Academy of Management The Academy of Management Review
Academy of Management The Academy of Management Review
Academy of Management The Academy of Management Review
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
http://about.jstor.org/terms
Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access
to The Academy of Management Review
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
? Academy of Management Review
1993, Vol. 18, No. 4, 694-734.
STEPHEN W. GILLILAND
Louisiana State University
Parts of this article were based upon the author's dissertation, and thanks are given to
the committee members: Neal Schmitt, Dan Ilgen, Steve Kozlowski, and Mike Lindell. The
author would also like to thank Jose Cortina, Cynthia Gilliland, and Craig Russell for their
helpful comments on various drafts of this article.
694
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 695
and hire highly qualified applicants, which, in turn, can influence the
overall utility of selection procedures (Boudreau & Rynes, 1985; Murphy,
1986). From an ethical perspective, organizations should be concerned
with the effects of selection procedures on the psychological well-being of
applicants. For example, the perceived fairness of selection testing may
influence the efficacy and self-esteem of rejected applicants (Robertson &
Smith, 1989). Finally, from a legal perspective, the perceived fairness of
the selection procedure may influence applicants' decisions to pursue
discrimination cases. Additionally, the 1991 amendments to the Civil
Rights Bill allow for jury trials in discrimination cases, creating the pos-
sibility that issues of face validity and perceptions of fairness may be-
come a more salient issue with statistically naive jurors.
In addition to these practical outcomes associated with the perceived
fairness of selection systems, research in this domain can be of theoret-
ical importance. Although few attempts have been made to empirically
assess reactions to selection procedures, even less attention has been
given to developing a theoretical model of these perceptual processes.
Preliminary models of reactions to selection systems have been proposed
(e.g., Arvey & Sackett, 1993; Schuler, 1993), but all of these lack a solid
theoretical framework. A natural theoretical orientation that can be used
to organize this area of research is that of organizational justice theories
(e.g., Greenberg, 1990b).
This article extends theories from the organizational justice literature
to the selection domain with the goal of advancing both organization
justice theory and the understanding of selection fairness. In this article
a comprehensive model that describes procedural and distributive factors
influencing applicants' reactions to selection systems is developed. The
model also links fairness perceptions to a variety of attitudinal and be-
havioral outcomes. To provide a basis for this model, the major tenets of
organizational justice theory are briefly reviewed, highlighting issues of
importance for the extension of justice literature to selection processes.
During the discussion of the model, selection research that has examined
reactions to selection procedures is reviewed in an effort to establish
what has been investigated and where further research is needed.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
696 Academy of Management Review October
Distributive Justice
Procedural Justice
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 697
Thibaut and Walker (1975) suggested that procedural justice and dis-
tributive justice are independent dimensions. Some researchers have
supported that procedural and distributive justice perceptions are statis-
tically independent (Alexander & Ruderman, 1987; Greenberg, 1986a).
Others have indicated that perceptions of procedural and distributive
justice are highly correlated (Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Fryxell & Gordon,
1989). A second question about the relationship between procedural and
distributive justice is the relative importance of each in relation to indi-
vidual reactions and organizational outcomes. Theorists have clearly
demonstrated that in many situations perceptions of procedural justice
account for more variance in a variety of dependent measures than do
perceptions of distributive justice (Alexander & Ruderman, 1987; Dipboye
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
698 Academy of Management Review October
& de Pontbriand, 1981; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Cropanzano,
1991).
The statistical interaction between procedural and distributive jus-
tice also has been examined. Greenberg (1987a) found that perceived
fairness of the outcome was related to the interaction between outcome
favorableness (which has tended to be related to distributive fairness)
and procedural justice such that just procedures led to perceptions of fair
outcomes, even when the outcome was not favorable. Similar findings
were obtained by Leung and Li (1990) when they evaluated individuals'
reactions to a new subway-fare scheme. In this case, increased process
control was found to be related to more favorable reactions with negative
outcomes, but it was less related to favorable reactions with positive
outcomes.
Summary
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 699
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
700 Academy of Management Review October
FIGURE 1
Model of Applicants' Reactions to Employment Selection Systems
PROCEDURAL
JUSTICE RULES OUTCOMES
search. In addition, some organizational justice issues that have not been
previously addressed are developed.
The Model
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 701
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
702 Academy of Management Review October
TABLE 1
Relationships Among Procedural Rules and Other Justice and
Reactions Models
Formal Characteristics
Job relatedness Accuracy rule (L), Job relatedness (A&S),
Representativeness (I&R), Transparency (S)
(S&L)
Opportunity to perform Voice (T&W), Soliciting Thoroughness of
input (G), Resource knowledge, skills, and
(S&L) abilities coverage
(A&S)
Reconsideration Ability to modify rule Opportunity for
opportunity (L), Ability to correct reconsideration (A&S),
(S&L), Ability to Review scores (A&S)
challenge (G)
Consistency Consistency rule or Consistency across
standard (L), (S&L), candidates (A&S)
(G), (T&B)
Explanation
Feedback Timely feedback (T&B), Feedback form (S),
Timeliness (S&L) Type/degree of
feedback (I&R)
Selection information Information (S&L), Information (S), (A&S),
Communication (S&L), System-development
Explanation (T&B) process (A&S)
Honesty Truthfulness (B&M) Feedback honesty (S)
Interpersonal Treatment
Interpersonal effectiveness Respect (B&M) Sympathetic treatment
(I&R)
Two-way communication Two-way communication Participation (S)
(G), Consider views
(T&B)
Propriety of questions Propriety of questions Illegal variables (A&S)
(B&M), Personal bias
(L), Bias suppression
(S&L), (T&B)
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 703
treatment composes the next category. The final three rules, interper-
sonal effectiveness of the administrator, two-way communication, and
propriety of questions, all relate to the interpersonal treatment of appli-
cants. Figure 1 and Table 1 also allow for the possibility of other proce-
dural rules that may influence fairness perceptions but that are not di-
rectly linked to organizational justice literature and have not been exam-
ined in the applicant reactions literature. Two such possible procedures
relate to concerns about the ease of faking answers and the invasiveness
of questions. The following discussion elaborates on each of these pro-
cedural rules.
Job relatedness. Perhaps the greatest procedural influence on fair-
ness perceptions is the job relatedness of the selection device. Job relat-
edness refers to the extent to which a test either appears to measure
content relevant to the job situation or appears to be valid. The appear-
ance of validity can be captured in both the content validity sense (i.e.,
test content related to job content) and the criterion-related validity sense
(i.e., performance on the test is likely related to performance on the job)
(Smither & Pearlman, 1991). Job relatedness should be distinguished from
face validity, which refers to "what the test appears to measure" or
"whether the test looks valid" (Anastasi, 1988: 136). A test can appear face
valid (appear to measure what it actually measures) without appearing
job related, and a test can appear job related (in the criterion-related
sense) without appearing face valid.
In the organizational justice literature, Leventhal (1980) defined the
accuracy rule as decisions being based on as much good information as
possible, and he suggested that procedural fairness is violated when
performance is evaluated on the basis of inappropriate information. Sim-
ilarly, Sheppard and Lewicki (1987) found evidence for a resource rule,
which suggests that decisions should be based on accurate resources and
expertise. Prior models of applicants' reactions to testing include job re-
latedness (Arvey & Sackett, 1993), job relevance (Iles & Robertson, 1989),
and task relevance (Schuler, 1993).
Many researchers have examined the job relatedness of tests and the
effects of this relatedness on perceptions of fairness. Positive reactions in
terms of perceived face validity, perceived accuracy, and perceived fair-
ness have been documented at assessment centers (Dodd, 1977) and with
work-sample tests (Schmidt et al., 1977), which are both highly job re-
lated. Schmitt, Gilliland, Landis, and Devine (1993) found that both per-
ceived job relevance and overall perceived fairness were higher with a
content-valid, computerized work-sample test than with simple typing
and dictation tests. In this case, job relevance was highly correlated with
perceived fairness. Smither, Reilly, Millsap, Pearlman, and Stoffey (1993)
assessed perceived job relatedness for different types of selection tests
and found that interviews, assessment-center tasks, and cognitive ability
tests that included concrete items were perceived to be more job related
than personality tests, biodata forms, and cognitive ability tests that in-
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
704 Academy of Management Review October
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 705
scoring were both issues proposed by Arvey and Sackett (1993) as impor-
tant in determining perceived fairness of selection systems. The only
testing domain in which reconsideration opportunity has been examined
with regard to applicant reactions is drug testing. Drug testing was given
high ratings of justifiability (a construct presumably related to fairness)
when test results were checked with a second testing method (Murphy,
Thornton, & Reynolds, 1990). Further research should consider the impact
that reconsideration opportunities have on applicant reactions.
Consistency of administration. Consistency or standardization is a
procedural factor discussed in much of the procedural justice literature
(Greenberg, 1986a; Leventhal, 1980; Sheppard & Lewicki, 1987; Tyler &
Bies, 1990). Consistency refers to ensuring that decision procedures are
consistent across people and over time. Arvey and Sackett (1993) sug-
gested that consistency across people refers to consistency in the content
of the selection system, in scoring, and in the interpretation of scores.
When considering consistency across people, Leventhal (1980) pointed out
that this rule is similar to the distributive rule of equality, which suggests
that all people should have an equal chance of obtaining the decision
outcome.
Concerns for standardization or consistency of test administration
have been demonstrated with drug-testing programs, which received
greater acceptance or perceived effectiveness when they involved all in-
dividuals or only those with a history of drug use and not a random
selection of individuals (Gomez-Mejia & Balkin, 1987; Murphy et al., 1990).
It seems reasonable to expect that concerns for consistency may be influ-
enced by the type of test and may be more salient for some types (e.g.,
interviews) than for others (e.g., paper-and-pencil tests). This procedural
rule suggests a prediction that structured interviews should be perceived
as more consistent in administration, and, therefore, they are considered
more fair than unstructured interviews. Although individuals may not
always be aware of the treatment given to others and, therefore, are
unable to evaluate consistency, a clear example of a violation of consis-
tency across people is the situation in which applicants receive jobs be-
cause of who they know, not what they know.
Given that consistency evaluations rely on comparisons with other
people or previous experiences, two factors that may influence the sa-
lience of the consistency rule are the previous experiences of the appli-
cant and the time at which perceived fairness is assessed. Applicants
who have experienced both structured and unstructured interviews may
be more likely to associate interview structure with consistency of admin-
istration. With regard to time or stage of the selection process, applicants
may be more aware of consistency after a selection decision has been
made and applicants have learned the basis for the decision from orga-
nizational sources or from conversations with other applicants.
Feedback. The provision of timely and informative feedback is cited
as an important factor in perceptions of interactional justice (Tyler & Bies,
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
706 Academy of Management Review October
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 707
(1989) found that attitudes toward testing were more favorable when peo-
ple were told how the test related to future job performance. It is possible
that validity evidence would be particularly useful for tests with low face
validity, such as cognitive ability tests (Huffcutt, 1990). With the current
interest in cognitive ability testing, the provision of validity evidence may
be one relatively cost-free method for improving the acceptance of such
testing.
Arvey and Sackett (1993) suggested that validity evidence may be
perceived differently by the public than by selection specialists. Specifi-
cally, whereas selection specialists are concerned with improved overall
prediction, the public may be more concerned with the extent to which the
selection system makes mistakes. This may be particularly true of integ-
rity testing and drug testing, where negative outcomes suggest that re-
jected applicants may engage in some form of deviant behavior.
Another type of selection information that may influence perceptions
of fairness is a priori information on the selection process. Arvey and
Sackett (1993) hypothesized that the reduction in uncertainty that such
information would provide, particularly with unfamiliar selection pro-
cesses, would reduce applicants' beliefs that they performed poorly be-
cause they did not know what to expect. Stone and Kotch (1989) found
more negative attitudes (defined in terms of perceived fairness and inva-
sion of privacy) toward drug-testing programs that provided no advance
notice of testing than those that did.
Honesty. Bies and Moag (1986) highlighted the importance of honesty
and truthfulness when communicating with recruitees. In particular, in-
stances of either candidness or deception would likely be particularly
salient. Schuler (1993) discussed the importance of openness and honesty
as components of feedback content. Although honesty may be inherent
with other forms of explanation (e.g., selection information or feedback),
research suggests that it is a distinct and important component of appli-
cants' reactions. For example, research on interviews has demonstrated
that interviewer correctness, sincerity, and believability are strong pre-
dictors of affect toward reactions to the interview, impressions of the
organization, and intentions to accept a job offer (Liden & Parsons, 1986;
Schmitt & Coyle, 1976).
Interpersonal effectiveness of administrator. The interpersonal effec-
tiveness of the test administrator refers to the degree to which applicants
are treated with warmth and respect. Bies and Moag (1986) analyzed by
content recruitees' descriptions of fair and unfair treatment and found
that one dimension of fairness was related to respect or, alternately,
rudeness. Similarly, Iles and Robertson (1989) discussed the possible im-
pact that sympathetic treatment may have on applicants during the se-
lection process.
Research on reactions to interviews demonstrated that the warmth
and thoughtfulness of an interviewer was the strongest predictor of im-
pressions of the company and expectations regarding job offers and ac-
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
708 Academy of Management Review October
ceptance of those offers (Schmitt & Coyle, 1976). Similarly, Liden and
Parsons (1986) found that the strongest predictor of general affect of an
interview was the extent to which the interviewer was personable. Rynes
(1991) summarized the research on recruiters' personality and behavior
traits and indicated the factor that explained the most variance in various
dependent variables was the affect of the recruiter (e.g., warmth and
empathy). It is important to note that interviewer and recruiter research
cited with regard to both honesty and interpersonal effectiveness did not
directly assess the impact of honesty and interpersonal effectiveness of
reactions to fairness. Although dependent variables included impres-
sions of recruiter, affect toward reactions to the interview, and probability
of accepting a job offer, the direct impact on reactions to fairness has not
been assessed.
Although interpersonal effectiveness is clearly an important factor in
applicants' reactions to interviews, it may also be an important factor in
other aspects of the selection process. For example, a test administrator
who simply administers a paper-and-pencil, work-sample, or drug test
may be able to affect the comfort and stress level of applicants and in-
fluence applicants' reactions to the testing process in general. Rynes
(1993) provided an example of an applicant who felt like a criminal during
drug testing because she was escorted by a uniformed guard to a doorless
toilet stall.
Two-way communication. Two-way communication refers to the op-
portunity for applicants to offer input or to have their views considered in
the selection process (Tyler & Bies, 1990), but it differs from the opportu-
nity to perform in that it relates primarily to interpersonal interaction. The
research by Martin and Nagao (1989) demonstrated the difference be-
tween two-way communication and opportunity to perform. Simulated
applicants for a high-status job expressed more anger and resentment
toward computerized and paper-and-pencil interviewing than toward tra-
ditional face-to-face interviewing. Though all interviewing formats pre-
sumably provided adequate opportunity to perform, the nontraditional
interview formats did not allow for the two-way communication that ap-
plicants appear to expect from interviews. Similarly, Schuler (1993) cited
research that demonstrated more favorable impressions and reactions to
nondirective interviews than to directive interviews. Although not a se-
lection situation, research has found that the opportunity for appraisees
to express their feelings was one of the strongest predictors of perceived
accuracy and fairness of performance appraisals (Landy, Barnes, & Mur-
phy, 1978).
Two-way communication also can refer to the opportunity to ask
questions regarding the job, the organization, or even the selection pro-
cess. The selection system must provide applicants with adequate oppor-
tunity to gain information that is relevant to making acceptance deci-
sions. If such opportunities are not found, applicants' satisfaction with
the selection process will likely be lessened. Clearly, procedural justice
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 709
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
710 Academy of Management Review October
Direct support for this proposition is limited, but the previously mentioned
research of Smither and colleagues (1993) demonstrated the link between
the type of selection test and perceived job relatedness, and Kluger and
Rothstein (1991) demonstrated a relationship between test type and op-
portunity to perform (perceived control). Indirect support for the influence
of test type on the other procedural rules was cited in the previous liter-
ature review pertaining to opportunity to perform (Bies & Shapiro, 1988),
feedback (Schmidt et al., 1978), and two-way communication (Martin &
Nagao, 1991).
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 711
The type of human resource policy will also influence the satisfaction
or violation of a number of procedural rules.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
712 Academy of Management Review October
about the types of factors that influence the salience of procedural rules,
although theoretical bases for these predictions are weak and they must
be examined through subsequent research. The following factors are pre-
dicted to influence the salience of procedural rules: (a) types of selection
procedures encountered, (b) the extent to which a procedural rule is vio-
lated, (c) previous experiences of the applicant, and (d) the time at which
fairness reactions are collected.
The types of selection procedures encountered will likely have an
impact on the relative weighting of procedural rules. For example, given
the predominance of research examining interviewer effects in selection
and recruiting interviews (Rynes, 1991), interpersonal treatment rules
(i.e., interpersonal effectiveness, two-way communication, and propriety
of questions) will likely be most salient when interviews are experienced
as part of the selection process. Opportunity to perform was also sug-
gested to be an important procedural rule regarding interviews. Simi-
larly, job relatedness has been a research concern with paper-and-pencil
tests (Smither & Pearlman, 1991), and it has been cited as an advantage
of work-sample tests and assessment centers (Dodd, 1977; Schmidt et al.,
1977); therefore, the job-relatedness procedural rule will likely be the
dominant component in perceptions of the fairness of these procedures.
The following proposition summarizes these possible relationships:
Although the research and theory behind this proposition are some-
what weak, initial support is offered from one study (Gilliland, 1992).
Newly hired individuals related incidents of fair and unfair treatment
during the selection processes of their latest job search, and these inci-
dents were sorted into procedural categories that were roughly correspon-
dent to the 10 procedural rules. Distribution of incidents in the procedural
categories differed for different selection procedures, suggesting that
concerns of job relatedness were most salient with paper-and-pencil tests
and work-sample tests, whereas ease of faking answers was the domi-
nant concern with honesty and personality tests.
A second factor that may influence the relative weighting of proce-
dural rules in forming fairness perceptions is the extent to which a rule is
violated. Research on impression formation and individual decision mak-
ing indicates that negative information is often more salient and, there-
fore, weighs more heavily when an individual forms an evaluation, com-
pared to either neutral or positive information (e.g., Fiske & Taylor, 1984;
Wright, 1974). Similar findings have been consistently observed in eval-
uations related to selection interviews (Schmitt, 1976) and performance
appraisals (Steiner & Rain, 1989). Extended to the domain of procedural
justice, this research suggests that rule violation is likely to be more
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 713
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
714 Academy of Management Review October
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 715
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
716 Academy of Management Review October
' In terms of formally expressed outcome to input ratios (e.g., Adams, 1965), equity can
be defined in the following manner:
IC Ip
0c = I'(Op/Ip) = (If/IP)OP
The combination of current inputs and past inputs and outcomes can be conceptualized as
performance expectations, or the extent to which a person thinks he or she will get the job.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 717
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
718 Academy of Management Review October
not qualified for the job but were given the job anyway, they responded
with higher work quality and increased self-perceived qualifications.
Work quantity often decreased in these studies, but as noted by Pritchard
(1969), this decrease may have had more to do with deflation of self-
esteem than with inequity.
It should be noted that overpayment effects of inequity have not been
as consistently demonstrated as underpayment effects, and in many
cases overpayment effects are short lived (Campbell & Pritchard, 1976;
Kanfer, 1990). In addition, Proposition 10 does not preclude the possibility
that the hiring decision will also have a main effect on fairness reactions.
Applicants who do not expect to get the job and then receive an offer will
likely see the decision as more fair than those who expect to get a job and
do not receive an offer.
Equality. Equality suggests that all individuals should have an equal
chance of receiving the outcome, regardless of differentiating character-
istics such as knowledge or ability. In the selection situation, the satis-
faction of equality would suggest random hiring rather than hiring based
on ability or experience. However, the equality rule may be more impor-
tant in terms of its violation rather than its satisfaction, and it may be
more salient for job-irrelevant differentiating characteristics (e.g., sex or
ethnic background) than for relevant characteristics (e.g., qualifications).
Thus, if a person is hired on the basis of ability, equality is not violated,
but if sex or ethnic background appears to bias the hiring decision, equal-
ity is clearly violated.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 719
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
720 Academy of Management Review October
eration during the hiring decision is made salient to job applicants, the
needs rule may become more dominant in fairness evaluations, and the
decision will be seen as more fair (less unfair) by both positively and
negatively affected applicants.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 721
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
722 Academy of Management Review October
Organizational Outcomes
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 723
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
724 Academy of Management Review October
example, Arvey and colleagues (1975) found that as the time lag between
initial application for a job and initiation of the selection process in-
creased (thereby violating the feedback rule), so did the percentage of
applicants withdrawing from selection. Reactions to interview proce-
dures also have been shown to influence individuals' intentions to accept
job offers (e.g., Linden & Parsons, 1986; Schmitt & Coyle, 1976). Although
most research has examined job acceptance in terms of acceptance in-
tentions, it is important to also examine actual job-acceptance decisions
because the two may not be as closely related as one would expect
(Rynes, 1992).
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 725
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
726 Academy of Management Review October
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 727
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
728 Academy of Management Review October
light the influence that individuals' reactions to the fairness of the selec-
tion process may have on their outcomes after they are hired.
CONCLUSIONS
REFERENCES
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 729
Adams, J. S., & Rosenbaum, W. E. 1962. The relationship of worker productivity to cognitive
dissonance about wage inequities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 46: 161-164.
Alexander, S., & Ruderman, M. 1987. The role of procedural and distributive justice in
organizational behavior. Social Justice Research, 1: 177-198.
Arvey, R. D., Gordon, M. E., Massengill, D. P., & Mussio, S. J. 1975. Differential dropout
rates of minority and majority job candidates due to "time-lags" between selection
procedures. Personnel Psychology, 28: 175-180.
Arvey, R. D., & Sackett, P. R. 1993. Fairness in selection: Current developments and per-
spectives. In N. Schmitt & W. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection: 171-202. San Francis-
co: Jossey-Bass.
Arvey, R. D., Strickland, W., Drauden, G., & Martin, C. 1990. Motivational components of
test taking. Personnel Psychology, 43: 695-716.
Bierhoff, H. W., Cohen, R. L., & Greenberg, J. 1986. Justice in social relations. New York:
Plenum.
Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. S. 1986. Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness.
Research on Negotiation in Organizations, 1: 43-55.
Bies, R. J., & Shapiro, D. L. 1988. Voice and justification: Their influence on procedural
fairness judgments. Academy of Management Journal, 31: 676-685.
Boudreau, J. W., & Rynes, S. L. 1985. The role of recruitment in staff utility analysis. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 70: 354-366.
Campbell, J. P., & Pritchard, R. D. 1976. Motivation theory in industrial and organizational
psychology. In M. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychol-
ogy: 63-130. Chicago: Rand McNally.
Campion, J. E., & Arvey, R. D. 1989. Unfair discrimination in the employment interview. I
R. W. Eder & G. R. Ferris (Eds.), The employment interview: Theory, research, and prac-
tice: 61-72. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Cascio, W. F., & Phillips, N. F. 1979. Performance testing: A rose among thorns? Personnel
Psychology, 32: 751-766.
Cohen, R. L. 1987. Distributive justice: Theory and research. Social Justice Research, 1: 19-
40.
Crant, J. M., & Bateman, T. S. 1990. An experimental test of the impact of drug-testing
programs on potential job applicants' attitudes and intentions. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 75: 127-131.
Crosby, F., Burris, L., Censor, C., & MacKethan, E. R. 1986. Two rotten apples spoil the
justice barrel. In H. W. Bierhoff, R. L. Cohen, & J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social
relations: 267-281. New York: Plenum.
Deutsch, M. 1975. Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as
the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3): 137-149.
Dipboye, R. L., & de Pontbriand, P. 1981. Correlates of employee reactions to performance
appraisals and appraisal systems. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66: 248-251.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
730 Academy of Management Review October
Ellis, R. A., & Taylor, M. S. 1983. Role of self-esteem within the job search process. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 68: 632-640.
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. 1984. Social cognition. New York: Random House.
Folger, R. 1987. Distributive and procedural justice in the workplace. Social Justice Re-
search, 1: 143-159.
Folger, R., & Greenberg, J. 1985. Procedural justice: An interpretive analysis of personnel
systems. Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, 3: 141-183.
Folger, R., & Konovsky, M. A. 1989. Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions
to pay raise decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 115-130.
Friedman, A., & Goodman, P. S. 1967. Wage inequity, self-qualifications, and productivity.
Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 3: 340-352.
Fryxell, G. E., & Gordon, M. E. 1989. Workplace justice and job satisfaction as predictors of
satisfaction with union and management. Academy of Management Journal, 32: 851-
866.
Gilliland, S. W. 1992. Fairness from the applicant's perspective: Reactions to employee se-
lection procedures. Unpublished manuscript.
Gist, M. E., & Mitchell, T. R. 1992. Self-efficacy: A theoretical analysis of its determinants
and malleability. Academy of Management Review, 17: 183-211.
Gomez-Mejia, L. R., & Balkin, D. B. 1987. Dimensions and characteristics of personnel man-
ager perceptions of effective drug-testing programs. Personnel Psychology, 40: 745-763.
Greenberg, J. 1982. Approaching equity and avoiding inequity in groups and organizations.
In J. Greenberg & R. L. Cohen (Eds.), Equity and justice in social behavior: 389-435. New
York: Academic Press.
Greenberg, J., & Tyler, T. R. 1987. Why procedural justice in organizations. Social Justice
Research, 1: 127-142.
Hartigan, J. A., & Wigdor, A. K. 1989. Fairness in employment testing: Validity generaliza
tion, minority issues, and the general aptitude test battery. Washington, DC: Nationa
Academy Press.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 731
Heilman, M. E., Simon, M. C., & Repper, D. P. 1987. Intentionally favored, unintentionally
harmed? Impact of sex-based preferential selection of self-perceptions and self-
evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72: 62-68.
Herriot, P. 1989a. Interactions with clients in personnel selection. In P. Herriot (Ed.), Assess-
ment and selection in organizations: 219-228. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Herriot, P. 1989b. Selection as a social process. In M. Smith & I. Robertson (Eds.), Advances
in selection and assessment: 171-187. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Hunter, J. E., & Hunter, R. F. 1984. Validity and utility of alternative predictors of job per-
formance. Psychological Bulletin, 96: 72-98.
Iles, P. A., & Robert, I. T. 1989. The impact of personnel selection procedures on candidates.
In P. Herriot (Ed.), Assessment and selection in organizations: 257-271. Chichester, En-
gland: Wiley.
Jacobson, M. B., & Koch, W. 1977. Women as leaders: Performance evaluation as a function
of method of leader selection. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 20:
149-157.
Jones, E. E., Rhodewalt, F., Berglas, S., & Skelton, J. A. 1981. Effects of strategic self-
presentation on subsequent self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
41: 407-421.
Kleiman, L. S., & Faley, R. H. 1988. Voluntary affirmative action and preferential treatment:
Legal and research implications. Personnel Psychology, 41: 481-496.
Kluger, A. N., & Rothstein, H. R. 1991. The influence of selection test type on applicant
reactions to employment testing. In R. R. Reilly (Chair), Perceived validity of selection
procedures: Implications for organizations. Symposium conducted at the 6th annual
conference of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, St. Louis.
Konovsky, M. A., & Cropanzano, R. 1991. The perceived fairness of employee drug testing as
a predictor of employee attitudes and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,
76: 698-707.
Landy, F. J., Barnes, J. L., & Murphy, K. R. 1978. Correlates of perceived fairness and accu-
racy of performance evaluation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63: 751-754.
Lawler, E. E., III, Koplin, C. A., Young, T. F., & Fadem, J. A. 1968. Inequity reduction over
time in an induced overpayment situation. Organizational Behavior and Human Perfor-
mance, 3: 253-268.
Lawler, E. E., III, & O'Gara, P. W. 1967. Effects of inequity produced by underpayment on
work output, work quality, and attitudes toward the work. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 51: 403-410.
Leung, K., & Li, W. 1990. Psychological mechanisms of process-control effects. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 75: 613-620.
Leventhal, G. S. 1980. What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study
of fairness in social relationship. In K. J. Gergen, M. S. Greenberg, & R. H. Willis (Eds.),
Social exchange: Advances in theory and research: 27-55. New York: Plenum.
Liden, R. C., & Parsons, C. K. 1986. A field study of job applicant interview perceptions,
alternative opportunities, and demographic characteristics. Personnel Psychology, 39:
109-122.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
732 Academy of Management Review October
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. 1988. The social psychology of procedural justice. New York: Plenum.
Lounsbury, J. W., Bobrow, W., & Jensen, J. B. 1989. Attitudes toward employment testing:
Scale development, correlates, and "known-group" validation. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 20: 340-349.
Martin, C. L., & Nagao, D. H. 1989. Some effects of computerized interviewing on job appli-
cant responses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74: 72-80.
McFarland, C., & Ross, M. 1982. Impact of causal attributions on affective reactions to
success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43: 937-946.
Morse, S., & Gergen, K. J. 1970. Social comparison, self-consistency, and the concept of self.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16: 148-156.
Murphy, K. R. 1986. When your top choice turns you down: Effects of rejected offers on utility
of selection tests. Psychological Bulletin, 99: 133-138.
Murphy, K. R., Thornton, G. C., III, & Prue, K. 1991. Influence of job characteristics on the
acceptability of employee drug testing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76: 447-453.
Murphy, K. R., Thornton, G. C., III, & Reynolds, D. H. 1990. College students' attitudes to-
ward employee drug testing programs. Personnel Psychology, 43: 615-631.
Nacoste, R. B. 1990. Sources of stigma: Analyzing the psychology of affirmative action. Law
& Policy, 12: 175-195.
Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. 1980. Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judg-
ment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Premack, S. L., & Wanous, J. P. 1985. A meta-analysis of realistic job review experiments.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 70: 706-719.
Pritchard, R. D. 1969. Equity theory: A review and critique. Organizational Behavior and
Human Performance, 4: 176-211.
Robertson, I. T., & Smith, M. 1989. Personnel selection methods. In M. Smith & I. Robertson
(Eds.), Advances in selection and assessment: 89-112. Chichester, England: Wiley.
Ryan, A. M., & Sackett, P. R. 1987. Pre-employment honesty testing: Fakability, reactions of
test takers, and company image. Journal of Business and Psychology, 1: 248-256.
Rynes, S. L. 1991. Recruitment, job choice, and post-hire consequences: A call for new re-
search directions. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational
psychology: 399-444. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Rynes, S. L. 1993. Who's selecting whom? Effects of selection practices on applicant atti-
tudes and behaviors. In N. Schmitt & W. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in organi-
zations: 240-274. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Rynes, S. L., & Barber, A. E. 1990. Applicant attraction strategies: An organizational per-
spective. Academy of Management Review, 15: 286-310.
Rynes, S. L., Bretz, R. D., Jr., & Gerhart, B. 1991. The importance of recruitment in job choice:
A different way of looking. Personnel Psychology, 44: 487-521.
Schmidt, F. L., Greenthal, A. L., Hunter, J. E., Berner, J. G., & Seaton, F. W. 1977. Job sample
v. paper-and-pencil trades technical tests: Adverse impact and examinee attitudes.
Personnel Psychology, 30: 187-197.
Schmidt, F. L., Urry, V. M., & Gugel, J. F. 1978. Computer assisted tailored testing: Exam-
inee reactions and evaluations. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 38: 265-
273.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
1993 Gilliland 733
Schmitt, N., Ford, J. K., & Stults, D. M. 1986. Changes in self-perceived ability as a functio
of performance in an assessment centre. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 59: 327-
335.
Schmitt, N., & Gilliland, S. W. 1992. Beyond differential prediction: Fairness in selection. In
D. M. Saunders (Ed.), New approaches to employee management: fairness in employee
selection, vol. 1: 21-46. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Schmitt, N., Gilliland, S. W., Landis, R. S., & Devine, D. 1993. Computer-based testing
applied to selection of secretarial applicants. Personnel Psychology, 46: 149-165.
Schmitt, N., Gooding, R. Z., Noe, R. A., & Kirsch, M. 1984. Metaanalyses of validity studies
published between 1964 and 1982 and the investigation of study characteristics. Person-
nel Psychology, 37: 407-422.
Schneider, B. 1987. The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, 40: 437-453.
Schuler, H. 1993. Social validity of selection situations: A concept and some empirical re-
sults. In H. Schuler, J. L. Farr, & M. Smith (Eds.), Personnel selection and assessment:
Individual and organizational perspectives: 11-26. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schwinger, T. 1986. The need principle of distributive justice. In H. W. Bierhoff, R. L. Cohen,
& J. Greenberg (Eds.), Justice in social relations: 211-225. New York: Plenum.
Seymour, R. T. 1988. Why plaintiffs' counsel challenge tests, and how they can successfully
challenge the theory of "validity generalization." Journal of Vocational Behavior, 33:
331-364.
Sheppard, B. H., & Lewicki, R. J. 1987. Toward general principles of managerial fairn
Social Justice Research, 1: 161-176.
Simon, H. A. 1955. A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69:
99-118.
Smither, J. W., & Pearlman, K. 1991. Perceptions of the job-relatedness of selection proce
dures among college recruits and recruiting/employment managers. In R. R. Reilly
(Chair), Perceived validity of selection procedures: Implications for organizations. Sym
posium conducted at the 6th annual conference of the Society for Industrial and Orga
nizational Psychology, St. Louis.
Smither, J. W., Reilly, R. R., Millsap, R. E., Pearlman, K., & Stoffey, R. W. 1993. Applic
reactions to selection procedures. Personnel Psychology, 46: 49-76.
Steiner, D. D., & Rain, J. S. 1989. Immediate and delayed primacy and recency effects in
performance evaluation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74: 136-142.
Stone, D. L., & Kotch, D. A. 1989. Individuals' attitudes toward organizational drug testing
policies and practices. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74: 518-521.
Stone, E. F., & Stone, D. L. 1990. Privacy in organizations: Theoretical issues, research find-
ings, and protection mechanisms. Research in Personnel and Human Resource Man-
agement, 8: 349-411.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms
734 Academy of Management Review October
Taylor, M. S., & Bergmann, T. J. 1987. Organizational recruitment activities and applicants'
reactions at different stages of the recruitment process. Personnel Psychology, 40: 261-
285.
Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. 1975. Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Tyler, T. R., & Bies, R. J. 1990. Beyond formal procedures: The interpersonal context of pro-
cedural justice. In J. S. Carroll (Ed.), Applied social psychology and organizational set-
tings: 77-98. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wright, P. 1974. The harassed decision maker: Time pressure, distractions, and the use of
evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59: 555-561.
This content downloaded from 37.128.225.218 on Tue, 12 Dec 2017 14:58:45 UTC
All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms