2008 - Autism and Asperger Syndrome - Frith PDF
2008 - Autism and Asperger Syndrome - Frith PDF
2008 - Autism and Asperger Syndrome - Frith PDF
Sufferers
from the syndrome are like autistic individuals in that they show the same kind of
impairments from early childhood; yet they are unlike them in being far more
verbally articulate and socially adapted. They can be highly intelligent and remini-
scent of eccentrics with their unusual interests, special skills and unworldliness.
In this volume several of the major experts in the field discuss the diagnostic
criteria of the syndrome, named after Hans Asperger who first described the
condition in the 1940s, and illustrate their views with case studies drawn from their
clinical practice. These clinical studies are complemented by personal accounts and
placed in a new theoretical framework. Significantly, Uta Frith provides the first
English translation of Asperger's paper: his work has long been neglected, but his
insights reflect a very modern awareness of the broad continuum of autistic
disorders. They also provide surprisingly practical suggestions on the education and
management of autistic children.
Current opinion on Asperger syndrome and its relationship to autism is fraught
with disagreement and hampered by ignorance. This book gives the first coherent
account of Asperger syndrome as a distinct variant of autism and will undoubtedly
arouse a good deal of debate.
Autism and Asperger syndrome
Hans Asperger. From the collection of Dr Maria Asperger-Felder
Autism and
Asperger syndrome
Edited by Uta Frith
MRC Cognitive Development Unit,
London
M CAMBRIDGE
f UNIVERSITY PRESS
CAMBRIDGE u n i v e r s i t y p r e s s
Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, Sao Paulo, Delhi
Cambridge University Press
The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge cb2 8RU, UK
Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title:www.cambridge.org/9780521386081
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library
ISBN 978-0-521-38608-1 paperback
Vll
viii Contents
IX
Acknowledgements
I would like to thank the contributors to this volume for the enthusiasm and
forbearance they have shown towards this project. I am particularly
indebted to Lorna Wing for allowing me to use and quote her own case
reports, and for tutoring me on the many varieties of the developmental
progress of autistic individuals. She was the instigator of the translation of
Asperger's 1944 paper, recognising its importance and topicality long
before anyone else. Christopher Gillberg and Digby Tantam who them-
selves have done much to increase awareness and knowledge of Asperger
Syndrome have also been instrumental in the origin of this book. To
Francesca Happe I am much indebted for stimulating discussions of the
theoretical issues and for her vital help in clarifying thoughts and words. My
writing has benefited immeasurably from Margaret Dewey's magic touch.
Dr Maria Asperger-Felder graciously provided materials pertaining to her
father as well as vivid biographical information. I am thankful also to Judith
Ayling for her careful reading of the translation and expert help in keeping
the text faithful to the original yet readable in English. Jacqueline Harvey's
meticulous copy editing has prevented innumerable infelicities and inconsis-
tencies. John Morton, who has been a constant provider of intellectual and
material support, is owed an enormous debt of gratitude. My deepest
thanks go to Chris Frith who read every line of innumerable drafts and
persuaded me that this volume preliminary as it is in the future history of
Asperger syndrome - must now meet the critical eye of its readers.
Chapter 2, '"Autistic psychopathy" in childhood' by Hans Asperger, was
first published as 'Die autistischen Psychopathen im Kindesalter' in 1944 in
the journal Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankenheiten, 117, 76136.
It was reprinted in Heilpadagogik (Springer-Verlag, Vienna, 1952, latest
edition 1968). We acknowledge the permission of Springer-Verlag, to print
this translation of Asperger's article.
Asperger and his syndrome
UTA FRITH
Both terms are widely used. There seems to me no virtue in being dogmatic about the letter
5, and for this reason Asperger syndrome and Asperger's syndrome appear in this volume.
Wing's (1981) paper was instrumental in kindling interest in Asperger syndrome; Tantam
(1988), Gillberg (1990) and Green (1990) provided annotations; diagnostic manuals
(World Health Organization, 1990) and textbooks (for example, Rutter and Hersov, 1985)
began to define the category, and systematic studies are now appearing (for example,
Schopler and Mesibov, in press).
2 Uta Frith
mark the phase of most troublesome difficulties for autistic children and
their families. At this stage autism produces a highly recognisable pattern of
behaviours, even though there is an enormous amount of individual
variation. All sorts of behaviour problems can worry parents at this time. In
almost all cases language learning is delayed, and in some cases language is
never acquired at all. Most young autistic children do not seem to
comprehend what others are saying to them or, indeed, what is going on
around them. Deafness is often suspected but ruled out. Social interaction is
severely limited. Imaginative pretend play is noticeably absent. The children
are often fixated on simple activities, and may inadvertently tyrannise their
family by intolerance of any change in routine. It has often been stated that
young autistic children behave as if other people did not exist. Again there
are degrees, but, taken with a grain of salt, this description sums up their
behaviour quite well.
Developmental changes which are rightly experienced as improvements
are often a marked feature between the ages of five and ten. From here the
paths begin to diverge to such an extent that the idea of subtypes cannot be
ignored.6 Progress will be very different for the autistic child who speaks
fluently and the child who has little or no language. Progress will also be
different for the child who shows evidence of ability in some areas and the
child who suffers from such pervasive brain damage that all his or her
intellectual abilities are impaired.7 Language and general intellectual ability
tend to go hand in hand, but there are exceptions. These exceptions are not
addressed in this volume, but deserve to be studied in their own right.
How should we tackle the question of subgroups? It may not be through a
distinctive pattern of signs and symptoms at a particular moment in time,
but rather through differences in developmental progress that we will be
able to discern variants of autism. In this volume we focus on those autistic
children who make good progress and are not crippled by multiple and
severe learning disabilities. How do they diverge from other autistic
children? Perhaps the main feature of children for whom we propose the
label Asperger syndrome is that they tend to speak fluently by the time they
are five, even if their language development was slow to begin with, and
even if their language is noticeably odd in its use for communication. Some
of these children show dramatic improvements despite having had severe
autistic symptoms as toddlers.8 As they grow older they often become quite
interested in other people and thus belie the stereotype of the aloof and
withdrawn autistic child. Nevertheless, they remain socially inept in their
6
Problems in diagnostic classification when the whole range of ability and course of
development are taken into account are discussed by Cohen, Paul and Volkmar (1987).
7
Rees and Taylor (1975), as well as Bartak and Rutter (1976), drew attention to differences
in developmental progress in autistic children with and without additional mental
retardation.
8
Rapid improvement in bright autistic children's social and communicative behaviour just
before the age of five was found in a questionnaire-based study by Shah (1988).
4 Uta Frith
very able intellectually and have coped well with their handicap will strike
one as strange. This strangeness may be perceived as anything from chilling
cold-bloodedness to endearingly old-fashioned pedantry. 13
Within this very brief and general outline of typical Asperger syndrome
individuals there is much variation. Some show extreme behaviour diffi-
culties, others are gentle and easy to manage. Some suffer from specific
learning disabilities and do badly at school, others do very well academi-
cally and have university degrees. Some may find a niche in society and lead
a reasonably contented life, but others become outcasts or remain misfits.
For all their strangeness, people with Asperger syndrome seldom find the
help and sympathy they deserve and need. As we shall see, theirs is a
devastating handicap.
So far, Asperger syndrome is the first plausible variant to crystallise from
the autism spectrum. 14 No doubt other variants will follow. In this volume
we address the question of how best to characterise the syndrome from our
present state of knowledge. A good starting point is to find out more about
Asperger and to see how his ideas differed from those of Kanner.
13
This brief generalised description roughly distils Asperger's clinical description of the
syndrome in its mature form. It also corresponds to descriptions of adults given by many
other authors (for example, Wing, 1989). It also fits in with descriptions of less
well-adapted cases as, for instance, in Tantam's (1986) study of a sample of adult
psychiatric patients, subsequently diagnosed as suffering from Asperger syndrome, whose
main characteristics were unusual interests, impaired non-verbal communication and
clumsiness. This study is discussed by Tantam in chapter 5.
14
Different forms of autism, seen as part of a spectrum of autistic disorders, have been
described by Wing and Attwood (1987).
15
A sympathetic appraisal of both men has been provided by Lutz (1981).
6 Uta Frith
joined the staff of this clinic, where he worked on his Habilitation, that is,
his second doctoral thesis. The topic of his thesis was what he called autistic
psychopathy and what we would call autism. He submitted his thesis in
1943 a n d it w a s published in 1944. It is this paper which appears in
translation in chapter 2.
By a remarkable coincidence, Asperger and Kanner independently
described exactly the same type of disturbed child to whom nobody had
paid much attention before and both used the label autistic.16 They were
pioneers in recognising that autism is a major developmental disorder and
not merely a rare and interesting childhood affliction. From the start
Asperger had an idea of what these children would be like as adults. He was
interested in the subtle, and possibly milder, manifestations of autism in
more able children. Nevertheless, he also emphasised that autism could be
seen throughout the whole range of ability and that it produced a
particularly striking picture when accompanied by mental retardation.
At the time of Asperger's and Kanner's pioneering studies a concern with
subgroups would have seemed remote. Both men were intent on one aim, to
convince colleagues that there was a previously unidentified entity, a highly
recognisable disorder, which was present from early childhood and per-
sisted for many years. It is only now, after autism has become almost a
household term, that refinement into subgroups begins to make sense. Just
where the category boundaries for such subgroups should be drawn is a
difficult question, and readjustments from time to time are to be expected.
The present volume is afirststep in this process.
I wish to express the hope that the translation of Asperger's paper will not
be used as a means for a false othodoxy. It contains some startling insights
which are still new to many. It also reveals some misconceptions that have
crept into secondary sources. Nevertheless, we cannot extract from this
paper a definitive view of Asperger syndrome, or of autism for that matter.
In the first description of a few cases of a puzzling clinical entity of unknown
aetiology it cannot be presumed that all the essential features and only the
essential features will at once be identified. After all, such a description
hinges on the happenstance of individual cases that come to the clinic. The
task of identifying the core symptoms of autism has taken many decades,
and a definitive answer will be reached only when we have full knowledge of
the biological origins and their effects on brain development. It will
undoubtedly be some years before Asperger syndrome is fully defined and
recognised. We shall now take a closer look at Asperger and his back-
ground, and then briefly compare Kanner's and Asperger's first thoughts on
autism.
16
This label, first used by Eugen Bleuler to describe the schizophrenic patient's loss of contact
with the world around him, was chosen presumably because detachment from the social
world strongly characterised the special children Kanner and Asperger were studying. An
English translation of Bleuler's textbook of psychiatry (1916) appeared in 1951.
Asperger and his syndrome 7
plight of neglected and deprived children following the First World War,
was involved in voluntary organisations that ran homes and day centres in
Vienna and provided help with forensic problems for children and adoles-
cents. His compassion and commitment to this work were no doubt an
inspiration to all who worked at the clinic.
Asperger frequently acknowledged his debt to the work of his predeces-
sors and colleagues at the university clinic. The special and novel feature of
the remediation practised at this clinic was its biological basis. This meant
that the design of any programme began with the identification of organi-
cally caused limitations or deficits of individual children. After the handicap
was identified, the children would be treated sympathetically. Education
and therapy were the same thing. It is fascinating to read of the development
of this work. To begin with, the remedial ward was like any other clinical
ward. As in the hospital's other wards, the children lay in neat little rows of
beds, and twice daily there was a ward-round. They were treated as sick
children who needed to be made well again. As the team's experience grew,
their ethos changed. Soon the children were out of bed during the day and
played and worked in a busy round of activities. Now the aim was to give
handicapped (rather than sick) children as positive an education as possible.
In 1926 the ward moved to the beautiful purpose-built Widerhofer Pavilion,
with its airy rooms, architect-designed furniture and artistic wall friezes.
The daily programme of play and lessons was led by a remarkable
woman, Sister Viktorine Zak. Asperger called her a genius. Her intuitive
diagnostic skills and therapeutic effects as a teacher were legendary. One of
Asperger and his syndrome 9
socially inept but often socially interested, who are articulate yet strangely
ineloquent, who are gauche and impractical, who are specialists in unusual
fields - these will always evoke Hans Asperger's name.
ZT
Bartak and Rutter (1976) compared progress in retarded and normally intelligent autistic
children, showing considerable differences between these groups. Newson, Dawson and
Everard (1982) conducted a questionnaire-based study in Britain on a sample of over ninety
very able people, most of them between sixteen and twenty-six years old. Discussions of
practical issues in the life-span development of autistic individuals of very different
capacities for adaptation can be found in Cohen, Donnellan and Paul (1987) and in the
series edited by Schopler and Mesibov (1983 onwards).
Z2
Szatmari et al. (1989), in a follow-up study of high-functioning autistic children, reported
some excellent and near-normal adaptation (four out of sixteen). On the other hand,
Rumsey, Rapoport and Sceery (1985) documented rather poor outcomes in able autistic
adults. Other cases of excellent adaptation have been described by Kanner, Rodriguez and
Ashenden (1972) and Brown (1978).
Asperger and his syndrome 13
z8
Rumsey, Rapoport and Sceery (1985) provide results which support this claim. However,
Szatmari, Bartolucci and Bremner (1989) found that stereotypic movements were observed
much less frequently in their Asperger group than in their high-functioning autistic sample.
zy
Rumsey, Andreasen and Rapoport (1986) highlighted the rinding that able autistic adults
who showed considerable similarities to schizophrenic patients with negative features, such
as poverty of speech, poverty of ideas and flattening of affect, were, nevertheless, much
more likely than schizophrenics to exhibit perseveration on a given topic and single-
mindedness in its pursuit.
30
Grandin (1978), for example, describes extremes of sensitivity to certain types of sensory
stimulation.
Asperger and his syndrome 15
other types of autistic individuals but, unlike the Asperger syndrome person,
they cannot tell us about their sensations. Unfortunately, we are far from a
clear understanding of the mechanisms by which human beings normally
interpret sensations and react to them.
These poorly understood features do not mean that Asperger syndrome is
impossible to identify. Asperger himself maintained that the persistent, if
sometimes subtle, impairments typical of the individuals he described are
very obvious once one has come to recognise them.
handicaps, and at the other end, where Asperger syndrome is situated, there
are mitigating factors which cushion the effect of the deficit. The problem is
to specify what such mitigating factors are. Here then is a chance for
speculating and producing testable ideas. To start with, could it be that one
such factor is the presence of a sociable disposition? Asperger syndrome
individuals seem to be distinguished from other autistic individuals by a
desire to communicate and be part of the social world.47 Their desire to
communicate is often demonstrated in their tendency to talk incessantly
about their pet interest. Their desire to be part of the social world is often
seen in their expressed frustration and sorrow at not finding friends or
spouses. Could it be that a sociable disposition drives the acquisition of very
detailed social learning, which can be applied to great effect in routine
situations, as well as in the SallyAnne test? One test of this hypothesis
would be to see whether meaningful subgroups of autistic children can be
identified at very young ages in terms of their basic sociability. Measures of
sociability would have to be independent of the ability to conceptualise
mental states.
Those Asperger-type individuals who have found effective ways of coping
in social and communicative interactions and still retain their autistic
oddness present a great challenge for theory and practice. In the next section
I shall tentatively explore the hypothesis that we are looking at compensa-
tory learning in the presence of a severe deficit rather than at a very mild
form of deficit. In line with this hypothesis I propose that well-adapted
Asperger syndrome individuals may have all the trappings of socially
adapted behaviour, may have learnt to solve belief attribution problems, but
yet may not have a normally functioning theory of mind. The hypothesis
allows us to describe behaviour as resembling the normal pattern but arising
from quite abnormally functioning processes.
47
Case studies s h o w that n o t all Asperger individuals are talkative and show a desire to be
sociable. Indeed, some are distinctly reticent and keep themselves to themselves (Wing,
1989). As a coping strategy this may be highly effective. Perhaps it is this type of person w h o
occasionally appears in family histories as a relative w h o w a s recognised as eccentric b u t
never needed specialist attention.
48
This title echoes that chosen by Kanner, Rodriguez and Ashenden (1972) in their follow-up
study.
22 Uta Frith
true nature. This led to the prince marrying someone else, and to her own
failure to gain a place in this world.
Superficial resemblance to normality is, as case histories in this volume
show, within the reach of at least some Asperger individuals. It may well be
that this capacity to achieve near-normal behaviour is the single most
distinctive feature of Asperger syndrome as opposed to other forms of
autism.
It is possible for the Asperger person to learn social routines so well that
he or she may strike others as merely eccentric. They would not consider
that there was anything wrong with them. Of course, such hard-won
adaptation is achieved only at a price. The Asperger person will have had to
learn with great effort what others absorb quite naturally. He or she will
have needed unstinting help and a high degree of motivation. Unfortunately,
achievements bought at high cost are often fragile, and he or she will have to
run where others stand still. The question arises whether such gains are
worth the high price. One has to acknowledge too that not all Asperger
syndrome individuals can achieve near-normal social integration for all
their strenuous efforts.
Just how high is the cost, and how much effort is being spent in keeping
up appearances? Often outsiders do not appreciate that there is a cost at all.
Parents must find it irksome to be told by someone who has seen their
Asperger son or daughter for a brief and pleasant interview that they are
fussing about nothing. If the family members bring up anecdotal examples
of difficulties, they will probably be told that these are normal problems that
could happen to anybody. For instance, they may mention the embarrassing
occasion of an autistic adult sitting in a crowded underground train and
readjusting his or her underwear. 'So what?' comes the well-meaning but
naive reply, 'Anybody in extreme discomfort might do this!' As for the
charmingly humorous example of the autistic man who always forgets to
take off his bicycle clips, the standard comment is 'Well, which bicyclist
hasn't!'
Many examples of Aspergerish behaviour - to do with being oblivious of
other people's reactions or with being over-concerned can be cited, but
there is always a retort handy that implies it is nothing out of the ordinary.
Even the more outlandish examples, such as the young man who came
down naked to a living room full of visitors asking where his pyjamas were,
can be put down to a healthy disregard for stuffy conventions. In terms of
behaviour the autistic person can be so well camouflaged that his or her
occasional slips are generously discounted. Is it truly generous to overlook
such problems? Or is it in fact a mistake not to acknowledge that there has
been a cover-up of much more serious problems?
Sometimes the incompatibility of interest groups the person with
Asperger syndrome, the family, the community and the specialists - has to
be kept in mind. Imagine a child with Asperger syndrome who is up for
Asperger and his syndrome 23
What would follow if the autistic individual saw himself or herself as just
like everyone else and were treated as such? When routines have been long
established and when things are going well, this attitude may be justified. It
can convey a well-earned feeling of triumph and success. But what if things
do not go well? And what if nobody, including the autistic person, is aware
of the deeper underlying problems? To what extent can Asperger people
themselves be aware of their difficulties? The uninformed employer might
request something that for a normal person would be a trifling effort
unwittingly demanding a major effort from the Asperger individual. Sudden
panic could result. For example, an autistic person who retains an extreme
dislike of breaks in routine may be asked to accept a change that seems
reasonable to the employer but which will throw him or her into despair.
Presumed normality does not make allowance for sudden gaps in the
carefully woven fabric of compensatory learning. A catastrophic reaction
like screaming can cost a job, then.
The person with Asperger syndrome may well ask, 'What is so good
about being normal anyway? So what if I am different?' The single-minded
pursuit of a special interest or an outstanding artistic or musical talent is
worthy of admiration and can lead to great social success. Those who care
for an autistic individual may be captivated by his or her beauty and
egocentric dependence. There is a fascination about eccentricity which is
missing in conformity. Much that is obnoxious can be forgiven. Many
people find autistic innocence appealing. 'Autistic is beautiful' could be a
24 Uta Frith
51
An example is given by Baron-Cohen (1988) who describes a case of a 21-year-old with
Asperger syndrome who often violently beat his 7i-year-old(!) girl-friend.
26 Uta Frith
Dear Dr Robertson,
Re: James Jones. Aged 16 years
Thank you for asking my opinion concerning James's diagnosis, which
presents an interesting problem. I saw his mother on 13 February 1987 and
she gave an account of his developmental history. I saw James himself on
15 February with two colleagues.
Infancy
James was the first and only child of his parents. Labour was prolonged
and delivery was by forceps, but his condition was said to be satisfactory at
birth. As a baby he was very placid and noticeably less demanding than
other babies of his mother's friends and relations. He was not cuddly and
did not positively reach out for attention and affection, although he smiled
when approached. He would lie in his pram and gurgle happily at the trees.
He was easily toilet-trained and slept well from birth onwards.
activities. He lacked curiosity and did not go through the usual childish
stage of asking questions about everything.
Behaviour at school
When James was three years old a nursery schoolteacher informed Mrs
Jones that he interfered with the other children, often knocking their toys
down, and was very restless. However, he was allowed to stay at the
nursery.
When he transferred to primary school, he was unco-operative, over-
active and disturbing to the class. He was slow at learning and was found
to be below average in intelligence. He was placed in a special school with a
structured, organised programme, and there he got along well. This seems
to have been the best placement of his childhood.
For his secondary education he was first placed in a school with a
permissive regime. His behaviour became extremely disturbed, worse than
anything before. Again, a move to a school with a more structured regime
produced improvement.
Motor skills
He can swim, horse-ride and cycle with a fair degree of skill. On the other
hand, he is hopeless at team games of any kind. He appears unable to
co-ordinate his actions with those of the rest of the team and ends by
aggravating everyone. He is quite likely to kick the ball into his own goal.
Current interests
At home he still likes his toy cars, which he continues to push around in
long lines.
28 Uta Frith
He has no interest in his own appearance and his mother has to choose
his clothes for him.
He also needs help with brushing his hair.
He likes watching television and has a remarkable memory for past
programmes. He also remembers routes to places, even those visited only
once. He memorises routes on the London Underground and likes looking
at maps. He seems to have no interests apart from these and spends much
time in his room when at home. He goes out alone for long rides on his
bicycle. His parents worry about him going out alone but do not want to
deprive him of one of the few things he enjoys.
Difficult behaviour
James has never been much trouble within his own home, where the main
concern has been his passivity and isolation from social interaction.
However, there has been a series of problems in other environments,
beginning with his disturbed behaviour on starting school. He tends to
make inappropriate comments to strangers in the street and will push past
other people or barge into them as if they did not exist. He sometimes
attempts to join in the street games of younger children and does odd
things such as throwing bricks or running away with their footballs or
cricket bats, to their bewilderment and fury. There have been several
incidents of his taking money, apparently to try to buy friends. He has also
been seriously assaulted by two of his age-peers, an incident which he
recounted with little emotion.
On examination
James is small for his age and has a naive, immature appearance and
manner. He was friendly and amenable and did not seem to be at all
disconcerted by the situation. He answered questions fully and elaborated
some of his answers. He was by no means monosyllabic in the interview.
He made good eye contact, used appropriate gestures and intonation when
speaking and was prepared to involve all three people present in the
conversation. He had none of the mannerisms, odd intonation and lack of
gesture typical of autism or Asperger syndrome.
James has an overall IQ of 67 on the WAIS with no marked discrepancies on
subtests. He was given tests of his ability to understand sequences of events in
picture stories and their consequences for the characters involved. These he
completed at a level a little above that predicted from his WAIS IQ.
Formulation
James is in the mildly retarded range of intellectual ability, but this on its
own does not account for his strange behaviour.
His developmental history, while showing elements of both conditions,
is not typical of Kanner's early childhood autism or of Asperger's
syndrome. His profile on the WAIS does not show the very marked
discrepancies found in these conditions in the classic form. In particular, he
has no special skill with block design, although he is quite good at object
assembly and is reported to be adept at jigsaw puzzles. He is able to
arrange pictures to tell a story which is unusual in typical autism and
Asperger syndrome.
He lacks some other common features of the above syndromes, that is,
the stereotypes, odd bodily movements, repetitive routines and
idiosyncratic language of typical autism, and the long-winded repetitive
speech on special topics found in Asperger's group. On the other hand, his
language development was delayed and he failed to develop imaginative
play. His mother says that he is capable of affection but his social
interaction skills and empathy with others are limited and, in practice,
disastrously inappropriate. His gross and fine motor development was
delayed, and he is inept when attempting to join in team activities,
although he can perform reasonably well in solitary physical activities. He
conspicuously lacks common sense which, Asperger emphasised, was a
cardinal feature of his syndrome. He appears to have little capacity to
describe his feelings and motivations. His pattern of interests is markedly
limited, and he has the unusual memory for routes, maps and television
programmes typical of conditions in the autistic continuum. His 'fantasies'
seem to be limited to scenes he has watched on television.
It can be argued that James has a developmental disorder affecting his
capacity to plan his own life and fit into the social world. Classification as
socially impaired and having a disorder within the autistic continuum
would seem to be appropriate. The assignment of a precise diagnosis for his
pattern of behaviour is of academic interest. From the practical point of
30 Uta Frith
view, it is important to realise that the prognosis is poor for young adults
with similar histories and behaviour patterns. Because of their lack of inner
resources of understanding or imagination, they are unable to benefit from
programmes of re-education that are based on psychotherapy, group
therapy or family therapy. They function best in a closed, structured,
organised but sympathetic environment, with staff who understand their
handicaps and a high staff-to-resident ratio. They need to be provided with
a range of activities but should not be under any pressure to achieve
beyond their level of ability. The fact that James did well at a school with
this type of regime is evidence of his needs.
Ideally, such an environment should be provided on a long-term basis.
Unfortunately, in most such cases, any improvement is specific to the
situation. Once the programme ceases, relapse to the former difficult
behaviour is only too likely to occur. People like James are, because of their
developmental impairment, unable to build an organised inner world of
ideas capable of regulating their own conduct. They need other people to
provide structure for them over the course of many years, and perhaps for
their whole lives.
Yours sincerely
X.Y.
A dash of autism
Hans Asperger deserves to be recognised as a pioneer and champion of all
Asperger children. His case studies of Fritz V. and Harro L. are enthralling
to read. These extraordinary boys have, unbeknown to themselves, contrib-
32 Uta Frith
References
SL
Seneca (c. 4BC - AD65), i n De tranquillitate animi, formulates the statement 'nullum
magnum ingenium sine mixtura dementias fuit', reflecting Aristotle's (384-322 BC) similar
beliefs about the relationship of melancholia and creativity.
Asperger and his syndrome 33
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G. & Bremner, R. (1989). Asperger's syndrome and autism:
comparison of early history and outcome. Developmental Medicine and Child
Neurology, 31, 709-20.
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremner, R., Bond, S. & Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up
study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 19, 213-25.
Szatmari, P., Tuff, L., Finlayson, A. J. & Bartolucci, G. (1990). Asperger's syndrome
and autism: neuro-cognitive aspects. Journal of the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry', 29, 1306.
Tantam, D. (1986). Eccentricity and autism. Unpublished Ph. D. thesis, University of
London.
Tantam, D. (1988a). Annotation: Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Child Psycho-
logy and Psychiatry', 29, 83640.
Tantam, D. (1988b). Lifelong eccentricity and social isolation: I. Psychiatric, social
and forensic aspects. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 77782.
Tantam, D. (1988c). Lifelong eccentricity and social isolation: II. Asperger's
syndrome or schizoid personality disorder? British Journal of Psychiatry, 153,
783-91.
Volkmar, F. R. (1987). Social development. In D. J. Cohen, A. M. Donnellan & R.
Paul (eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. New
York: Wiley.
Wing, L. (1981). Asperger's syndrome: a clinical account. Psychological Medicine,
11, 115-30.
Wing, L. (1989). Autistic adults. In C. Gillberg (ed.), Diagnosis and treatment of
autism. New York: Plenum Press.
Wing, L. & Attwood, A. (1987). Syndromes of autism and atypical development. In
D. J. Cohen, A. M. Donnellan & R. Paul (eds.), Handbook of autism and
pervasive developmental disorders. New York: Wiley.
Wing, L. & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and
associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification. Journal of
Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 9,11-29.
Wolff, S. & Barlow, A. (1979). Schizoid personality in childhood: a comparative
study of schizoid, autistic and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 20, 2946.
Wolff, S. & Chick, J. (1980). Schizoid personality in childhood: a controlled
follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 10, 85100.
World Health Organization (1990). International classification of diseases: tenth
revision. Chapter V. Mental and behavioural disorders (including disorders of
psychological development). Diagnostic criteria for research (May 1990 draft for
field trials). Geneva: WHO (unpublished).
Wurst, E. (1976). Autismus. Berne: Huber.
'Autistic psychopathy 5 in childhood
HANS ASPERGER
Translated and annotated by Uta Frith
37
38 Hans Asperger
* The Swiss psychiatrist Eugen Bleuler, who wrote an influential textbook of psychiatry,
coined not only the term autism but also the term schizophrenia. Bleuler differentiated
autism and autistic thinking. In English translations of his writings these are usually
translated as dereism and dereistic thinking, referring to a loss of contact with reality.
4
For Bleuler autistic thinking meant thought associations driven by affects. He applied this
concept not only to schizophrenic patients but also to normal people. 'Autistic thinking
turns the boy playing soldiers into a general, the girl playing with her doll into a happy
mother... it enables the dreamer to express his wishes and fears.' In 1919 Bleuler
published The autistic-undisciplined thinking in medicine and how to overcome it, where
he castigated then current medical practice. He gave examples of beliefs in medicine that are
wishful rather than truthful, analogical rather than logical, fantasy rather than reality. It is
clear from this bitter treatise that autistic thinking in Bleuler's sense has nothing to do with
autism as we know it. This is also recognised by Asperger, who in his later revision for his
textbook (1952), omits any reference to dereistic thinking.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 39
Fritz V.
We start with a highly unusual boy who shows a very severe impairment in
social integration. This boy was born in June 1933 and came for
observation to the Heilpadagogische Abteilung (Remedial Department) of
the University Paediatric Clinic in Vienna in the autumn of 1939.5 He was
referred by his school as he was considered to be 'uneducable' by the end of
his first day there.
Fritz was the first child of his parents. He had a brother two years
younger who was also somewhat difficult but not nearly as deviant as Fritz.
Birth was normal. Motor milestones were rather delayed. He learnt to walk
at fourteen months, and for a long time was extremely clumsy and unable
to do things for himself. He learnt the practical routines of daily life very
late and with great difficulty.6 This will be looked at in more detail later. In
contrast, he learnt to talk very early and spoke his first words at ten
months, well before he could walk. He quickly learnt to express himself in
sentences and soon talked 'like an adult'.7 Nothing was reported about
unusual childhood illnesses and there was no indication of any brain
disease.
From the earliest age Fritz never did what he was told. He did just what
he wanted to, or the opposite of what he was told. He was always restless
5
This famous clinic was founded in 1918 by Erwin Lazar and pioneered a combination of
6
special education and paediatrics.
Practical routines include self-help skills such as washing, dressing and, generally, keeping
clothes and body clean, and probably also some typical social skills, such as eating properly
at table, and sitting still and paying attention at school. Toilet training is never mentioned
7
while it looms large as a problem in Kanner's cases.
Donald, Kanner's first case, also appears to have had rather early and unusual development
of speech. By the age of two, he was said to be able to name large numbers of pictures and to
recite poetry and prose. Asperger's descriptive phrase 'talking like an adult' suggests
oddness over and above precocity.
4<D Hans Asperger
and fidgety, and tended to grab everything within reach. Prohibitions did
not deter him. Since he had a pronounced destructive urge, anything that
got into his hands was soon torn or broken.8
He was never able to become integrated into a group of playing children.
He never got on with other children and, in fact, was not interested in
them. They only 'wound him up'. He quickly became aggressive and lashed
out with anything he could get hold of (once with a hammer), regardless of
the danger to others. For this he was thrown out of kindergarten after only
a few days. Similarly, because of his totally uninhibited behaviour, his
schooling failed on the first day. He had attacked other children, walked
nonchalantly about in class and tried to demolish the coat-racks.
He had no real love for anybody but occasionally had fits of affection.
Then he would embrace various people, seemingly quite unmotivated. The
effect, however, was not at all pleasant. This behaviour never felt like the
expression of genuine affection, instead, it appeared to be as abrupt as a fit.
One could not help thinking that Fritz might never be able to love anyone
and would never do something solely to please somebody else. He did not
care if people were sad or upset about him. He appeared almost to enjoy
people being angry with him while they tried to teach him, as if this were a
pleasurable sensation which he tried to provoke by negativism and
disobedience.9
Fritz did not know the meaning of respect and was utterly indifferent to
the authority of adults. He lacked distance and talked without shyness even
to strangers. Although he acquired language very early, it was impossible
to teach him the polite form of address ('Sie'). He called everybody 'Du'.
Another strange phenomenon in this boy was the occurrence of certain
stereotypic movements and habits. 10
Family history
The mother stemmed from the family of one of the greatest Austrian poets.
Her side of the family were mostly intellectuals and all were, according to
her, in the mad-genius mould. Several wrote poetry 'quite beautifully'.
A sister of the maternal grandfather, 'a brilliant pedagogue', lived as an
eccentric recluse. The maternal grandfather and several of his relatives had
been expelled from state schools and had to attend private school. Fritz
strongly resembled this grandfather. He too was said to have been an
exceptionally difficult child and now rather resembled the caricature of a
While conduct problems are highly prominent symptoms in Asperger's cases, they are not in
Kanner's sample although the problems mentioned there do include aggressive and
destructive behaviour. This difference can perhaps be explained by the more child-centred
attitudes prevalent in the United States at the time, while in Europe the instilling of respect
and discipline had remained a major aspect of education.
The social impairment described here closely resembles the picture of the 'odd' rather than
the 'aloof or 'passive' type, using Wing and Gould's (1979) terminology.
Examples later on show that Fritz's stereotypic (repetitive) movements and habits include
jumping, hitting and echoing speech. The critical feature of such activity is its fragmentary
nature. Often it seems to be generated without external provocation.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 41
scholar, preoccupied with his own thoughts and out of touch with the real
world.
The mother herself was very similar to the boy. This similarity was
particularly striking given that she was a woman, since, in general, one
would expect a higher degree of intuitive social adaptation in women, more
emotion than intellect. In the way she moved and spoke, indeed in her
whole demeanour, she seemed strange and rather a loner. Very
characteristic, for instance, was the situation when mother and son walked
to the hospital school together, but each by themselves. The mother
slouched along, hands held behind her back and apparently oblivious to
the world. Beside her the boy was rushing to and fro, doing mischief. They
gave the appearance of having absolutely nothing to do with each other. 1'
One could not help thinking that the mother found it difficult to cope not
only with her child but with the practical matters of life. She was certainly
not up to running the household. Even living, as she did, in the upper
echelons of society, she always looked unkempt, unwashed almost, and
was always badly dressed.IZ She was also, clearly, not coping with the
physical care of her son. It has to be said, however, that this was a
particularly difficult problem. The mother knew her son through and
through and understood his difficulties very well. She tried to find similar
traits in herself and in her relations and talked about this eloquently. She
emphasised again and again that she was at the end of her tether, and this
was indeed obvious as soon as one saw them both together.
It was clear that this state of affairs was due not only to the boy's own
internally caused problems, but also to the mother's own problems in
relating to the outside world, showing as she did a limited intuitive social
understanding. Take the following typical trait: whenever things became
too much for her at home she would simply walk out on her family and
travel to her beloved mountains. She would stay there for a week or more
at a time, leaving the rest of the family to struggle for themselves.
The boy's father came from an ordinary farming family, with no
reported peculiarities. He had made a successful career for himself,
eventually becoming a high-ranking civil servant. He married late and was
fifty-five years old when hisfirstchild was born. The father was a
withdrawn and reticent man who did not give much away about himself.
He clearly hated to talk about himself and his interests. He was extremely
correct and pedantic and kept a more than usual distance.
It is interesting to compare Kanner and Eisenberg's (1955) description of the autistic boy
George and his mother: 'As they come up the stairs, the child trails forlornly behind the
mother, who does not bother to look back'. Here the authors seem to sympathise with the
child while being somewhat censorious of the mother. Asperger instead points out the
similarity of mother and son in the way they ignore each other.
Kanner and Eisenberg's (1955) account of George's mother is again strikingly similar: 'His
mother, a college graduate, looked bedraggled at the time of the first visit. She felt futile
about herself, was overwhelmed by her family responsibilities and gave the impression of
drabness and ineffectualness.'
42 Hans Asperger
Kanner (1943) does not talk of mischievous behaviour. However, Donald showed
behaviour that Asperger would almost certainly have labelled spiteful: 'He still went on
chewing on paper, putting food on his hair, throwing books into the toilet, putting a key
down the water drain, climbing onto the table and bureau, having temper tantrums.'
One of the most controversial of Asperger's ideas is his contention that the autistic children
he describes display intentionally spiteful or malicious behaviour. This idea has to be seen
together with his other observations of the children's general indifference to other people's
feelings. Examples that Asperger gives suggest that the child had only a physical effect in
mind, not a psychological one, as, for instance, when Fritz provoked his teacher because he
enjoyed seeing her display anger.
44 Hans Asperger
MEMORY FOR DIGITS He very readily repeated six digits. One was left
with a strong impression that he could go further, except that he just did
not feel like it. According to the Binet test, the repetition of six digits is
expected at the age of ten, while the boy was only six years old.
Enough examples from the intelligence test. We did not obtain an accurate
picture of the boy's intellectual abilities. This, of course, was hardly to be
expected. First, he rarely reacted to stimuli appropriately but followed his
own internally generated impulses. Secondly, he could not engage in the
lively reciprocity of normal social interaction. In order to judge his abilities it
was therefore necessary to look at his spontaneous productions.
As the parents had already pointed out, he often surprised us with
remarks that betrayed an excellent apprehension of a situation and an
accurate judgement of people. This was the more amazing as he apparently
never took any notice of his environment. Above all, from very early on he
had shown an interest in numbers and calculations. He had learnt to count
to over 100 and was able to calculate within that number-space with great
fluency. This was without anybody ever having tried to teach him - apart
from answering occasional questions he asked. His extraordinary
calculating ability had been reported by the parents and was verified by us.
Incidentally, we found, in general, that the parents had an excellent
understanding of their child's intellectual abilities. Such knowledge as the
boy possessed was not accessible by questioning at will. Rather, it showed
itself accidentally, especially during his time on the ward, where he was
given individual tuition. Even before any systematic teaching had begun, he
had mastered calculations with numbers over ten. Of course, quite a
number of bright children are able to do this before starting school at six.
However, his ability to use fractions was unusual, and was revealed quite
incidentally during his first year of instruction. The mother reported that
at the very beginning of schooling he set himself the problem what is
bigger lA(, or l/,8 - and then solved it with ease. When somebody asked for
fun, just to test the limits of his ability, 'What is ZA of 120?', he instantly
gave the right answer, '80'. Similarly, he surprised everybody with his
grasp of the concept of negative numbers, which he had apparently gained
wholly by himself; it came out with his remark that 3 minus 5 equals '2
under zero'. At the end of the first school year, he was also fluent in solving
problems of the type, 'If 2 workers do a job in a certain amount of time,
how much time do 6 workers need?'
We see here something that we have come across in almost all autistic
individuals, a special interest which enables them to achieve quite
extraordinary levels of performance in a certain area. This, then, throws
some light on the question of their intelligence. However, even now the
answer remains problematic since the findings can be contradictory and
46 HansAsperger
17
Asperger and Kanner were both impressed by the isolated special abilities found in almost
all their cases. Fritz shows superior rote memory and calculating ability; Donald likewise
has excellent rote memory and could count to TOO at the age of five.
18
Recent findings of an impairment in the understanding of emotion in voice and face confirm
Asperger's impression. See Hobson (1989) for a review of research and theoretical
interpretation. Asperger believed autistic children to have a disturbed relation to the
environment in general, and not merely to the social environment. It follows that their lack
of emotional understanding is a consequence of the same underlying problem (that is,
contact disturbance) which also results in their helplessness in practical matters of everyday
life. Kanner (1943), instead, contrasts the 'excellent relation to objects with the non-existent
relation to people', a highly influential view which has become the basis of many theories of
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 47
Let us consider this issue again from a different point of view. It is not
the content of words that makes a child comply with requests, by
processing them intellectually. It is, above all, the affect of the care-giver
which speaks through the words. Therefore, when making requests, it does
not really matter what the care-giver says or how well-founded the request
is. The point is not to demonstrate the necessity of compliance and
consequence of non-compliance - only bad teachers do this. What matters
is the way in which the request is made, that is, how powerful the affects
are which underlie the words. These affects can be understood even by the
infant, the foreigner or the animal, none of whom is able to comprehend
the literal meaning.
In our particular case, as indeed, in all such cases, the affective side was
disturbed to a large extent, as should have become apparent from the
description so far. The boy's emotions were indeed hard to comprehend.
It was almost impossible to know what would make him laugh or jump
up and down with happiness, and what would make him angry and
aggressive. It was impossible to know what feelings were the basis of his
stereotypic activities or what it was that could suddenly make him
affectionate. So much of what he did was abrupt and seemed to have no
basis in the situation itself. Since the affectivity of the boy was so deviant
and it was hard to understand his feelings, it is not surprising that his
reactions to the feelings of his care-givers were also inappropriate. 19
In fact, it is typical of children such as Fritz V. that they do not comply
with requests or orders that are affectively charged with anger, kindness,
persuasion or flattery. Instead, they respond with negativistic, naughty and
aggressive behaviour. While demonstrations of love, affection and flattery
are pleasing to normal children and often induce in them the desired
behaviour, such approaches only succeeded in irritating Fritz, as well as all
other similar children. While anger and threats usually succeed in bending
obstinacy in normal children and often make them compliant after all, the
opposite is true of autistic children. For them, the affect of the care-giver
may provide a sensation which they relish and thus seek to provoke. 'I am
so horrible because you are cross so nicely', said one such boy to his
teacher.
It is difficult to know what the appropriate pedagogic approach should
be. As with all genuine teaching, it should not be based primarily on logical
deduction but rather on pedagogic intuition. Nevertheless, it is possible to
state a few principles which are based on our experience with such
children.
The first is that all educational transactions have to be done with the
affect 'turned off. The teacher must never become angry nor should he aim
to become loved. It will never do to appear quiet and calm on the outside
while one is boiling inside. Yet this is only too likely, given the negativism
and seemingly calculated naughtiness of autistic children! The teacher must
at all costs be calm and collected and must remain in control. He should
give his instructions in a cool and objective manner, without being
intrusive. A lesson with such a child may look easy and appear to run along
in a calm, self-evident manner. It may even seem that the child is simply
allowed to get away with everything, any teaching being merely incidental.
Nothing could be further from the truth. In reality, the guidance of these
children requires a high degree of effort and concentration. The teacher
needs a particular inner strength and confidence which is not at all easy to
maintain!
There is a great danger of getting involved in endless arguments with
these children, be it in order to prove that they are wrong or to bring them
towards some insight. This is especially true for the parents, who
frequently find themselves trapped in endless discussions. On the other
hand, it often works simply to cut short negativistic talk: for example, Fritz
is tired of doing sums and sings, 'I don't want to do sums any more, I don't
want to do sums any more', the teacher replies, 'No, you don't need to do
sums', and continuing in the same calm tone of voice, 'How much is . . .?'
Primitive as they are, such methods are, in our experience, often successful.
There is an important point to be made here. Paradoxical as it may seem,
the children are negativistic and highly suggestible at the same time.
Indeed, there is a kind of automatic or reflex obedience. This behaviour is
known to occur in schizophrenics. It could well be that these two disorders
of the will are closely related! With our children we have repeatedly found
that if one makes requests in an automaton-like and stereotyped way, for
instance, speaking softly in the same sing-song that they use themselves,
one senses that they have to obey, seemingly unable to resist the command.
Another pedagogic trick is to announce any educational measures not as
personal requests, but as objective impersonal law. But more of this later.
I have already mentioned that behind the cool and objective interaction
with Fritz and all similar children there needs to be genuine care and
kindness if one wants to achieve anything at all. These children often show
a surprising sensitivity to the personality of the teacher. However difficult
they are even under optimal conditions, they can be guided and taught, but
only by those who give them true understanding and genuine affection,
people who show kindness towards them and, yes, humour. The teacher's
underlying emotional attitude influences, involuntarily and unconsciously,
the mood and behaviour of the child. Of course, the management and
guidance of such children essentially requires a proper knowledge of their
peculiarities as well as genuine pedagogic talent and experience. Mere
teaching efficiency is not enough.
It was clear from the start that Fritz, with his considerable problems,
could not be taught in a class. For one thing, any degree of restlessness
around him would have irritated him and made concentration impossible.
For another, he himself would have disrupted the class and destroyed work
done by the others. Consider only his negativism and his uninhibited,
impulsive behaviour. This is why we gave him a personal tutor on the
ward, with the consent of the educational authority. Even then, teaching
was not easy, as should be clear from the above remarks. Even
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 49
mathematics lessons were problematic when, given his special talent in this
area, one might have expected an easier time. Of course, if a problem
turned up which happened to interest him at that moment (see previous
examples), then he 'tuned in' and surprised us all by his quick and excellent
grasp. However, ordinary mathematics sums made for much tedious
effort. As we will see with the other cases even with the brightest children
of this type, the automatisation of learning, that is, the setting up of routine
thought processes, proceeds only with the utmost difficulty. Writing was an
especially difficult subject, as we expected, because his motor clumsiness, in
addition to his general problems, hampered him a good deal. In his tense
fist the pencil could not run smoothly. A whole page would suddenly
become covered with big swirls, the exercise book would be drilled full of
holes, if not torn up. In the end it was possible to teach him to write only by
making him trace letters and words which were written in red pencil. This
was to guide him to make the right movements. However, his
handwriting has so far been atrocious. Orthography too was difficult to
automatise. He used to write the whole sentence in one go, without
separating the words. He was able to spell correctly when forced to be
careful. However, he made the silliest mistakes when left to his own
devices. Learning to read, in particular sounding out words, proceeded
with moderate difficulties. It was almost impossible to teach him the
simple skills needed in everyday life. While observing such a lesson, one
could not help feeling that he was not listening at all, only making
mischief. It was, therefore, the more surprising, as became apparent
occasionally, for example through reports from the mother, that he had
managed to learn quite a lot. It was typical of Fritz, as of all similar
children, that he seemed to see a lot using only 'peripheral vision', or to
take in things 'from the edge of attention'. Yet these children are able to
analyse and retain what they catch in such glimpses. Their active and
passive attention is very disturbed; they have difficulty in retrieving their
knowledge, which is revealed often only by chance. Nevertheless, their
thoughts can be unusually rich. They are good at logical thinking, and the
ability to abstract is particularly good. It does often seem that even in
perfectly normal people an increased distance to the outside world is a
prerequisite for excellence in abstract thinking.
Despite the difficulties we had in teaching this boy we managed to get
him to pass successfully a state school examination at the end of the school
year. The exceptional examination situation was powerful enough to make
him more or less behave himself, and he showed good concentration.
Naturally, he astounded the examiners in mathematics. Now Fritz attends
the third form of a primary school as an external pupil, without having lost
a school year so far. Whether and when he will be able to visit a secondary
school we do not know.
Differential diagnosis
Considering the highly abnormal behaviour of Fritz, one has to ask
whether there is in fact some more severe disturbance and not merely a
50 Hans Asperger
Harro L.
Our second case is a boy who also shows the main characteristics of autism
in highly typical form, except that the relationships to the outside world are
not as severely disturbed as in our first case. Instead, the positive aspects of
autism become more obvious: the independence in thought, experience and
speech.
This eight-and-a-half-year-old boy was referred to us by his school as
unmanageable.20 He was in his third year at school, but was repeating the
second year because he had failed in all subjects.21 The teacher believed
that he 'could if he only wanted to'. Occasionally, he made surprisingly
clever remarks of a maturity way beyond his age. On the other hand, he
often refused to co-operate, sometimes using bad language, for example,
'this is far too stupid for me', which threatened to undermine the discipline
of the whole class. He hardly ever did his homework. Worse still were his
iO
As in the case of Fritz V., conduct problems seem to have been the main reason for referral.
zr
Failure to reach the required standard at the end of the school year resulted in repetition.
The child was placed with a younger age group going through the same syllabus again. This
would have carried a stigma for the family and the child.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 51
conduct problems. He rarely did what he was told but answered back and
with such cheek that the teacher had given up asking him so as not to lose
face in front of the class. On the one hand, Harro did not do what he was
supposed to do, on the other, he did exactly what he wanted to do himself
and without considering the consequences. He left his desk during lessons
and crawled on the floor on all fours. One of the principal reasons for his
being referred by the school was his savage tendency tofight.Little things
drove him to senseless fury, whereupon he attacked other children,
gnashing his teeth and hitting out blindly. This was dangerous because he
was not a skilled fighter. Children who are skilledfightersknow exactly
how far they can go and can control their movements so that they hardly
ever cause real trouble. Harro was anything but a skilledfighter,and since
he was very clumsy, could not control his movements and had no idea
where to aim, he often allegedly caused injury to others. He was said to be
extremely sensitive to teasing, and yet in many ways, with his strange and
comical behaviour he directly provoked teasing.
He was said to be an inveterate 'liar'. He did not lie in order to get out of
something that he had done this was certainly not the problem, as he
always told the truth very brazenly but he told long, fantastic stories, his
confabulations becoming ever more strange and incoherent.22
His early independence in certain things was outstanding. Since his
second school year, that is, since he was only seven years old, he had
travelled alone by train to school in Vienna. His parents lived in a village
approximately 25 km from Vienna. The father, who wanted his son to
have better opportunities, disdained the village school and therefore sent
him to school in Vienna.
On a more unpleasant note, Harro also showed his social unconcern in
sexual play with other boys, allegedly going as far as homosexual acts,
coitus attempts.
From the family history, we note that Harro was an only child. He was a
forceps delivery, but no disturbances were observed that might relate to
any birth injury. His mental and physical development was unremarkable.
As a small child, he was supposed to have been perfectly ordinary, except
that his stubbornness and independence were evident very early.
The father, who brought the child to us, was a strange man, and very
similar to his son. He appeared to be something of an adventurer. He
originally came from Siebenburgen (Transylvania) and during the First
World War, under great danger from the Romanian army, fled to Austria
via Russia. By profession he was a painter and sculptor, but out of financial
necessity he was making brooms and brushes. While there was severe
unemployment at the time we saw the boy, the contrast of the two jobs was
certainly striking. The father, who himself comes from peasant stock, is a
typical intellectual. He professed to be completely and painfully self-
Sadly, examples of fantastic stories told by Harro are not given. In their absence it is difficult
to know whether these stories were imaginative in the ordinary sense of the word, that is
whether Harro was fully aware of their fictitious nature. Donald (Kanner's first case) was
also said by his mother to dramatise stories, again without examples to illustrate what was
meant by this.
52 H a n s Asperger
taught. One could make out from what he said that he had nothing to do
with anyone in the village where he lived and where he must have been
considered highly eccentric. He said himself that he was nervous and highly
strung but that 'he controlled himself to such an extent that he appeared to
be indifferent'.
The mother, whom we never saw (we felt that the father did not want us
to see her) was also supposed to be highly strung. In both the father's and
mother's families there were said to be many highly strung people. No
more details were obtained.
Intelligence testing
First, some general remarks about the testing methods that we use in our
department. The main difference from traditional testing (for example, the
Z3
'Ich bin ein ganz fiirchterlicher Linkser'.
i4
Harro's social impairment included being aloof as well as odd. He showed, for instance,
aggressive disrespect for teachers, and kept himself apart from his peers.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 53
Binet test, from which we have taken some subtests) is that we use a clinical
approach where we are not interested merely in the passing and failing of
single tests but, instead, in the qualitative aspects of performance. First we
score the performance according to the level reached, and represent this
graphically to obtain a test profile. In this way, one can see the
discrepancies between the performances in various tests, which would
otherwise have been submerged in the overall IQ score/ 5 More
importantly, we observe how the child solves various problems, his method
of working, his individual tempo, his concentration and, above all, his
ability to relate and communicate. We adapt the way we test according to
the personality of the child, and we try to build up good rapport. Of
course, every good tester would do this anyway. Thus, it is important to
help the anxious, inhibited child who lacks self-confidence, for example, by
starting off the task for him or by helping him along. The chatty and
hyperactive child, or the child who does not keep a distance, on the other
hand, has to be restrained and somehow made to do the required work.
Obviously, any help given needs to be taken into account later when
scoring, which is not easy. We also try to find out what special interests
each child may have. We always let the children produce something
spontaneously, and we let them expand on their responses by asking more
questions. If there is a particular failure or if there are specific problems on
some subtest, then we ask questions that are not part of standard
procedure until we have clarified the reason for the difficulty.
This method of testing demands much greater experience than
schematically laid down methods with rigid scoring. However, if carried
out well, it can tell us not only about the intellectual ability of the child, but
also about important personality functions.
It was almost as difficult to carry out the testing with Harro as it was
with Fritz. Very often, he shut off completely when a question did not
interest him. Sometimes he did not seem to hear the question. A lot of
energy went into simply making him do the tasks. Again and again he went
off on a tangent and had to be brought back. However, once his attention
was engaged, his performance could be remarkably good.
Any tests that did not yield anything of special interest will be omitted,
but I will describe in detail the results of the similarities subtest. Here,
where Harro was able to produce answers spontaneously, he became lively
and interested, and one even had to cut him off, since he threatened to go
on for ever.
T R E E / B U S H 'The bush, that is where the branches grow straight off the
ground, completely jumbled up, so that it can happen that three or four
cross over each other, so that one has a knot in one's hand. The tree, that is
where there is first a stem and only then the branches, and not so jumbled
up, and rather thick branches. This happened to me once, that is where I
The examples Asperger gives of IQ test questions are chosen to illustrate the quality of the
autistic child's way of thinking, but they do not go beyond a clinical impression. His
colleague Elisabeth Wurst, in 1976, published profiles of IQ test performance which show a
characteristically uneven pattern such as is generally found in autism. The nature of this
pattern is discussed in Frith (1989).
54 HansAsperger
cut into a bush, I wanted to make myself a sling, I cut off four branches and
then I have an eight-part knot in my hand. This comes when two branches
rub against each other, then there is a wound there, then they grow
together.'
LAKE/RIVER 'Well, the lake, it doesn't move from its spot, and it can
never be as long and never have that many branches, and it always has an
end somewhere. One can't compare at all the Danube with the Ossiach
Lake in Corinthia - not in the least little bit.'
READING He read a story shoddily and with errors. However, one could
notice clearly that he read for meaning and that the content of the story
interested him. He wanted to read faster than he was able to and for this
reason was not very accurate. As this observation suggests his reading
comprehension was excellent. He could reproduce what he had read in his
own words, and he could say what the moral of a story was even though
the moral was not explicitly presented in the text (the fable of the fox who
was punished for his vanity).
bottle with a cork costs I.IO schillings, the bottle costs just one more
schilling than the cork, how much does each cost? After five seconds
he gave the correct solution and explained when asked: 'When a
bottle costs i schilling more, then you have to leave one schilling
aside, and something of the 10 groschen still needs to be left, so I
have to divide by 2, so the cork costs 5 groschen and the bottle costs
1 schilling and 5 groschen.'
Fascinating as his mastery of numbers may be, we can nevertheless see the
disadvantages of his original methods. They were often so complicated -
however ingenious that they resulted in errors. To use the conventional
methods that are taught at school, for example, starting with tens and then
units when subtracting, did not occur to Harro.
Here we come to an important insight: in autism there is a particular
difficulty in mechanical learning, indeed there is an inability to learn from
adults in conventional ways. Instead, the autistic individual needs to create
everything out of his own thought and experience. More often than not this
results in defective performance, even in the more able autistic individuals.
In this way we can explain why such a bright boy as Harro was unable to
attain the end of his form year and had to repeat it. Of course, in school he
was more difficult than during individual testing, where we made
allowances for his problems and provided an opportunity for him to give
spontaneous and original answers. On the ward too, we were able to
observe how much worse his performance was when he was taught in a
group. Being taught in a group, of course means that everybody has to pay
attention and do what the teacher asks. Harro could do neither of these.
His mind wandered off on his own problems and he would not know what
the lesson was about. He took away from the lesson only those things for
which he had a particular affinity and could think about in his own way.
According to the school report he hardly ever knew what homework he
had to do, and could not therefore do the appropriate work at home
despite the father's efforts. It is not surprising, then, that in the previous
year he had not been able to advance to the next form despite his
undoubted ability which was recognised by the school.27
Almost certainly, the ideas Asperger has in mind here are those discussed in a classic work
of German literature, Heinrich von Kleist's (1810) essay about the puppet theatre. Kleist
contrasted the natural grace of the unconsciously moving child with the artifice of
mechanical puppetry. Similarly, Asperger contrasts 'intellectualised' behaviour, which is
formal and stilted, with spontaneous behaviour, which is naturally graceful and appealing.
58 H a n s Asperger
show negativism and malice to seemingly petty and routine demands, and
that it is there that serious conflicts often arise.
How, then, should one treat these difficulties? We have already noted in
the first case that more can be achieved by 'switching off one's affect and
by using an impersonal, objective style of instruction. Here, with the more
able and less disturbed Harro, we found a way which we believe to be
successful with more autistic children. The boy was more amenable when a
request appeared not to be directed towards him in particular, but was
verbally phrased in a very general, impersonal way, as an objective law,
standing above the child and the teacher, for instance, 'One always does
such and such . . .', 'Now everyone has to . . .', 'A bright boy always
does . . .'.
Another important point is this: normal children acquire the necessary
social habits without being consciously aware of them, they learn
instinctively. It is these instinctive relations that are disturbed in autistic
children. To put it bluntly, these individuals are intelligent automata.
Social adaptation has to proceed via the intellect. In fact, they have to learn
everything via the intellect. 29 One has to explain and enumerate
everything, where, with normal children, this would be an error of
educational judgement. Autistic children have to learn the simple daily
chores just like proper homework, systematically. With some children who
admittedly were somewhat older than Harro, it was possible to achieve a
relatively smooth integration by establishing an exact timetable in which,
from the moment of rising at a particular time, every single occupation and
duty was outlined in detail. When such children left the hospital they were
given a timetable. It was, of course, made up in consultation with the
parents and adapted to the individual needs of each family. The children
had to give an account of how well they followed the timetable, sometimes
by keeping a diary. They felt that they were firmly tied to this 'objective
law'. In any case, many of them have pedantic tendencies veering towards
the obsessional, and it was possible to use such tendencies for this
regulatory purpose.
In this way Harro too achieved better adaptation, though not without
difficulty. He certainly began to respond better to the demands of group
teaching. Several months after he left, we heard that he was much happier
at school. Unfortunately, we have not heard from him since, as his parents,
we believe, have moved.
The difficulties these children have with instinctive adaptations are, then,
amenable to partial compensation through an intellectualising approach.
The better the intellectual ability the more successful this approach. Now,
the autistic personality is certainly not only found in the intellectually able.
It also occurs in the less able, even in children with severe mental
Ernst K.
This seven-and-a-half-year-old boy was also referred to us by his school
because of severe conduct and learning problems.
The following points from the family history deserve to be mentioned.
Birth and physical development were normal. Ernst was an only child. His
speech was somewhat delayed (first words at the age of one-and-a-half).
For a long time, the boy was reported to have had speech difficulties
(stammering). Now, however, his speech was exceptionally good, he spoke
iike an adult'.
He was reported to have been a very difficult toddler, paying heed to
neither his indulgent mother nor his strict father. He was said to be unable
to cope with the ordinary demands of everyday life. The mother believed
that it was because of his clumsiness and impracticality that he had more
difficulties than other children. For instance, it was still necessary to dress
him, since, by himself, he would dawdle endlessly and also make a lot of
mistakes. He had learnt to eat by himself only recently and was still a messy
eater. The mother also reported that occasionally he could be very naughty
and would not do what he was told.
He was never able to get on with other children. It was impossible to go
to a park with him, as he would instantly get embroiled in fighting.
Apparently, he hit or verbally abused other children indiscriminately. This
had become more of a problem since he started school. He acted like a red
rag to his class and was teased mercilessly. However, rather than keeping
away from the other children, he acted as a trouble-maker. For instance, he
would pinch or tickle other children or stab them with his pen. He liked to
tell fantastic stories, in which he always appeared as the hero. He would
tell his mother how he was praised by the teacher in front of the class, and
other similar tales. 31
The report said that it was difficult to know how bright he was. Before
he entered school, everyone was convinced that he would learn particularly
well, since he was always making clever remarks and original observations.
Moreover, he had by himself learnt to count to twenty, as well as picking
up the names of various letters. At school, however, he failed miserably. He
just managed to move up from the first form (wrongly, as we had cause to
observe later), but now, in the second form, according to the teacher, he
was not performing adequately. Instead of listening and answering when
appropriate, he constantly argued with the teacher as to how to hold his
pen. According to the report, he had a strong tendency to argue with
The important insight that autism can occur at all levels of intellectual ability, including the
subnormal range of intelligence, has often been overlooked, even by Asperger himself in his
later papers.
As in the case of Harro L., the information given does not tell us if Ernst himself believed his
fantastic stories to be true or if he was aware that they were not and told them deliberately
to mislead his mother.
60 Hans Asperger
Family history
The father was said to be very highly strung and irritable. By profession he
was a tailor's assistant. Although we had known the boy for many years,
we had seen the father only once. He was clearly eccentric and a loner. The
mother did not like to talk about her domestic circumstances. However, it
was plain that her life could not have been very happy due to the husband's
difficult character.
The mother was a very bright and extremely nice woman whose life was
not easy. She complained of nervousness and headaches. She was also very
sensitive. She found it hard to cope with the fact that her son, who was
obviously her one and only interest in life, was such an odd child and did so
badly at school. She constantly tried to take his side against the school and
fought desperately against a transfer into a special school for retarded
children.
The rest of the family was said to be without any special peculiarities, the
information being given with some reticence.
*2 This observation clearly relates to the phenomenon which Kanner calls insistence on
sameness, and which he believes to be a cardinal symptom of autism. Asperger observes but
does not particularly focus on this symptom.
'* Graf Bobby, the butt of popular jokes in German-speaking countries and particularly in
Austria, is a refined aristocrat who constantly finds ordinary events incomprehensible. This
image of the gentle innocent may well have been inspired by the existence of able autistic
people.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 61
By the examples he gives Asperger implies deeper links between insistence on sameness,
obsessive pedantry, narrow preoccupations and tenaciously held ideas.
6z Hans Asperger
attention was disturbed. It was typical that during testing he seemed either
to be somewhere else or as if he had just fallen from the sky. He was clearly
not tuned in for proper responding and was clueless on most of the test
questions. Thus he only managed a very poor performance even when one
held him down long enough by look or by word.
Very characteristic again was the performance on the similarities subtest.
Here are some examples:
FLY/BUTTERFLY 'The fly has wings like glass. From the wings of the
butterfly you can make silk [this apparently referred to the silky shine].
They are colourful. The butterfly, when it gets colder goes down, and in the
spring he turns into a caterpillar and then again a butterfly, first he is a
cocoon and this is all silvery.' Then he talked about some events that had
happened to him involving moths in his room and worms in the soup,
which had nothing to do with the question.
RIVER/LAKE 'In the river the water flows, and in the lake it stands still,
and on the top is green slime.'
STAIRS/LADDER 'The ladder is leaning like this, and the stairs go like
that, and up there like this [he draws steps by gesture]. The stairs have a
kind of surface for treading on, the ladder has rungs.'
CHILD/DWARF 'The dwarf is small, the child big. The dwarf looks
completely different. It has a pointed hat, but this is red. The child has a
bonnet.'
Asperger here describes a problem that is strongly suggestive of classic dyslexia, a disorder
marked by severe problems in phoneme segmentation (see Snowling (1987) for a detailed
discussion). It is not known as yet whether dyslexia co-occurs with autism more than one
would expect by chance.
64 Hans Asperger
*6 Asperger's claim that autistic features are even more striking in retarded individuals than in
those of normal intelligence is important to note. His clinical picture of autism is not limited
to able children alone.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 65
intelligence. Water supplies in the house are particularly popular targets for
mischief (and one can indeed do a lot of it there!), but equally popular is
throwing things out of windows, even when these are opened only for an
instant. Then there is the instinctive aggression which is characteristic of
both clinical groups, shown frequently in pinching, biting and scratching.
Brain-injured patients often distinguish themselves by masterful spitting,
especially if they have plenty of material due to hyper-salivation! In short,
the disturbance of contact which we have already described in autistic
children with its characteristic features can be found in a very similar form
in many post-encephalitic cases.
It is often not easy to differentiate diagnostically whether, in such cases,
the disturbance is constitutional (that is, autistic psychopathy), or a sequel
to acquired brain damage. Important factors to consider are family history,
birth history, presence of high fever with dizziness, sleepiness, vomiting, fits
at any time, and other neurological symptoms. Among these are signs or
hints of spastic paresis such as dysarthric speech, stuttering, oculo-
muscular symptoms, strabismus, vegetative signs such as increased
salivation (in our experience, hardly ever absent in the brain-injured),
increased eye brilliance (which, together with some other elusive features,
forms the basis of the 'encephalitic glance') and profuse sweating. Lastly,
there are endocrine disturbances, in particular, obesity. It is increasingly
believed that endocrine disturbances are caused by primary cerebral
disturbances, in particular, disturbances of the hypophysis. With endocrine
disturbances go certain trophic disturbances such as double-jointedness,
especially of thefingers,or a particular prominence of the middle of the
face. The alveolar appendices can become enlarged and coarse, and the
gums become hypertrophic. These signs are particularly striking when they
are seen in children who earlier were of an elfin beauty. Three, four or five
years after encephalitis, they have a badly misshapen face. As an example,
another case will be described briefly.
Hellmuth L.
This boy is the fourth child of his parents, who are themselves without any
peculiarities. He was born seven years after the third child, when the
mother was forty-one years old. He had severe asphyxia at birth and was
resuscitated at length. Soon after his birth he had convulsions, which
recurred twice within the next few days, but have not since. His
development was delayed and he started walking and talking towards the
end of his second year. However, he then learnt to speak relatively quickly,
and even as a toddler he talked 'like a grown up'.
He was always grotesquely fat, despite a strict, medically supervised diet.
He gained weight without having a big appetite. When we met him six
years ago, at the age of eleven, he had distinctly formed 'breasts and hips'.
He has remained thus up to now (we have recently seen him again). He had
bilateral cryptorchidism (for about a year he had been masturbating a good
deal). The boy had been treated with hormone preparations, especially
66 Hans Asperger
thymus and hypophysis preparations, since his early childhood but without
any effect on his condition. He was double-jointed to a high degree. When
one shook his hand, it seemed as if it had no bones and were made of
rubber. He had knock knees and flat feet. He had noticeably increased
salivation, and when he talked one could hear the saliva bubbling in his
mouth.
His appearance was grotesque. On top of the massive body, over the big
face with flabby cheeks, was a tiny skull. One could almost consider him
microcephalic. His little eyes were closely set together. His glance was lost
and absent but occasionally lit up with malice. As is to be expected from his
whole appearance, he was clumsy to an extraordinary degree. He stood
there in the midst of a group of playing children like a frozen giant. He
could not possibly catch a ball, however easy one tried to make it for him.
His movements when catching and throwing gave him an extremely
comical appearance. The immobile dignity of the face which accompanied
this spectacle made the whole even more ridiculous. He was said to have
been clumsy in all practical matters from infancy, and has remained so ever
since.
Listening to the boy talking, one was surprised how clever he sounded.
He kept his immobile dignity while speaking and talked slowly, almost as if
in verse, seemingly full of insight and superiority. He often used unusual
words, sometimes poetical and sometimes unusual combinations. This was
consistent with an interest in poetry as reported by the mother. He clearly
did not have any feeling for the fact that he did not really fit into this world.
Otherwise he would not have shown off in his peculiar way, especially
not in front of other children. It was not surprising, then, that he was
continuously taunted by other children who ran after him in the street.
Of course, he could never do anything to his fleet-footed tormentors,
becoming only more ridiculous in his helpless rage. This was the reason the
mother had arranged for him to be taught privately over the last school
years. He managed, surprisingly, to attain the fifth grade of primary
school.
His school knowledge was very uneven. He was an excellent speller and
never made mistakes. He also had quite a good style. On the other hand,
his arithmetic was very poor, not only in terms of the mechanical aspects,
but also when problems were presented in verbal form. One noticed the
degree of his disability and his ignorance of worldly things when
questioning him about ordinary matters of everyday life. This was where
he failed abysmally, giving empty, pompous-sounding answers. The
mother was quite right when she said that he was always 'in another
world'. However, this did not prevent him from doing a lot of malicious
things to the people he lived with and to other children. He enjoyed hiding
or destroying objects, especially when he was little.
He was reported to have been pedantic from earliest childhood, for
instance, he created scenes when something was occasionally placed in a
slightly different position from usual. In everything he did, it was said, he
had his particular rituals. He was especially concerned with his clothes, did
not tolerate a grain of dirt on them, washed his hands very frequently and
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 67
observed his body and its functions very closely. His pedantries tyrannised
the household and he was in general very difficult to cope with.37
Much of his description is reminiscent of the earlier cases. This boy was
'an autistic automaton', impractical and instinctually disturbed. His
relationships with the outside world were extremely limited. He did not
have any genuine human relationships, was full of pedantries and also
showed spiteful behaviour.
In Hellmuth's case there were clear indications that his autism was due
to brain injury at birth. His medical history asphyxia, fits, endocrine
disorder, hyper-salivation, neurologically based apraxia - clearly pointed
to an organic cause.
We can therefore draw the preliminary conclusion that there are cases
where an organic disorder can result in a picture that, in numerous critical
points, is closely similar to the picture presented by 'autistic personality
disorder' of constitutional origin.38
glance goes into the far distance or is turned inwards, just as one never
knows what the children are preoccupied with at a particular moment or
what is going on in their minds. The disturbance is particularly clear when
they are in conversation with others. Glance does not meet glance as it does
when unity of conversational contact is established. When we talk to
someone we do not only 'answer' with words, but we 'answer' with our
look, our tone of voice and the whole expressive play of face and hands. A
large part of social relationships is conducted through eye gaze, but such
relationships are of no interest to the autistic child. Therefore, the child does
not generally bother to look at the person who is speaking. The gaze goes
past the other person or, at most, touches them incidentally in passing.
However, autistic children do not look with a firmly fixed glance at
anything, but rather, seem to perceive mainly with their peripheral field of
vision. Thus, it is occasionally revealed that they have perceived and
processed a surprisingly large amount of the world around them. There is
one situation, however, in which the eye gaze of these children becomes
extremely expressive; their eyes light up when they are intent upon some
malicious act, which is then perpetrated in an instant.
It will have become obvious that autistic children have a paucity of facial
and gestural expression. In ordinary two-way interaction they are unable to
act as a proper counterpart to their opposite number, and hence they have
no use for facial expression as a contact-creating device. Sometimes they
have a tense, worried look. While talking, however, their face is mostly
slack and empty, in line with the lost, faraway glance. There is also a paucity
of other expressive movements, that is, gestures. Nevertheless, the children
themselves may move constantly, but their movements are mostly stereoty-
pic and have no expressive value.44
Next in importance to eye gaze as a channel for expression is language.45
As we saw with our first case, Fritz V., language expresses interpersonal
relationships as much as it provides objective information. Affect, for
instance, can be directly expressed in language. We can hear from the tone
of voice what relationship people have to each other, for instance superior
and subordinate, and whether they are in sympathy or antipathy. This is
regardless of the often deceptive content of the words themselves. It is this
aspect of language which tells us what someone really thinks. In this way the
perceptive listener can get behind the mask. He can tell from an individual's
expressions what is lie and truth, what are empty words and what is
genuinely meant.
44
While Kanner rarely mentions poverty of expression in autistic children, Asperger goes into
great detail to convey the nature of this to him essential s y m p t o m of autism. It m a y be that
the disturbance of expressive functioning that Asperger highlights is more striking in cases
with fluent language a n d paradoxically p o o r communication.
45
Asperger addresses a n d anticipates here that aspect of language we n o w k n o w as
pragmatics. It is well established that autistic individuals have a specific failure in
pragmatics, that is, in the use of language. This is true even if their speech is otherwise
linguistically sophisticated.
jo Hans Asperger
It is impossible to list all that can be revealed in volume, tone and flow of
speech since these aspects are as varied as the human character. In any case,
we do not intellectually understand many of these qualities and can only feel
them intuitively.46
Again, it will come as no surprise that contact-creating expressive
functions are deficient in people with disturbed contact. If one listens
carefully, one can invariably pick up these kinds of abnormalities in the
language of autistic individuals, and their recognition is, therefore, of
particular diagnostic importance. The abnormalities differ, of course, from
case to case. Sometimes the voice is soft and far away, sometimes it sounds
refined and nasal, but sometimes it is too shrill and ear-splitting. In yet other
cases, the voice drones on in a sing-song and does not even go down at the
end of a sentence. Sometimes speech is over-modulated and sounds like
exaggerated verse-speaking. However many possibilities there are, they all
have one thing in common: the language feels unnatural, often like a
caricature, which provokes ridicule in the naive listener. One other thing:
autistic language is not directed to the addressee but is often spoken as if
into empty space. This is exactly the same as with autistic eye gaze which,
instead of homing in on the gaze of the partner, glides by him.
In a wider sense, the choice of words too must be considered among the
expressive functions of language. This will become clear in the following
section.
Autistic Intelligence
The skills that a child acquires grow out of a tension between two opposite
poles: one is spontaneous production, the other imitation of adult knowl-
edge and skills. They have to balance each other if the achievement is to be
of value. When original ideas are lacking achievement is an empty shell:
what has been learnt is merely a superficial and mechanical copy. Autistic
intelligence is characterised by precisely the opposite of this problem.
Autistic children are able to produce original ideas. Indeed, they can only be
original, and mechanical learning is hard for them. They are simply not set
to assimilate and learn an adult's knowledge. Just as, in general, somebody's
good and bad sides are inextricably linked, so the special abilities and
disabilities of autistic people are interwoven.
This becomes clearer when we look at the language production of autistic
children. They, and especially the intellectually gifted among them,
undoubtedly have a special creative attitude towards language. They are
46
In Asperger's remarks about expressive functions the influence of Ludwig Klages (for
example, 1936) is strongly evident. Klages was a widely read author of the time who
attempted to find a scientific basis for character-reading from phenomena observed in each
person's style of behaviour. Target behaviour included voice, movement, facial expression
and also handwriting.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 71
47
T h e original w o r d s a n d phrases p r o d u c e d by autistic children are m o r e often t h a n n o t
characterised by a disregard for t h e listener's ability t o c o m p r e h e n d their meaning, a n d
particularly the reason for their use. Asperger tends t o stress the originality and overlook the
inappropriateness of idiosyncratic language.
48
'Die Leiter geht so spitz u n d die Stiege so schlangenringelich'.
49
'Miindlich k a n n ich das nicht, aber kopflich.'
50
'Fur ein Kunstauge sind solche Bilder vielleicht schon, aber mir gefallen sie nicht.'
5
' 'Ich m d c h t nicht sagen dass ich u n f r o m m bin, aber ich h a b so kein M e r k m a l von G o t t . '
52
An interesting example of a seven-and-a-half-year-old b o y q u o t e d by Asperger in his 1 9 5 2
t e x t b o o k also serves t o illustrate the capacity t o m a k e original observations a n d the ability
to d r a w causal inferences t h a t one can find in bright autistic children: G L A S S / M I R R O R 'A
mirror is n o t m u c h different, a sheet of glass t h a t is painted with mercury o n the back; it
mirrors back t h e picture before the glass; w h y mercury is able to d o this, I d o n ' t k n o w ,
perhaps because it is so dark. I have found o u t t h a t y o u can see yourself w h e n there is
something d a r k behind the glass. W h e n there w a s light behind it, I have never been able to
see myself. In o u r house w e have a glass d o o r ; you can see yourself in it only w h e n the light
behind it is n o t switched o n . ' Incidentally, this example shows the same mixture of
personally remembered episodes a n d factual knowledge as is found in t h e answers given by
Fritz and H a r r o .
53
Very often the interests of autistic children cannot be described as advanced; rather they are
outside the interests of their normal peers. However, their reasoning with biological or
physical concepts appears to be ahead of their reasoning with psychological concepts
(Baron-Cohen, 1989) while the opposite is true of normal children.
72 Hans Asperger
case, Harro L. Often a very narrow, circumscribed and isolated special area
can show hypertrophic development.
We know an autistic child who has a particular interest in the natural
sciences. His observations show an unusual eye for the essential. He orders
his facts into a system and forms his own theories even if they are
occasionally abstruse. Hardly any of this has he heard or read, and he
always refers to his own experience. There is also a child who is a 'chemist'.
He uses all his money for experiments which often horrify his family and
even steals to fund them. Some children have even more specialised
interests, for instance, only experiments which create noise and smells.
Another autistic boy was obsessed with poisons. He had a most unusual
knowledge in this area and possessed a large collection of poisons, some
quite naively concocted by himself. He came to us because he had stolen a
substantial quantity of cyanide from the locked chemistry store at his
school! Another, again, was preoccupied by numbers. Complex calculations
were naturally easy for him without being taught. We are reminded here of
our first case, Fritz V., which, however, also shows us the possibility of
failure. The same child who astounded others by solving complex maths
problems had the most serious learning disabilities at school, and could not
learn the simple calculation methods that were taught there. Another
autistic child had specialised technological interests and knew an incredible
amount about complex machinery. He acquired this knowledge through
constant questioning, which it was impossible to fend off, and also to a
great degree through his own observations. He came to be preoccupied with
fantastic inventions, such as spaceships and the like, and here one observes
how remote from reality autistic interests often are. 54
Another distinctive trait one finds in some autistic children is a rare
maturity of taste in art.55 Normal children have no time for more
sophisticated art. Their taste is usually for the pretty picture, with kitschy
rose pink and sky blue. The artfully stylised children's books, so fashionable
fifteen to twenty years ago, are therefore as unchildlike as possible.
Fortunately, matters have now improved in this respect. Autistic children,
on the other hand, can have a surprisingly sophisticated understanding,
being able to distinguish between art and kitsch with great confidence. They
may have a special understanding of works of art which are difficult even
for many adults, for instance Romanesque sculpture or paintings by
Rembrandt. Autistic individuals can judge accurately the events represented
in the picture, as well as what lies behind them, including the character of
54
A b o u t ten years later, Asperger points o u t in his t e x t b o o k (1952) that spaceships are n o
longer a fantastic invention. H e jokingly suggests that the inventors might have been
autistic.
55
The claim that autistic children have a special gift for art appreciation is very surprising.
One can imagine, however, that bright autistic children may well give refreshingly
unconventional responses to high art and literature.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 73
the people represented and the mood that pervades a painting. Consider
that many normal adults never reach this mature degree of art appreciation.
Related to this skill is the autistic person's ability to engage in a particular
kind of introspection and to be a judge of character. 56 While the normal
child lives unself-consciously and appropriately interacts with others as an
integrated member of his community, these children observe themselves
constantly. They are an object of interest to themselves, and they direct their
attention towards the functions of their body. Here is an example: a
nine-year-old autistic boy suffered badly from homesickness57 in the
evening (homesickness always being worst at this time), saying: 'If one lays
one's head on the bolster, then there is such a strange noise in the ear and
one has to lie very quietly for a long time and that is nice.' The same boy
also described an occasional micropsy: 'At school, I sometimes see that
teacher has a tiny head, then I don't know what it is; it is very unpleasant to
me that I see this way. Then I press my eyes very hard [demonstrates how he
does this], and then it gets better.'
These peculiarities lead us to a digression. As always, the miraculous
automaticity of vegetative life is at its best when left unconscious. When
attention is directed towards it we invariably find disturbances of these
functions. Hamburger has always emphasised that educators should never
direct the child's attention towards eating, sleeping or elimination, since this
would only disturb these automatic functions.58 With autistic children,
however, their own bodily functions are in the forefront of their conscious-
ness anyway. The functions are not only registered and taken seriously, but
they are also often disturbed. Especially frequent are eating and sleeping
difficulties, which can lead to serious conflicts within the family.
Just as these children observe themselves to a high degree, so they also
often have surprisingly accurate and mature observations about people in
their environment. They know who means well with them and who does
not, even when he feigns differently. They have a particular sensitivity for
56
Introspection a n d self-reflection usually refer to mental rather than physiological processes.
Asperger's examples all pertain to observations of physiological states. If the autistic child
has difficulty in conceiving of mental states, then biological concepts might well take on
special prominence in such a child's life. T h e idea of personality that w o u l d follow from a
biological theory w o u l d be quite different from that derived from a psychological o n e . F r o m
this viewpoint the claim that autistic children can read character in others is a most unlikely
one. However, autistic children m a y unerringly k n o w which person really loves them
precisely because they tend t o be behaviourists. They w o u l d ignore the person w h o merely
talks sweetly b u t does n o t in fact help them.
57
Experience in b o a r d i n g schools for autistic children over m a n y years confirms t h a t severe
homesickness can occur b u t is n o t particularly c o m m o n . Asperger later on suggests a reason
for the type of homesickness he observed: the missing of daily routines. As highlighted by
Kanner, great unhappiness can result from even apparently trivial changes in familiar
habits.
58
Asperger pays homage here to his mentors at the University Paediatric Clinic where he was
trained. Hamburger was the director at the time this work was carried out and no doubt
influenced it.
74 Hans Asperger
From this middle group there is a smooth transition further along the
range to those mentally retarded people who show highly stereotyped
automaton-like behaviour. Sometimes they have crackpot interests which
are of no practical use. They also include 'calendar people', who know the
name of the saint for every day of the year, or children who, long before
they enter a special school, know all the tram lines of Vienna with their
terminals, or children who show other feats of rote memory. 60
So far, we have looked at the intelligence of autistic children from the
point of view of their own spontaneous productions and their own interests.
Now we shall turn to learning and schooling. Obeying only spontaneous
impulses and never paying attention to social demands may well lead to
originality but will also lead to learning failure. The truth of this statement
is borne out in almost all our cases. The very same children who can
astonish their teachers with their advanced and clever answers fail miserably
at their lessons. What they find difficult are the mechanical aspects of
learning which the least clever, even somewhat retarded, pupils find easy, in
other words, reading, writing and arithmetic (multiplication tables!). Some-
times, school subjects happen to coincide with the child's special interest.
For instance, some of these children may learn to read particularly easily
because they absorb all reading material from an unusually early age, say six
or seven years (normally, children become bookworms around the age of
ten).61 'Savant' calculators can certainly do well at school arithmetic,
although there are some noticeable paradoxes here. The obsession to go his
own way in all circumstances and the exclusive use of his own self-invented
procedures can prevent the child from assimilating the calculation methods
the school wishes to instil. These children make life difficult for themselves.
They are bound to make errors and to arrive at the wrong results. Examples
are described in the first case (Fritz V.) and the second (Harro L.). Another
example is of an autistic boy who was just starting school, but could pose
and solve the problem of how many seconds there are in two hours.
However, when asked to work out 5 plus 6, he said, 'I don't like little sums,
I'd much rather do a thousand times a thousand.' After he had produced his
'spontaneous' calculations for a while, we insisted that he solve the given
problem. He then presented the following original, but awkward method:
'Look, that's how I work it out. 6 and 6 equals 12 and 5 and 6 is 1 less,
therefore 11.' This boy was also particularly prone to being distracted, that
is, distracted from within. This type of distraction impairs the performance
of many autistic children.
60
Why special skills in autistic individuals so often involve feats of rote memory is as yet
unexplained. The spontaneous predilection for calendar skills and transport is also an
unsolved mystery. Through Asperger's observations we know that these odd interests were
as conspicuous then as they are now.
61
Asperger himself is known to have read all of the works of Grillparzer, one of Austria's
greatest playwrights, by the time he was nine.
j6 Hans Asperger
64
Again, we hear about acts of malice; this time, however, Asperger suggests that the cause
might be a poorly developed sense of how much another person may be hurt. This idea fits
well with the hypothesis that autistic individuals usually fail to take account of mental
states. For further discussion of this topic see chapter 1.
6s
Negativistic reactions include refusing to do what one is required to do or doing exactly the
opposite.
78 HansAsperger
their own, at school they are almost always referred to child guidance
centres because they cannot be handled in the ordinary way.
In the first two cases we pointed out the learning and conduct problems in
school that are due to autism. It was also mentioned that autistic children
are often tormented and rejected by their classmates simply because they are
different and stand out from the crowd. Their conduct, manner of speech
and, not least, often grotesque demeanour cries out to be ridiculed. Children
in general have a good eye for this and show great accuracy in their mocking
of conspicuous character peculiarities.
Thus, in the playground or on the way to school one can often see an
autistic child at the centre of a jeering horde of little urchins. The child
himself may be hitting out in blind fury or crying helplessly. In either case he
is defenceless. The situation can be so bad that the mother must accompany
the child to protect him from this sort of cruelty. The child may need a
minder to the end of his school years and often beyond.67 In favourable
cases, however, it is possible for autistic children to earn respect, even if it is
mixed with ridicule, either through sheer intellectual prowess or through
particularly ruthless aggression.
shall take a knife one day and push it in your heart, then blood will spurt
out and this will cause a great stir.' 'It would be nice if I were a wolf. Then I
could rip apart sheep and people, and then blood would flow.' Once, when
the mother cut her finger, 'Why isn't there more blood? The blood should
run!' When he injured himself on one occasion, he was said to have been
utterly thrilled, so that the doctor who tended the wound remarked on the
child's state as extremely odd. At the same time, the boy was particularly
anxious. He was afraid to fall over in his chair and extremely afraid of
fast-moving vehicles on the road. There is also not infrequently a tendency
to use obscene words which may stand in strange contrast to the otherwise
often stilted language of these children.
Thus, with the sexual aspect of affective life there is often a definite
disharmony, either a weakness or precocity and perversion, but no har-
monious integration of sexuality into the developing personality. The same
is also true for other areas of affective life. Over-sensitivity and blatant
insensitivity clash with each other. Here are some examples.
In the sense of taste we find almost invariably very pronounced likes and
dislikes. The frequency of this phenomenon provides yet more proof of the
unity of the type. There is often a preference for very sour or strongly spiced
food, such as gherkins or roast meat. Often there is an insurmountable
dislike of vegetables or dairy produce.68 It is no different with the sense of
touch. Many children have an abnormally strong dislike of particular tactile
sensations, for example, velvet, silk, cotton wool or chalk. They cannot
tolerate the roughness of new shirts, or of mended socks. Cutting fingernails
is often the cause of tantrums. Washing water too can often be a source of
unpleasant sensations and, hence, of unpleasant scenes. In the hospital we
have observed hypersensitivity of the throat which was so strong that the
daily routine inspection with the spatula became an increasingly difficult
procedure. There is hypersensitivity too against noise. Yet the same children
who are often distinctly hypersensitive to noise in particular situations, in
other situations may appear to be hyposensitive. They may appear to be
switched off even to loud noises.69
The impression of disharmony and contradiction only increases when we
consider the higher feelings as they are manifested in relationships to
objects, to animals and to other people. As soon as one starts to work with
these children, one is struck by a distinctive emotional defect which one may
well consider an ultimate cause of their social disturbance. This defect is
apparent in their isolation while they are in the midst of other people and in
their contrariness with their environment and especially their closest family.
68
It is possible t h a t food allergies, if investigated, might have h a d links with some of the
aversions mentioned.
69
In contrast to Kanner (1943) Asperger remarks on the paradoxical p h e n o m e n o n of hyper-
a n d hyposensitivity to s o u n d , light and touch. This p h e n o m e n o n has since been observed in
autistic children of all ability levels. N o consensus has been reached, however, on the
diagnostic value or meaning of this feature.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 81
They lack the displays of affection which normally make life with a small
child so richly rewarding. One never hears that they try to flatter or try to be
nice. Indeed, they often turn nasty when one tries to be nice to them. Their
malice and cruelty too clearly arise from this impoverished emotionality.
Autistic children are egocentric in the extreme. They follow only their
own wishes, interests and spontaneous impulses, without considering
restrictions or prescriptions imposed from outside. They lack completely
any respect for the other person. They treat everyone as an equal as a
matter of course and speak with a natural self-confidence. In their dis-
obedience too their lack of respect is apparent. They do not show deliberate
acts of cheek, but have a genuine defect in their understanding of the
other person.
For personal distance too they have no sense or feeling. Just as they
unconcernedly lean on others, even complete strangers, and may run their
fingers over them as if they were a piece of furniture, so they impose
themselves without shyness on anybody. They may demand a service or
simply start a conversation on a theme of their own choosing. All this goes,
of course, without any regard for differences in age, social rank or common
courtesies.70
Autistic children's relations to objects, too, are abnormal. With the
normal child, particularly the infant, things become alive because he fills
them with life through his vivid relationship with the world around him. He
gains experience and maturity through lavishing his attention and love on
objects. This does not happen with autistic children. Either they take no
notice of the objects in their environment, for instance, they take little
interest in toys, or they have abnormal fixations. Perhaps they fixate on a
whip or a wooden brick or a doll that they never let out of their sight, and
cannot eat or sleep when the 'fetish' is not there. There can be the most
severe tantrums at any attempt to take away the object of such passionate
attachment.71
Very often, the relationship of autistic children to things is limited to
collecting, and here again, instead of the harmonious order and richness of a
normally balanced affective life, we find deficiencies and empty spaces, in
70
All the autistic features which Asperger considers in the three preceding paragraphs can be
explained by the theory that autistic individuals lack a proper conception of mental states:
first, they do not display affection or try to be nice, because they do not try to manipulate
other people's feelings towards them; secondly, they are totally egocentric because they do
not distinguish their own from other people's mental states and do not recognise that they
may differ; thirdly, they appear to be rude because they are unaware of the social niceties
that allow smooth mutual understanding. Likewise, the disobedience and bad behaviour
that seemed to be the major reasons for referral of Asperger's cases may arise from a defect
in understanding the effect of their behaviour on another person's mental state.
71
The potential link between disturbance of active attention, attachment to objects and
narrow preoccupations is referred to by Asperger as a poor relationship to the world of
objects. While Kanner makes a distinction between poor relations to people and good
relations to objects, Asperger points out examples of poor relations in both spheres.
82 HansAsperger
which singular areas develop to an excessive extent. The collections that are
favoured by autistic children appear like soulless possessions. The children
accumulate things merely in order to possess them, not to make something
of them, to play with them or to modify them. Thus, a six-year-old-boy had
the ambition to collect 1,000 matchboxes, a goal which he pursued with
fanatical energy. The mother, however, never saw him play trains with them
as other children do. Another boy collected cotton threads; a third
'everything' that he found on the street, but not like the street urchin, who
has everything in his trouser pocket that he might need for his pranks. The
autistic individual just stacks boxes full of useless junk. He constantly
orders things and watches over them like a miser. Thus, there are serious
rows when the mother dares to throw anything away. In adulthood the
passion for collections often becomes more interesting and selective, in short
more 'rational', and their mental attitude to collecting improves. The real
collector-type is often an eccentric with pronounced autistic traits. 7Z
Autistic children also do not have a proper attitude towards their own
bodies. It is often well nigh impossible to teach them the numerous
requirements of cleanliness and physical care. Even as adults they may be
seen to walk about unkempt and unwashed, including those who have taken
up an academic career. Up to the end of their childhood autistic children
tend to be extremely messy eaters. They may smear or 'paint' with their
food while being preoccupied with some strange problem of their own.
Another characteristic of autistic children is the absence of a sense of
humour.73 They do not 'understand jokes', especially if the joke is on them.
This is another reason for their often being the butt of teasing: if one can
laugh at oneself, one can take the edge off ridicule. However, autistic
children are rarely relaxed and carefree and never achieve that particular
wisdom and deep intuitive human understanding that underlie genuine
humour. When they are in a merry mood, as sometimes happens, then this
often strikes one as unpleasant. The mood is exaggerated and immoderate.
They jump and rampage around the room, infringe other people's space, are
aggressive and annoying. When making puns, however, autistic people
sometimes shine, and may even be highly creative. This can range from
simple word-play and sound associations to precisely formulated, truly
witty remarks.
Nevertheless, if one focused only on the features just described, one
would gain a false impression of the emotional side of autistic individuals.
7Z
Later examples of collections mentioned by Asperger include collecting toys and collecting
sewing thread. Collecting as a peculiarly autistic feature is not mentioned in Kanner's
original paper. This has since been frequently documented in autistic individuals of all levels
of ability.
73
This original observation of Asperger's has been amply confirmed by later case descriptions.
A sense of humour depends crucially on an intact ability to understand the use of language
in communication, that is, pragmatics.
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 83
There are also observations that do not show such a decidedly negative
picture.
Again and again, we have been surprised by the severe bouts of
homesickness of autistic children when newly admitted to the ward. At first,
this phenomenon did not seem to us to fit at all with the otherwise blatant
signs of emotional poverty. Ordinary children, even those who have a very
strong and genuine emotional bond to their family, adapt to their new
environment after a short period of grief. This is because they can soon feel
the love and care offered to them, and because they increasingly become
interested in the new environment and the various activities that fill their
days. Autistic children suffer from homesickness much more severely. For
days they may cry desperately, especially in the evenings, when the pain
always breaks out anew. They talk about their poor tormented parents and
about their home with the tenderest words with the mature language that
we have already mentioned and also with an exceptionally differentiated
emotion, which children of that age cannot usually express. In a peculiar
mixture of naivety and sophistication they give reason upon reason why
they cannot stay, why they definitely have to go home today. They write
imploring and quite shattering letters home. This all lasts very much longer
than the homesickness of normal children, until at last they too get used to
us and start to feel happy under the inescapable structure and guidance that
we impose. It is possible that an exceptional degree of bonding to the objects
and habits of the home, bordering on the obsessional, causes these children
to suffer so much at separation. Therefore, it may be their general limitation
in the normal freedom of action which lies at the root of this reaction.
Nevertheless, the phenomenon of severe homesickness shows that autistic
children are capable of strong feelings.
There are other examples. One boy, whose highly creative verbal
expressions have already been quoted, had two white mice for which he
cared tenderly, and which he preferred to all human beings, as he frequently
pointed out. This boy deeply upset his parents by his spitefulness and cruelly
tormented his little brother. There are similar examples of undoubted
emotional attachments to animals and also to particular people which can
regularly be observed in autistic children.
In view of these facts, the problem of the emotionality of autistic children
is made extremely complicated for us. In any case, the children cannot be
understood simply in terms of the concept 'poverty of emotion', used in a
quantitative sense. Rather, what characterises these children is a qualitative
difference, a disharmony in emotion and disposition. They are full of
surprising contradictions which makes social adaptation extremely hard to
achieve.
84 Hans Asperger
76
Asperger's c o m p a r i s o n of the sexes in terms of underlying t h o u g h t processes and interests is
very m u c h in accord with cultural stereotypes. As yet we have little scientific basis for these
widely held beliefs. So far there is no empirical evidence to suggest t h a t autistic boys differ
from autistic girls in terms of abstract thinking.
77
In his t e x t b o o k (1952) Asperger still maintains t h a t the only y o u n g girls he has seen with the
full clinical picture of autism are those w h o have acquired autism after presumed
encephalitis. T h a t y o u n g girls t o o can s h o w the typical Asperger variant of autism has been
well established. Several cases are described elsewhere in this volume. It remains true,
however, t h a t girls are vastly o u t n u m b e r e d by boys. A sex-linked m o d e of inheritance is
compatible with this p a t t e r n .
78
Epidemiological studies have n o t confirmed that autistic children are p r e d o m i n a n t l y only
children.
86 HansAsperger
79
This refers to the pressure which the fascist regime then p u t on families to p r o d u c e m o r e
children. This single r e m a r k is the only one in the whole paper to refer to a point of fascist
ideology at a time w h e n it w o u l d have been o p p o r t u n e to m a k e m a n y m o r e such references.
Apparently, acceptance of Asperger's thesis w a s delayed because he was n o t a party activist.
80
It has recently been s h o w n t h a t superimposition of schizophrenia on autism can occur. A
specific case is described by T a n t a m (chapter 5).
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 87
pathy is that psychopaths are people who suffer from themselves, and from
whom the environment suffers in turn. The latter part of the saying certainly
applies to autistic individuals but it is hard to know whether they suffer
from themselves. They are strangely impenetrable and difficult to fathom.
Their emotional life remains a closed book. Given their behaviour problems
in childhood, it is to be expected that their closest relatives or spouses find
them difficult to get on with. However, it is a different matter where their
work is concerned.
In the vast majority of cases work performance can be excellent, and with
this comes social integration. Able autistic individuals can rise to eminent
positions and perform with such outstanding success that one may even
conclude that only such people are capable of certain achievements. It is as if
they had compensatory abilities to counter-balance their deficiencies. Their
unswerving determination and penetrating intellectual powers, part of their
spontaneous and original mental activity, their narrowness and single-
mindedness, as manifested in their special interests, can be immensely
valuable and can lead to outstanding achievements in their chosen areas. We
can see in the autistic person, far more clearly than with any normal child, a
predestination for a particular profession from earliest youth. A particular
line of work often grows naturally out of their special abilities.
Here is an example. For almost three decades we were able to observe an
autistic individual from boyhood to manhood. Throughout his life he
showed grossly autistic behaviour. It was as if he never took any notice of
other people. He behaved so absent-mindedly that he often did not
recognise his closest acquaintances. He was extremely clumsy and gauche,
and there were all the difficulties we described earlier in learning to deal
with the practical chores of daily life. He remained awkward and socially
unconcerned in his demeanour. For instance, one could see him as a young
man sitting in the tram and picking his nose with great care and persistence!
When he was at school there were constant serious difficulties; he learnt or
did not learn as the whim took him. For languages he had no talent at all. In
secondary school he never advanced beyond the elementary grade of Greek
and was able to get by only on the basis of his other abilities.
Even as a toddler, one could see in him a most unusual and spontaneous
mathematical talent. Through persistent questioning of adults he acquired
all the necessary knowledge from which he then worked independently. The
following scene is reported from his third (!) year of life. The mother had to
draw for him, in the sand, a triangle [Dreieck or three-corner], a square
[four-corner] and a pentangle [five-corner]. He then took a stick himself,
drew a line and said 'And this is a two-corner [Zwei-eck], isn't it?', then
made a dot and said 'And this one is a one-corner [Ein-eck]'. All his play and
all his interest centred on mathematics. Before he even started school he was
able to work out cubic roots. It must be emphasised that the parents had
never drilled the child in calculating skills, but that the boy quite spontan-
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 89
eously, sometimes against the wishes of his teachers, forced them to teach
him these skills. In secondary school he surprised his teachers by his
specialised mathematical knowledge which had already advanced to the
most abstract areas. Thanks to this extraordinary talent, and despite his
impossible behaviour and failure in other subjects, he managed to advance
without having to repeat classes, and was able to take the university
entrance examinations. Not long after the start of his university studies,
reading theoretical astronomy, he proved a mathematical error in
Newton's work. His tutor advised him to use this discovery as the basis for
his doctoral dissertation. From the outset it was clear that he was destined
for an academic career. In an exceptionally short time he became an
assistant professor at the Department of Astronomy and achieved his
Habilitation.83
This case history is by no means exceptional. To our own amazement, we
have seen that autistic individuals, as long as they are intellectually intact,
can almost always achieve professional success, usually in highly specialised
academic professions, often in very high positions, with a preference for
abstract content. We found a large number of people whose mathematical
ability determines their professions: mathematicians, technologists, indus-
trial chemists and high-ranking civil servants. We also found some unusual
specialisations. For instance, there is a heraldry expert who is said to be an
authority in his field. There are also several musicians of considerable
stature who were observed by us when children. The superficially surprising
fact that such difficult and abnormal children can achieve a tolerable, or
even excellent, degree of social integration can be explained if one considers
it a little further.
A good professional attitude involves single-mindedness as well as the
decision to give up a large number of other interests. Many people find this a
very unpleasant decision. Quite a number of young people choose the
wrong job because, being equally talented in different areas, they cannot
muster the dedication necessary to focus on a single career. With the autistic
individual, on the other hand, the matter is entirely different. With collected
energy and obvious confidence and, yes, with a blinkered attitude towards
life's rich rewards, they go their own way, the way to which their talents
have directed them from childhood. Thus, the truth of the old adage is
proved again: good and bad in every character are just two sides of the same
coin. It is simply not possible to separate them, to opt for the positive and
get rid of the negative.
We are convinced, then, that autistic people have their place in the
organism of the social community. They fulfil their role well, perhaps
83
Asperger notes in 1952 that the young man in question has long since become a university
professor. Interestingly, the following sentence, which here states that the case is by no
means exceptional, is changed in the 1952 volume to say that this is a very exceptional case.
90 Hans Asperger
better than anyone else could, and we are talking of people who as
children had the greatest difficulties and caused untold worries to their
care-givers.84
The example of autism shows particularly well how even abnormal
personalities can be capable of development and adjustment. Possibilities of
social integration which one would never have dreamt of may arise in the
course of development. This knowledge determines our attitude towards
complicated individuals of this and other types. It also gives us the right and
the duty to speak out for these children with the whole force of our
personality. We believe that only the absolutely dedicated and loving
educator can achieve success with difficult individuals.
Conclusion
Now, at the end of the paper, one ought to discuss the literature, but this
would not be very fruitful at present. One should investigate in what way
the type of child described here relates to existing typologies. While I do
not believe in a perfect systematic typology, the concept of type can be
useful in certain cases, and this I have tried to prove in the present
investigation.
The literature on personality types certainly includes those who show
similarities to the autistic personality. There is E. Kretschmer's schizo-
thymous personality, E. R. Jaensch's disintegrated personality and, above
all, the introverted personality described by C. G. Jung. In the description of
the introvert, in particular, there is much that is reminiscent of the children
described here. Introversion, if it is a restriction of the self and a narrowing
of the relations to the environment, may well be autism in essence.85
However, none of the authors mentioned has anything to say about the
behaviour of their particular personality types in childhood. Hence the basis
for comparison is largely lacking, and the descriptions are situated on quite
a different level from ours. The debate will undoubtedly become more
fruitful when we know what becomes of our autistic children when they are
adults. This awaits a later comprehensive study, in which we intend not only
to research more fully the biological and genetic basis, but also to look at
development beyond childhood. This, then, will offer the opportunity to
84
T h e historical b a c k g r o u n d to this passionate defence of the social value of autism w a s the
very real threat of Nazi terror which extended to killing mentally h a n d i c a p p e d a n d socially
deviant people.
85
The remark that introversion may in essence be the same as autism is odd but also
fascinating in view of the fact that Asperger considered himself, and was considered by
others, to be a typical introvert. Aspects of autism are not alien to normal experience and
comparison to introversion may be relevant. However, it should not be overlooked that
there is a world of difference between a well-adjusted introverted personality (who does not
have to try hard to adapt) and an essentially no more than precariously adjusted autistic
personality (who is constantly struggling).
'Autistic psychopathy' in childhood 91
Asperger's References
Bleuler, E. (1922). Das autistisch-undisziplinierte Denken in der Medizin und seine
Ueberwindung. Berlin: Springer.
Bleuler, E. (1930). Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. 5th edn. Berlin: Springer.
Hamburger, F. (1939). Die Neurosen des Kindesalters. Vienna and Berlin: Urban &
Schwarzenberg.
Heinze, H. (1932). Freiwillig schweigende Kinder. Zeitschrift fur Kinderforschung,
40,235-56.
Jaensch, E. R. (1929). Grundformen menschlichen Seins. Berlin: Eisner.
Jaensch, E. R. (1936). Der Gegentypus. Leipzig: Barth.
Jung, C. G. (1926). Psychologische Typen. Zurich and Leipzig: Rascher.
Klages, L. (1936a). Grundlegung der Wissenschaft vom Ausdruck. 5th edn. Leipzig:
Barth.
Klages, L. (1936b). Die Grundlagen der Charakterkunde. Leipzig: Barth.
Kretschmer, E. (1928). Korperbau und Charakter. Berlin: Springer.
Schneider, K. (1934). Die psychopathischen Personlichkeiten. Leipzig and Vienna:
Deuticke.
Schroder, P. (1931). Kindliche Charaktere und ibre Abartigkeiten, mit erlduternden
Beispielen von Heinze. Breslau: Hirth.
Schroder, P. (1938). Kinderpsychiatrie. Monatsschrift fur Psychiatrie und Neurolo-
g/e, 99, 269-93.
References
Asperger, H. (1944). Die 'Autistischen Psychopathen' im Kindesalter. Archiv fiir
Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76-136.
Asperger, H. (1952). Heilpddagogik. Berlin: Springer.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Are autistic children 'behaviourists'? An examination of
their mental-physical and appearance-reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 19, 579600.
Bleuler, E. (1916). Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. Trans. A. A. Brill (1951), Textbook of
psychiatry. New York: Dover.
Sadly, Asperger never carried out this promised study. His later work was almost solely
concerned with consolidating his views and propagating the approach of remedial
pedagogics (Heilpadagogik).
92 HansAsperger
93
94 Lorna Wing
3. 'A fascination for objects, which are handled with skill in fine motor
movements'. Some children become intensely attached to specific objects or
collections of similar objects such as empty detergent packets, tin lids, dead
holly leaves, and show resistance to any interference with these possessions.
These objects are used only for repetitive activities such as making them spin
with amazing dexterity, twisting them into identical complex shapes or
arranging them in long straight lines, or are amassed for no obvious
purpose. Normal pretend play is conspicuous by its absence, or, if present, is
narrow, limited and repetitive in form.
4. 'Mutism, or a kind of language that does not seem intended to serve
interpersonal communication'. The latter includes immediate and delayed
echolalia, the reversal of pronouns ('you want biscuit' meaning 'I want. . .')
and the idiosyncratic use of words or phrases, often due to irrelevant
associations formed by one chance incident and remaining fixed thereafter.
Kanner (1943) gave the example of a child who invariably exclaimed
'Peter-eater' when he saw a saucepan. This association began at two years
old when his mother accidentally dropped a saucepan at the same time as
reciting a nursery rhyme to him, beginning 'Peter, Peter, pumpkin eater'.
Sometimes autistic children invent new words or adapt names, such as the
child, known to me, who called a dish-washing machine a 'gaslectric
dish-pie-er'.
Those who have at least some comprehension of language tend to make
literal interpretations of the meaning. For example, a boy, when asked
'Have you lost your tongue?' anxiously looked around for it, and a little
girl, being told to 'walk on ahead', stopped and touched her head as if
wondering how it was possible to obey that instruction.
Those who have speech use it repetitively and those with large vocabu-
laries tend to be long-winded and pedantic in their utterances. Kanner
quoted one child who asked endless questions about light and darkness.
When persuaded to answer some questions his replies were 'painstakingly
specific'. For example, he defined a balloon as 'made out of dried rubber and
has air in it and some gas and sometimes they go up in the air and sometimes
they can hold up and when they get a hole in it they'll bust-up; if people
squeeze they'll bust. Isn't it right?'
5. 'The retention of an intelligent and pensive physiognomy and good
cognitive potential manifested, in those who can speak, by feats of memory
or, in the mute children, by their skill on performance tests, especially the
Seguin form board'. Kanner explained failure on intelligence tests or
subtests as due to lack of co-operation. The islets of ability, as the special
skills came to be called, also include a remarkable memory for music, the
ability to play musical instruments, drawing with accurate perspective,
calculations based on calendars for past and sometimes future years, and
unusual skill in mental arithmetic, even, in some cases, extending to
negative numbers. These skills can be manifested from an early age.
Asperger's syndrome and Kanner's autism 95
In his early writings Kanner stated that the behaviour pattern was present
from birth, though he later modified this view (see below).
on, sometimes on fantastic themes. Although they were socially isolated, they
were not unaware of the existence of others, but their approaches tended to
be inappropriate and sometimes malicious in effect. Asperger described them
as odd in appearance in contrast to the alert, attractive look often mentioned
as typical of Kanner's children. He also commented on their 'originality of
thought' and the frequency with which their interests were 'canalised into
rather abstract subjects of little practical use' (Asperger, 1979).
Asperger, unlike Kanner, did not compile a list of essential diagnostic
criteria for his syndrome. The features mentioned here are extracted from
his clinical descriptions and discussions of cases.
From my own case material I include as illustration one case (already
published, Wing, 1981a) which is a typical example of Asperger syndrome,
showing social ineptness, communication problems and peculiar interests.
(Reprinted by permission of the publishers, Cambridge University Press.)
From early in his life he liked toy buses, cars and trains. He amassed a
large collection and would notice at once if a single item were missing. He
would also make models with constructional kits. He played with such toys
on his own for as long as he was allowed to. He had no other pretend play
and never joined in with other children. The interest in means of transport
has remained with him. In his spare time he reads factual books on the
subject, watches cars and trains and goes on trips to see trains with fellow
train enthusiasts. He has no interest in fiction or any other type of
non-fiction.
K. has been employed for many years in routine clerical work. He enjoys
his job and his hobby, but is very sad and anxious because he is aware of
his social ineptness and would like to have friends and to marry. He writes
many letters to advice columns in magazines, hoping for help with these
problems. His concern over what he terms his 'shyness'finallymade him
ask for help from a psychiatrist.
The WAIS gave K. an IQ in the low normal range, with similar verbal
and non-verbal scores. He was particularly poor at subtests requiring
comprehension of a sequence of events.
the position that the two syndromes were different in nature. Van Krevelen
(1971) produced a scheme of the points he considered to be the major
distinguishing features that, in his opinion, 'made it unmistakably clear that
early infantile autism and autistic psychopathy are two entirely different
nosological syndromes'. These points were: early infantile autism is mani-
fested in the first month of life, whereas autistic psychopathy is not
manifested until the third year of life or later; the child with early infantile
autism walks earlier than he speaks, speech is retarded or absent, and
language is not used to communicate, whereas in autistic psychopathy the
child walks late but speaks earlier and tries to communicate although in a
one-sided manner; in early infantile autism, eye contact is poor because, for
the child, other people do not exist and he lives in a world of his own,
whereas the child with autistic psychopathy evades eye contact and lives in
our world but in his own way; early infantile autism is a psychotic process
and the social prognosis is poor, whereas autistic psychopathy is a
personality trait and the social prognosis is rather good.
The major problem with this formulation is that, in clinical practice, the
features are not neatly divided as in this account; many, perhaps even most,
individuals manifest elements from both lists (this point is pursued in detail
in other parts of this chapter). Van Krevelen went on to suggest, on the basis
of Asperger's and his own observations of the personalities of the parents,
especially the fathers, that autistic psychopathy is a familial trait transmitted
via the male line and that early infantile autism occurs when a child who
inherits the trait also suffers organic brain damage. Thus, he recognised a
relationship between the two conditions even though he considered them to
be entirely different.
Wolff and Chick (1980) described a group of children with an abnormal
pattern of behaviour that they classified as schizoid personality disorder.
They defined the core characteristics as solitariness, rigidity of mental set
and an unusual or 'odd' style of communication, and considered the group
to resemble Asperger's syndrome in all respects except that a small minority
were girls. Wolff and Chick stated that, although there were similarities to
Kanner's autism, children with schizoid personality never showed what they
considered were the essential features of autism, namely, absent or impaired
language development with echolalia, lack of emotional responsiveness with
gaze avoidance, ritualistic and compulsive behaviour, all beginning under
thirty months of age. They agreed with Van Krevelen that Asperger's
syndrome is an unchanging personality trait, whereas changes can be seen in
autistic children over the years. Unfortunately, Wolff and Chick did not give
any details of the pre-school years of their 'schizoid' subjects, so it is difficult
to tell whether any typical autistic behaviour was present or absent at that
age.
Wolff and Barlow (1979) compared eight matched pairs of 'schizoid' and
well-functioning autistic children. They also included eight matched control
ioo Lorna Wing
children attending normal schools. The subjects were given tests of intelli-
gence, language, memory, cognitive processes and emotional constructs.
The results showed that there were some differences and some similarities
between the 'schizoid' and the autistic children and that, in general, the
'schizoid' group were intermediate in function between the autistic and the
normal children. The authors concluded that the study confirmed their
hypothesis that the two handicapped groups are distinct from each other
and from normal children, though the results could be interpreted as
evidence for the hypothesis of a continuum of autistic disorders, as
discussed later in this chapter.
Kay and Kolvin (1987), in their discussion of the borderlands of
childhood psychosis, considered that Asperger's syndrome was biologically
linked to childhood autism. However, they also hypothesised that the
syndrome was a type of 'personality disorder' whereas autism was a
'psychosis', but they did not define the meaning of these terms.
Nagy and Szatmari (1986) considered that Asperger's syndrome was the
same as schizotypal personality disorder found in adults (American Psy-
chiatric Association, 1980) but with onset in childhood. They also equated
it with the clinical picture in the group studied by Wolff and her colleagues
(Wolff and Barlow, 1979; Wolff and Chick, 1980) although these workers
used the term 'schizoid'. Nagy and Szatmari carried out a case record study
of twenty children with social isolation and oddities of behaviour. They
concluded that these children could be diagnosed as having pervasive
developmental disorders as well as fitting the criteria for schizotypal
personality. They noted that a subgroup may represent a mild form of
infantile autism despite the fact that the children concerned did not meet the
DSM-III criteria for this condition. (Nagy and Szatmari also suggested that
some of the children displayed symptoms of schizophrenia, which raises
questions concerning the specificity of the criteria used by the authors for
this diagnosis but that is another story.) However, in a later paper,
Szatmari et al. (1986) argued for the retention of the term Asperger's
syndrome, at least for the present, since firm evidence for the unity of
Kanner's infantile autism and Asperger's syndrome was not yet available.
They suggested that Asperger's group might comprise a subtype of children
with similarities to and differences from classic autism. Retaining the former
term would have the advantage of opening up a wider field of enquiry than
autism alone.
above 70, but not in those with lower IQs. Fifteen of the nineteen probands
with IQs above 70 had the clinical picture of Asperger's syndrome as well as
showing the DSM-III criteria for autism or pervasive developmental
disorder. The authors suggested that high- and low-functioning autism are
different conditions; Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism are
largely equivalent and have a predominantly familial aetiology, in contrast
to low-functioning autism in which there is a high incidence of evidence of
neuropathology.
Gillberg and Gillberg (1989) examined all six-year-old children who
were not mentally retarded living in Gothenburg in 1977. They found
fourteen individuals who had a combination of deficits in attention, motor
control and perception, referred to as the DAMP syndrome. The group
was followed up at ten, thirteen and sixteen years of age. Eight of the
fourteen children showed 'autistic type traits', that is, impairments of
social relationships, speech and language and stereotyped behaviours or
interests. One had the DSM-III-R criteria for autism, three had all the six
diagnostic features of Asperger's syndrome listed by the authors, and four
had three or four of these features. The authors reported that among
mentally retarded children in the same area studied in 1984, the only child
found with the diagnostic features of Asperger's syndrome was one mildly
retarded boy. The authors suggested that the clinical findings fitted the
hypothetical construct of a continuum of disorders with, at one extreme,
severely or profoundly retarded children with the triad of impairments
described by Wing and Gould (1979) (see below), followed by Kanner's
syndrome, then Asperger's syndrome and, towards the mildly handicapped
end of the scale, the DAMP syndrome (see chapter 4 for further details of
this argument).
The idea of a continuum fits very well with findings of family-genetic
studies. Burgoine and Wing (1983) published a clinical account of teenage
identical male triplets diagnosed as having Asperger's syndrome but who
had some typically autistic features in their histories and current behaviour.
They differed along a continuum in the severity of their impairments, the
degree to which they showed autistic features and the amount of peri- and
post-natal trauma each had experienced. Bowman (1988) described a family
in which two of the four sons satisfied the diagnostic criteria for autism
except that one had no history of speech delay, a third son had the features
of Asperger's syndrome, and the father was a loner with circumscribed
interests and odd language-use. Only the mother and the eldest son did not
show any clinical features of either syndrome. All the family were of normal
overall intelligence but all the males, including the eldest son, had an
unusually wide scatter of subtests on WISC or WAIS performance scores,
with highest scores on block design and object assembly and lowest on digit
symbol and picture arrangement. In the case of the two boys with the
autistic clinical picture, the mother had had ante-partum haemorrhages, but
Asperger's syndrome and Kanner's autism 103
she had had no problem with the births of the other two sons. A similar
family (the case of the lawyer) is presented in chapter 4.
Perhaps the strongest argument for a seamless continuum from Kanner
autism to Asperger's syndrome comes from clinical case material where the
same individual was typically autistic in his early years but made progress
and as a teenager showed all the characteristics of Asperger's syndrome. The
following (earlier published in Wing, 1981a) is such a case. (Reprinted by
permission of the publishers, Cambridge University Press.)
and citizens' band radio. Using the latter, he enjoys talking to a wide circle
of fellow enthusiasts despite his problem in sustaining face-to-face
interactions. He has no other friends and leads an isolated life.
editions, both the WHO and the APA emphasise the importance for
diagnosis of impairments of social interaction and communication and the
presence of repetitive stereotyped routines. In contrast, the definition
adopted by the Autism Society of America stresses disturbances in the rate
of appearance of physical, social and language skills; abnormal responses to
sensations; speech and language absence or delay; and abnormal ways of
relating to people, objects and events. This definition overlaps with the
WHO and APA systems, but would produce some significant differences in
deciding which children should be included as autistic and which excluded
(see table 3.3).
ders and the psychoses usually occurring in adult life (Waterhouse, Wing
and Fein, 1989).
The last question was dealt with by Kolvin and his colleagues (Kolvin,
1971; Kolvin et al. 1971ae). In a study of a series of children presenting
with childhood psychoses, these authors found that, on the basis of the
clinical phenomena, intelligence testing and personal and family history,
they could differentiate between children with autism and autistic-like
conditions on the one hand and on the other those with symptoms of the
adult psychoses, especially schizophrenia. The latter were rare and were
virtually never seen before seven years of age, while the former were
comparatively much more frequent and, in almost all cases, began before
three years of age.
With regard to the relationship to mental retardation, a number of studies
have shown that the clinical phenomena of infantile autism and of
autistic-like conditions can occur in association with any level of intelligence
as measured on standardised tests, though the majority of those affected are
mentally retarded, more than half being in the severely retarded range, even
though they may have isolated skills at a higher level (DeMyer et al., 1974;
Lockyer and Rutter, 1969; Wing and Gould, 1979).
A population study
In order to tackle the questions that were still unanswered and still the
subject of debate, workers in the MRC Social Psychiatry Unit decided to
carry out a study of children from one geographical area, Camberwell in
south-east London (Wing and Gould, 1979; Wing, 1981b, 1988).
The aim was to examine the full range of clinical phenomena in the
children in order to see if the syndromes named in the literature could be
identified and separated from each other and from other childhood
disorders. The children were then followed-up into adolescence or early
adult life to observe any changes that might occur over time. The children
selected were aged under fifteen years and were functioning as severely
mentally retarded (that is, were catered for or known to the services for
severe mental retardation) or were of any intelligence level and in any
service but had any one or more of the features described as occurring in
autism or autistic-like conditions.
The details of the method used are given in other papers (Wing and
Gould, 1978, 1979). The results showed that the children studied could be
divided into two main groups. Some were on the borderlines and were
difficult to classify but most could be assigned to one or other category with
reasonable confidence. One group comprised children who were normally
sociable in the light of their mental ages, although, because of the method of
selection for the study, they were all functioning as severely mentally
retarded even if they had IQs of 50 or above on some tests (see table 3.1).
Asperger's syndrome and Kanner's autism 109
The other group contained the children whose social interaction was
impaired and would have been abnormal for any mental age. This group
included all those with any autistic features and, because of the method of
selection, their intelligence levels covered the whole range from profoundly
retarded to normal, although most were mentally retarded. Most were also
especially impaired in language development, and examples of all kinds of
other specific developmental delays in cognitive and motor skills could be
found scattered among the children.
It was found that social impairment was closely associated with impair-
ment of two-way social communication and impairment of the development
of imaginative activities, especially those related to social understanding,
referred to by the authors as a 'triad' of impairments. Furthermore, when
these problems were present, the pattern of activities, instead of being
flexible and creative, was rigid, repetitive and stereotyped.
Each of these abnormalities was manifested in different ways in different
children. Impairment of social interaction could be shown as aloof indiffer-
ence to others, passive acceptance of approaches, or active but odd,
repetitive one-sided approaches. Impairment of communication (as distinct
from formal language skills) varied from absence of any attempts to
communicate, through communication of needs only, to repetitive question-
ing or lengthy monologues regardless of the responses of the listener.
Imaginative activities could be totally absent, copied from other children in
a meaningless way, or spontaneous but carried out repetitively in an
identical fashion regardless of suggestions from other children. The rigid
repetitive pattern of behaviour could be manifested as simple bodily
directed stereotypies such as rocking or tooth-grinding, collecting and
organising objects into meaningless patterns or insistence on repetition of
sequences of actions or routines, or they could be verbal or abstract such as
repetitive questioning or concentration on circumscribed interests in specific
subjects such as railway timetables, calendars, chemistry, complex arith-
metical calculations. No subject was too abstruse or bizarre to become a
special interest of someone in this group.
An attempt was made to identify the named syndromes and to fit each
socially impaired child into some diagnostic group. All the syndromes
mentioned in this chapter were represented among the socially impaired
children in the study but this exercise highlighted a number of problems.
First, the longer the list of essential diagnostic criteria, the fewer the children
that were eligible. Thus, as shown in table 3.T, seventeen children fitted
Kanner and Eisenberg's (1956) two criteria for infantile autism (see page
104) but only seven out of the seventeen fitted Kanner's original five criteria
(see page 93). Secondly, although it was possible to identify the diagnostic
criteria when present in typical form, for every item there were problems of
delimiting the borderlines. Thirdly, the criteria for different syndromes
overlapped so much that some children could be given two or more
no LornaWing
Sociable,
IQ Kanner's Kanner's Other functioning
(non-verbal syndrome becoming Asperger's socially as severely Total
tests) (two criteria) Asperger's syndrome impaired retarded in study
<5o 6 40 33 79
50-69 6 2 13 23 44
70+ 2 3 2 4 11
Total 14 3 4 57 56 134
Notes:
1. The total population of children aged under 15 years numbered approximately 35,000 at
the time of the study.
2. There were 29 non-ambulant children not included in the above table, 16 of whom were
also socially impaired.
3. The use of non-verbal tests gave, in most, though not all, cases, higher estimates of levels of
abilities than verbal tests.
4. The number of children with Asperger's syndrome with normal level of ability was an
underestimate (degree unknown).
Table 3.2. The autistic continuum (features most often used in diagnosis*
Item Manifestations15
1 Tend to be seen in the 4 Tend to be seen in the
most severely least severely
handicapped/retarded handicapped/retarded
a Social interaction Aloof and indifferent Approaches for physical Passively accepts approaches Makes bizarre one-sided
needs only approaches
b Social communication No communication Needs only Replies if approached Spontaneous, but repetitive,
(verbal and non-verbal) one-sided, odd
c Social imagination No imagination Copies others mechanically Uses dolls, toys correctly but Acts out one theme (e.g.
limited, uncreative, repetitive Batman) repetitively, may
use other children as
'mechanical aids'
d Repetitive pattern of self- Simple, bodily directed (e.g. Simple, object-directed (e.g. Complex routines, Verbal, abstract (e.g.
chosen activities face-tapping, self-injury) taps, spins, switches lights) manipulation of objects, or timetables, movements of
movements (e.g. bedtime planets, repetitive
ritual, lining up objects, questioning)
attachment to objects,
whole-body movements)
e Language-formal system No language Limited - mostly echolalic Incorrect use of pronouns, Grammatical but long-
prepositions; idiosyncratic winded, repetitive, literal
use of words/phrases; odd interpretations
constructions
f Responses to sensory Very marked Marked Occasional Minimal or absent
stimuli (over-sensitive to
sound, fascinated by
lights, touches, tastes,
self-spinning; smells
objects or people;
indifferent to pain, cold,
1
There arc other clinical features seen in disorders in the autistic continuum, but they are not mentioned in the various sets of criteria considered essential
for diagnosis.
h
The manifestations of each item (numbered t to 4 under each heading) are arbitrarily chosen points along a continuum. In reality, each shades into the
next without any clear divisions.
Table 3.3. Comparison of essential diagnostic criteria (the numbers refer to the features listed in table 3.2)
Asperger's
Abnormalities of Typical autisma syndrome Autistic
continuum
Kanner Lotter Rutter DraftICD-iob DSM-1HC DSM-UI-R3 Wing Draft ICD-10 Wing and Gould
1943 1966 1978 WHO, 1990 APA, 1980 APA,1987 1981a WHO, 1990 1979
a Social 1)2. 1,2 1,2 r,2-,3,4 1,2,3 1,2,3,4 3,4 1,2,3,4 1,2,3,4
interaction
b Social 1,2 1,2 i,2,3,4 1,2,3,4 1,2,3 1,2,3,4 3,4 1,2,3,4
communication
c Social i,3 1,2,3,4 1,2,3,4 3,4 i,2,3,4
imagination
d Repetitive 3 3 3,4 2,3,4 2,3,4 1,2,3,4 4 3,4 1,2,3,4
activities
e Language !,2.,3 1,^,3 4
f Sensory 1,2
responses
g Movements 1,2
h Special skills 3 4 4
i Other Attractive No Clumsy May be
appearance delusions clumsy
no organic or hallucinations No language
aetiology or cognitive
delay
Age of onset of
signs of abnormal
development 0-2 Vi 0-2 Vi 0-3 o-zVi 3 + , but may
be earlier
motor delay
a
Most workers imply that the characteristic features have to be present in early life, but may become less obvious or change in manifestation in middle or
later childhood. However, none has specified precisely the age up to which the features must be present to make a diagnosis, except Lotter, who specified up
to 7 or 8 years, and Rutter who specified up to around 5 years.
b
At least three examples from a, two from b or c and two from d must be present.
c
Examples (number unspecified) from a and b or e and d or f or h must be present.
d
At least eight examples, including at least two from a, one from b or c and one from d must be present.
Asperger's syndrome and Kanner's autism 115
When these aims are achieved it would be preferable to find new diagnostic
terms since those in current use overlap sufficiently to cause confusion, as
can be seen from the variations in definitions in different diagnostic systems
(see table 3.3).
Social interaction, communication and imagination are just three of a
large number of developmental skills, any of which can be absent, delayed
or deviant on their own or in combination with others. Impairment in any
such skill causes problems for the individuals concerned, so why should
particular attention be paid to abnormalities of social interaction? The
reason is that any degree of impairment of the social skills has a particularly
profound effect upon the development of the child as a whole person and
upon his or her chances of becoming an independent adult, able to work,
marry and raise a family. Only those socially impaired people who have few
or no other disabilities and who have sufficient skills and determination to
compensate for their problems manage to become reasonably well-
functioning and even, in a few cases, markedly successful as adults. The
majority need help and guidance in sheltered settings for all their lives.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders. 3rd edn. Washington: APA.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders. 3rd edn, revised. Washington: APA.
Anderson, M. (1986). Understanding the cognitive deficit in mental retardation.
journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 297306.
Asperger, H. (1944). Die 'Autistischen Psychopathen' im Kindesalter. Archiv fur
Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76136.
Asperger, H. (1979). Problems of infantile autism. Communication. 13, 45-52.
Bender, L. (1947). Childhood schizophrenia: clinical study of 100 schizophrenic
children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 17, 4056.
Bender, L. (1961). The brain and child behaviour. Archives of General Psychiatry', 4,
531-47-
Blank, M., Gessner, M. & Esposito, A. (1979). Language without communication: a
case study. Journal of Child Language, 6, 32952.
Bosch, G. (1970). Infantile autism. New York: Springer.
Bowman, E. P. (1988). Asperger's syndrome and autism: the case for a connection.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 15 2, 3 77-82.
Burgoine, E. & Wing, L. (1983). Identical triplets with Asperger's syndrome. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 2615.
Creak, E. M. (Chairman) (1961). Schizophrenic syndrome in childhood: progress
report of a working party (April 1961). Cerebral Palsy Bulletin, 3, 501-4.
Creak, E. M. (1964). Schizophrenic syndrome in childhood: further progress report
of a working party (April 1961). Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
6, 530-5.
Asperger's syndrome and Kanner's autism 119
Introduction
Asperger syndrome whatever it is cannot be dismissed at the drop of a
hat as 'mild autism' and thereby relegated to the status of eccentricities in a
textbook on child psychiatry. Since the seminal paper by Wing (1981),
interest and work in thefieldhave so grown that one senses the presence of a
true diagnostic entity at least from the clinical point of view which for
many years has haunted child and adult psychiatrists alike, variously
alluded to as 'autism in high-functioning individuals', 'MBD (minimal brain
dysfunction) with autistic traits', 'borderline personality', 'schizoid per-
sonality disorder' or 'schizotypal personality disorder'.
Hans Asperger, in his original 1944 paper (see chapter 2), described a
group of patients suffering from 'the autistic psychopathy of childhood'
who in some respects (particularly with regard to superior language skills,
perhaps also with respect to worse motor skills) appeared to differ markedly
from the group of cases described by Kanner in 1943. Van Krevelen, in
1971, made a devoted attempt to conceptualize autistic psychopathy as a
separate disorder. Nevertheless, Asperger's original descriptions did not, as
it were, catch on, until Lorna Wing drew attention to them in her 1981
paper, in which she acknowledged the usefulness of the term Asperger
syndrome while at the same time announcing that she considered it to be
one among several entities within the 'autism spectrum disorders'. Since
then, Schopler (1985) has argued against the use of the term, while Nagy
and Szatmari (1987) and Gillberg and Gillberg (1989) have albeit on
different grounds made claims for its usefulness.
In Gothenburg we recently carried out a clinical and neurobiological
study of twenty-three children (twenty-one boys and two girls) with
Asperger syndrome who were diagnosed according to specific criteria
(Gillberg, 1989). The criteria are outlined in tables 4.1 and 4.2. We
122
Six family studies 123
a
For full diagnostic criteria of infantile autism, with examples of behaviours see DSM-III
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980), pp. 87-90.
a
For full diagnostic criteria of autistic disorder with examples of behaviours see DSM-III-R
(American Psychiatric Association, 1987), pp. 389. Autistic disorder is classified as a severe
form of pervasive developmental disorder with onset in infancy or childhood (pp. 3 39).
compared them with twenty-three sex-, age- and IQ-matched children who
had been diagnosed as autistic on DSM-III criteria (table 4.3). These autism
cases also fulfilled DSM-III-R criteria for autistic disorder (table 4.4). The
present chapter examines three of the Asperger cases from this sample and
three more recent cases in detail. These cases and their families provide a
basis for discussion which is complementary to group studies. The paper
goes on to discuss the implications of the cases as regards the arguments on
Asperger syndrome as a clinical entity. Delineations vis-a-vis autism, and
the possible neurobiological basis of the disorder are also discussed.
In our study there were no requirements regarding the child's intellectual
level. This is in line with Asperger's own viewpoint, as is evident from the
clinical status of the patients he described. However, the six cases, with their
families, presented here are all of rather high general ability. This allows one
to see the most subtle features of Asperger syndrome unconfounded by
additional handicaps. For purposes of cross-reference the cases are given
aide-memoire labels.
Asperger syndrome
Asperger traits
^Kk Autism
contact with him for just over twenty years, from the time when he was
pre-pubertal. He was then a highly intelligent but very peculiar, tall and
skinny boy of twelve. He had no friends and did not seem to care. He had
the most amazing interest and knowledge in the field of Rommel's desert
wars: he knew the 'exact' numbers of soldiers who had been killed in
various battles and would enact the major battles in the same fashion over
and over again using a set of tin soldiers. His speech was almost hilariously
formal, and children and adults who did not know him often burst out
laughing on hearing him for the first time, believing him to be joking and
putting on a show (which, of course, he was not). His gait was stiff and all
his motor movements were awkward and ill co-ordinated.
He had been slow to start walking (sixteen months) and comparatively
slow as regards speech development. He did not say much until almost
three years of age, when in a very short space of time he started saying long
and complicated sentences. These perfectly echoed things he had heard his
mother say during the day. He never showed an interest in anything but
mechanical toys and electric equipment of various kinds. The parents state
that they never noticed any kinds of stereotypies but, at the age of
thirty-three, it is plain that 11:4 shows highly stereotyped repetitive
spreading of the fingers, hundreds of times each day, and has a tendency to
make slight but clearly stereotyped nods for no obvious reason. These
repetitive movements have been present since I first saw him. 11:4 does not
like water and the parents had a tough time trying to teach him to swim
when he was nine years old. However, he learnt to swim (in a very stiff
manner) one afternoon at 5 o'clock. The parents remember this very clearly
because, since that day, whenever he has been to their summer house he has
gone for a swim at 5 o'clock sharp.
He was perceived as odd by his parents in particular the father from
around the time when he was five years old and did not want to mix with
age-peers. Throughout childhood, adolescence and early adult life he has
remained completely without friends. He is now a lawyer and has to meet
126 Christopher Gillberg
people every day. This works out all right, he thinks, because he is very
interested in their 'cases' (various economical and financial problems). He
does not participate in social gatherings at or in connection with his work.
He has had one 'girl-friend', when he was twenty-three - a fellow student
whom he once invited for a Saturday to the parents' summer house, but
with whom he has had no further contact since. At the age of thirty-three
he seems to be perfectly happy with his job, his very restricted daily routine
and visiting his parents once a month. He has a small flat which he keeps
very neat and tidy. He is always clean and well dressed. His voice is a
high-pitched monotone, and his speech is still almost unbearably pedantic.
11:4 is f normal appearance and right-handed.
Unfortunately, it has never been possible to make a complete
neurobiological work-up in his case. However, the case is of great interest
because the family history reveals both Asperger syndrome and autism
among its members.
His eldest brother, II: i, ten years his senior, has Kanner-type autism and
mild mental retardation. He was first diagnosed when he was four years
old. He now lives in a group home for mildly mentally retarded people. He
belongs to that group of autistic individuals who are passive and friendly
(Wing, 1983). The mother had rubella during the first trimester of this
brother's pregnancy. The mother herself has several Asperger-type traits:
she does not have any friends, is very formal, pedantic, obsessive and has
difficulty understanding some abstract expressions in spite of overall very
good intelligence (she took a Ph D in history at the age of sixty, after having
been a housewife for twenty-five years). However, her personality would
probably have been seen as on the extreme end of a normality continuum
had it not been for her sons, one with autism, the other with Asperger
syndrome.
The middle brother, 11:2, also has several traits suggestive of Asperger
syndrome: formal, pedantic and staccato-type speech; difficulty mixing
socially with other people; and stiff, awkward gait. He is married and
works as a chief dentist in a laboratory. Tragically, hisfirst-bornson by a
healthy unrelated woman shows at the age of three all the signs of infantile
autism (and probably normal intelligence). This boy had severe perinatal
asphyxia.
The father of 11:4,1:2> a v e r v distinguished civil servant with a lifelong
interest in geography, is a quiet, warm but socially rather shy person with a
few very good friends with whom he has kept in contact throughout his life
(he is now seventy-two years old).
This family history suggests that there may be important genetic links
between Asperger syndrome and autism, at least in some cases. There are
similarities between this family and the one recently described by Bowman
(1988), in which there seemed to be a continuum of disorders ranging from
Asperger-type traits to typical Asperger syndrome and autism.
If Asperger-type traits have indeed been inherited in this family, pure
X-linked inheritance is excluded as a candidate mode of transmission unless
II: 2 has more than one X-chromosome. The fragile-X abnormality could not
otherwise account for thesefindings.The physical phenotype in all cases in
Six family studies 12.7
IV
Asperger syndrome
Asperger traits
Autism
Alzheimer's disease
(autopsy - verified)
Elective mutism
Fig. 4.2 Family 2
and store in the attic with such fervour that her parents became extremely
concerned. This interest was at its peak from the age of eight to ten years
and was then replaced, rather suddenly, by an almost fanatic
preoccupation with the idea that research on animals must be prohibited.
She would write long letters on this subject to various state officials, but
would not expect an answer. She had a pet, a guinea pig which she treated
in a rather careless fashion and which eventually died, presumably because
of starvation in connection with a family holiday when it had been left
behind for several days.
Throughout her early school years, IV: 1 insisted on wearing the same
dress every day (except at Christmas and Easter). Her mother had to wash
the garment secretly at night and then try to make it smell 'non-clean' in
order for the girl not to notice and throw a tantrum.
She had a very formal and repetitive way of responding to questions and
always ended each statement on a very high note. Much of what she said
spontaneously seemed to be exact repetitions of what she had heard other
people say. If asked 'What do you think?' she would invariably respond
'Oh, I don't know'. She had several 'friends' at the age of twelve but never
made any demands on them, except in so far as animal research was
discussed. She passively accepted what her peers proposed. She was well
liked but the parents saw how her friends marvelled at her complete lack of
intuitive notions about other people and deficient appreciation of irony and
her inability to understand such concepts. She was clumsy and her gestures
and movements ill co-ordinated. She got very poor reports for PE but
participated seemingly without self-criticism in spite of the fact that her
performance was 'comical' to say the least. She also had problems with
Six family studies i 29
Asperger syndrome
Asperger traits
From about the age of two years, it became obvious that III: 1 was
extremely pedantic in all his undertakings. He always demanded to sit in
exactly the same position at the breakfast table and to wear the same red
trousers every day. His mother had to wash them at night when he had
gone off to sleep. Afterwards, he would complain for hours that the
trousers smelt and felt 'wrong'.
From around the age of four years he began to concentrate on
circumscribed interests, first aeroplanes, then trams then trains and now
chess and stage productions. These interests have always excluded all other
activities, at least until very recently when he had to some extent,
successfully - taken part in an amateur actors' group.
His manner of speech is very pedantic and formal. Up till about the age
of fourteen he always echoed by whisper what people said to him.
Nowadays he always hums before he answers a question. Asked why, he
says that 'under the hum' he repeats your question.
Children at school have always found him strange and even 'weird'.
Adult people such as the parents' friends, on the other hand, have always
found him amicable.
At eighteen he has no friends, attends a normal school (achieving like
average teenagers) and considers going on to university to study trains! He
shuts himself in his room most of the time when he is not at school, except
when he goes out to his actors' group.
He is the firstborn of three. His younger brother has very mild mental
retardation, an operated cleft palate and a tendency towards both
hyperactivity and obsessional traits. His sister, who is ten years younger, is
healthy in all respects. Sadly, his mother died of cancer when he was fifteen
years old. Before she died he said to her in his very formal way: 'How
unfortunate that you should have cancer.' His father has mild Asperger
132 Christopher Gillberg
^2 Asperger traits
\p\ Suicide
has a social handicap. The boy's eleven-year-old brother is normal but has
suffered from recurrent depression since the age of seven. Both paternal
grandparents suffered from severe recurrent depressions. A paternal aunt
suffers from 'paranoia', according to psychiatric hospital records.
The boy's psychosocial circumstances are the best possible and the
family is well-to-do. His mother took very good care of him at home during
his first three years of life. Before the work-up in connection with
diagnosing Asperger syndrome, he had been admitted to hospital once for a
two-day period at the age of three. Apart from this and half a dozen
middle-ear infections, he has so far been physically well.
From the neurobiological point of view, there are several interesting
features in this case. The family history is one of Asperger syndrome,
depression and 'paranoia' on the paternal side of the family. There are
signs of brainstem dysfunction on the ABR, and the CSF mono amine
balance is similar to that encountered in autism.
Clinically, this case is interesting in that it shows many similarities with
autism. Also of particular importance is that this boy seemed at first to be
very severely handicapped in the field of expressive language skills, an area
in which after a few years' follow-up (into primary-school age) he excelled
on formal testing. A diagnosis of mild mental retardation was suspected
when he was four years old, but this is no longer so at the age of six when
he scores above average in many areas. A particularly interesting point is
that on formal testing with the Griffiths test his gross motor skills are
average. Yet the clinical impression is one of considerable motor
clumsiness.
In summary, this case illustrates clearly that autism and Asperger
syndrome are sometimes not clearly separable diagnostic entities.
Typical from very early on were his obstinacy, impatience and 'lack of
respect for other people's integrity' (mother's words). He needed little sleep
(much less than his sister and his parents). He appeared to be restless,
fixated on his own interest, 'not listening', hyperactive and his attention
could rarely be gained. Around fourteen months of age he was placed in a
day nursery where he remained until seven, the normal starting age for
school. He never showed an interest in the other children. He was regarded
by the pre-school teachers as 'domineering'.
From the age of three he showed an extreme interest in mathematics and
before age four years he knew how to add, subtract, multiply and divide
three-figure numbers.
At the age of seven, when Ifirstsaw him, he could instantly extract the
square root of any number under 200. His only interest in life is
mathematics and he turns everything into a mathematical problem. He
solves the problem but is totally uninterested in any connecting aspects and
fails to see similarities between problems which are not completely
identical. His skills in the mathematical area at the age of seven
corresponded to those offirst-yearuniversity students in mathematics
according to psychological testing and comparison with current university
norms. The parents described him as totally disinterested in other children
and minimally interested in members of his own family. He would treat
people as objects, move them to suit his needs and bite them if they did not
comply with his demands. He could not tolerate changes of routine, but if
changes could be explained in terms of the solution to a mathematical
problem, he reluctantly agreed to accept them. He incessantly asked the
same set of questions and did not really care what the answers were. His
gaze was very stiff and 'non-reciprocal'. He laughed when other people
cried. He has always had great difficulties understanding human facial
expressions. He was described as having had a very early development of
speech (considerably earlier than the other children in the day nursery).
From about the age of four he has often expressed himself by way of
neologisms. Very often he has spoken only in sentences in which every
word begins with the same letter. His voice has always (from the first
month of life) been a little hoarse and his intonation flat.
Six family studies i37
His gross motor performance was very poor and he could not manage to
hop on one foot more than two or three times. Diadochokinesis
performance corresponded to that of a five-year-old (at the age of seven
years nine months). His WISC score was 108 (performance 100 and verbal
188). His poorest performance was in coding (82) and his best in block
design and arithmetic (138). On the ITPA his scores range from (visual
memory) to auditory reception (118) and visual analogy (125).
His mother is very matter-of-fact, highly obsessional and always takes
notes even when conversing with acquaintances. Both parents have traits
reminiscent of Asperger syndrome, and the mother in particular is formal,
pedantic and shows circumscribed interests. However, she is deeply
devoted to her son's progress and has always tried to care for him in the
best possible way. She has suffered two episodes of major depression.
The boy now attends a special class for bright children with autism and
autistic-like conditions. He started school in normal class but his dislike of
the other children and theirs of him made it impossible for him to continue
there.
It is likely that the hypothyroidism which remained untreated for the first
seven weeks of the boy's life has played some part in the pathogenetic chain
of events in this case. It is equally likely that hereditary factors have played
some part. The hypothyroidism may well have acted by way of producing
hyperbilirubinemia (which the boy had), which in turn could perhaps have
made Asperger-sensitive brain areas dysfunctional.
1
I
Asperger syndrome
Asperger traits
secretary . . .', but he did not seem to listen and, instead, immediately
asked, 'How many letters per second can you type?' My secretary said:
'Well, it used to be 1100 in three minutes.' He then proceeded to her desk,
made a quick computation and shouted: 'Six point one one one one one
one in all eternity one one one.' My secretary looked astounded and said:
'Is that so?' He then pointed at me and stared into thin air and said: 'He
does look like Christopher Gillberg! What a coincidence!'
He then entered my office and started picking out various books and
papers from one of the shelves. Quite by chance he found a Swedish leaflet
for parents on Asperger syndrome. He said: 'This is something I've never
heard anybody say a word about before. I think I'll call it AS for short.' On
reading the text aloud he soon remarked, as though in passing: 'It seems I
have AS! By golly, I do have AS. Wait till my father hears about this!' He
went on reading and soon decided: 'My parents just might have AS too,
you know, my father in particular, he too has all-absorbing interests
and . . .' He did not seem to react emotionally to what he read. 'Now I can
tell my classmates the reason why I pace the school-yard briskly ten times
up and down each break all the year round is I have AS. And it will get my
teacher off my back. If you have a "handicap condition" they have to
tolerate you.'
111:2 was the firstborn child to healthy unrelated parents of superior
intelligence who both show mild but clear traits of Asperger syndrome
(the father in particular). A little sister, seven years of age, also has
Asperger-type traits but nothing near as severe as those of her elder
brother. There are reports of extreme Asperger traits in both the maternal
and the paternal grandmothers. Pregnancy and delivery were uneventful.
111:2 was a little slow to start walking (fifteen months) and spoke very little
before two and a half years but then started 'conversing with adult
strangers' almost at once. He showed virtually no interest in age-peers
other than as short-cuts to computers, his favourite pastime. His way of
talking was very fast and formal, 'like a professor'. When very young he
showed a number of stereotyped hand movements and grimaced in
stereotyped ways. He soon started to amass facts about chemistry and at
Six family studies 139
Discussion
These six case histories of Asperger syndrome were chosen to illustrate
various clinical and neurobiological issues which might be relevant for a
discussion concerning the delineation of the syndrome vis-a-vis autism, the
changing manifestations over time and possible aetiological pathogenetic
mechanisms. They do not constitute a representative sample of children
with Asperger syndrome but are fairly typical of the series of twenty-three
patients seen in my own practice (Gillberg, 1989). The last three cases in
particular are extraordinarily similar to the cases describea by Asperger.
The first three show the developmental course to adulthood of some
relatively mild cases and there is the case of a girl with Asperger syndrome
whose clinical picture is also rather mild. In the absence of population-based
studies of Asperger syndrome these cases will have to serve as a reasonable
clinical sample for formulating hypotheses which can be put to the test in
future epidemiological studies.
From the diagnostic point of view, two of the cases (from families 3 and
4) fulfilled DSM-III-R criteria for autistic disorder in early childhood but
later better fitted the prototype of Asperger syndrome. This is similar to
what Wing (1981) reported in her series of thirty-four patients, some of
whom had been autistic in the pre-school years but who later appeared to be
classic cases of Asperger syndrome.
The main question that demands discussion is the relationship of
Asperger syndrome to autism. It is clear that Asperger syndrome overlaps
with autism in several key aspects, perhaps most notably with regard to
some of the core symptoms, the persistence of handicap for many, many
years and certain associated neurobiological markers.
Let us look at the core symptoms first. It is striking that all six cases
described here show a reduced capacity for conceiving of other people as
creatures who think and feel. This symptom has recently been proposed as a
core deficit that underlies all autistic spectrum disorders, and this proposal
is presented in detail in chapter 1. In particular, it is argued that this deficit
140 Christopher Gillberg
very precisely accounts for the quality of the social impairment of autistic
people.
We turn next to the symptom of language problems. All six cases had
language problems, in particular with respect to semantics, pragmatics and
comprehension. These same problems are also always encountered in
autism. However, unlike individuals with autism all had good or very good
expressive language skills at a level which far exceeds that usually found in
autism. Furthermore, they all had developed a near-normal level of speech
by the age of five.
It is often surmised that stereotypies are particularly typical of autism
with mental retardation. Indeed severe motor stereotypies, such as exces-
sive hand-flapping and rocking, are probably more often encountered
when autism and mental retardation coincide. Nevertheless, it is clear that
stereotypic motor behaviours occur in Asperger syndrome too. All six
cases described showed pronounced behaviour of this kind in early child-
hood (hand-flapping, head-banging and tiptoeing in one case each). How-
ever, they later learnt how to hide these behaviours and perform them only
in socially acceptable settings. Nevertheless, some still exhibited clear-cut
repetitive finger or hand stereotypies or stereotyped facial grimacing (in
one case each). Obviously, even though stereotypies of this kind in adult
age are less conspicuous (and perhaps (?) less frequent) in Asperger
syndrome than in typical autism cases, they cannot be used as a differen-
tiating criterion.
Perhaps the most striking feature of Asperger cases is their odd all-
absorbing interests. It may be thanks to their normal intelligence that they
have turned those interests into areas of expertise in which they shine.
Clearly, comparison with less able autistic individuals would be unfair in
this respect. Nevertheless, autistic people too, even some who are totally
mute and aloof, can develop rare circumscribed interests and skills, such as
drawing. While the present symptom itself does not discriminate autism
from Asperger syndrome, the attempts to impose the interest (or any special
routines) on others in everyday life appears to be particularly pronounced in
Asperger cases. However, the nature of this symptom is as yet too little
understood for specific claims to be made.
Finally, with regard to possible distinguishing features in clinical diagno-
sis, the issue of gross and fine motor skills needs to be discussed.
Unequivocal cases of classical Kanner autism usually exhibit relatively
excellent skills in the motor domains, relative that is to their overall severe
deficits in so many otherfields.Asperger syndrome cases, on the other hand,
are often described as showing clumsiness, a lack of co-ordination of
movements and a stiff, awkward gait. In a recent comparison of twenty-
three Asperger syndrome cases and twenty-three sex- and age-matched
Kanner syndrome cases, motor clumsiness did indeed appear to be a
hallmark of the former and not of the latter (Gillberg, 1989). Motor
Six family studies 141
with the general criteria for Asperger syndrome outlined at the beginning of
this chapter.
However, children with Asperger syndrome do not come all in one shape
with regard to early symptoms and this is well illustrated by the six cases.
Some present with a suspicion of autism, but others do not cause major
worry or concern until well into their school years. This is not to say that
such cases do not have Asperger-type problems from very early in their
development, but there must exist relatively wide inter-individual variation
with regard to severity of problems.
Szatmari, Bartolucci and Bremner (1989) have made claims for a
distinction of autism vis-a-vis Asperger syndrome on the basis of well-
developed early attachment behaviour and absence of severe language
problems in the latter. In all the six cases described here and in a majority
of the clinical series of twenty-three referred to previously (Gillberg, 1989)
according to interview with the mother, early attachment behaviours were
not quite normal, and neither was the child's language development. I do
not think that at present we have a firm enough basis for subdividing cases
with 'autism spectrum' or 'autism associated' disorders according to early
attachment behaviour and language development. Again, it appears that, at
least with regard to disturbed attachment behaviour, which can be seen as
one aspect of overall social impairment, quantitative rather than qualitative
differences between autism and Asperger syndrome prevail.
In respect of language development, there are certainly considerable
differences with regard to the level of skills between cases with classic
Kanner type autism and classical Asperger syndrome when examined in
later childhood, but differences may be less pronounced in early childhood
(perhaps in particular with regard to language comprehension) at an age
when laymen do not yet react strongly to what they perceive as subtle
problems. Many authors, including Hans Asperger, stress the excellent
verbal skills of children with Asperger syndrome to such an extent that all
the major problems (major, that is, in relation to overall IQ and other skills)
in the field of semantics, pragmatics and comprehension in general are
obscured. Seventeen of the twenty-three children with Asperger syndrome
seen in my clinic over the last ten years (Gillberg, 1989) were slow to start
talking, according to parental report. Once they started talking, however,
their speech and language soon turned adult-type and formalistic. This
perfect quality of their speech has tended to detract from the proneness of
many of them to concrete misinterpretations.
One boy of fifteen reacted to his mother's statement 'She's on the way to
getting better' by asking 'Whereabouts is that?' He then put a number of
questions which showed that he believed that 'the way to getting better' was
a street address. This boy had a full scale WISC IQ at the age of fourteen of
132. Many more examples of concrete misinterpretation are given in
chapter 7.
Six family studies 143
Four of the six children showed late speech development and the fifth had
echolalia. All the cases had problems with regard to prosody and language
comprehension. Other speech-language characteristics included whispering
echolalia (cases 2 and 4) repetitive questioning (all six cases) and galloping
speech (case 6). Thus if one accepts the diagnosis of Asperger syndrome in
these six cases, one would also have to accept that it is impossible to
subdivide cases of Asperger syndrome and autism according to absence or
presence of language problems, which seems to be what Szatmari and his
group (Nagy and Szatmari, 1987) suggest.
The careful neurobiological analysis of the six cases described here (table
4.5) does not yield a consistent picture except in so far as Asperger-type
problems heredity is concerned. All six cases had a close relative with
Asperger syndrome (this was true for three of the cases) or Asperger-type
traits (which was true for the other three). Two even had close relatives with
autism. It is relatively uncommon for parents of children with classical
autism to be themselves autistic-like or Asperger type. Four out of five
examined cases showed signs of brainstem pathology according to auditory
brainstem response examination. One case was probably triggered by the
effects of congenital hypothyroidism and hyperbilirubinemia in the first
seven weeks of life. The neurobiological problems encountered in these six
cases are similar to, but not as pronounced as those seen in children of
near-normal or normal IQ who have been diagnosed as suffering from
autism. In the study of twenty-three Asperger syndrome and twenty-three
children with autism referred to above, major indications of brain
damage/dysfunction were found in 6 1 per cent of the former and 78 per cent
of the later group.
One hypothesis which could account for most of the findings in the field
so far is that we are dealing with three groups in this connection: (1) Kanner
autism which is usually the result of environmentally determined brain
damage or specific hereditary factors causing brain dysfunction; (2) Asper-
ger syndrome which is usually caused by polygenic hereditary influences;
and (3) a clinically less specific group with traits of both conditions, a
developmental course during which both diagnoses are considered and a
background of both hereditary Asperger-type problems and laboratory
indications of brain damage.
The early histories of at least four of these cases seemed to indicate
attentional problems early in life. All six cases had some motor-control or
motor-perceptual problems. Courchesne (1987) has suggested that autistic-
type disorders could arise as a consequence of disruption of normal
development of attentional processes early in life, followed by development
only of some higher brain centres which have been spared from the effects of
stimulus deprivation due to the attentional deficits. Brainstem pathology
was common in the present material and could mirror damage to crucial
'neural attentional circuitry1.
144
First or second
degree relative Recurrent
with Asperger depressions in
Age at syndrome (AS) First degree first and second
examination or Asperger relative with degree relatives Late speech EEC Brainstem CT-scan Medical
Case (years) Sex IQ-V IQ-P traits (AT) autism development pathology dysfunction pathology diagnosis Other comment
b
12-33 M ? AS mother Brother no Familial
AS brother Nephew (but echo) clustering of AS
and autism
5 7-9 M 118 100 AT both parents no yes yes yes +ABRa Hypo- Neonatal hyper-
WISC (moderate thyroidism bilirubinemia
increase of (congenital) -
low treated from
frequency age 7 weeks
activity)
6 12 M > i 5 o > i 5 o AT both parents no (yes)e yes no +ABR a
WISC (mild in mother}
a
ABR = auditory brainstem response examination (+ = pathology).
b
IQ exceeds 130 according to clinical judgement.
c
indicates 'not performed'.
d
Fulfilled DSM-III-R criteria for autistic disorder in early childhood.
e
Severe recurrent depression in several paternal relatives according to maternal report.
Six family studies 145
It has been suggested that there may exist a spectrum of autistic disorders
(Wing, 1989). The disorders range from severe specific social impairment in
conjunction with severe mental handicap, through Kanner's syndrome
(autistic disorder), with moderate mental retardation and Asperger syn-
drome, in cases with low to normal or normal intelligence, to even more
subtle social deficits seen in children with so-called DAMP (deficits in
attention, motor control and perception) - often referred to as 'MBD-type
problems' (minimal brain dysfunction) (Gillberg and Gillberg, 1989). The
overlap both with autism and DAMP seen in several of the present cases
could be seen as supportive of such a continuum.
Nevertheless, no firm conclusions can be drawn on the basis of the data
presented here except that there is a great need for continued epidemiologi-
cal clinical and neurobiological research in the field of autism spectrum
disorders. The nature of the social deficits and attentional problems
encountered in individuals, either from within or outside the autistic
spectrum, warrants systematic study now.
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders. 3rd edn. Washington: APA.
Asperger, H. (1944). Die 'Autistichen Psychopathen 1 im Kindesalter. Archiv fur
Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 1 17, 76-136.
Bowman, E.P. (1988). Asperger's syndrome and autism: the case for a connection.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 15 2, 3 7782.
Courchesne, E. (1987). A neurophysiological view of autism. In E. Schopler & G.B.
Mesibov (eds.), Neurobiological issues in autism. New York: Plenum.
DeMyer, M.K. (1979). Parents and children in autism. Washington: Winston.
Gillberg, C. (1989). Asperger syndrome in 23 Swedish children. Developmental
Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 52031.
Gillberg, C. & Svennerholm, L. (1987). CSF-monoamines in autistic syndrome and
other pervasive developmental disorders of early childhood. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 151, 89-94.
Gillberg, I.C. & Gillberg, C. (1989). Asperger syndrome - some epidemiological
considerations: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30,
631-8.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, z,
217-50.
Nagy, J. &c Szatmari, P.A. (1986). A chart review of schizotypal personality
disorders in children. Jorunal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16,
351-67.
Schopler, E. (1985). Convergence of learning disability, higher-level autism, and
Asperger's syndrome. (Editorial.) Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
ders, 15,359.
Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G. &c Bremner, R. (1 989). Asperger's syndrome and autism:
146 Christopher Gillberg
It's only by logic and lack of emotions that I get through. Hiding feelings
came after I became the victim. All emotions are a sign of weakness. I'm
about as flexible as a thick bar of metal in a barrel of nitrogen . . . I shall
turn out a mechanical, inflexible person who [sic] nobody likes, nobody
loves and who everyone will be glad about when I'm in my grave. I'd only
be concerned with money . . . It's a vicious circle, r. I get teased z. I make
myself miserable and cynical 3. I get teased again . . . The best school
would be one where I spent my time working with machines remove the
human factor. If the people were very nice I could probably do very well.
What I find difficult about learning, as well as the teasing, is that there's a
massive great group of us and they're all unruly . . . I can break out of the
vicious circle, but I can't take down the barriers. The clay has set I've
moulded my personality. The wall's there for good. I'm no good at
changing. My flexibility was one of the first things I lost lost completely.
147
148 DigbyTantam
In adulthood
Lack of non-verbal expressiveness, associated either with 1. idiosyncratic facial
expressions, gestures, voice prosody or posture; or 2. an inability to recognise socially
important cues; or 3. both.
Unusual 'special1 interests which are narrow and private. The special interest may be
idiosyncratic or pursued obsessively, or both. Special interests often involve collecting
objects or memorising facts.
Difficulty in behaving according to socially accepted conventions, particularly when these
conventions are normally implicit.
Pragmatic abnormalities of speech.
Lack of close peer relationships often but not always, as a result of social advances being
rebuffed by peers.
Impression of clumsiness.
In childhood
Symptoms as above, or symptoms of autism.
If childhood history unavailable, symptoms cannot be attributed to psychosis occurring
after early childhood.
Asperger triad described above. Patients who were unable to give a clear
account of themselves and their families were excluded from the study so
that only able autistic people were included, as in the Nottingham study; 6j
per cent of them were currently socially impaired (see below for details), 91
per cent had marked abnormalities of non-verbal expression currently, and
95 per cent had unusually restricted or asocial 'special' interests currently.
Corresponding figures for the remaining fourteen, non-autistic, subjects
were: 14 per cent, 38 per cent and 43 per cent. Ninety per cent of the autistic
subjects had both abnormal non-verbal expression and special interests
currently, compared to only 8 per cent of the non-autistic group with this
combination.
The Nottingham and MRC samples were collected at the same time, but
only one case is known to have been included in both. Comparison of the
two samples (table 5.2) shows the expected preponderance of men, but the
ratio of approximately six men to one woman in the MRC study is lower
than that reported in Nottingham and by others (for example, Gillberg,
1989). There were more parents with professional or managerial occupa-
tions than in the general population in both samples, but this may be an
effect of selection bias (see Wing (1980) for a general discussion). Newson,
Dawson and Everard's subjects were more likely to have been previously
diagnosed as autistic, to have had special schooling (usually in schools
specialising in autism) and to have had language impairments in infancy.
The age at which their child's problems were first acknowledged by parents
was also lower.
Other findings quoted in this chapter are from the MRC study unless an
alternative citation is given.
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 15 1
spent much of each day checking that there was no one at the front
door, that the tablets in the bathroom cabinet were as he had left them
and that the bath was not full of water. He was perturbed by intrusive
thoughts that he might kill someone or that he had already done so
without realising it. He sometimes thought that he was telepathic. He
never made eye contact at interview, but his mutual gaze when talking
to peers was normal. His major interest was in pop groups, about
which he was knowledgeable, football and television. He collected
miniature liqueur bottles and stamps.
Asperger Non-Asperger
% affected % affected
interviewer, for example hailing the doctor, at a first meeting and across a
crowded room, by his first name. Another abnormality of perspective was a
lack of the guardedness which would have been appropriate to the
beginning of an interview with a stranger and a lack of curiosity about the
purpose and consequences of the interview. Abnormal choice of topic was
rated as 'idiosyncrasy' or 'fanaticism', depending on how forcefully the
topic was imposed on the conversation and how odd it was. Ratings were
also made of the assumption by the subject of knowledge which the
interviewer could not be expected to have, an example of the failure to use
the illocutionary act of 'introduction', and of gaucheness, a residual
category of pragmatic abnormality.
Pragmatic abnormalities of the type described occurred in the speech of
two-thirds of the Asperger group (table 5.3) and were significantly more
common in the subjects with lower verbal IQ scores and clumsiness.
Semantic and syntactic abnormalities tended to be more common in subjects
with lower verbal IQ, but were not associated with clumsiness. Nor was
there a tendency for semantic and pragmatic abnormalities to occur more
frequently together.
Semantic abnormalities were associated in the MRC study with deviant
language development, lower verbal than non-verbal IQ, and presence of
Rutter's (1978) criteria for autism in early childhood, but not with
abnormality of non-verbal expression (table 5.4). These associations can be
interpreted as indications that the syntactic and semantic abnormalities are
a language handicap which is distinct from the handicaps associated with
pragmatic errors.
The severity of language handicap was significantly (p<.O5) associated
with an excessive use of routines in childhood and an early age of
recognition of abnormality, these associations probably contributing to a
highly significant (p<.oi) association with a history of Kanner syndrome.
On a principal components analysis of ratings on the various standard-
ised assessments used in the study (table 5.5), 60 per cent of the variance of
the scores of the combined ratings of the subjects in the MRC study was
156 DigbyTantam
the two-minute exposure, and between viewing with and without sound.
The rating of oddness was, however, almost completely restricted to the
Asperger subjects, about whom it was common: 89 per cent of the general
professional audience, for example, rated the Asperger subjects as odd,
whereas only 3 per cent gave this rating to any of the control subjects. What
conveyed the impression of oddness? Raters gave the following reasons: odd
posture, lack of expressiveness, unusual clothes and, in the case of the sound
playback, unusual speech patterns.
When the assessments of the forty-six subjects with Asperger syndrome
were compared to the fourteen isolated and eccentric subjects without a
developmental disorder, a similar pattern of expressive abnormality emer-
ged (see table 5.3). Abnormal non-verbal expression is common among
Asperger subjects, and co-occurs with clumsiness. It is also more common
than speech abnormality, but less specific.
Non-verbal expressive abnormalities were rated in the MRC study using
a specially designed rating instrument (the express scale). This scale differed
from otherwise similar rating scales for negative symptoms in schizophrenia
(for example, Andreasen, 1989) in differentiating abnormal expressions
from an abnormal lack of expression. Abnormalities of speech prosody,
facial expression, gaze and gesture made the largest contribution to the
rating of abnormal non-verbal expression. More detailed analysis of
video-recorded interviews has shown that the frequency of expressive
movements, including other-directed gaze, does not differentiate Asperger
and non-Asperger subjects, nor are there forms of expression that are
restricted to Asperger syndrome subjects (Tantam, Holmes and Cordes, in
press). Our analysis suggests that the impression of abnormality results, in
fact, from a lack of integration of expression, speech and gaze. This can
have the effect of making an ordinary gesture seem incongruous or
inexplicable. We postulate at the moment that the lack of integration results
from a failure to orientate, by gaze or change of expression, to actions by
158 Digby Tantam
Special interests
These self-selected leisure activities are both unusually narrow and unusu-
ally engrossing. They are pursued privately and with no eye to their social
implications. They often involve an element of systemisation or repetition.
The simplest interests, usually found in the least intellectually able, involve
either routines or collections. Examples of routines are arranging a particu-
lar toy car-track in a certain configuration and putting every object in the
sitting-room into a particular place every day. The collections of some of the
people with Asperger syndrome I have met have included toy mobile cranes,
records and photographs of cats, pictures and books about cathedrals, TV
Times issues, maps, pictures of trains and of car washes. More complex
interests involve memorising facts, producing stereotyped drawings or lists
or pursuing a detailed line of abstract study. Examples of facts memorised
by people with Asperger syndrome include carrot varieties, rose varieties,
boxing records, heights of tallest buildings, dates of historical events, the
Dewey Decimal Classification, names of jockeys, bus routes, addresses of
courts and the livery of Great Western trains.
The following case history contains an example of a special interest.
Rosalind, the daughter of a music teacher, had a particular interest in
singing. She said that she would like to sing in a choir but became upset on
160 DigbyTantam
one occasion singing in public. She developed the habit of practising with
her mother and would have a temper tantrum if her mother refused or if
the practice was shorter than half an hour. On occasions she had literally
dragged her mother away from guests to another room so that she could
practice. Her mother complained that she did not put any expression into
her singing. She liked to sing the same songs over and over again and
refused to practise alone. Her mother found these demands difficult, and so
often left it uncertain whether there would be a practice session until the
last minute, and sometimes cut practices short.
Rosalind's interest had an obvious relation to her parents' life, but it was
pursued in an unusually solitary way and was rather narrow in that she
showed little interest in other branches of music than singing and disliked
religious music particularly.
suit, because I love him with all my heart, and he looks so fine to me
that he makes me feel great?'
When the teachers at my school said that I wouldn't be seeing him
any more, I was so upset I just didn't want to live any more. So I
asked the people in the Real World to take away all the interesting
things which I liked to see or do, so that 1 completely forgot about
them, and to put me to sleep in a dark, underground cave which no
daylight could get into . . .
But so far he has never come to see me again so there I am still fast
asleep in that dark, underground cave.
Wing (1981) has described the lack of imagination typical of the play of
children with Asperger syndrome, but this description needs some modifi-
cation to account for James and other people with Asperger syndrome who
make up stories, imaginary worlds or imaginary play companions. My
experience has been that the crucial element lacking in the play of more able
children with Asperger syndrome, and its later development into interests
and hobbies, has been an ability to role-play, to switch into a different
persona. Children with Asperger syndrome, in my experience, do not
spontaneously dress up or play cowboys and Indians or cops and robbers.
Adults do not have the ability deliberately to charm, seduce or disguise and
are often unable to detect this sort of behaviour in others. When they act a
part on stage, they have difficulty in infusing it with a character other than
their own.
The hypothesis needs testing, but some substantiation is offered by a
further case example.
Elspeth is the elder of two daughters who has been markedly delayed in her
social and motor development since birth, although her language was
neither delayed nor anomalous. She attended a normal school but was
socially isolated at the time. Her play was stereotyped. She lacked normal
facial expressiveness, her speech was delayed and she had many other
features of autism. However, one feature of my home assessment that did
not fit in was that Elspeth dressed up as a fairy and came back to show me
her costume.
On review a year later she had rapidly progressed in her social
relationships and was playing normally with age-peers. Her parents
thought that she had lost most of the autistic features that had previously
concerned them.
Clumsiness
Children and adults with Asperger syndrome typically give an impression of
clumsiness, which is borne out by delayed motor development and a lack of
aptitude for all ball games. Yet they can be surprisingly dextrous in pursuing
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 16 3
their interests, for example, building toy engines or drawing objects which
fascinate them.
Ninety-one per cent of the forty-six Asperger subjects studied in detail
were judged to be clumsy, and a number of tests were devised to investigate
this further. Clumsiness was assessed by means of two tests developed to
assess right and left hemisphere brain damage (Kimura and Archibald,
1974; Kimura and Vanderwolf, 1970), a catching test and a test of balance
on the right and left legs. The scores on all these tests were significantly
inter-correlated and the first factor in principal components analysis,
accounting for 50 per cent of the total variance, had factor loadings on all of
the tests. Gesture-copying, catching and leg-balancing were all significantly
more impaired in the Asperger subjects, with the most errors relative to the
non-Asperger subjects being made in the catching test.
Many parents described the abnormal routes used by their children to
acquire motor skills, although this was not confined to the autistic children
in the MRC study. Walking 'just happened' without prior crawling or
shuffling, and talking could be acquired after reading. On the other hand, if
a motor skill was not 'invented' in this way by the child, it proved very
difficult to acquire. Demonstration, the normal means of teaching motor
skills, is of limited help to autistic children or adults. Poor performance on
the tests described above may therefore also have reflected this difficulty in
copying movements that another person is making. It is consistent with this
that copying simple designs was performed by the Asperger group as well as
the non-Asperger controls, although unusual strategies were also in evi-
dence here. For example, some subjects drew the Necker cube by superim-
posing two diamonds and then joining them up.
The most typical Asperger syndrome patients, who were usually the least
neurologically impaired, were often the most clumsy. If it is in learning
movement, rather than in producing it, that the abnormality lies, then it
would be expected that movements which require little learning would be
dextrous while those that require considerable learning, such as socially
determined behaviours, would be poorly learnt and therefore poorly
performed. This may be one explanation of why people with Asperger
syndrome, who are less handicapped than people with classical autism, are
more clumsy: their behaviour contains a much higher proportion of
attempts to imitate the behaviour of others and since these are the source of
the clumsiness, the appearance of clumsiness preponderates.
Clumsiness may not be entirely attributable to motor abnormality. It is,
for example, a feature of the self-consciousness of adolescents who are
striving to adopt new and more socially acceptable motor schemata.
Movement patterns are subject to social structuring: certain postures are
condemned or emulated, for example. Children may be instructed in the use
of gaze 'Look in my eyes', they may be told, or 'Look at the ball and not
the thrower'. Such instruction focuses on the importance of integrating gaze
164 Digby Tantam
with motor sequences. Many people with Asperger syndrome seem to have
particular difficulty with this. One who liked to play table-tennis was said to
do so without ever seeming to look at the ball. Another had difficulty
sighting down a telescope.
The problem of co-ordinating gaze and movement seems to be one aspect
of a general problem of integrating individual movements into actions
which may have its basis in a brain lesion, but may also reflect the autistic
person's inability to model behaviour on that of others and thus to acquire
socially transmitted motor schemata. Gaze is discussed in greater detail in a
later section.
30 i
Autistic
Non-autistic
20-
10-
Aetiology
Cases of Asperger syndrome have been reported in association with
Tourette syndrome (Kerbeshian and Burd, 1986), aminoaciduria (Miles and
Capelle, 1987) and ligamentous laxity (Tantam, Evered and Hersov, 1990).
Gillberg (1989) found a variety of neurological abnormalities in 60 per cent
of twenty-three people with Asperger syndrome that he examined. Some of
the organic handicaps associated with Asperger syndrome in the MRC
study are shown in table 5.6. A history of epilepsy could be reliably
ascertained only in a proportion, and only a proportion had had a thorough
physical examination or neurological investigations. The frequencies of
abnormalities may therefore be overestimated, but it is clear that organic
brain disease is over-represented.
166 DigbyTantam
Heredity
Two twin pairs were included in the Asperger group in the MRC study. One
pair, whose uniovularity had been confirmed by blood grouping, were
concordant for Asperger syndrome. The second pair, who were identical but
in whom uniovularity had not been confirmed, were concordant for many
features of Asperger syndrome, but one was less severely impaired than the
other. The father of this latter pair also had some features of Asperger
syndrome.
The numbers are too small to draw any conclusions but the findings are
consistent with the model of Burgoine and Wing (1983) that the severity of
the expression of the Asperger/autism genotype can be influenced by other
factors, so that the same genotype can result in severe autism or milder
Asperger syndrome.
Birth injury
Perinatal complications were retrospectively reported in 51 per cent, but
this figure was lower, although not significantly so, than the 62 per cent of
non-autistic subjects in whom perinatal complications were reported. This
finding contrasts with previousfindingsin autism (for example, Finegan and
Quarrington, 1979) in which a normal control group was used which
showed a lower rate of perinatal complications than the non-autistic control
group in this study. This could be explained if birth injury is actually linked
to a non-specific feature, such as coming to medical attention, rather than to
Asperger syndrome. One possible non-specific link might be the increased
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 167
likelihood that mothers will remember birth problems if their children are
impaired in any way.
Brain lesions
It has been suggested that Asperger syndrome is the behavioural expression
of a lesion of the right cerebral hemisphere on the basis of the similarity of
the communicative abnormalities of Asperger syndrome to the aprosodia
described in adults (Weintraub and Mesulam, 1983) and children (Voeller,
1986) with acquired right hemisphere lesions. Such a view does not take
into account the plasticity of cerebral development when lesions occur
sufficiently early in life, although the extent of the compensation possible as
the result of the development of other structures is, admittedly, not known.
If the suggestion is correct, an excess of right hemisphere structural and
functional abnormalities would be expected in Asperger syndrome. There
was some evidence for this from the MRC study in which the Asperger
subjects had an excess of right hemisphere lesions in the CT scans or EEG
168 DigbyTantam
(see table 5.9). Unfortunately, not every subject was fully investigated by
this means, and selection bias cannot therefore be ruled out. Furthermore,
EEG abnormalities may be an unreliable means of lateralising dysfunction.
In some cases (one of which is reported in detail on Tantam, Evered and
Hersov, 1990), the apparent focus changed from being right- to left-sided
over a period of years. Balancing on the left leg was slightly worse than
balancing on the right leg in the Asperger group, but this also fell short of
significance and may, anyway, have been an effect of right leg dominance.
Although the mean verbal IQ was 92.2 and non-verbal IQ was 86.7, there
was a considerable scatter of scores between subjects, with one subject
having a verbal IQ of 65 and a performance IQ of 104 and another having a
verbal IQ of 94 and a performance IQ of 6$. The IQ distribution was
skewed towards a lower than average score. Five out of forty-two subjects
were mentally handicapped 'overall' but rather more were handicapped
either verbally or non-verbally. Asperger syndrome does not have an
association with mental handicap but, as others have reported (Burgoine
and Wing, 1983; Gillberg <?#/., 1986), does not exclude it.
Van Krevelen (1971) has made the suggestion that autism results from a,
possibly heritable, handicap in association with brain damage. When there
is no brain damage, Asperger syndrome results. This suggestion is partly
supported by some of the findings of the MRC study. Many of the patients
in the study had been physically examined and had had EEGs or CT scans
performed. When the results of these are compared to the results of one of
the balancing tests, it is apparent that the clumsier the subjects the more
likely they are to have EEG or CT scan abnormalities and to have soft
neurological signs. They are also more likely to have had a fit at some time
in their lives, to make syntactic or semantic speech errors, to have lower
verbal IQs, and they are also more likely to have met Rutter's criteria for
early childhood autism (table 5.10).
These findings are evidence for an association of psycholinguistic abnor-
malities, brain damage and Kanner syndrome, and are consistent with Van
Krevelen's hypothesis that brain damage may lead to Kanner syndrome.
However, even the least clumsy subjects and the subjects without evidence
of semantic or syntactic problems have an increased risk of EEG or CT scan
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 169
Percent with
Balancing
on right leg Mean IQ Soft Psycholinguistic History EEG or CT
(seconds) Verbal Performance signs abnormalities of fit abnormal
>IO 102.8 91.4 1 5-3 25.0 12.5 .6.7
5-TO 87.8 84.6 20.0 ^0.0 25.0 33-3
<5 82.6 85.2 44.4 50.0 36.4 66.7
Number affected
Mania only 4
Mania alternating with depression 4
Depressive psychosis only 2
Schizophrenia 3
Epileptic psychosis 1
Hallucinosis 4
All psychosis 18 (21%)
Depression only 5
Depression and anxiety 2
Anxiety only 4
Obsessive-compulsive disorder 2
a
One man who experiences hallucinations and has
obsessional symptoms counted only once in final total.
Mental illness
Thirty (35 per cent) of eighty-five adults with Asperger syndrome examined
by me met criteria for a psychiatric disorder other than a developmental
disorder (criteria taken from the Ninth Revision of the International
170 DigbyTantam
own carer whom he met once a week, but there was no change in his social
avoidance or his lack of sensitivity to social cues. He did, however, became
more able to tolerate change.
Several of the cases of depression were severe and associated with
biological symptoms and suicidal ideas. Five were associated with delusions
and one with stupor. The biological symptoms in these cases were typical of
depression but the content of the illness was coloured by previous, autistic
preoccupations such as in the man who threw himself into the Thames
because the Government refused to abolish British Summer Time. He
believed that watches were damaged by being altered twice a year when the
switch to or from GMT occurred. People with Asperger syndrome who
become depressed may also not seek help and, because of their impairment
in non-verbal expression, may not have a depressed appearance. The
diagnosis may therefore be missed. Similar problems may be presented by
mania, although sleep disturbance is as reliable a diagnostic feature in
Asperger syndrome as it is in non-autistic people. In less able autistic people,
this may not be so as their sleep may normally be disturbed. Lithium was
effective in reducing the frequency of mood disorders in several patients.
The excess of affective disorder over schizophrenia is a further point
against Asperger syndrome being genetically related to schizophrenia as has
been suggested by some workers (Wolff and Chick, 1980). It is not possible
to say whether there is an excess of affective disorder compared to the
general population from the present sample because of selection bias but it
seems likely. Leff, Fischer and Bertelsen (1976) have estimated the incidence
of mania to be 2.6 per 100,000 per year in Camberwell. For the risk period
from which most of the present subjects were drawn, aged fourteen to
thirty, this gives a likely prevalence of 44 per 100,000. On that basis, the
eight cases in the present study would have been drawn from a notional
population of 55,250. Since eighty-six cases of Asperger syndrome were
also drawn from this population, the absence of any particular association
between mania and Asperger syndrome leads to a prevalence estimate of
detected Asperger syndrome of 1.6 per 1,000. This is higher than many
estimates but lower than one (Gillberg, 1988).
Only unequivocal cases of mania have been considered as such: the
remaining cases are difficult to classify. A fairly confident diagnosis of
schizophrenia can be made in the presence of a clear episode of increased
disturbance associated with new first-rank symptoms but even these may be
misleading when they are described by an autistic person whose experi-
ences, self-image and vocabulary may all be idiosyncratic (see Clarke et al.
(1989) for an example). None of the people with schizophrenia experienced
a clearly deteriorating course, although one had become a hospital resident.
A diagnosis of schizophrenia, often simple schizophrenia, was made in a
majority of the group that I studied in detail, and most had received
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 173
20-
n n
16-
Score
on
scale
4-
Individual subjects
I Express scores I Schizoid scores
Fig. 5.2 The distribution of observed abnormalities of non-verbal expression (express scores) and
ratings of schizoid personality (schizoid scores) in a sample of thirty-six eccentric psychiatric patients
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 175
Antisocial behaviour
Many adults with Asperger syndrome have rather strict, law-abiding
attitudes, but in a minority these may coexist with a lack of empathy that
can result in unpredictable violence towards others. My own clinical
experience suggests that this may be more common in those innocent-
looking autistic people who seem to have particular impairment in their
ability to interpret non-verbal expressions.
Lack of empathy may result in inappropriate emotional reactions, for
example, some would laugh when others were hurt, and some would
respond to domestic crises caused by sudden illness by complaining of a
disrupted meal or television-viewing. Lack of empathy may also have been
linked to the serious, unheralded violence to vulnerable individuals that
occurred in a few cases, examples of this included attacks on younger
siblings, on younger, unrelated children, on young animals and on mothers.
These attacks were rarely understandable. Frustration often seemed to have
been involved, although the target of the aggression was often not respon-
sible, often being chosen, it seemed, for their vulnerability or proximity.
The attacks were occasionally serious. Four people with Asperger syn-
drome known to me set fire to a building while there were people in it, and
one started a forest fire. Another killed a school-mate, probably as an
experiment, and has subsequently been detained in a special hospital.
Violence as a result of one person dominating another in a fight, in an
explosion of rage or in sexual excitement - is rarely shown by people with
Asperger syndrome. Sexual offending is generally rare, although indecent
exposure may sometimes occur. Property offences are also rare except when
they are side-effects of the pursuit of a special interest.
Unconcern for others is not a universal characteristic of people with
Asperger syndrome. Many of those showing it had experienced separation
from one or both parents for a significant period of their childhood, but
further study is needed to find out whether this is a contributory factor.
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 177
Nottingham MRC
Higher education 11 4
Employed currently 22 9
In residential care i6 53
Living independently 7 3
Living with parents 7 1
4i
Interest in sexual relations 76 76
Heterosexual relations ever 15 1
Married 1 2
Outcome
The social adjustment of the Nottingham (Newson, Dawson and Everard,
1982) and the MRC subjects are shown in table 5.12. The latter were a
more socially handicapped group than the Nottingham subjects. Only two (4
per cent) had had any education after school, compared to ten (11 per cent) in
the Nottingham sample, and only four (9 per cent) were working compared
to twenty (22 per cent) in that sample. The number attending a social group
regularly was probably also greater in the Nottingham sample, but the
proportion who had had a regular girl-friend (defined as dating for longer
than one month) was the same. Information about the IQs of the Nottingham
sample is lacking, but the pattern of their developmental handicaps (see table
5.12) suggests that it is unlikely that they had a higher than normal
intelligence. Their better social adjustment is thus likely to be due to some
other factor. Fewer of them had seen a psychiatrist and more of them were
living with parents, perhaps indicating that they were less disabled.
My clinical experience strongly suggests that, while the fundamental
handicap of Asperger syndrome is not produced by emotional factors, its
long-term effects may be altered by them. Of course, it is not possible to be
sure about this because of the complicating effect of other associated
physical handicaps and the difficulty of measuring severity. However, there
is a marked variation in adult adjustment, especially in sociability and
adaptability, which is most readily explained by the differences in upbring-
ing, including the strategies that parents have used to deal with autistic traits
and the differences in the emotional environment of the family.
The provision and receipt of help or support was one of the measures
used to assess the strength of social networks in the present study. It is
consistent with the greater dependence of the autistic people in the present
study that only one of thirty-seven questioned (3 per cent) considered that
178 Digby Tantam
they were able to help others in exchange for the help that they received,
while thirty-seven (40 per cent) of the Nottingham sample thought that they
did.
Eight (22 per cent) of the present subjects in whom a confident rating
could be made were thought to be close to those at home: usually this rating
was given only to those in hostels or living with their parents. A similar
proportion, 20 per cent, of the Nottingham sample was reported to be close
to their mothers. Many of the parents in the Nottingham study reported
concern about their child in the future. In the MRC study, parents of
twenty-eight subjects, out of the twenty-nine subjects who had parents who
could be questioned, reported that their child was a source of concern to
them. The commonest worry, troubling 5 5 per cent of parents, was the fate
of the child after the parents' death. This was the main worry of rather
fewer, 21 per cent, of the parents interviewed in the Nottingham study. The
parents of only three subjects (10 per cent) complained of the burden
imposed by their child. However, the home situation of many people with
Asperger syndrome was not satisfactory: 39 per cent reported regular
conflict which in half had led to violence.
Only one parent reported guilt about their child's condition. Most had
been told at some stage that they were over-reacting or that their child's
disorder was an emotional reaction with the implication of some fault in
their upbringing, and reassurance that this was not so was requested at
some time during almost every interview with parents.
The parents could be reassured that Asperger syndrome is not a conse-
quence of child-rearing practice, but it was noticeable that only some of the
Asperger subjects presented conduct or emotional problems. These tended
to be more common when there was familial stress or separation of parents
early in the child's development. The latter may itself be due to the
considerable difficulties posed by the upbringing of a child with autism, and
more evidence would be needed before it could be assumed that these
familial factors led to the emotional disturbance.
Asperger considered that the prognosis for the syndrome he described
was good and, indeed, my own clinical experience has shown that able
people with autism can sometimes have childhood histories indicating much
more severe impairment. Recent follow-up studies (Fombonne et al., 1989;
Szatmari et al.9 1989) have also tended to confirm Asperger's view, although
the characteristics that predict a good outcome are still unclear.
The general trend towards amelioration of the symptoms must, however,
be balanced against the often poor social outcome. A minority of subjects in
both the Nottingham and MRC studies were working (see table 5.12), fewer
than 10 per cent were living independently, and very few had established
any sort of intimate relationship outside the family. Asperger syndrome,
despite being a milder form of autism in terms of the apparent severity of its
symptoms, is, clearly, still a highly socially disabling condition.
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 179
Conclusions
Wing and Gould's (1979) triad of social impairment persists throughout the
vicissitudes of autistic development as the cardinal feature that distinguishes
autism from other developmental disorders. My own study of Asperger
syndrome in adulthood quoted here provides further evidence for this, at the
same time suggesting some slight modifications to Wing and Gould's
formulation of the triad as it is expressed in Asperger syndrome.
Two variants of the triad can be usefully distinguished: passivity in
social interaction coupled with reduced, but not obviously unusual, non-
verbal expression and, I have previously hypothesised, particular impair-
ment in the recognition of facial and other non-verbal expression; and,
corresponding to Wing and Gould's (1979) category of 'active, but odd', a
greater degree of social initiation, odd and idiosyncratic non-verbal
expression and, according to my hypothesis, less impairment of the
interpretation of non-verbal cues than the first group. My clinical impres-
sion is that the former group tend to contain disproportionately more of
the minority of callous individuals, to be more immature in person and
manner and to be less intelligent. Those in the latter group are not always
deficient in imagination, as Wing (1981) suggests, but may lack the ability
to role-play.
The more evidence of brain damage there is in the form of soft
neurological signs, abnormalities on CT scan or EEG, epilepsy or low IQ
the more likely the affected person is to be clumsy or to have a language
problem affecting semantic and syntactic competence, or both.
One interpretation of this is that the core neurological defect producing
the trait often coexists with more severe neurological abnormalities which
may affect speech areas or motor cortex, or both. This would provide an
explanation of the greater likelihood of mental handicap in Kanner
syndrome compared to Asperger syndrome: that the greater language
impairment in the former results from neurological involvement of the
speech areas. This core defect may correspond to a specific anatomical
lesion or, as seems more likely, to a specific loss of function equivalent to a
180 DigbyTantam
References
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders. 3rd edn, revised. Washington: APA.
Andreasen, N. C. (1989). The scale for the assessment of negative symptoms
(SANS): conceptual and historical foundations. British journal of Psychiatry, 155,
supplement 7, 5962.
Asperger, H. (1979). Problems of infantile autism. Communication, 13, 45-52.
Bowman, E. P. (1988). Asperger's syndrome and autism: the case for a connection.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 377-82.
Burgoine, E. & Wing, L. (1983). Identical triplets with Asperger's syndrome. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 261-5.
Clarke, D. J., Littlejohns, C. S., Corbett, J. A. & Joseph, S. (1989). Pervasive
developmental disorders and psychoses in adult life. British Journal of Psychiatry,
155,692-9.
Asperger syndrome in adulthood 18 i
Volkmar, F. R., Paul, R. & Cohen, D. J (1985). The use of 'Asperger's syndrome'.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 15, 4379.
Weintraub, S. &c Mesulam, M. M. (1983). Developmental learning disabilities and
the right hemisphere: emotional, interpersonal and cognitive components.
Archives of Neurology, 40, 4638.
Wing, L. (1980). Childhood autism and social class: a question of selection. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 137, 410-17.
Wing, L. (1981). Sex ratios in early childhood autism and related conditions.
Psychiatry Research, 5, 12937.
Wing, L. (1987). The continuum of autistic characteristics. In E. Schopler & G. B.
Mesibov (eds.), Diagnosis and assessment of autism. New York: Plenum.
Wing, L. & Gould, J. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and
associated abnormalities in children: epidemiology and classification. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 1 129.
Wolff, S. & Barlow, A. (1979). Schizoid personality in childhood: a comparative
study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 20, 29-46.
Wolff, S. & Chick, J. (1980). Schizoid personality disorder in childhood: a follow-up
study. Psychological Medicine, 10, 85JOO.
Wolff, S. &C Cull, A. (1986). Schizoid personality and antisocial conduct: a
retrospective case note study. Psychological Medicine, 16, 67787.
World Health Organization. (1978). International classification of diseases: ninth
revision. Geneva: WHO.
World Health Organization. (1990). International classification of diseases: tenth
revision. Chapter V. Mental and behavioural disorders (including disorders of
psychological development). Diagnostic criteria for research (May 1990 draft for
field trials). Geneva: WHO (unpublished).
Living with Asperger's syndrome
MARGARET DEWEY
Test instructions
In the following stories some parts are in italics. Immediately following
there is a pair of brackets ( ). Rate the behaviour which is illustrated by the
184
Living with Asperger's syndrome 185
portion in italics according to how you think most people would judge that
behaviour if they witnessed it. Use this scale.
Fairly normal behaviour in that situation (A)
Rather strange behaviour in that situation (B)
Very eccentric behaviour in that situation (C)
Shocking behaviour in that situation (D)
his diaper had opened. Rather than bother the mother in the park, Keith
quickly checked the baby's clothing to see whether he could feel an open
pin. ( )
Roger said, 'but if you don't mind I will wait another hour to eat. I just had
some food an hour ago.'
shocking. Other autistic subjects varied, as some may have learnt rules
about sharing combs.
'Mummy, I shall take a knife one day and push it in your heart, then blood
will spurt out and this will cause a great stir.' 'It would be nice if I were a
wolf. Then I could rip apart sheep and people, and then blood would flow.'
Once, when the mother cut her finger, 'Why isn't there more blood? The
blood should run!' When he injured himself on one occasion, he was said
to have been utterly thrilled, so that the doctor who tended the wound
remarked on the child's state as extremely odd.
I do not see in this account a sadistic child, but one who is obsessively
interested in the way blood is forcefully pumped through the body. Even his
own injury is viewed as an exciting occasion to observe the phenomenon.
The first remark is shocking because it appears to be a threat to his mother.
But autistic children are socially far younger than their actual age. Many
young children resort to exaggerated threats to show displeasure. I know a
Living with Asperger 's syndrome 193
Overcoming misunderstandings
trial and error. That may still be the best way to handle some unique
problems of autistic individuals. Eventually, one comes to recognise what
kinds of intervention work for each child and what kinds are counter-
productive.
Today Jack is a reasonably contented middle-aged man, gainfully
employed as a piano tuner and living in his own small house. Younger
parents sometimes ask 'How did you do it?1 Actually, he did it by dint of
sustained effort as well as ability. My insights stem from a forty-year
perspective and fortuitous contacts. Certainly, I cannot write as an infallible
parent.
Asperger's paper is a convenient reference point from which to discuss the
problems of high-functioning autistic people. Obviously, they are capable of
more independence than those who are severely handicapped. But that does
not necessarily ensure that life will be easier for them. Each step towards
greater independence is accompanied by new pitfalls. Asperger dwelt on one
such transition, the start of school. He observes, 'Parents can often cope
with the oddities of small autistic children, but at school they cannot be
handled in the ordinary way.'
The phrase 'in the ordinary way' is rich with meaning. It suggests that
readers will know what everybody, even little children starting school,
should know. There are expectations surrounding the school situation.
Unfortunately, many of the most elementary things which everybody is
assumed to know can elude an autistic child. (Don't ask for a list: it is too
variable). An unspoken expectation is nothing but an idea in other people's
minds, something which quite baffles an autistic person. Even when the
message is put into words it is apt to be misinterpreted. The phrases most
commonly used in everyday speech can seldom be taken literally. If 'Fold
your hands!' seems like an impossible order, an autistic child does not look
around to see what others are doing. He is more likely to look for a way out.
The teacher is perplexed because this child leaves his seat for no apparent
reason and wanders away. She is no more perplexed than he. The same
comedy of errors will be repeated on the occasion of every drastic change in
an autistic person's life.
The more capable an autistic person appears to be, the more likely it is
that he will be expected to manage his own affairs without supervision. It
would seem that he should be able to handle at least simple social
interactions of the everyday variety. But that is not necessarily so. The
oddness of an autistic person shows up most starkly in commonplace
situations. He does not intuitively know what everybody is presumed to
know, and he lacks the awareness of other minds which would enable him
to get guidance from subtle cues on the spot. By contrast, in situations
where almost everybody is baffled by expectations, autistic people are less
obvious misfits. They may do well travelling abroad, for example, seeming
just a bit more odd than other tourists.
196 Margaret Dewey
changing behaviour. That sometimes works, but if the reward is valued, the
cost of failure is also dear. The autistic person becomes so anxious that one
slip can provoke a catastrophic emotional reaction.
I have in mind continuous positive reinforcement by favourable notice
and well-timed approval. It is selectively directed at any sign of desired
change up to the point of victory and well beyond that.
Consider the child's game, Hot and Cold. One child is kept in the dark
about the location of a hidden prize. (This may be compared to the way an
autistic person may be in the dark about appropriate social behaviour.) The
player starts out blindly, counting on others to direct him. If he moves away
from the prize, they chant words like 'You're coolcoolercoldfreezing!'
So he changes direction and continues to change until he hears 'You're
warmwarmerhotburning!' Even the tone of the guiding voices heats up
with excitement as the triumphant player closes in on his trophy. Ultimate
success is assured with this step-by-step reinforcement. The cool signals are
positive because they are not delivered as a punishment to deter him from
his reward, but as an additional guide to help him win. (Remember this
game. You can use it to explain to an autistic person why it is sometimes
necessary to point out errors as a short-cut to success.)
In real life, the guidance game is often played quite differently. Correct
behaviour is scarcely noticed because it is taken for granted. Every situation
has implied rules, and every person is assumed to be marginally acquainted
with them, at the least. The person who drives in the correct direction down
a one-way street is not singled out for praise. But if you are aware that he
frequently goes in the wrong direction you may take notice. (How you
notice and what you say depends on the individual's personality. All the
autistic adults I have known respond favourably to sincere compliments. Do
not be afraid you will provoke misbehaviour by saying something nice.)
Here is an excerpt from a letter written by one mother about her autistic
son:
I have been trying positive reinforcement with surprising success. Deciding
on what behavior I wish he would develop, I compliment him greatly when
I see the slightest sign of it. It works like magic! He is so guileless he doesn't
see through it as you or I might. He always monopolizes the dinner table
conversation, so one day I waited for a pause as he was eating, and I said
'You know, Barry, you talk much less at the table than you used to.' (It was
true, although he still talked far too much.) 'And sometimes you listen to
what others say and follow the dinner conversation.' Well, this statement
brought a marked change, so much so that his sisters noticed when they
came home.
In the above example, the family should repeat the compliment often to
encourage him to keep on trying to control his compulsion to spill out all
the ideas he has been mulling over. The follow-up compliments would be
even more appropriate, and should be phrased in different ways. When an
198 Margaret Dewey
conformity even in non-conformity. Each season has its 'look' which the
autistic person is unaware of.
Asperger made many references to family conflicts which arise from the
difficulty of teaching autistic children the practical chores of daily life. This
tense situation may be eased when one understands that the basic difficulty
is not wilful obstruction but a need for precise and patient instruction.
Autistic people can be guided to develop grooming habits which help them
look (and smell) acceptable in most situations. The trick is to keep it as
simple as possible with a minimum of contingencies. For example, a man
wearing corduroy slacks and a clean shirt will pass at a picnic or a funeral,
even though jeans are more suitable for the former occasion and a dark suit
is traditional for the latter. Habits of cleanliness should be the basic ones,
without too many daily rituals.
Under pressure of time, an autistic person will not intuitively know which
details of grooming to eliminate. Tooth-brushing, nail-trimming, deodorant
use, shampoo, underwear-changing, body-washing, hair-cutting, sock-
changing, clothes-cleaning and many more common details of grooming
need to be put in perspective for an autistic person. The best tactic is to set
up a schedule - and keep instructions basic. Interestingly, Asperger himself
made the suggestion of a timetable for everyday practical necessities.
Beyond that, a degree of discrimination can be taught by use of a one-to-ten
scale applied just to grooming. Other common activities also have scales
and the activities themselves fit somewhere in an overall scale of importance
for survival.
Normal people compromise with their own schedules more than they are
willing to admit when they give advice to others. There is no way to cram
one day with all of the proper procedures for care of one's self and
possessions. Each gadget and each garment comes with rules for care and
dire warnings for failing to heed them. This can quite overwhelm an autistic
person who cannot discriminate the essential from the trivial.
The more sheltered the individual the less he has to worry about. Those
who venture out on their own deserve candid advice which includes
short-cuts and acceptable omissions. Sirens do not sound for failing to floss
one's teeth!
said he couldn't drive me to jobs any more, so I bought a car. It was a long slow
process gradually getting used to driving. I was very cautious, planning my routes
ahead. I had to automate my driving skills one by one. The way I handle the
hurry-up pressure on the job is choosing who I work for. I now refuse to do concert
tunings because I know there is a deadline tofinishthe piano. I don't like to have the
artist saying, 'Hurry up! Get that done so I can practise!'
Margaret: Do you have a philosophy for dealing with your reaction to
people who seem disappointed in you or angry with you?
Anne: I think, That person might be upset about something in his or her own life.
Maybe I can gain something from this experience.'
Jack: (after a moment of silence) I am very impressed with Anne's philosophy for
facing people who are disappointed in her. It has always been one of the worst
traumas for me to feel I have displeased somebody. I tend to remember it years
afterwards. It hurts more than I can bear, practically. One thing I do is daydream
about how I can be reconciled with people I have displeased and change their
opinion of me.
Margaret: Do you have a philosophy for dealing with feelings of failure?
Anne: I think, 'Maybe this wasn't meant to happen right now. Maybe this wasn't
meant to work out, but something else will.' I believe strongly in God and often it
works out that if I say, This wasn't meant to be', it does turn out to be for the best. I
believe that there is a good and benevolent force beyond us. If I say, 'I am going to
leave everything up to God' after I have done my best, things seem to work out
eventually.
Jack: My most profound feelings of failure are as far as music composition is
concerned. My musical compositions have had virtually no impact on the profes-
sional music world. The philosophy I have for dealing with that is like the fox and
sour grapes story. I say, 'If I were successful I might get some very caustic reviews
and I would be crushed because of the way I feel about criticism.' Also, the way to
lose all of your private life is to become famous. You can't reverse that, and I like my
privacy.
Margaret: Does either of you suffer from loneliness?
Anne: I don't feel as though I have enough friends but I've been feeling a lot better
lately because of all the things I've been doing. I work part-time organising the
library for the Autistic Society. And I do some part-time work with the Developmen-
tal Disability group. Right now I am a volunteer with the Dukakis campaign. There
are some benefits from being alone, too, I can think things through and be more
ready to go out again. I often feel lonely for a man. I would really like to have
somebody but I am glad that I haven't gotten attached yet so I make further progress
and be more acceptable.
Jack: Actually, I don't suffer from loneliness. If I have to relate to people too much I
become nervous and uncomfortable. As I said before, it is important to me to please
people. Once I have pleased people it is not that important that I see them often. I
seem to crave a lot of privacy. I am practically an after-work recluse. Since I am
pathologically sensitive to criticism if I were married, quite frankly, I'd feel sorry for
my wife. All she'd need to do is criticise me and I might have such a fit it would ruin
our marriage. It would be a bad thing for her.
Margaret: Couldn't you learn to control your reaction to criticism?
Jack: No! In fact, so much not that I actually plan my life in such a way that I make
204 Margaret Dewey
sure it doesn't happen. For example, the only customers I accept for my tuning work
are the two 're's', referrals and repeats. I never have trouble with those people.
Margaret: Have you thought about controlling your reactions rather than
trying so hard to control the circumstances?
Jack: Of course, I've been conditioned not to scream when I am criticised, but the
feeling is there inside me just the same. It's like being poisoned.
Margaret: Now, here is a question for both of you. Do you have trouble
letting go of ideas?
Anne: Yep I sometimes drive my mother up the wall by talking too much about
the same subject. I do have fixations.
Jack: Oh, yes, I have quite a number of things I talk about and can't let go of. But
since you've conditioned me not to bore people, I talk to myself incessantly.
Anne: Oh, I talk to myself all the time, too!
Margaret: I have heard of this happening in so many cases that it must
serve some useful purpose for you.
Anne: I think it does. You know, I like the sound of my own voice because it keeps me
from feeling lonely. I think there is also a little fear that if I don't talk a lot I may lose
my voice. I didn't talk until I was almost five, you know. Before I started talking I
noticed a lot of things, and now when I tell my mother she is amazed I remember them.
I remember that the world was really scary and everything was over-stimulating.
Jack: Talking to myself helps me figure out and practise how to express ideas well.
[On other occasions Jack had mentioned many reasons he feels compelled to talk to
himself, though he avoids doing so in public places. Apparently it organises his
thoughts and serves as a memory aid. Like Anne, he enjoys the companionship of his
own voice.]
Margaret: Can you say a few words about your reaction to being called
autistic?
Anne: The autism diagnosis was like a great big weight being lifted from my chest!
[She celebrates the anniversary of that diagnosis as a special day.]
Jack: Me too! I was relieved to know that it was a real thing and it wasn't just my
fault for not trying hard enough.
Margaret: You had other diagnoses before autism. Why didn't those
explanations work for you?
Anne: People thought I was mainly visually impaired at first. My lenses were
removed when I was a baby because of cataracts. I didn't talk or indicate to people
that I saw things, so they thought I was practically blind. I was started in a blind
class in school. When I started to read instead of learning braille, they realised I
could see. I didn't even need large print. Then they called me hyperactive with
minimal brain dysfunction and tried various things for that. For a while, I thought
my problems were all psychological and I had psychotherapist after psychotherapist.
I won't say some of them didn't help a little bit, but they weren't able to make the
autistic behaviours go away. It was a relief when Dr Maltz identified my problem as
autism, and suggested the behavioural approach.
Jack: First they thought I was retarded, then brain-damaged. I was often accused of
not paying attention or not listening even though I was trying my best. The way I
knew when I did the right thing was when people were pleased, not because it was
clear to me or obvious. [Maybe this is why Jack is so senstive to criticism and eager
to please people.]
Living with Asperger's syndrome 20 5
Anne: I still have to work on my problems caused by autism. I even get new ones.
Recently I've developed obsessive-compulsive behaviours. I go to bed and wonder,
'Did I turn off the stove and slide the dead bolt?' I get so anxious I have to get up and
check again.
Jack: I know what you mean. The same thing happens to me. I always have to make
sure! I am not comfortable until I have checked certain things.
Here we end our discussion. It seems we have come full circle to the topic of
anxiety once again. Autistic people are likely to feel anxious about
safeguarding their achievements after their tremendous effort to attain
independence. Is it all worth it, then? Jack and Anne obviously think so. In
some follow-up conversations they talked eagerly about the things that give
them joy. There are many small daily pleasures as well as weekly events and
special occasions which are anticipated for months.
Jack has always been involved with music for pleasure as well as work,
both creating and listening. As a baby, he walked around beaming with joy
and singing songs long before he could talk. Now he gets excited about
using a sampling keyboard at a recording studio. He says he loves to
orchestrate musical sounds in ways that have never been done before. I
believe his claim that the combinations are unique because he never forgets
a sound-combination. His ability to recognise and identify classical music
seems limitless. It is as if all the music he has ever heard and he is a
prodigious listener has been forever recorded in his mind for instant recall.
Anne seems attracted to people by nature. She enjoys occasions which
give her a chance to mingle with people in small groups. (Large gatherings
can be confusing, she admits.) On a typical Sunday recently she rose early,
joined a friend for a festive breakfast and walked a considerable distance to
the little church she attends. After the formal meetings she remained at
church for a pot-luck dinner. Planning her contribution and seeing it
appreciated was another pleasure of that day.
Both Anne and Jack enjoy watching nature documentaries on television.
Travel films are favourites too. Jack is drawn to documentaries about
science while Anne confesses to favouring those about people. Both have
been avidly following a recent series about the mind. In fact, they were
thrilled because the first program included a segment about a young man
who is autistic. They easily recognised symptoms they share with him,
although his interests are different.
Jack sometimes relaxes at home by playing the organ or piano. He bought
a large used organ which the movers were able to fit into his bedroom by
removing the door frame. A grand piano from his grandmother dominates
the living room. Just as Jack enjoys expressing himself on all sorts of musical
keyboards, Anne finds pleasure exploring ways to use her personal compu-
ter. She is looking forward to adding components from time to time.
When asked what she enjoys doing with free time, to relax, Anne said she
loves to read. She likes to write too, poetry, prose and letters. In fact, she
206 Margaret Dewey
The terms 'Asperger syndrome' and 'able autistic' are used interchangeably throughout this
chapter. While more precise criteria for Asperger syndrome are discussed in other chapters
of this volume, the term is used here to apply to autistic individuals with fluent language and
normal intelligence.
207
208 Francesca G. E. Happe
light the writings of these able autistic adults have much to tell us about
autism in general and the nature of autistic communication in particular.
changes of topic that do not run smoothly for the reader. An enlightening
example is where she switches abruptly from talking about her squeeze
machine to talking about how to handle cattle, in the passage quoted above.
At this point in the article, the reader does not know the origins of the
squeeze machine, and the topic change is puzzling and hard to follow.
Knowing about this link, on the other hand, makes the change of topic
appear smooth. It is as if she fails to appreciate that her reader does not
share the important background information that she possesses. Similarly,
after many references to the squeeze machine, Temple waits until page 152,
after the passage quoted above, to describe the machine - thus giving
information vital to understanding earlier references only after these
references. On page 167, in the middle of a description of her nervous
episodes, she introduces, with no further explanation: 'After I swam
through the dip vat, the organophosphates greatly reduced the "nerves".'
Again, it is as if she does not realise that the reader does not share her
personal knowledge, or perhaps that, without further explanation, swim-
ming through a dip vat will seem a pretty odd thing to do.
The dip vat episode also raises an interesting point about Temple's ability
to empathise not just in terms of understanding her reader's state of
knowledge, but also in terms of emotion and feeling. Temple's motive for
swimming through the dip vat, which is the vital piece of information for
making her reader understand this strange action, is revealed only in the
reference at the end of the article: 'Grandin, T. 1978. How stressful is
dipping I jumped in to find out. Calf News (November), pp. 646.' This,
in turn, is interesting in two ways. Firstly, it suggests perhaps a lack of
ability to empathise, since she felt it necessary to put herself through the
same experience in order to feel the same feelings. When we empathise with
another person we generally mean that we feel with them, despite the fact
that we are not actually suffering with them. It might also be an inability to
empathise to the normal extent that leads Temple to fail to distinguish what
is unusual in her experience from what is universal. So, for example, she
tells us that 'Even as an adult, I find that it is easier to learn something if I
can actually do it instead of watching' (p. 164) an almost universal
experience, in much the same way as she tells us of her extraordinary early
'fixation on spinning objects, refusing to be touched . . . destructive beha-
viour . . . inability to speak . . . and intense interest in odours' (p. 144).
The second point of interest raised by the dip vat episode, is Temple's
attitude to animals. She quotes animal research data throughout her article,
much more than she quotes experiments with human subjects, and uses this
data without any suggestion that some allowance might have to be made
when reasoning from animal to human subjects. She does not seem to see
the divide which, rightly or wrongly, most people feel the need to construct
between animals and humans. This is evident in her creation of the squeeze
machine, and it is interesting that while Temple is certainly aware that her
Autobiographical writings 211
teachers and family find the machine odd and disturbing, she herself does
not feel this perhaps irrational, but none the less intuitive, unease. The
photographs in her book capture this disregard for the usual barriers we
construct between ourselves and other animals, and it must be in part due to
this disregard that Temple has been so successful in her job designing
livestock facilites in ranches and meat plants.
One explanation for Temple's merging of human and animal data may be
that she ignores or discounts the importance of our affective or emotional
life. Evidence for this comes mainly from a lack of reference to her own
emotional life, the reader's potential emotional reactions or her family
relationships in her account of her childhood. It is also revealed in what she
does say, for example, in discussing the beneficial effects of the presence of
younger 'therapist' monkeys on adult monkeys reared in isolation, she
writes: 'The beneficial effect could possibly be due to a combination of
tactual stimulation and increased motor activity' (p. 150). The social and
affective effects of the presence of the therapist monkeys are not seen to be
important.3 Again in her description of her own anxiety attacks, it seems
that Temple discounts the emotional significance of events in favour of a
more physiological analysis of her reactions: 'An intermittent itch would be
totally insignificant to most adults, but in me it created the same reaction
as being chased by a mugger' (p. 167). Elsewhere she talks about her
'emotions' where most people would feel that stress or anxiety (in some
sense, the physiology of emotion without the affective meaning) better
describes her state: 'The only relapse I have had was during an extremely
emotionally arousing event when some new equipment I designed was being
started up at a meat packing plant' (p. 168). Such examples leave the reader
with the feeling that Temple's emotional life is not like most people's.
This lack of interest in the affective and emotional significance of events is
perhaps most striking where she talks about her view of the fundamental
deficits in autism. She seems throughout to disregard the fact that most
people would feel the social handicap to be the most striking aspect of
autism. Instead she is more interested in the autistic person's differences in
cognitive style, perceptual experiences and underlying nervous system. One
example of this is where she talks about Genie, the girl who was found after
spending her first thirteen years tied to a chair with almost no social contact.
Temple writes: 'Genie . . . had many autistic behaviours. "Genie is an
'appositional' thinker, visually and tactually oriented, better at holistic than
sequential analytic thinking" (Curtiss, 1977)' (p. 149). This seems an odd
thing to say, since Genie was notably not autistic in terms of forming
affectionate relationships with her nurses soon after being rescued a
feature which most people would feel was of more significance in any
3
Even behaviourists, who make a point of not appealing to mental states, acknowledge the
importance of affect and social factors.
212 Francesca G. E. Happe
on the back' in the sense of actual physical contact as praise (p. 144). This
literal use of language, as we shall see, may be an important feature of
autistic communication.
Overall, the writing is remarkable and an achievement of which almost
anyone would be proud. There can be no doubt that Temple Grandin is a
capable and intelligent woman and a success story to encourage parents,
teachers and those who themselves receive a diagnosis of autism. Her
success lies, perhaps, in her lack of interest in social matters, and hence her
lack of distress at her relative isolation. It is interesting too, as a last
thought, that she has succeeded in a field which requires some 'empathy' for
farm animals since this is an area in which we are all left guessing and
using scientific facts to infer 'feelings'.
Barry: letters
Barry is a young man who was in his twenties when he wrote these letters.
He lives in the United States and started writing to his correspondent - a
middle-aged woman after visiting her and her autistic son. The correspon-
dence lasted for about a year, when it suddenly stopped as abruptly as it had
begun. Barry wrote sometimes as frequently as three times a month letters
which were often several pages long. These letters are exceptional in being
totally self-motivated and without any external editing. The following is the
first letter Barry wrote to his correspondent, and gives some impression of
the style of the letters:
Dear Mrs Jones,
I'm glad you enjoyed our visit very much. I am eager to write to your son
Tom, but I don't know his home address. Please give me his own home
address.
Also too Yd like for you to send me that old fashioned peanut butter if
you would. That old fashioned peanut butter here is too expensive.
Would you please send me a list of addresses of all the people I met at the
meeting in Flint, Michigan when Christmas comes around, so that I can
write them Christmas cards.
Speaking of girls, I've met two girls, one named Darlene, age 20, the
other Denise, age 17 and getting friendly with them. I began thinking that it
would be better for now that I can have one or two more girlfriends for
three years and find another one or two girlfriends or more for the next
three years and so forth. Lots of guys do that. Here almost all of the girls
are getting married, which are those of my age. They seem to be marrying
very rapidly. When I think about girls of my age I think of the passenger
railroad trains which are going off the tracks here in the USA.
So write soon. My address is included in this letter.
Yours truly, Barry
Barry's letters are quite different from Temple's published works. As one
would expect from the less formal genre, the writing is less polished, less
214 Francesca G. E. Happe
fluent, and the grammar is sometimes awkward. However, all of this might
be expected of someone of similar intellect and education, without autism.
Some aspects of his writing style, on the other hand, seem peculiar in a more
significant way. Here we shall look at a number of these features, including
his idiosyncratic use of language, his flitting between topics, his persevera-
tion, parroting and social naivety about other minds.
One of the most striking aspects of Barry's writings is his use of idiosyncra-
tic terms, which are not explained, and which the reader could not be
expected to understand by normal conventions. For example, in the letter
above, he writes, 'When I think about girls of my age I think of the passenger
railroad trains which are going off the tracks here in the USA'. This rather
opaque comparison appears several times in the letters, with no direct
explanation as to what aspect of the girls reminds him of trains. The only clue
is an aside in a letter over a year later, about passenger trains being cut in
number: 'Now, we hardly have any passenger trains left anymore'. Presum-
ably Barry, who is his twenties, feels that girls of his age are slowly
disappearing as they get engaged and married to other men. Another example
which, interestingly, Barry seems to realise is idiosyncratic, although that
does not lead him to explain why he employs it is his use of the phrase
'student nurses' age group' for those aged seventeen to twenty-one.
This use of idiosyncratic terms is important and revealing. Kanner wrote
about similar cases in 1946, referring to the autistic subjects' 'irrelevant and
metaphorical language'. Kanner showed that apparently nonsensical
phrases could often be traced back to a first occurrence, from which the
child had derived his or her own meaning. He gave the example of J.S., a
three-year-old autistic boy who referred to himself as 'Blum' whenever his
veracity was questioned. This was explained when the boy, who read
fluently, pointed out an advertisement claiming 'Blum tells the Truth!'
Kanner pointed out that this association is just the same as that which we
would recognise in our culture between, say, all lovers and Romeo, except
that it is essentially a private rather than cultural and shared association.
Hence, 'the basic difference consists of the autistic privacy and original
uniqueness of the transfers' (Kanner, 1946). The important point is,
perhaps, that even an autistic writer who is trying to communicate will use
such private 'transfers' without explanation. Thus it seems that, like
Temple, Barry does not recognise what knowledge is shared and what is
personal. In accord with this explanation, he also fails to explain to his
reader the meanings of several train abbreviations which, although conven-
tional, are obscure to non-rail enthusiasts (such as his correspondent). That
his own idiosyncratic terms are based on normal parallels and equivalences
is shown in one letter where he coins and explains a new term: 'Steam
locomotives have been the most expensive and the most painful locomotives
ever to run on any railroad track. For this reason, I shall call steam
locomotives girls.'
Autobiographical writings 215
when he wrote this autobiography. His interests are primarily in the normal
desires of life - getting a job, being liked, finding a girl-friend. This, plus his
enthusiasm for writing, makes him a very open and potentially very
informative subject. In view of all this, it is perhaps surprising that on
reading the autobiography the most striking impression is of a man who is a
little simple, but more importantly, is simply lonely, bored and depressed.
Of course some social gaucheness shows through which in part accounts for
his loneliness, but he shows a degree of social understanding not seen even
in Temple's edited work. The question of parroting cannot, of course, be
ruled out - but if David's remarks are his own they are surprising and
challenging.
David wrote his autobiography as a sort of personal advertisement, in the
hope that 'someone that can offer me some love and affection will get in
touch' (p. 101). It follows a loose chronological order with diversions on
various topics. It has not been edited except to correct spelling errors, add
commas and split the text into more paragraphs than David himself used.
The following extract is representative of the writing in this autobiography:
I think religion helps some people but it doesn't help others. I think I'm one
of those people that it doesn't help much. I've led a more or less pure life
for so long that I'm sick of it. Mind you [Jane] smokes a lot and I don't
think she minds drinking a bit too. She just turned up at our house last
Sunday; perhaps if she'd have rung up it would have been an idea. She gave
Peter her address and said I can bring Peter up to call on her sometime. I
hope she doesn't go out with Peter. My mother told me that you needn't be
afraid of him. She said girls like lads to look clean. My mother sees that I
look clean. She's just brought me a new pair of shoes and I've put them on
this morning. Sometimes new shoes are murder to wear. I wear the soles
out on my shoes very quickly because I do an awful lot of walking. My
mother seems to always have the washing machine on, it's on again this
morning. I don't think my mother likes anything to go dirty and it's on
most days, the washer. I think it's one of those washers with a programme
so she just puts it on and leaves it. We've had this washer about 2 years. I
think she wore the last washer out by using it too much, no doubt. Odd
times during the winter the pipes on the washer get frozen up and water
spills out of the washer then. This winter was that cold the water wouldn't
run out of the bath one day because it was that cold. The water eventually
went down the pipe; the hot bath water must have melted the ice after an
hour or so being in the pipe (p. 90).
in the story' (p. 76); and both Jane and Peter who are mentioned in the
passage above, have been fully introduced to the reader earlier. Such
consideration for the reader's state of knowledge is not seen in Temple's or
Barry's writing. This is only one example of David's apparently good
understanding of others' minds, or as it has come to be known theory of
mind. A rather different example is where he writes about going into a pub,
with two badges pinned 'on a funny place on my chest, and a fellow in the
pub was very amused and he said "you've got your badges stuck in the
wrong place". I think he thought I was trying to imitate a woman because of
the places I'd stuck those badges' (p. 72). Here David seems to show that he
can appreciate another person's mistaken ideas about his intentions (thus
understanding someone's ideas about his thoughts).
Understanding another person's false belief about a situation or mental
state has been accepted as good evidence in the experimental literature (for
example Baron-Cohen, Leslie and Frith, 1985) for the possession of a theory
of mind. David clearly passes this test and, indeed, shows an appreciation of
and interest in more subtle social interactions. He writes of his family, for
example: 'My mother doesn't always get on well with my grandmother . . . I
think my mother gets out of her way by going to French classes a l o t . . .
These classes help her take her mind off things' (p. 100). Understanding of
jokes and deception is often thought to be a skill that even able autistic
adults do not master, and Barry's and Temple's (unedited) works seem to
bear this out. David, however, seems to have a grasp of these forms of
communication: 'I also know a group called 3-D fiction,' he writes, 'I think
they've got some of my poems . . . They said they might write some music to
my poems but I think they might be joking' (p. 82). Later on he writes about
trying to impress girls: 'I usually try to kid them I'm doing well as a writer
but they don't fall for that one very easy' (p. 101).
Along with an apparently quite astute understanding of other minds (by
autistic standards), David seems to have some degree of social awareness.
So, for example, he writes about going for a job interview: 'This woman
asked me why I wanted the job. I said to get some money. It was the wrong
answer . . . By the end of the interview I knew I hadn't got the job' (p. 68).
Later in the book he describes going along to a meeting where they started
celebrating someone's birthday: 'That was one reason why I didn't like
stopping, because I didn't know the person whose birthday it was' (p. 83).
Although obviously socially gauche in his attempts to find a girl-friend,
David does have some accurate ideas about what girls do not like, for
example: 'I think I'd get more girls interested in me if I lived on my own, it
seems to put girls off if they know you live with your parents, I think' (p.
85); and later, 'I find it hard to talk to girls, I never seem to know the right
things to say to them. I'm in a bad set-up, so no matter what I say won't
make them take much interest in me . . .' (p. 101). Again, he seems to have
some idea of social dos and don'ts, as, for example, when he writes about
Autobiographical writings 219
Jane in the passage above: 'She just turned up at our house last Sunday;
perhaps if she'd have rung up it would have been an idea' (p. 90).
This last example leads us onto the question of parroting, since it sounds
rather like something his mother might have said about the girl's rudeness.
There is always the possibility that with autistic children and adults, who
often seem to have such excellent rote memory for overheard material, some
expression they use which may seem to show startling social skill is simply
an echoed phrase remembered from a previous and similar context. In
David's work, and in all the writings considered here, it must be borne in
mind that some instances of social insight may simply be copied or taught
expressions. While this seems an unlikely explanation for all David's
insights, there are some instances of obvious parroting in his book which
leave the reader feeling cautious. In many cases he will make a remark and
then add, 'Well, that's what my mother says.' But an example from page 84
makes it clear that not all David's borrowed thoughts are so clearly
accredited: 'I think the thing about the pop music industry is that everything
has to be new . . . I had an old guitar teacher . . . He said the only reason for
a 12-string guitar is that everything has to be new in pop music' It is
interesting, however, that David adds his own (?), contrary opinion 'Mind
you, I think you get a fuller sound with a 12-string.' The possibility of
parroting must, therefore, cause doubt on the significance of apparently
insightful remarks such as 'I read somewhere that certain tablets can destroy
brain cells . . . Mind you, you can't believe everything you read in the
papers'(p. 97).
It would be easy to overstress David's abilities, especially when reading
his book in the context of other writings by autistic individuals after
which one tends to forget what a 'normal' standard of writing is. It is
important not to lose sight of the many odd features of David's writing. It is
clear from what he says that his behaviour has not always been as normal as
it appears to be at the time of writing. He describes his delusions, obsessions
and stereotypes when not on his present medication - and it is certain that if
he had written his autobiography at that time we would get a very different
picture of the degree of David's handicap. He says of himself, 'I think my
mind is clearer taking those Haloperidol tablets. At one time I used to think
I was in tune with the whole world. So if something happened on the news
or something I used to think it was affecting me when I wasn't feeling very
well . . . I also thought we'd got a 3-D TV at our house' (p. 87). This
perhaps sheds some light on what Asperger, in his 1944 paper, referred to as
the autistic child's 'long, fantastic stories, his confabulations . . . ever more
strange and incoherent', it being unlikely that his patients either meant to
deceive or were being overly creative in a make-believe sense.
David also writes about his obsessions at the time: 'I also had to do
certain actions at certain times, I got so mixed up. Like if I didn't have a cup
of coffee at 10am, something terrible would happen' (p. 87). The topic of
220 Francesca G. E. Happe
Hazards of interpretation
If we hope to draw conclusions about the nature of the autistic handicap
from the writings of the three authors quoted above, we must be aware of a
number of factors that are potentially misleading. Analysis of these writings
is obviously a subjective method of investigation and hence prey to all the
problems of that type of method. Some authors, for example Wolff and
222 Francesca G. E. Happe
are dissociated, and for some of the less intelligent autistic writers this
comparison may be informative. On the other hand, recent work looking at
Asperger syndrome subjects has taken schizophrenic subjects as controls
(Bowler, 1989). This choice highlights the fact that in the very able group,
some autistic people may not have seen a clinician until adolescence or later
and are therefore not suitably matched with people of subnormal IQ who
have been in special schools and homes all their lives. Unlike autobiogra-
phies of normal amateurs, writings by schizophrenics are relatively plentiful
and accessible in published form. A good anthology of these, which takes
one away from the glossier novels, is Dale Peterson's A mad person's guide
to madness, which contains many first-hand accounts of the experience of
mental illness from 1436 to 1976. A full bibliography of the accounts of
mental patients, from i960 to 1982, is also available (Sommer and
Osmond, 1983). Of the single accounts published, that by Mary Barnes
(Barnes and Berke, 1971) is in some ways a good comparison work for
Temple's book. It is a good match because the women were of similar ages
at the time of writing, and Mary Barnes rightly or wrongly would say
that she had been 'odd' almost from birth: 'Much of me was twisted and
buried, and turned in upon itself, as a tangled skein of wool to which the
end has been lost. . . The big muddle started before I was born. It went on,
getting worse. My mother and I battled with feelings. My father was in it,
then my brother barged in. My two sisters came and the mess got bigger' (p.
i3)-
An interesting parallel between the two accounts is that both Mary and
Temple have a machine they appeal to for relief from their problems. As we
have seen, for Temple this is her squeeze machine. For Mary it is a box
which her therapists have built down in the cellar: 'We try it, it's beautiful.
A big wooden box. You bend down to go in the opening. There's coloured
lights inside. . . . It's super. Stay in the box and you really go places. I want
to try i t . . . This was my biggest delight, the box. I sat still in there . . . I was
"going somewhere" in the box. It was to give me experiences out of this
world. The lights went on and off. You watched them' (pp. 98, 102).
Compare Temple's description of her machine: 'The squeeze chute I
ultimately built was that secret, coveted cubby hole of childhood dreams.
Sometimes I worried that the squeeze chute would overpower me, and I
would not be able to survive without it. Then I realised that the chute was
just a restrictive device made from scrap plywood. It was a product of my
mind' {Emergence, p. 96).
Striking too is a similar, extreme reaction to tactile stimuli: 'Although I
lay in a stupor for most of the time, I was very aware of what was going on.
Touch seemed to mean everything. By it I moved away or inwardly moved
nearer' (Barnes and Berke, p. 113). Compare this with Temple's description
of her tactile experiences: 'As a child I wanted to feel the comfort of being
held, but then I would shrink away from fear of losing control . . . I could
Autobiographical writings 225
In this section autism and Asperger syndrome are again used interchangeably on the
understanding that the underlying communication handicap is the same in both, although
Asperger syndrome is a mild manifestation of this handicap.
226 Francesca G. E. Happe
- as seen in our authors' works - will have implications for our definitions
of Asperger syndrome, since relatively good language is perhaps the only
unanimously agreed distinguishing feature of this group.
In what follows, the premises and conclusions of Relevance theory are
outlined, with suggestions at each stage as to the possible breakdown of
normal functioning in autism.
In autism?
It is a common observation that autistic people seem to miss what we would
regard as salient in a situation, and pay close attention to what seems to us
irrelevant. This is also reflected in experimental findings, for example,
Rincover and Koegel (1975) found that, in training, autistic children tended
to learn responses to irrelevant details of the teaching situation such as the
teacher's dress and so on, which severely limited the generalisation of learnt
responses. There are at least three possible reasons why autistic children
may fail to turn their attention to what we would regard as relevant. The
first is suggested by Frith (1989), who argues that autistic children do not
process stimuli for meaning, a tendency which is pervasive in normal human
subjects. In her words, 'A good decision [about what to attend to] would be
based on large amounts of pooled information. If coherence at this central
decision-making point is weak, the direction of attention would be quite
haphazard.' Frith suggests that it is the autistic child's inability to take
account of context that both produces excellent block design performance
and leads to their inability to process stimuli for meaning (as demonstrated
in Frith, 1970). As we have seen, it is the processing of information in
context that gives the contextual effects which are weighed against pro-
cessing effort in the estimation of relevance. An individual who cannot
process information as part of a larger context, therefore, would obviously
be peculiar in his calculation of relevance and hence deviant in his
deployment of attention.
On the other hand, just as attention would be oddly focused in a person
unable to derive normal contextual effects, it is also possible that the
autistic child's odd attention springs from peculiarities in the costs of
processing. Throughout Relevance theory it is stressed that the interpre-
tation of ostensive behaviours and utterances is guided by the inability to
tolerate nonsense and irrelevance. For example, it is said to be a character-
istic of powerful ostensive stimuli such as speech signals that they are
'stimuli which both pre-empt the attention . . . and are irrelevant unless
treated as ostensive stimuli'. Thus the inability of humans to treat utter-
ances as if irrelevant leads to a type of disambiguation of speaker's
meaning and, as we shall see, ultimately an appeal to speaker's attitudes
and intentions. But what if the cognitive system did not demand meaning
and did tolerate the speaker's message being interpreted as irrelevant? We
began with the idea that new, unconnected information that could only be
228 Francesca G. E. Happe
processed piecemeal would not be worth processing, since the costs would
outweigh the meagre benefits and so violate the principle of Relevance. But
what if such piecemeal processing was not too costly to be pursued? Is it
possible that in at least some autistic people the same cognitive architec-
ture that allows extraordinarily good rote memory, and even outstanding
'savant' abilities, could lead to an abnormally low level of processing
costs? This idea is, of course, only the other face of Frith's theory of lack
of central coherence leading to an inability to process for meaning. It may
be that such unusual memory ability is a coping mechanism for this
inability to process information in context for meaning. As usual in
development the causal directions are hard to establish without experi-
ment. It may be that the inability to process for meaning is the cause of
compensatory rote memory skills. But it is also possible that someone with
such good rote memory would never be driven to the cognitively economic
measure of processing and storing meaning or gist. The breakdown of the
usual search for relevance due to a peculiarity in either the costs or the
benefits of processing are not incompatible hypotheses. It may, however,
be interesting to investigate the effects of excellent rote memory on
language by looking at communication as a function of 'savant' abilities,
which, after all, also occur in retarded but non-autistic individuals. Cer-
tainly, it seems likely that the normal cost/benefit analysis that underlies
the directing of attention towards what is relevant is deviant or defective in
(many) autistic individuals.
A third and last possible breakdown in the assessment of relevance, and
one that might have more subtle effects on communication than the inability
to calculate normal costs or effects, also derives from the possibility that
autistic people have a peculiar memory organisation. Sperber and Wilson
stress 'the crucial importance of the organisation of encyclopedic memory in
the pursuit of relevance'. It is only due to some assumed universality in
memory organisation, and hence in the accessibility and so cost of retrieving
contexts, that the speaker can estimate the relevance her remark will have
for her listener. If autistic people have radically different memory organisa-
tions, this system will break down, and with it communication. It is likely
that whether due to innately different rote memory abilities, the develop-
ment of superior rote memory to compensate for an inability to process
information in context for meaning or just because of this last inability -
autistic memory will show differences in organisation. Certainly this is a
hypothesis that should be amenable to test. In its turn, this difference in
memory organisation would alter the costs of processing, which, as we have
seen, are a vital component in the usual calculation of relevance and hence
of the normal focus for one's attention. Because of the balance of costs and
benefits underlying relevance, Sperber and Wilson claim that 'At every stage
in disambiguation, reference assignment, and enrichment the hearer should
choose the solution involving the least effort, and should abandon this
Autob iograp hical writings 229
In autism?
There is less wrong with autistic language than with its usage. Indeed, in the
works above it is hard to find anything formally wrong, rather the reader is
left with an overall impression of oddness. While it is true that some autistic
people never develop language, it is more striking that even those who do
still fail somehow to communicate fully. At its extreme, this presents as
those cases of hyperlexia where language seems far in advance of communi-
cation.
While there is little work that looks directly at the inferential abilities of
230 Francesca G. E. Happe
than coding in normal intercourse (that is, irony, metaphor and less special
cases of indirectness), which seem incomprehensible to even the more able
autistic person (recall Temple and her 'door').
In autism?
Research into the autistic child's theory of mind (Baron-Cohen, Leslie and
Frith, 1985) has found a severe impairment in most autistic subjects' ability
to comprehend another person's false belief. Leslie (1987), in his metare-
presentational conjecture, has shown how representing such mental states,
along with pretence, requires a more advanced form of representation (the
representation of representations). This level of secondary representation or
metarepresentation seems to be lacking in the autistic person's processing of
social situations. Thus most autistic people are handicapped in the under-
standing of others' mental states. For such people, inferential communica-
tion - which requires the recognition of intentions - may be an unattainable
goal. This would leave them, perhaps, with only coded communication,
which may be what underlies the repetitious echolalic or single word
instrumental speech of many less able autistic individuals. Without the
ability to recognise the intention to inform, many autistic people may be
unable to recognise ostensive behaviour, or to distinguish it from non-
ostensive behaviour. This might explain the apparent 'deafness' and delay in
9
Inferential in the sense that the audience infers the communicator's intentions from evidence
provided for this purpose. For example, in reply to the question 'How are you feeling?' I
may do a cartwheel and three back-flips. There is no code to tell you this means I am pretty
well, but I have made clear my intention to show you just that.
23 2 Francesca G. E. Happe
example, where pedantry is avoided. If someone asks you how much you
earn, you will in most cases not give the precise figure but a rounded
estimate that is less costly for the hearer to process (in the absence, that is, of
any indication of need for greater precision that would justify the greater
processing costs). Sperber and Wilson claim that there is no discontinuity
between such loose uses and figurative speech. Both occur simply as a result
of the speaker's search for relevance, which leads her to adopt a more or less
literal interpretation of her thought.
It is because in communication we are concerned with the speaker's
intentions, that, as Sperber and Wilson put it, 'the same piece of evidence
can be used, on different occasions, to make manifest different assumptions,
even mutually inconsistent assumptions, as long as it makes manifest the
intention behind the ostention'. The initial message we derive from the
speaker saying 'The weather is lovely', for example, is not <The weather is
lovely> but rather <The speaker is saying that 'The weather is lovely'>.
Thus the speaker's attitude to what she is saying, her intention in saying it,
becomes of vital importance. Why is the speaker saying this? If the weather
is obviously horrible then the search for relevance may lead the hearer to
understand the speaker as being sarcastic. Thus the same surface form may
make manifest different assumptions, according to the different communi-
cative intent underlying its utterance. And, as before, it is the criterion of
consistency with the principle of Relevance that allows us to decide which
assumptions are warranted.
Metaphorical expression then, is just another way of striving for rele-
vance in one's communication. Therefore, 'whatever abilities and pro-
cedures are needed to understand it [metaphorical expression] are indepen-
dently needed for the interpretation of quite ordinary, nonfigurative utter-
ances'. As above, in metaphors the logical form of the utterance is not an
explicature (that is, not part of the intended interpretation). So, as with the
weather example, the explicature the hearer derives from the utterance,
'This room is a pig-sty' is not <This room is a pig-sty>, but rather <The
speaker is saying that 'This room is a pig-sty'>. (Note the similarity
between this form and Leslie's form for metarepresentations, Agent-
informational relation-"expression", for example, I PRETEND "this empty
cup contains water".)
In autism
If, as I have suggested above, even able autistic people are communicating
without the guarantee of relevance (due to an inability to represent
second-order intentions), then communication should be most likely to
break down for them where the speaker's attitude must be taken into
account in modifying the literal meaning of the sentence used. Those autistic
people who lack even first-order theory of mind may be operating with the
propositional form of the utterance as the explicature they derive in
234 Francesca G. E. Happe
communication, that is, they will be doing precisely what we have just said
normal communicators using ostention do not do.
It is widely reported that even the most verbally able autistic people (that
is, people with Asperger syndrome) fail to understand non-literal speech
such as sarcasm, joking and metaphorical expressions. From Sperber and
Wilson's theory it follows that these autistic people must also be handi-
capped in their understanding of literal, non-figurative utterances. This is
likely in view of the common finding that able autistic speakers are
inappropriately pedantic in their communication (for example, Szatmari et
al. (1989) found that 60 per cent of their sample of able autistic adults
showed 'overly formal speech'). Similarly they seem to fail to recognise the
connotations behind words the attitude people have to the words they use
or the intentions behind their choice of word (recall David's 'mousey
coloured hair'). Without the principle of Relevance to guide them the
autistic person may fall back on a literal interpretation of all utterances. So
when someone says 'The weather is lovely' the autistic person derives the
explicature <The weather is lovely>. This may serve well enough to get by
when the speaker is speaking literally, but the autistic person must be
baffled by ironic or figurative usage - and also, perhaps, where the speaker
is simply mistaken, or is lying. A highly intelligent person with autism/
Asperger syndrome, such as Temple, may learn to recognise situations
where people 'do not mean what they say' working on simple rules such
as:
literally false or puzzling speech + smile = joke
literally false or puzzling speech + frown = sarcasm
But without the principle of Relevance to guide them, the transparency of
intentions that allows us to use language in a truly flexible way is not open
to autistic communicators. In the face of the puzzle that our ostensive
communication must pose them, they may have no choice but to adopt a
rigid interpretation a default value of the propositional form of the
utterance in place of our shifting and mercurial intentions.
THE PROPOSITIONAL
FORM OF AN UTTERANCE
is an
INTERPRETATION
of
an INTERPRETATION of a DESCRIPTION of
Fig. 7.1 The possible interpretive and descriptive relations between the
propositional form of an utterance, the speaker's thought and what that
thought represents (from Sperber and Wilson, 1986)
the speaker. Figure 7.1 adapted from Sperber and Wilson, shows the two
sorts of representative relations at play in various types of speech act, plus
some examples of the types of utterance that might fall into each category.
According to the figure, some types of utterance are interpretive representa-
tions of interpretive representations of states of affairs. It is tempting to
equate Sperber and Wilson's two types of representation with Leslie's
(1987) representation/metarepresentation distinction. This leads to some
interesting predictions. If 'interpretations' are metarepresentations, and
'descriptions' are primary representations, then every utterance is under-
stood as a metarepresentation, since an utterance is 'an interpretive
expression of a thought of the speaker and the hearer makes 'an interpretive
assumption about the speaker's informative intention'. As such, the implica-
tion is that without a theory of mind, autistic communication never works
like normal communication, and perhaps succeeds only by keeping to a
strictly literal expression of the meaning, where the explicature they derive
is simply the propositional form of the utterance. So, in the figure, the
propositional form of the utterance will always be taken as a literal
interpretation of the speaker's thought. In other words this parameter will
236 Francesca G. E. Happe
similes. And indeed we find many examples of simile in the works of our
three autistic writers. For example, Temple writes: 'relations between
people are like a glass sliding door. The door must be opened gently, if it is
kicked it may shatter' ('My experiences', p. 145). Barry too uses similes, for
example, he says about having a new girl-friend: 'I think it wouldn't be wise
behaviour to both spend money on her and practice behaviour with her at
the same time. It's just like you cannot build the roof of a house first then the
foundation later.'
Metaphor, on the other hand, requires some understanding of intentions.
In a metaphor the propositional form of the utterance is a more or less loose
interpretation of the speaker's thought. Therefore metaphors cannot be
fully understood or properly used without a first-order theory of mind -
using a default value of literalness will not work. Not surprisingly, then,
examples of metaphors are much harder to find in the writings. Temple, for
example, seems to have a tendency to take metaphors literally - as in the 'I
am the door' example. Rather like examples of humour, metaphors are
conspicuous by their absence. Barry does use some metaphors, in passages
of the letters which appear to be parroted, but in a strange way: 'I read an
article on the trouble of our transportation. They are finding cures for the
illness of our transportation. Transportation has been getting sick before I
was born, and is getting sicker every year.' Interestingly, Mary Barnes, the
schizophrenic writer quoted above, often uses rich and evocative metaphors
in her book, for example, she writes: 'Slowly I worked free from the past,
from the web . . . A whole lifetime could be spent making, outside oneself,
webs to match how one is inside. To go into madness, to start to come out,
to leave the web, is to fight to get free, to live . . .' (p. 162).
Lastly, irony is more demanding still, requiring as it does an understand-
ing of second-order metarepresentations since it is an interpretation of an
interpretation (an attributed thought). There is not, to my knowledge, a
single example of irony in any of the writings by our autistic authors. Here
again control works would be useful to tell us the usual incidence of irony,
metaphors, similes, exclamatives and so on - and a more direct test of the
comprehension of such forms may circumvent the problems of parroting or
rote learning.
Relevance theory can lead us to make some non-trivial and non-intuitive
predictions about autistic language use and, with theory of mind
explanations of autism, may help us to understand many of the features of
our autistic authors' writings. In addition, Relevance theory may shed light
on other elements of autistic behaviour not explained by a lack of theory of
mind. For example, I have mentioned the poor understanding we have of
obsessive behaviours and interests in Asperger syndrome. One explanation
may be that these behaviours are a consequence of the failure to calculate
relevance normally, and part of the autistic person's odd focus of attention.
Imagine, for example, if the driver of a car paid as much attention to the
Autobiographical writings 239
position of his seat-belt as to what was going on in the road in front of him.
Or if someone placed as much importance on the fact that their lunch was
late as most people would on the very late return of a loved one who is
usually prompt. Such odd focusing of attention would appear obsessional. It
may be, then, that it is not the degree of attention or anxiety, but its odd
focus and subject - the peculiar attachment of importance to apparently
irrelevant things - that makes the autistic person seem obsessive. Much the
same point could be made about the incidence of fixations in autistic people
- it may be the oddness and incompleteness of their interests that earns this
label rather than any great difference in passion between them and the
normal train-spotter. A child who talks about electricity pylons all the time
is more likely to be thought oddly fixated than one who talks about horses
or football teams. Such phenomena as obsessions and fixations may be seen
as in part a result of the breakdown of normal relevance.
Conclusions
Current interest in Asperger syndrome may be seen as a response to the
puzzle which autistic people as able as our three authors present to
researchers in this field. These writers represent the most optimistic answer
to the question 'How far can autistic children go in matters of social
adaptation?' (Kanner, 1973). We asked at the start of this chapter just what
it could be that makes us say these impressively able people are still autistic.
Here we have looked at their writings, which are a testament to their
success, and picked out some elements of style and content that seem to be
characteristic of the autistic handicap. 11 To do this I have applied the most
stringent analysis - judging these writers by normal rather than handi-
capped standards. As can be seen, they come off very well, but I would
suggest that what flaws their writings do show are significant. My sug-
gestion is that even these very able autistic adults show some characteristic
handicap in communication.
Having suggested that there is a communication handicap, Relevance
theory was presented as a framework for understanding this deficit. This
theory helps us to understand the autistic communication handicap by
showing how a theory of mind, and specifically the ability to handle
metarepresentations such as intentions, may be vital to the full ostensive-
inferential communication that we as human beings enjoy. In Sperber and
Wilson's words, 'Communication exploits the well-known ability of
humans to attribute intentions to each other.' Relevance theory, therefore,
allows us to reason from the now well-known work showing a deficit in
autistic subjects' theory of mind (Baron-Cohen, Leslie and Frith, 1986) to
the well-documented autistic communication handicap. It goes further than
TT
I would like to thank Temple Grandin and David Miedzianik for their kind permission to
quote from their writings.
240 Francesca G. E. Happe
References
Asperger, H. (1944). Die 'Autistischen Psychopathen' im Kindesalter. Archiv fur
Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten, 117, 76-136.
Barnes, M. & Berke, J. (1971). Mary Barnes - two accounts of a journey through
madness. New York: Penguin.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989a). The autistic child's theory of mind: a case of specific
developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry', 30, 285-98.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989b). Do autistic children have obsessions and compulsions?
British journal of Clinical Psychology', 28, 193-200.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a
'theory of mind'? Cognition, 21, 3746.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioural and
intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 4, 113-25.
Bemporad, J. R. (1979). Adult recollections of a formerly autistic child, journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 17997.
Bowler, D. (1989). Theory of mind in Asperger's syndrome. Paper presented at the
conference of the British Psychological Society, London.
DesLauriers, A. M. (1978). The cognitive-affective dilemma in early infantile
autism: the case of Clarence, journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 8,
219-28.
Frith, U. (1970). Studies in pattern detection in normal and autistic children: I.
Immediate recall of auditory sequences, journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76,
413-20.
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.
Grandin, T. (1984). My experiences as an autistic child and review of selected
literature, journal of Orthomolecular Psychiatry, 13, 14475.
Grandin, T. (in press). An inside view of autism. In E. Schopler and G. B. Mesibov
(eds.), High-functioning autism. New York: Plenum.
Grandin, T. & Scariano, M. (1986). Emergence labelled autistic. Tunbridge Wells:
Costello.
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2,
217-50.
Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism.
American journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242-6.
Kanner, L. (1973). How far can autistic children go in matters of social adaptation?
In L. Kanner, Childhood Psychosis: initial studies and new insights. Washington:
Winston.
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretence and representation: the origins of 'theory of mind'.
Psychological Review, 94, 412-26.
Miedzianik, D. C. (1986). My autobiography. Intro, by Elizabeth Newson. Notting-
ham: Child Development Research Unit, University of Nottingham. (Available
from publisher, 2.50 inch inland postage. All proceeds go to author.)
Peterson, D. (1982). A mad person's guide to madness. Pittsburgh: University of
Pittsburgh Press.
Rincover, A. 8c Koegel, R. L. (1975), Setting generality and stimulus control in
autistic children, journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 8, 23 5-46.
242 Francesca G. E. Happe
243
244 Name index
Kanner 4, 34, 39, 42., 43, 46, 68, 80, 92, 93, Rutter & Schopler 148, 182
94, 96, 104, i n , 119, 122, 146, 207, 214, Rutter etal. 15,35
220,239,241
Kanner & Eisenberg 41, 92, 104, 109, 119 Schneider 91
Kanner etal. 12,21,34 Schopler 101,116,120,122,146
Kay & Kolvin 100, 119 Schopler & Mesibov T , 12,35
Kerbeshian & Burd 165,181 Schreibman 16,35
Kerbeshian et al. 13,34 Schroder 91
Kimura & Archibald 163, 181 Scott 158, 182
Kimura &C Vanderwolf 163,181 Semrud-Clikeman &c Hynd 13,35
Klages 11,34,70,91,92 Seneca 32
Kleist 57,92 Shah 3,35
Kolvin 108, 119 Shah & Frith 162, 182
Kretschmer 13,34,91 Shea &: Mesibov 111,120
Snowling 63,92
Langdell 158,181 Sommer & Osmond 224,242
Leff etal. 172, 181 Sparrow et al. 4,35,107,120
Leslie 18,34,230,231,235,241 Sperber & Wilson 20, 3 5, 207, 225, 226,
Leslie & Frith 18, 19,34 228-30, 232-5, 242
Leslie & Thaiss (in press) 18,34 Szatmari et al. 12, 13, 14, 36, 100, 117,
Lockyer t Rutter 4, 34, 108, 119 120, 142, 146, 178, 182, 234, 242
Losche 2,34 Szurek 106, 120
Lutz 5,34
Tager-Flusberg 154, 182, 230, 242
Mahler 106, 120
Tantam 1, 13, 36, 101, 120, 148, 14979,
Mesibov 147, 181
182
Mesibov &C Stevens 24, 34
Tantam et al. 157,158,165,168,182
Miedzianik 21621,241
Miles & Capelle 165, 181 VanDijk 154,182
Morton 19,34 Van Krevelen 99, 104, 116, 120, 122, 146,
Nagy & Szatmari 13,34,100,120,122, 168, 182
143, 146 Voeller 167,182
Newson 106, 120 Volkmar 4,36
Newsonetal. 12, 34, 149, 150, 177, T 8 I Volkmar & Cohen 222, 242
Volkmaretal. 101,120,148,182,183
O'Gorman 106, 120
Ozonoff, Pennington et al. (in press) 15,35 Waterhouse et al. 108,116,120
Ozonoff, Rogers et al. (in press) 13, 19,35 Weintraub &C Mesulam 107, 121, 167, 183
White & White 222,242
Perneretal. 18,35
WHO 1,36,104,121,148,170,183
Peterson 224, 241
Wing 1, 5, 21, 36, 97, 103, T08, i n , 117,
Rank 106, 120 121, 122, 126, 140, 145, 146, 148, 150,
Rapin 107, 120 161, 179, 183, 221, 242
Rapin &t Allen 107, 120 Wing & Attwood 5,17,36
Rees & Taylor 3,35 Wing & Gould 17,36,40,92,102,108,
Rifkin et al. 173, 181 121, 148, 149, 179, 183
Rincover & Koegel 227, 241 Wing &c Wing 111,121
Rourke et al. 107,120 Winner & Leekam (in press) 236,242
Rumsey 15,35 Wolff 6c Barlow 13, 36, 99, 100, 121, 158,
Rumsey et al. 12,14,35 183, 2212, 242
Rumsey &c Hamburger 4,15,35 Wolff &c Chick 13, 36, ^9, 100, 116, 121,
Rutter 155, 182 172, 183
Rutter & Hersov 1,35 Wolff & Cull 173,183
Rutter &c Lockyer 4,35 Wurst 4 , 3 6 , 5 3 , 9 2
Subject index
able autistic 12, 88, 207, 221, 240 Bleuler, Egon 6,38
abstract thinking 49, 74 borderline disorders 100,101
adolescents 4,110,147,179, brain,
Anne 24,2016 abnormality 16, 1659
anti-social behaviour 1767 damage 64, 65, 67
anxiety 14,80,211
Asperger, Hans 512, 312, 938, 105, calculating ability 45, 46, $$, 75, 88
122, 192 calendar skills 75,94,109
Asperger's syndrome Camberwell childrens' study 10811
controversy 2, 116-18, 148, 223 camouflage 22,30,31
definition 12,13,1056,114,240 cases
diagnostic criteria amateur actor 13 0-2
Gillbergs' 123 late talker 1034
Wing's 114 lawyer 1247
Tantam's 149 little frog expert 13 2-5
prevalence 2,102,110 little professor 13 79
subcategories 164,240 lonely cyclist 2630
attention, abnormalities of 49, 62, 76, 162, mathematician 135-7
180, 227, 236, 238 one-dress girl 12730
autism, train enthusiast 978
diagnostic criteria clumsiness 11, 14, 49, 55, 141, 142, 162-4
DSM-III 124 cognitive explanation 1621, 22539
DSM-III-R 124 coherence, lack of 4, 220, 2278, 236
Wing's 112-14 collections 812, 159
positive aspects of 32, 50, 74, 8790 communication 22939
subtypes of 2 - 3 , 12, 116 problems 173,22539
autistic/dereistic thinking 38 compensatory learning 213
autistic continuum 5, 19, 3 1, 102, 11117 conduct problems 40, 50, 79
intelligence 623,707 construction test 44
autobiographical writings 20742 contact disturbance 44, 46, 56, 74
awareness of other minds 140, 195, 196, control groups 223
200, 214, 216, 218 conventions, awareness of 18 8-91,199
creativity 70, 216, 219
Barnes, Mary 224-5,238
Barry 21316 DAMP syndrome 102,145
birth injury 50, 64, 166-7 delusions 219
245
246 Subject index