climate; such changes include altered plant and
animal phenology, new distributions of species, shorter or longer
growing seasons, and the changing frequency of extreme weather
events (Deressa et al., 2011; Orlove et al., 2000; Roncoli et al., 2002;
Smit et al., 1997; Thomas et al., 2007; Tschakert et al., 2010;
Weatherhead et al., 2010; West et al., 2008). Such research has
almost exclusively been carried out at the local scale using case
studies of one or a small number of communities. The communities
in these studies tend to be rural, with most residents engaged in
livelihoods based on agriculture, ranching, or shing. Because their
livelihoods are dependent on the weather, residents have a strong
incentive to pay attention to the variability of local weather and
climate, and indeed they tend to notice changes. The changes that
people notice tend to be closely related to the aspects of the
weather that have the most direct effect on the livelihoods in
which they engage, to such an extent that residents of the same
community may identify different changes depending on their
occupation (Hartter et al., 2012; Meze-Hausken, 2004; Osbahr
et al., 2011).
1489
1490
3. Methods
This study is based on data from a nationally representative
survey of the United States in April and May 2011, conducted by
Knowledge Networks using a probability-based online panel.
Survey respondents were randomly sampled from a panel of over
50,000 members originally recruited using random-digit dialing
and address-based sampling. To ensure that the panel is nationally
representative, members without internet access receive a netbook and internet service. The survey was elded from April 22 to
May 11, 2011, with a total of 1,010 completed responses from
adults aged 18 or older. The survey had a recruitment rate of 15.7%
(contacted individuals who opt to join the panel), a prole rate of
60.9% (panel members who complete the prole survey), a
completion rate of 66.1% (invited panel members who completed
the survey), and a cumulative response rate of 6.3% (the product of
the recruitment, prole, and completion rates; see Callegaro and
DiSogra, 2008).
The geographic distribution of respondents roughly reected
that of the U.S. population. By state, the greatest number of
respondents lived in California (12%), followed by Texas (8%), New
York (8%), and Florida (7%). The global warming audience
segmentation was consistent with previous U.S. representative
surveys, with the majority of respondents classied as the
Concerned (27%) followed by the Cautious (25%), the Doubtful
(15%), the Alarmed (12%), the Disengaged (10%), and the
Dismissive (10%) (Leiserowitz et al., 2011).
Four survey items are the dependent variables in this study.
These items measure perceived departures from normal in average
temperature and precipitation during the preceding winter (winter
20102011) and the preceding summer (summer 2010). The
questions were presented as follows: (1) Has this winter in your
local area been warmer, colder, or no different than normal?, (2)
Has this winter in your local area brought more snow or rain, less
snow or rain, or was it no different than normal?, (3) Thinking
back to last summer, in your local area was it warmer, colder, or no
different than normal? and (4) Thinking back to last summer, in
your local area, did it bring more rain, less rain, or was it no
different than normal? The text of the survey did not specify a
normal period with respect to these items, since individual
1491
1492
Fig. 1. Mean temperature anomaly (panels A and B, top) and percent of normal precipitation (panels C & D, bottom) during climatological winter (DecemberFebruary) 2010
2011 and summer (JuneAugust) 2010. Data derived from monthly PRISM (2004) analysis.
case if an individuals understanding about what period constitutes the winter season does not correspond with the months of
climatological winter (DecemberFebruary). For example, astronomical winter in the northern hemisphere extends to the vernal
equinox on March 20, and snowfallassociated with winter
conditionsfrequently occurs after that date in the northern U.S.
Because of this disparity, we include temperature and precipitation
data for March and April 2011 in our analysis to control for the
effect of weather conditions immediately before and during the
survey.
4. Results and discussion
4.1. Descriptive survey results
Between 969 and 977 respondents in the contiguous U.S.
completed each of the four seasonal climate items (Table 1). The
majority of respondents reported that the previous winter had
been colder than normal (58%) and brought more rain and snow
than normal (59%). For the previous summer, the largest portion
(43%) of respondents reported that the season had been warmer
than normal, while nearly as many (42%) reported that the summer
had been no different from normal. A similar number (47%)
reported that precipitation during the previous summer had been
no different from normal.
Survey respondents experienced a wide range of local climate
conditions due to the spatial variability of climate in the U.S. On
average, local temperature anomalies among sampled respondents
differed more dramatically between the summer and winter
seasons than did local precipitation (Table 2). The average
respondent experienced above-normal temperatures and slightly
below normal precipitation in summer 2010, and below-normal
1493
Table 1
Seasonal climate perception items.
Temperature
Colder
Warmer
Winter 20102011a
Summer 2010b
603
136
61.8
14.0
259
420
26.6
43.2
113
417
11.6
42.9
Precipitation
Winter 20102011c
Summer 2010d
128
266
a
b
c
d
13.2
27.2
222
484
22.9
49.5
619
227
63.9
23.2
Has this winter in your local area been warmer, colder, or no different than normal?
Thinking back to last summer, in your local area was it warmer, colder, or no different than normal?
Has this winter in your local area brought more snow or rain, less snow or rain, or was it no different than normal?
Thinking back to last summer, in your local area, did it bring more rain, less rain, or was it no different than normal?
Table 2
Summary statistics for temperature anomaly and percent of normal precipitation at survey respondent coordinates. Data derived from PRISM (2004), based on 19712000
normals.
Mean
Median
1.2
0.9
0.1
1.4
1.1
0.3
1.0
0.9
1.0
2.5
3.3
3.3
3.0
2.1
3.3
85.5
16.9
62.7
92.7
94.2
106.0
93.0
88.7
109.0
40.0
39.4
48.5
0.6
11.8
0.6
216.9
342.1
273.4
40.5
33.2
57.3
Std. dev.
Minimum
Maximum
Percent above
19712000 mean
1494
Fig. 2. Relative risk surfaces of the probability of responses to each item, conditional on the overall spatial distribution of responses. Each map depicts the ratio of two bivariate
kernel density estimates, the rst for case locations (respondents who responded as labeled), and the second for control locations (all remaining respondents). Darker shading
indicates a higher probability of responding as labeled. Optimal kernel smoothing bandwidths were selected by cross-validation: winter temperature, 276 km; winter
precipitation, 220 km; summer temperature, 517 km; summer precipitation, 294 km. Light blue contour lines indicate signicantly elevated probabilities at p < .05 (upper
tailed). Dark blue contour lines indicate signicantly elevated probabilities at p < .01 (upper tailed). Signicance contours based on asymptotic p-value surfaces (Hazelton and
Davies, 2009; Kelsall and Diggle, 1995). Dots indicate approximate household locations of labeled respondents. (For interpretation of the references to color in gure legend,
the reader is referred to the web version of the article.)
1495
Fig. 3. Time series plots showing mean climate conditions among groups of respondents to the seasonal climate perceptions items. Values are three-month moving averages.
Panels A (winter temperature) and B (summer temperature) report mean temperature anomaly (8C) by response; panel C (winter precipitation) and D (summer precipitation)
report mean percent of normal precipitation by response. Shaded gray box indicates the season referenced by each item. Vertical dotted line indicates the date of the survey.
Data derived from PRISM (2004).
1496
Table 3
Logistic regression models predicting perceived positive departure from normal in seasonal temperature, including climate change belief segments and local temperature and
precipitation anomalies.
Dependent variable
Winter, warmer
Model
A1
Summer, warmer
A2
B1
0.46 (0.45)
Demographics
Gender (female)a
Age (3044)b
Age (4559)b
Age (60+)b
Education (high school)c
Education (some college)c
Education (bachelors or higher)c
Black, non-Hispanicd
Other races, non-Hispanicd
Hispanicd
Two or more races, non-Hispanicd
Conservativee
Liberale
0.28
0.11
0.27
1.06
0.97
0.67
0.72
0.81
0.60
0.35
0.74
0.05
0.05
(0.21)
(0.29)
(0.29)
(0.33)**
(0.34)**
(0.33)*
(0.35)*
(0.30)**
(0.50)
(0.32)
(0.59)
(0.27)
(0.27)
0.28
0.14
0.37
1.14
0.99
0.78
0.78
0.82
0.57
0.04
0.56
0.08
0.03
(0.21)
(0.30)
(0.29)
(0.34)***
(0.33)**
(0.33)*
(0.36)*
(0.31)**
(0.51)
(0.35)
(0.61)
(0.27)
(0.27)
0.19
0.10
0.15
0.06
0.02
0.14
0.73
0.20
0.40
0.57
0.48
0.13
0.08
(0.14)
(0.22)
(0.21)
(0.21)
(0.25)
(0.25)
(0.27)**
(0.23)
(0.39)
(0.24)*
(0.49)
(0.17)
(0.18)
0.20
0.14
0.19
0.03
0.09
0.01
0.51
0.12
0.53
0.44
0.48
0.11
0.04
(0.14)
(0.22)
(0.21)
(0.21)
(0.25)
(0.26)
(0.27)
(0.23)
(0.41)
(0.25)
(0.49)
(0.17)
(0.18)
0.68
0.77
0.20
0.90
0.46
(0.33)*
(0.34)*
(0.40)
(0.40)*
(0.43)
0.57
0.70
0.14
0.86
0.39
(0.33)
(0.34)*
(0.40)
(0.41)*
(0.44)
0.29
0.49
0.74
1.20
1.65
(0.23)
(0.24)*
(0.29)*
(0.27)***
(0.32)***
0.25
0.46
0.72
1.16
1.67
(0.23)
(0.23)
(0.29)*
(0.27)***
(0.32)***
0.18
0.89
0.24
0.12
(0.14)
(0.26)***
(0.12)*
(0.29)
Local climate
Mean temperature anomaly, MA 2011 (8C)
Percent normal precipitation, MA 2011 (/100)
Mean temperature anomaly, DJF 201011 (8C)
Percent normal precipitation, DJF 201011 (/100)
Mean temperature anomaly, JJA 2010 (8C)
Percent normal precipitation, JJA 2010 (/100)
n
Deviance (2*log-likelihood)
AIC
Nagelkerke pseudo r2
Spatial autocorrelation of residualsg
Change in residual spatial autocorrelation
0.65 (0.61)
B2
Intercept
0.58 (0.35)
0.58 (0.43)
0.17 (0.10)
0.20 (0.17)
0.19 (0.09)*
0.25 (0.19)
975
650
688
0.10
975
633
679
0.13
973
1253
1291
0.10
973
1236
1276
0.12
0.05
0.03
37.4%
0.05
0.03
48.6%
1497
Table 4
Logistic regression models predicting perceived positive departure from normal in seasonal precipitation, including climate change belief segments and local temperature
and precipitation anomalies.
Dependent variable
Model
C1
Intercept
Demographics
Gender (female)a
Age (3044)b
Age (4559)b
Age (60 + )b
Education (high school)c
Education (some college)c
Education (bachelors or higher)c
Black, non-Hispanicd
Other races, non-Hispanicd
Hispanicd
Two or more races, non-Hispanicd
Conservativee
Liberale
Climate change beliefs
Concernedf
Cautiousf
Disengagedf
Doubtfulf
Dismissivef
Local climate
Mean temperature anomaly, MA 2011 (8C)
Percent normal precipitation, MA 2011 (/100)
Mean temperature anomaly, DJF 201011 (8C)
Percent normal precipitation, DJF 201011 (/100)
Mean temperature anomaly, JJA 2010 (8C)
Percent normal precipitation, JJA 2010 (/100)
n
Deviance (2*log-likelihood)
AIC
Nagelkerke pseudo r2
Spatial autocorrelation of residualsg
Change in residual spatial autocorrelation
0.60 (0.35)
D1
D2
2.06 (0.49)***
0.73 (0.38)
1.69 (0.52)**
0.09
0.29
0.47
0.50
0.11
0.14
0.20
0.38
0.63
0.77
0.88
0.04
0.03
(0.14)
(0.22)
(0.21)*
(0.21)*
(0.25)
(0.26)
(0.27)
(0.23)
(0.39)
(0.23)***
(0.44)*
(0.17)
(0.19)
0.11
0.47
0.70
0.69
0.23
0.12
0.14
0.33
1.05
0.55
0.72
0.00
0.19
(0.15)
(0.24)*
(0.23)**
(0.22)**
(0.28)
(0.28)
(0.29)
(0.25)
(0.42)*
(0.28)*
(0.49)
(0.19)
(0.20)
0.25
0.10
0.20
0.13
0.33
0.37
0.70
0.18
0.33
0.13
0.33
0.05
0.13
(0.16)
(0.25)
(0.23)
(0.24)
(0.27)
(0.27)
(0.29)*
(0.26)
(0.42)
(0.25)
(0.57)
(0.19)
(0.20)
0.23
0.15
0.25
0.10
0.30
0.33
0.70
0.31
0.42
0.37
0.26
0.05
0.09
(0.16)
(0.25)
(0.24)
(0.24)
(0.28)
(0.28)
(0.30)*
(0.26)
(0.44)
(0.27)
(0.58)
(0.20)
(0.21)
0.16
0.26
0.28
0.52
0.67
(0.25)
(0.25)
(0.31)
(0.27)
(0.30)*
0.03
0.43
0.32
0.70
0.82
(0.27)
(0.27)
(0.33)
(0.29)*
(0.33)*
0.32
0.25
0.85
0.21
0.40
(0.25)
(0.26)
(0.36)*
(0.29)
(0.34)
0.38
0.29
0.80
0.26
0.40
(0.26)
(0.27)
(0.36)*
(0.30)
(0.35)
0.04
1.45
0.20
1.08
(0.11)
(0.20)***
(0.10)*
(0.26)***
0.21 (0.11)
0.34 (0.19)
0.09 (0.10)
0.69 (0.21)**
969
1216
1254
0.07
969
1070
1116
0.25
977
1039
1077
0.03
977
1008
1054
0.08
0.28
0.16
44.2%
0.05
0.02
50.8%
1498
Summer 2010
Probability of response
80%
60%
40%
20%
ture, for both the winter and summer seasons. Holding winter
201011 precipitation and temperature anomalies constant at the
sample median, respondents in the Dismissive segment were 16%
less likely than those in the Alarmed segment to perceive the
winter as having more snow or rain than normal, while those in the
Doubtful segment were 13% less likely. Summer 2010 brought
above-normal precipitation to only 33% of the sample. Holding
precipitation and temperature anomalies constant at the sample
median, respondents in the Disengaged segment were 15% less
likely than those in the Alarmed segment to perceive the summer
as having more rain than normal. The differences between the
remaining segments were not signicant.
5. Discussion
0%
Alarmed
Dismissive
The results provide support for our rst hypothesis that the
spatial distribution of seasonal climate perceptions would coincide
with the spatial distribution of temperature and precipitation
anomalies. Spatial analysis of responses indicated that the
distribution of seasonal climate perceptions was non-random,
and analysis of local clusters found broad agreement between the
patterns of seasonal climate perceptions and patterns of local
climate anomalies. For instance, the extreme heat wave of summer
2010 in the eastern U.S. was visible in a map of the probability that
respondents would report that the summer was warmer than
normal, and the droughts of winter 20102011 in the Southwest
were visible in a local concentration of respondents who reported
that the winter was drier than normal. Furthermore, seasonal
climate conditions were signicantly different among groups with
different perceptions of seasonal temperature and precipitation.
However, the perception of a single climatic variable during a
specic season was not solely dependent on that variable,
particularly in regard to the perception of seasonal precipitation,
which was consistently associated with both local temperature
and precipitation. This nding reects the physical relationship
between temperature and the type and amount of local
precipitation. For instance, a colder-than-normal winter may lead
to more persistent and visible accumulations of snow.
The results also support our second hypothesis that global
warming beliefs may bias recollections of seasonal climate through
motivated reasoning. If global warming beliefs do affect seasonal
climate perceptions, we would expect perceptions to be biased
toward a state of the world that is consistent with beliefs about
global warming, e.g. toward seasons that are warmer than normal
for those who believe that global is happening, and seasons that are
not warmer than normal for those who doubt that global warming
is happening. Our results provide some evidence that global
warming beliefs bias perceptions of seasonal temperature, but the
effect is strongest among those who do not believe global warming
is happening. Controlling for local conditions, those who believed
in the existence and threat of global warming (the Alarmed and the
Concerned) were indeed signicantly more likely to recall the
summer of 2010 as being warmer than normal than those who
believed that global warming is not happening or is not a threat
(the Dismissive and the Doubtful). Conversely, for the winter of
20102011, a season that was colder than normal for most of the
population, those who believed in the existence and threat of
global warming were not signicantly more likely to recall the
winter as being warmer than normal than those who most strongly
believed that global warming is not happening and is not a threat
(the Dismissive), and only somewhat more likely than those who
believe global warming may be happening but is not a threat (the
Doubtful). These results therefore suggest that motivated reasoning may bias individual recollections of seasonal climate, but the
effect is asymmetric and more pronounced among those who most
strongly believe that global warming is not happening. The results
1499
References
Adger, W.N., Agrawala, S., Mirza, M.M.Q., Conde, C., OBrien, K., Pulhin, J., Pulwarty,
R., Smit, B., Takahashi, K., 2007. Assessment of adaptation practices, options,
constraints and capacity. In: Parry, M.L., Canziani, O.F., Palutikof, J.P., van der
Linden, P.J., Hanson, C.E. (Eds.), Climate Change 2007: Impacts, Adaptation and
Vulnerability. Contributions of Working Group II to the Fourth Assessment
Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 717743.
Akerlof, K., Maibach, E.W., Fitzgerald, D., Cedeno, A.Y., Neuman, A., 2013. Do people
personally experience global warming, and if so how, and does it matter?
Global Environmental Change 21, 8191.
Baddeley, A., Turner, R., 2005. Spatstat: an R package for analyzing spatial point
patterns. Journal of Statistical Software 12, 142.
Borick, C.P., Rabe, B.G., 2010. A reason to believe: examining the factors that
determine individual views on global warming. Social Science Quarterly 91,
777800.
Callegaro, M., DiSogra, C., 2008. Computing response metrics for online panels.
Public Opinion Quarterly 72, 10081032.
Christensen, J.H., Hewitson, B., Busuioc, A., Chen, A., Gao, X., Held, I., Jones, R., Kolli,
a Rueda, V., Mearns, L., Menendez, C.G.,
R.K., Kwon, W.-T., Laprise, R., Magan
Raisanen, J., Rinke, A., Sarr, A., Whetton, P., 2007. Regional climate projections.
In: Solomon, S., Qin, D., Manning, M., Chen, Z., Marquis, M., Averyt, K.B., Tignor,
M., Miller, H.L. (Eds.), Climate Change 2007: The Physical Science Basis. Contribution of Working Group I to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,
UK.
Daly, C., Gibson, W.P., Taylor, G.H., Johnson, G.L., Pasteris, P., 2002. A knowledgebased approach to the statistical mapping of climate. Climate Research 22, 99
113.
Davies, T.M., Hazelton, M.L., Marshall, J.C., 2011. Sparr: analyzing spatial relative
risk using xed and adaptive kernel density estimation in R. Journal of Statistical Software 39, 114.
1500
Deressa, T.T., Hassan, R.M., Ringler, C., 2011. Perception of and adaptation to climate
change by farmers in the Nile Basin of Ethiopia. Journal of Agricultural Science
149, 2331.
Egan, P.J., Mullin, M., 2012. Turning personal experience into political attitudes: the
effect of local weather on Americans perceptions about global warming.
Journal of Politics 74, 796809.
Goebbert, K., Jenkins-Smith, H.C., Klockow, K., Nowlin, M.C., Silva, C.L., 2012.
Weather, climate and worldviews: the sources and consequences of public
perceptions of changes in local weather patterns. Weather, Climate, and Society
4, 132144.
Hamilton, L.C., Stampone, M.D., 2013. Blowin in the wind: short-term weather and
belief in anthropogenic climate change. Weather Climate, and Society 5, 112
119.
Hansen, J., Marx, S., Weber, E.U., 2004. The Role of Climate Perceptions, Expectations, and Forecasts in Farmer Decision Making (IRI Technical Report No. 04-01)
IRI Technical Report. International Research Institute for Climate Prediction,
Palisades, New York.
Hansen, J., Sato, M., Ruedy, R., 2012. Perception of climate change. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109, E2415
E2423.
Hartter, J., Stampone, M.D., Ryan, S.J., Kirner, K., Chapman, C.A., Goldman, A., 2012.
Patterns and perceptions of climate change in a biodiversity conservation
hotspot. PLoS ONE 7, e32408.
Hazelton, M.L., Davies, T.M., 2009. Inference based on kernel estimates of the
relative risk function in geographical epidemiology. Biometrical Journal 51,
98109.
Hitchings, R., 2011. Coping with the immediate experience of climate: regional
variations and indoor trajectories. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate
Change 2, 170184.
Howe, P.D., Markowitz, E.M., Lee, T.M., Ko, C.-Y., Leiserowitz, A., 2013. Global
perceptions of local temperature change. Nature Climate Change 3, 352356.
Hulme, M., Dessai, S., Lorenzoni, I., Nelson, D.R., 2009. Unstable climates: exploring
the statistical and social constructions of normal climate. Geoforum 40, 197
206.
Joireman, J., Barnes Truelove, H., Duell, B., 2010. Effect of outdoor temperature, heat
primes and anchoring on belief in global warming. Journal of Environmental
Psychology 30, 358367.
Kahan, D., 2013. Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reection. Judgment
and Decision Making 8, 407424.
Kahan, D.M., Jenkins-Smith, H., Braman, D., 2011. Cultural cognition of scientic
consensus. Journal of Risk Research 14, 147174.
Kelsall, J.E., Diggle, P.J., 1995. Non-parametric estimation of spatial variation in
relative risk. Statistics in Medicine 14, 23352342.
Koehler, J.J., 1996. The base rate fallacy reconsidered: descriptive, normative, and
methodological challenges. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19, 117.
Kunda, Z., 1990. The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 108, 480.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., 2009. Global Warmings Six Americas
2009: An Audience Segmentation Analysis. Yale University and George Mason
University. Yale Project on Climate Change Communication, New Haven, CT.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G., Howe, P., 2012. Extreme
Weather and Climate Change in the American Mind, Yale Project on Climate
Change Communication. Yale University and George Mason University, Yale
Project on Climate Change Communication, New Haven, CT.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G., Howe, P., 2013. Extreme
Weather and Climate Change in the American Mind: April 2013, Yale Project on
Climate Change Communication. Yale University and George Mason University,
Yale Project on Climate Change Communication, New Haven, CT.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Smith, N., 2011. Global Warmings Six
Americas in May 2011, Yale Project on Climate Change Communication. Yale
University and George Mason University, Yale Project on Climate Change
Communication, New Haven, CT.
Leiserowitz, A.A., 2005. American risk perceptions: is climate change dangerous?
Risk Analysis 25, 14331442.
Leiserowitz, A.A., 2006. Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: the
role of affect, imagery, and values. Climatic Change 77, 4572.
Li, Y., Johnson, E.J., Zaval, L., 2011. Local warming: daily variation in temperature
affects beliefs and concern about climate change. Psychological Science 22,
454459.
Maibach, E.W., Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., Mertz, C.K., 2011. Identifying likeminded audiences for global warming public engagement campaigns: an
audience segmentation analysis and tool development. PLoS ONE 6, e17571.
Marx, S.M., Weber, E.U., Orlove, B.S., Leiserowitz, A., Krantz, D.H., Roncoli, C.,
Phillips, J., 2007. Communication and mental processes: experiential and
analytic processing of uncertain climate information. Global Environmental
Change 17, 4758.
McCright, A.M., Dunlap, R.E., 2011. The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American publics views of global warming, 20012010. Sociological Quarterly 52, 155194.
Meehl, G.A., Stocker, T.F., Collins, W.D., Friedlingstein, P., Gaye, A.T., Gregory, J.M.,
Kitoh, A., Knutti, R., Murphy, J.M., Noda, A., Raper, S.C.B., Watterson, I.G., Weaver,
A.J., Zhao, Z.C., 2007. Global climate projections. In: Solomon, S., Qin, D.,
Manning, M., Chen, Z., Marquis, M., Averyt, K.B., Tignor, M., Miller, H.L.
(Eds.), Climate Change 2007: The Physical Science Basis. Contribution of Working Group I to the Fourth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on
Climate Change. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
Meehl, G.A., Tebaldi, C., 2004. More intense, more frequent, and longer lasting heat
waves in the 21st century. Science 305, 994997.
Meehl, G.A., Tebaldi, C., Walton, G., Easterling, D., McDaniel, L., 2009. Relative
increase of record high maximum temperatures compared to record low
minimum temperatures in the U.S. Geophysical Research Letters 36, 15.
Meyer, W.B., 2000. Americans and their Weather. Oxford University Press, New
York.
Meze-Hausken, E., 2004. Contrasting climate variability and meteorological
drought with perceived drought and climate change in northern Ethiopia.
Climate Research 27, 1931.
Myers, T.A., Maibach, E.W., Roser-Renouf, C., Akerlof, K., Leiserowitz, A.A., 2012. The
relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global
warming. Nature Climate Change 3, 343347.
NCDC, 2010. National Temperature and Precipitation Maps: June 2010August
2010. National Climatic Data Center, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration.
NCDC, 2011a. Spring 2011 U.S. Climate Extremes. National Climatic Data Center,
National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration.
NCDC, 2011b. National Temperature and Precipitation Maps: December 2010
February 2011. National Climatic Data Center, National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration.
Orlove, B., Roncoli, C., Kabugo, M., Majugu, A., 2010. Indigenous climate knowledge
in southern Uganda: the multiple components of a dynamic regional system.
Climatic Change 100, 243265.
Orlove, B.S., Chiang, J.C.H., Cane, M.A., 2000. Forecasting Andean rainfall and crop
o on Pleiades visibility. Nature 403, 6871.
yield from the inuence of El Nin
Osbahr, H., Dorward, P., Stern, R., Cooper, S., 2011. Supporting agricultural innovation in Uganda to respond to climate risk: linking climate change and variability
with farmer perceptions. Experimental Agriculture 47, 293316.
Peterson, T.C., Stott, P.A., Herring, S., 2012. Explaining extreme events of 2011 from a
climate perspective. Bulletin of the American Meteorological Society 93, 1041
1067.
PRISM, 2004. PRISM Climate Group, Oregon State University [WWW Document]..
http://prism.oregonstate.edu (accessed 05.01.12).
Rahmstorf, S., Coumou, D., 2011. Increase of extreme events in a warming world.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
108, 1790517909.
Rebetez, M., 1996. Public expectation as an element of human perception of climate
change. Climatic Change 32, 495509.
Risen, J.L., Critcher, C.R., 2011. Visceral t: while in a visceral state, associated states
of the world seem more likely. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100,
777793.
Roncoli, C., Ingram, K., Kirshen, P., 2002. Reading the rains: local knowledge
and rainfall forecasting in Burkina Faso. Society & Natural Resources 15,
409427.
Ruddell, D., Harlan, S.L., Grossman-Clarke, S., Chowell, G., 2012. Scales of perception: public awareness of regional and neighborhood climates. Climatic Change
111, 581607.
Sanchez-Cortes, M.S., Chavero, E.L., 2011. Indigenous perception of changes in
climate variability and its relationship with agriculture in a Zoque community
of Chiapas, Mexico. Climatic Change 107, 363389.
Smit, B., Blain, R., Keddie, P., 1997. Corn hybrid selection and climatic variability:
gambling with nature? Canadian Geographer/Le Geographe Canadien 41, 429
438.
Smith, N., Leiserowitz, A., 2012. The rise of global warming skepticism: exploring
affective image associations in the United States over time. Risk Analysis 32,
10211032.
Spence, A., Poortinga, W., Butler, C., Pidgeon, N.F., 2011. Perceptions of climate
change and willingness to save energy related to ood experience. Nature
Climate Change 1 1, 4649.
Strauss, S., Orlove, B.S., 2003. Weather, Climate, Culture. Berg, New York.
Thomas, D.S.G., Twyman, C., Osbahr, H., Hewitson, B., 2007. Adaptation to climate
change and variability: farmer responses to intra-seasonal precipitation trends
in South Africa. Climatic Change 83, 301322.
Tschakert, P., Sagoe, R., Ofori-Darko, G., Codjoe, S.N., 2010. Floods in the Sahel: an
analysis of anomalies, memory, and anticipatory learning. Climatic Change 103,
471502.
Weatherhead, E., Gearheard, S., Barry, R.G., 2010. Changes in weather persistence:
insight from Inuit knowledge. Global Environmental Change 20, 523528.
Weber, E.U., 1997. Perception and expectation of climate change: precondition for
economic and technological adaptation. In: Bazerman, M., Messick, D., Tenbrunsel, A., Wade-Benzoni, K. (Eds.), Psychological Perspectives to Environmental and Ethical Issues in Management. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, pp. 314
341.
Weber, E.U., 2010. What shapes perceptions of climate change? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Climate Change 1, 332342.
West, C.T., Roncoli, C., Ouattara, F., 2008. Local perceptions and regional climate
trends on the Central Plateau of Burkina Faso. Land Degradation & Development
19, 289304.
Whitmarsh, L., 2008. Are ood victims more concerned about climate change than
other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural
response. Journal of Risk Research 11, 351374.
Whitmarsh, L., 2009. Whats in a name? Commonalities and differences in public
understanding of climate change and global warming. Public Understanding of Science 18, 401420.