Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

HHS Public Access: Brains On Video Games

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 13

HHS Public Access

Author manuscript
Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Author Manuscript

Published in final edited form as:


Nat Rev Neurosci. ; 12(12): 763–768. doi:10.1038/nrn3135.

Brains on video games


Daphne Bavelier,
Brain & Vision Lab, Department of Brain & Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester,
Rochester, New York 14627, USA

C. Shawn Green,
Center for Cognitive Science, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis,
Author Manuscript

Minnesota 55455, USA

Doug Hyun Han,


Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Chung Ang University, 224-1 HeukSeok-Dong,
Dong Jack-Gu, Seoul, Korea 156-755

Perry F. Renshaw,
Department of Psychiatry and the Brain Institute, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah 84108,
USA

Michael M. Merzenich, and


W.M. Keck Foundation Center for Integrative Neuroscience, University of California San
Francisco, California 94143, USA

Douglas A. Gentile
Author Manuscript

Media Research Lab, Department of Psychology, Iowa State University, Ames, Iowa 50011, USA
Daphne Bavelier: daphne@cvs.rochester.edu; C. Shawn Green: csgreen@umn. edu; Doug Hyun Han:
hduk@yahoo.com; Perry F. Renshaw: Perry.Renshaw@hsc.utah.edu; Michael M. Merzenich: merz@phy.ucsf.edu;
Douglas A. Gentile: dgentile@iastate.edu

Abstract
The popular press is replete with stories about the effects of video and computer games on the
brain. Sensationalist headlines claiming that video games ‘damage the brain’ or ‘boost brain
power’ do not do justice to the complexities and limitations of the studies involved, and create a
confusing overall picture about the effects of gaming on the brain. Here, six experts in the field
shed light on our current understanding of the positive and negative ways in which playing video
games can affect cognition and behaviour, and explain how this knowledge can be harnessed for
Author Manuscript

educational and rehabilitation purposes. As research in this area is still in its early days, the
contributors of this Viewpoint also discuss several issues and challenges that should be addressed
to move the field forward.

FURTHER INFORMATION
Douglas A. Gentile’s homepage: www.douglasgentile.com
ALL LINKS ARE ACTIVE IN THE ONLINE PDF
Competing interests statement
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
Bavelier et al. Page 2

Does playing video or computer games have beneficial effects on brain and
Author Manuscript

behaviour? If so, does the evidence point to general improvements in


cognitive function?
Daphne Bavelier & C. Shawn Green
Although the popular media has a strong tendency to produce breathless headlines about the
effects (or lack of effects) of video games, it is worth noting that the term ‘video games’ is
far from a single construct and thus, has almost no scientific predictive power. One can no
more say what the effects of video games are, than one can say what the effects of food are.
There are millions of individual games, hundreds of distinct genres and sub-genres, and they
can be played on computers, consoles, hand-held devices and cell phones. Simply put, if one
wants to know what the effects of video games are, the devil is in the details.
Author Manuscript

Studies that have examined perception and spatial cognition (from our lab and many others)
have focused on one specific genre of games — the so-called ‘action’ video games. Indeed,
playing this type of game results in a wide range of behavioural benefits, including
enhancements in low-level vision, visual attention, speed of processing and statistical
inference, among others. Furthermore, properly controlled training studies have repeatedly
demonstrated a causal link between video game playing and enhanced abilities. Hence, it is
not just that people who naturally choose to play games have better perceptual skills. The
ability to improve one’s abilities through practice has obvious practical ramifications, from
rehabilitation of visual skills in individuals with amblyopia (also known as a ‘lazy eye’) to
the training of surgeons.

Doug Hyun Han & Perry F. Renshaw


Author Manuscript

The extent to which playing video and online games affects the brain and behaviour is
uncertain. It is likely that the specific beneficial or harmful effects are determined by the
characteristics of both the individual and of the game. Several studies have reported that
video and online game play may improve visuospatial capacity, visual acuity, task
switching, decision making and object tracking in healthy individuals. However,
methodological limitations to these studies have also been noted. For example, cross-
sectional comparisons of gamers and non-gamers may reflect baseline differences in
cognitive abilities rather than the effects of game playing. Moreover, video game training
studies that involve the recruitment of non-gamers and that provide game experience have
not generally shown that gaming enhances performance on higher level reasoning and
problem solving tasks.
Author Manuscript

Michael M. Merzenich
The potential benefits that can be achieved through video-game play are, of course, a
function of the specific task requirements, and of the cognitive and social demands and
values represented by the game(s) in play. Games that require progressively more accurate
and more challenging judgments and actions at higher speeds, that require focused attention
and the suppression of progressively stronger distracting lures, that increase working
memory spans, that provide pro-social training contexts, and that offer increasingly harder

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 3

cognitive challenges — among many other possible game dimensions — can be expected to
Author Manuscript

drive positive neurological changes in the brain systems that support these behaviours.

There is growing direct evidence that intensive use of video games results in significant
generalized improvements in cognitive function. Video games are controlled training
regimens delivered in highly motivating behavioural contexts. The documented gains in
processing speed, attentional control, memory, and cognitive and social control that result
from playing specific games are expected. Because behavioural changes arise from brain
changes, it is also no surprise that performance improvements are paralleled by enduring
physical and functional neurological remodelling.

At the same time, it should be noted that the daily time spent playing video games in school-
age children has been shown to be inversely correlated with academic achievement,
arguably because time spent playing video games is time stolen from reading and
Author Manuscript

curriculum-related academic study.

We and others have used video-game design strategies to create training exercises that drive
targeted changes in perception, cognition, and cognitive and social control more efficiently
and more effectively. We have shown that these game-like exercises drive positive changes
in perceptual, cognitive and action control abilities paralleled by ‘corrective’ neurological
changes in the brains of trainees. Importantly, in these trained populations, intensive
exercise results in generalized benefits and, in the case of student populations, predicts
future academic success.

Douglas A. Gentile
Several lines of research demonstrate that video games can have beneficial effects. One
Author Manuscript

excellent programme of research has been conducted by Daphne Bavelier and C. Shawn
Green, examining the effects of playing what they call ‘action’ video games — in practice,
these are usually highly violent first-person shooting games such as Unreal Tournament.
Besides including violent content, action games include high speed, high perceptual and
motor load, unpredictability and an emphasis on peripheral processing. In several training
studies, these games have been found to influence various aspects of perceptual processing,
including multiple object tracking spatial resolution and central and peripheral attention
skills. In other words, when you constantly need to scan the screen to detect little differences
(because they may signal an enemy) and then orient attention to and target that area, you
become better at those perceptual and attentional skills. These are probably not so much
general improvements in cognitive functioning as they are specific skills that can be
transferred only to other similar tasks (such as the perceptual skills needed by air traffic
Author Manuscript

controllers). One recent study, for example, found that although experienced video gamers
were better at spatial navigation in computer-mediated tasks than non-experienced players,
they were not better at the same type of navigation in a real-world environment. So, what is
learned may not be a broad, general improvement in skill.

Although there are fewer studies that have examined the positive effects of video gaming on
social behaviour, there are now a couple by Tobias Greitemeyer and also by my lab. We
conducted experimental studies in the US, Japan and Singapore and found a causal short-

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 4

term effect, namely that playing pro-social games led to more ‘helping’ behaviour, whereas
Author Manuscript

playing a violent game led to more ‘harming’ behaviour. In a longitudinal study, we found
that children who played more pro-social games early in a school year demonstrated
increased helpful behaviours later in the school year.

Does playing video or computer games have negative effects on brain and
behaviour?
D.B & C.S.G
There is no question that the same characteristics that make many games effective teachers
of perceptual and cognitive skills can also be harnessed to produce maladaptive effects on
brain and behaviour. There is an extremely large body of research demonstrating a
relationship between playing certain types of violent video games and increases in measures
Author Manuscript

of aggressive thoughts. However, the subtleties regarding the size of the effects reported in
published research are often sorely lacking in popular treatments of the topic. Violent video
games alone are unlikely to turn a child with no other risk factors into a maniacal killer.
However, in children with many risk factors, the size of the effect may be sufficient to have
practical negative consequences.

In terms of the possibility of video games potentially causing ‘reduced attention’, we have
yet another concept that means different things to different people. If one means the ability
to rapidly and efficiently filter visual distractors that are quickly presented (that is, visual
attention), then clearly playing action games greatly enhances this ability. However, if one
means the ability to sustain focus on a slowly evolving stream of information, such as
paying attention in class, there is recent work that suggests that total screen time, and video
game playing time in particular, may have negative effects.
Author Manuscript

Thus, although parents and politicians typically want to view the world as black or white
(and seek yes or no answers to questions such as ‘Should I let my child play video games?’),
there is simply no getting around the fact that any complex training regimen is likely to
produce a myriad of behavioural effects. Simply put, the world really is grey, and the answer
to the above question is always ‘It depends’.

D.H.H & P.F.R


Early studies on internet addiction (involving game playing) reported altered social
behaviour, increased aggression, loneliness, reduced attention and depressed mood in
patients with internet addiction. Recent studies have also reported relatively high rates of co-
morbid psychiatric illness in people with internet addiction, including major depressive
Author Manuscript

disorder, bipolar disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and anxiety
spectrum disorders. In addition, dysfunctional family structures are thought to constitute an
important risk factor for internet and on-line game addiction. Tragically, several horrific
cases of family collapse due to internet addiction have been reported in Taiwan and Korea.
For example, a mother with an on-line gambling addiction throttled her two-year old son
who had annoyed her by asking for a meal. Similarly, a thirteen year-old son beat his mother
to death because she accused him of spending too much time playing on-line games.

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 5

Further, a couple who were immersed in an on-line game ignored their 30 month old
Author Manuscript

daughter to the point where she starved to death. We believe that there is strong evidence in
support of the view that excessive internet use or game play is associated with adverse
consequences on behaviour in some individuals.

M.M.M
Intensive game-play practices have been shown to have several negative effects on
cognition. First, exposure to fast action games, on a play level that applies to the average
regular gamer, has been shown to contribute to an increase in ADHD-related behaviours,
and — it has been argued — can lead to listlessness and discontent in slower-paced and less
stimulating academic, work or social environments.

Second, time spent playing such games is time spent away from other school- (or work-)
related, social or outdoor activities. As noted earlier, despite the cognitive gains that are
Author Manuscript

potentially attributable to it, heavy game play, especially to a level of addiction, is inversely
correlated with academic, occupational and social success.

Third, action games with anti-social (violent) content — which are particularly addictive and
provide particularly strong motivational bases for driving positive cognitive changes — have
been shown to reduce empathy, to reduce stress associated with observing or initiating anti-
social actions, and to increase confrontational and disruptive behaviours in the real world.
These effects can be expected to increase as the images and scenarios in action games
become more realistic. The increasingly heavy use of video games and related virtual-reality
simulation environments for training combat military personnel provides clear testimony of
their effectiveness for inuring the ‘player’ against the social challenges and stresses
associated with observing or voluntarily initiating aggressive and violent behaviours.
Author Manuscript

Although we can appreciate the value of such training for soldiers, policemen or emergency
room technicians, there is a serious question as to whether or not intensive exposures to such
scenarios contribute positively to empathy and human understanding in the greater society.

Fourth, as discussed below, games can be addictive. Addiction generates its own special
destructive class of neurological and social burdens.

D.A.G
There is evidence that games can have negative effects, which makes sense when one
considers that most of the effects reported are learning effects at their core. As stated by
Donald Hebb in 1940, neurons that fire together wire together. Whatever we practice
repeatedly affects the brain, and if we practice aggressive ways of thinking, feeling and
Author Manuscript

reacting, then we will get better at those. This is not to say that violent games necessarily
cause violent behaviours, because human aggression is complex and multi-causal. But it
does suggest that when we practice being vigilant for enemies and then reacting quickly to
potentially aggressive threats, we are rehearsing this script. In fact, this is what has been
shown in several studies: playing violent video games increases what is called a ‘hostile
attribution bias’, a perceptual and cognitive bias to attribute hostile intentions to others’
actions. When people with such a bias are bumped into in the hallway, they assume that it
was done with hostile intent rather than by accident, and the most automatic response is to

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 6

retaliate in some way. The most comprehensive meta-analysis conducted to date included
Author Manuscript

136 papers detailing 381 independent tests of association conducted on 130,296 research
participants. The analyses found that violent game play led to significant increases in
desensitization, physiological arousal, aggressive cognition and aggressive behaviour. By
contrast, pro-social behaviour was decreased.

This is not to say that there isn’t some disagreement about this question in the scientific
community, for example over how to interpret the size of the effect and whether it is of
sufficient practical significance. On which side of the debate an investigator falls seems, in
my opinion, to depend on whether they care most about criminal level violence or low-level
aggression. The evidence that playing video games induces criminal or serious physical
violence is much weaker than the evidence that games increase the types of aggression that
happen every day in school hallways. As a developmental psychologist, I care deeply about
this everyday aggression (verbal, relational and physical), whereas critics of the research
Author Manuscript

seem to be mostly interested in criminal violence.

With regard to attention, there are not many studies into the effect of playing video games
on the types of directed and sustained attention that is needed in the classroom, but those
that exist seem to suggest that there is a relationship between video gaming and attention
problems in school. My current interpretation is that the same attentional skills that are
learned by playing action games (such as a wider field of view and attention to the
periphery) are part of the problem. Although these are good skills in a computer-mediated
environment, they are a liability in school when the child is supposed to ignore the kid
fidgeting in the chair next to him and focus on only one thing.

How strong is the evidence that gaming can be addictive?


Author Manuscript

D.B & C.S.G


Although the lack of firmly established standards has definitely hindered research in this
field (for example, the American Medical Association does not currently recognize video-
game or internet addiction as a psychiatric disorder), there does seem to be an emerging
scientific consensus that video-game play has the potential to become pathologically
addictive. At present, the best research uses scales adapted from those developed to diagnose
pathological gambling. It is important to note that ‘pathologically addicted’ implies more
than simply spending a considerable amount of time playing games. Being pathologically
addicted means, among other things, an actual reduction in the ability to function normally
in society. Thus, an individual who plays video games forty hours per week may not meet
the criteria for being a pathological user, whereas others may exhibit pathological signs
Author Manuscript

despite substantially less total usage.

A key issue for future research concerns the characterisation of the neural pathways
underlying this pathological use of technology. We know that the frontostriatal pathway,
which strongly mediates both drug addiction and behavioural disorders such as pathological
gambling, is also activated by some video games. Unfortunately, relatively little is known
about the developmental time course of the relevant neural pathways, and even less about
how their development is affected by the use of technology.

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 7

D.H.H & P.F.R


Author Manuscript

Several recent studies have suggested that internet addiction may be harmful enough to be
categorized as a psychiatric disorder. Internet addiction is sometimes classified as a
‘behavioural addiction’ in light of its natural course, clinical symptoms, tolerance,
comorbidities and neurobiology. In other studies, internet addiction has been regarded as a
subtype of impulse-control disorder.

Possible genetic vulnerability to on-line game addiction has been reported in studies on the
genes encoding the dopamine D2 receptor, catecholamine-o-methyltransferase and the
serotonin transporter, and is consistent with the view that individuals with internet addiction
have high novelty seeking and exhibit high reward dependence behaviour. Similarly, some
games seem to have much more of an addictive potential than others.

Neuroimaging studies have documented changes in brain activity during on-line game play.
Author Manuscript

There is now evidence that brain areas that respond to game stimuli in patients with on-line
game addiction are similar to those that respond to drug cue-induced craving in patients with
substance dependence. These brain regions include the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,
orbitofrontal cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, nucleus accumbens, thalamus and caudate
nucleus.

The functional impairments observed in individuals with on-line game addiction are also
thought to be similar to the impairments observed in other addictions. For example, video-
or on-line game play has been associated with dysfunction in five domains: academic,
social, occupational, developmental and behavioural. Subjects who are typically recruited
for research studies engage in on-line game play for more than 4 hours per day or 30 hours
per week. These subjects reported a persistent desire for online gaming and unsuccessful
Author Manuscript

efforts to cut down or control on-line game playing. School grades and work performance
decreased. They also showed disruption of their daily routines (sleeping during the day and
gaming at night, irregular meals and poor hygiene) and were irritable, aggressive, and
violent when family members asked them to stop playing. Some patients borrowed
enormous amounts of money ($30,000 over three months) to support their on-line game
play. Other patients reported on-line game play after finishing school and that this prevented
them from obtaining a job or participating in significant social roles.

M.M.M
About 1 in 5 regular gamers (4–10% of school-age children and young adults) seem to meet
the medical criteria that would define them as ‘addicted’. Recent studies have revealed
alterations in the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices of dependent gamers that
Author Manuscript

parallel those recorded in alcoholics and individuals addicted to other substances of abuse. It
should be noted that addiction-related distortions in reward systems involved in learning
processes contribute to broad-ranging cognitive deficits in every other studied form of
addiction.

Our younger-age population is strongly attached to screen-delivered media in a variety of


forms. Most regular video-game players are also heavy consumers of other media; on
average they are engaged with screens for more than 10 hours per day (projecting forward,

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 8

over 20 years of their life by age 60). There is thus a massive and unprecedented difference
Author Manuscript

in how their brains are plastically engaged in life compared with those of average
individuals from earlier generations, and there is little question that the operational
characteristics of the average modern brain substantially differ from that of our ancestors. A
better understanding of the consequences of these differences in brain use for societal and
individual brain health should be high on our research agenda.

D.A.G
I began studying the issue of video-game ‘addiction’ because I was highly sceptical of it. I
believed that people were misusing the expression to mean ‘spends a lot of time gaming’,
because addictions are not defined by how much one engages in an action (for example,
drinking), but by how much it damages one’s life. I began studying gaming from this more
clinical approach, using criteria adapted from those for pathological gambling. Based on this
Author Manuscript

much stricter set of criteria — which assess dysfunction in multiple areas of life (school,
social, family, psychological and emotional functioning) — about 8% of US gamers
between 8 and 18 years of age could be considered pathological or ‘addicted’. There are
now scores of studies showing that the pattern of problems that pathological gamers face are
very similar to the problems that people with substance or gambling addictions have. Thus,
the problem seems to have some construct validity.

There are only two published longitudinal studies on this topic to date. One focused on 881
Chinese adolescents between 13 and 16, using Young’s 20-item Internet Addiction Scale.
Adolescents were surveyed twice, nine months apart. Pathological internet use predicted
increased risk of depression (but not general anxiety) nine months later, after controlling for
several potential confounding factors such as sex, age, family dissatisfaction and illness,
among others. A larger study of 3,034 Singaporean children and adolescents followed over
Author Manuscript

two years gave some of the first clear evidence of whether variables such as depression and
poor school performance are predictors of or are predicted by pathological video gaming.
Because of the large sample size, this study was able to classify gamers into four types:
those who never exhibited pathological behaviour over the two years, those who became
pathological gamers, those who were pathological at the start but stopped being
pathological, and those who were and stayed pathological gamers. As in the first study with
Chinese adolescents, depression became worse if adolescents became pathological gamers.
Anxiety, social phobia and school performance also became worse in adolescents who
became pathological gamers. Interestingly, if they stopped being pathological gamers, their
depression, anxiety, social phobia decreased and school performance improved. These
findings suggest that these variables may be outcomes of pathological technology use rather
than predictors of it. At a minimum, they suggest that these variables are co-morbid with
Author Manuscript

pathological gaming, such that they can influence each other.

To my knowledge, however, there haven’t been any published studies looking at the effects
of gaming addiction on the brain, although there are some demonstrating that dopamine is
released and that brain reward centres are activated during video-game playing.

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 9

Is there a place for using video or computer games in education and


Author Manuscript

rehabilitation?
D.B & C.S.G
Some of the recent successes using off-the-shelf games (which were designed with no
particular outcome in mind, other than being a fun game) in the rehabilitation of, for
instance, amblyopia, are certainly a cause for optimism. However, although the idea of using
video games in educational and rehabilitative settings has been around for decades, as a field
we’re probably still only in the very early stages of learning how to effectively harness the
power of video games while simultaneously attempting to produce a desired outcome. For
instance, many of the earliest educational video games were little more than slightly dressed
up flashcards — full of sounds and interesting looking graphics, but lacking most of the
characteristics that truly define a video game. Too often those developing the games were
Author Manuscript

individuals who knew a lot about the content they wanted to teach, but very little about how
make a game compelling and fun. As more true game developers turn their attention to
educational and/or clinical applications, new fields are emerging in which educational and
medical practitioners are collaborating with game designers to develop fun and attractive
activities that will guarantee time on task and at the same time have the educational or
rehabilitation impact that experts in the field are seeking.

D.H.H & P.F.R


There have been several trials of video games in educational and rehabilitation settings. In
the rehabilitation of patients with post-traumatic stress disorder following motor vehicle
accidents, the virtual-reality experience (through a computer game) of driving or riding in a
car may improve clinical symptoms and promote recovery. In addition, several games have
Author Manuscript

been developed for screening or rehabilitation of people with dementia. Furthermore, we


have reported that eight weeks of internet-game play reduced delusional thinking and extra-
pyramidal symptoms in patients with schizophrenia. In addition, pro-social video games
have been associated with increased empathy and decreased reported pleasure at another’s
misfortune, compared with the effects of neutral games in healthy subjects. In a pilot study
in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders, we noted increased social behaviour and
increases in fusiform gyrus activity in response to emotional words and emoticons during a
six week, pro-social on-line game playing period. Although existing research is limited, we
believe that a growing number of clinical applications for video-game play will emerge over
time.

M.M.M
Author Manuscript

Video games per se don’t have a special role to play in these arenas. Video games exploit
well-established principles of motivation and learning that have been established by
experimental psychology and neuroscience research. Those same principles have also been
applied by us and by others in designing ‘brain training’ exercises to drive targeted,
positively empowering and, if necessary, ‘corrective’ behavioural and neurological changes
in the brains of children and adults who are in need of help. These forms of game-like
training have already strengthened or recovered the abilities and improved the prospects and

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 10

quality of life of millions of individuals. There is, of course, a convergence in the design of
Author Manuscript

successful video games and effective plasticity-based brain training exercises, because
enthusiastic engagement by a gamer or trainee is a key goal in both arenas. However,
because education- or rehabilitation-directed training is necessarily designed to address
neurological impairments that apply to specific learning problems or for specific clinical
indications, there are inevitably practical constraints on such exercise designs. It should also
be noted that the educational and medical applications of brain training are most effectively
deployed by using internet-based strategies, so that trainee compliance and progress can be
monitored, and by applying internet-delivered assessment tools to assure that generalized,
targeted benefits are being achieved. Video games do not implement these monitoring and
outcome-assessment methods.

These game-like computer-based training programmes represent the first wave in an


impending revolution for brain training in schools, medicine and in the broader society. In
Author Manuscript

the future, such computer-guided brain training may be employed to substantially improve
the performance of almost every child in school. In parallel, using this approach to drive
strengthening, ameliorative or corrective changes that increase resilience in people who are
at risk for certain illnesses, or to treat patients whose brain function is impaired by illness, is
rapidly emerging as an important new dimension of psychiatric and neurological medicine.

We must remember that the application of this technology in humans has a potentially
destructive side. It is easy to impair human abilities by training, even while other abilities
are being improved or refined. Furthermore, video games shall continue to evolve in forms
that are increasingly addictive. Time spent on screen-delivered media can be expected to
steal more time away from real life. Video game attraction strategies that have been
empirically developed to capture the hearts and minds of the player are already being more
Author Manuscript

extensively applied by the persuaders than by the educators or the medical practitioners. The
Genie (neuroscience-guided brain plasticity) is out of the bottle, for good and — if we let it
loose without more guidance and restraint — almost certainly, for ill.

D.A.G
Games offer significant promise for education. They use many of the techniques that a truly
exceptional teacher uses. For example, they have clear objectives that are set at multiple
difficulty levels to adapt to the prior knowledge and pace of each learner; they require
learning to be active, with immediate feedback and sufficient practice to the point of
mastery; practice on a game continues until much of what is learned becomes automatic;
mastery of a game is reinforced extrinsically, by points and levels, and intrinsically, by a
feeling of accomplishment and social status; levels of progress are well-sequenced, such that
Author Manuscript

success at later levels is contingent upon mastering earlier levels; games encourage
distributed practice across time; and games enable the gamer to practice the same concepts
in different contexts, therefore encouraging transfer of skills. Unfortunately, games have yet
to find a way to live up to their promise. Authors such as James Gee have documented the
theoretical value of games for education. Studies of educational software demonstrate that
children do learn from playing educational games. Nonetheless, the amount of money spent
on educational games is a tiny fraction of the amount spent on a commercial entertainment

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 11

game. Therefore, most educational games aren’t as interesting, fun or good as even a
Author Manuscript

mediocre commercial game.

What are the challenges and future directions for neuroscience research in
this field?
D.B & C.S.G
The mechanisms by which video-game play triggers such widespread brain plasticity remain
to be elucidated. And because behavioural and non-invasive brain imaging methods can only
take us so far towards this goal, pharmacological studies in humans and complementary
studies in animal models (yes, rats playing Call of Duty — or at least the animal equivalent)
may move the field forward. Beyond the clear theoretical interest, findings from such studies
will be of great practical benefit when attempting to design games that result in transferable
Author Manuscript

learning, be it for rehabilitation purposes, education or training.

One of the remaining challenges is to better understand which game components are crucial
for promoting a given skill in a particular individual. Although our current knowledge is at
the group level — for example, some overarching game components, such as the need to
constantly predict when and where events of interest may occur, are crucial in training
attention and executive functions — the most efficient learning regimens are unlikely to be
one-size fits-all. Games in the future will have the ability to gather data about the player
while simultaneously building the exact game needed in real time. A handful of pilot
training schools are already exploring this type of highly personalized tutoring.

D.H.H & P.F.R


First of all, objective diagnostic criteria for gaming addiction should be established. In
Author Manuscript

addition, we need to better understand the differences between pure on-line gaming
addiction and on-line gaming addiction that is co-morbid with other psychiatric disorders.
This is important as some investigators have argued that internet addiction does not exist but
is merely a symptom of psychiatric illnesses such as major depressive disorder or ADHD.
Second, the vulnerabilities to on-line gaming addiction, including genetic and cognitive
factors, need to be more clearly defined. Third, standard and effective treatments need to be
developed and validated. Several pilot studies have suggested that the antidepressant drugs
citalopram and bupropion may be effective for the treatment of on-line gaming addiction.
Similarly, cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) has been reported to be effective for
reducing internet use time and improving daily life patterns and family coherence. However,
future studies on on-line gaming addiction treatment will require larger populations and
longer follow-up periods. In addition, the relationship between clinical symptoms and
Author Manuscript

changes in brain activity needs to be more clearly defined. Finally, studies of internet-game
play are currently somewhat polarized and this area of research is likely to be improved if
investigators acknowledged both the potential beneficial and harmful effects of video
games.

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 12

M.M.M
Author Manuscript

Scientists and technologists have now developed practical strategies that strongly engage a
large proportion of people from all over the world (especially those of younger ages) at a
level of positive motivation that can progress to addiction in many of them. Modern
societies have come to be massively media-engaged and media-dependent, over an
incredibly short time-span in our history. The application of motivationally-powerful
strategies to help children and adults change their behaviours (and brains) for the better has
already begun in earnest. Because of their great didactic efficiencies, and because brain
plasticity-based exercises can improve the performance characteristics of the brain of almost
every child, these new game-like tools shall be at the core of a schooling revolution. They
might also be widely deployed to treat the specific neurological problems that characterize
psychiatric and neurological illnesses, and the normal ageing process.
Author Manuscript

How can we intelligently control this development? First, we should work to further
integrate cognitive neuroscience with educational science and clinical medicine. Our
understanding of the differences between the operational brains of normal versus
developmentally, neurologically or psychiatrically impaired individuals is rapidly
increasing, as is our understanding of the neural bases of human intelligence and ability.
This knowledge will provide the foundation for both designing and confirming the
effectiveness of targeted training tools. Second, the entry of new game-like training
programmes into schools and clinics must be based on controlled, high-standard trials. The
scientific community and the public should insist that any medical claims about training
programmes are based on formal review processes (by the US Food and Drug
Administration or equivalent). Third, the public would benefit from standards organizations
that objectively quantify the positive and negative consequences of the intensive use of
specific video games. Finally, we should intensify our efforts to determine how our growing
Author Manuscript

screen dependence in our everyday lives is changing us in ways that both strengthen and
weaken us, as individuals and as a society.

D.A.G
Perhaps the greatest challenge facing us right now is the trap of biased and dichotomous
thinking. Most people (including many scientists) are either critics or proponents of games
and gaming research. This has a detrimental effect on the field, and serves to increase
rhetoric and to limit research. I have proposed that there are at least five dimensions along
which video games can have effects on brain and behaviour — the content, context,
structure and mechanics of games, and the time spent game playing. When all these
dimensions are taken into account it is often possible to explain how research findings that
Author Manuscript

initially seem to be contradictory are actually congruent.

The amount of time that people spend on recreational games can have effects on them,
regardless of specific game features. Some studies have demonstrated that the amount of
time spent playing games predicts poorer school performance. This effect is likely to be due
to displacement of other academically beneficial activities. Other studies have demonstrated
a relationship between the amount of sedentary gaming and obesity, repetitive strain
disorders and video-game addiction.

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.
Bavelier et al. Page 13

Most of the research on video game effects has focused on the content dimension. In short,
Author Manuscript

people learn the content of whatever games they play. If they play educational games, they
learn the educational content and can apply it in school; if they play games designed to teach
health content, they learn those concepts and apply them to their lives; if they play violent
games, they learn the violent content and may apply it to their lives.

The context of game play may produce differential effects, and this is the least researched
dimension at this time. Context can be defined within the game or outside of the game. One
type of within-game context can be seen in violent games that allow for either team-based or
‘free-for-all’ modes of play. Both may be equally violent, but playing in an ‘everyone-for-
oneself mode’ might lead to more aggressive thoughts, lower empathy and greater
desensitization. If the in-game context requires players to cooperate to achieve goals, this
might also teach teamwork and social coordination skills. Furthermore, the social context
outside of the game may matter. Playing a violent game in a room (virtual or real) with other
Author Manuscript

individuals might increase the effects on aggression because players are giving each other
social support for aggression. However, it might actually reduce these effect if one’s
motivations are pro-social (that is, to help your friends in the game). No studies have yet
tested these hypotheses.

The way in which the game is structured and displayed on the screen can also have effects.
Screen structure provides information that is learned, similar to how we learn to perceive
visual information in general. Some studies have demonstrated that gaming improves visual
attention skills, including the ability to acquire three-dimensional information from flat
screens. Playing games that require those skills has also been shown to improve mental
rotation skills.
Author Manuscript

Finally, the mechanics dimension refers to what can be learned from playing a game with
different types of game controllers. Depending on the type of controller, different fine and
gross motor skills and balance skills can be improved and these effects could be harnessed
for therapeutic purposes. The intersection of structure and mechanics is the continuous
feedback loop that is often referred to as ‘hand–eye coordination’.

Considering these different dimensions when analysing the effects of video games should,
hopefully, reduce dichotomous thinking in the field. Playing video games is neither good nor
bad — existing research shows that they are powerful teaching tools and therefore we need
to harness that potential, aiming to maximize the benefits while minimizing the potential
harms.

Acknowledgments
Author Manuscript

D.H.H. thanks the Korean Game Culture Foundation for data and technical support.

Nat Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 November 04.

You might also like