Davis Et Al. 2015 JCP Decision To Forgive
Davis Et Al. 2015 JCP Decision To Forgive
Davis Et Al. 2015 JCP Decision To Forgive
Prominent models and interventions designed to promote forgiveness have distinguished one’s decision
to forgive from achieving forgiveness as an end state, but because of a lack of a strong measure, there
is a weak research base on making a decision to forgive. Thus, in three studies, the authors developed
the Decision to Forgive Scale (DTFS) and examined evidence for its reliability and construct validity.
The article focused on distinguishing making a decision to forgive from achieved level of forgiveness.
Scores on the DTFS showed evidence of reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranging from .92
to .94, and a 1-week temporal stability coefficient of .68. Using several strategies, the authors demon-
strated that the DTFS is empirically distinct from the Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivations
scale (TRIM; McCullough et al., 1998). Namely, a 3-factor confirmatory factor analysis that included the
DTFS and the 2 TRIM subscales showed excellent fit, suggesting these instruments assess 3 different
constructs. The DTFS was only moderately related to the TRIM subscales, was more strongly related to
stage of change than the TRIM, and predicted subsequent TRIM scores in a cross-lagged model. Finally,
although decisions to forgive generally suggested greater forgiveness, these constructs interacted to
predict existential distress. Namely, as decisional forgiveness increased, revenge was more strongly
related to existential distress. Overall, the DTFS shows considerable promise for further clinical and basic
research applications.
Scientific research on forgiveness has burgeoned in recent years, Malcolm, 2008) and enrichment (Worthington et al., in press), or
with work linking forgiveness to mental and physical health and treatment of older adults (Allemand, Steiner, & Hill, 2013); (b)
identifying key correlates (for a review, see Fehr, Gelfand, & Nag., theory and research on forgiveness and appraisals of justice
2010), such as empathy (McCullough, Worthington, & Rachal, (Worthington, 2006) and on stress-related outcomes (Berry &
1997), benevolent attributions about the offender (Fincham, 2000), Worthington, 2001; Orcutt, 2006; Wade, Vogel, Liao, & Goldman,
and cultural influence (Hook, Worthington, & Utsey, 2009; 2008); and (c) theory and research on adapting of forgiveness to
Sandage, Hill, & Vang, 2003). Furthermore, there is a thriving client’s specific needs and culture (Davis et al., 2013; Hook et al.,
applied literature focused on forgiveness interventions (for a meta- 2009; Worthington et al., 2010).
analytic review, see Wade, Hoyt, Kidwell, & Worthington, 2014). One such idea is the distinction between making a decision to
Counseling psychologists have played prominent roles in this forgive and one’s actual level of forgiveness (Worthington,
work. For example, they have developed (a) forgiveness interven- Witvliet, Pietrini, & Miller, 2007). This distinction is included
tions for a variety of specific contexts, including group therapy
in much prominent theorizing and some research on promoting
(Wade et al., 2014), couples therapy (Greenberg, Warwar, &
forgiveness (Wade et al., 2014). For example, Worthington et
al. (2007) adduced research evidence to suggest that emotional
forgiveness was related more to physical and mental health than
This article was published Online First January 26, 2015. was a decision to forgive, but decisions were more related to
Don E. Davis, Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, relational and spiritual outcomes. However, despite the theo-
Georgia State University; Joshua N. Hook, University of North Texas; rizing and qualitative research support, the research base on
Daryl R. Van Tongeren, Hope College; Cirleen DeBlaere and Kenneth G. decisions to forgive has been hampered because prior attempts
Rice, Georgia State University; Everett L. Worthington, Jr., Virginia Com-
to measure the construct resulted in a scale that did not show
monwealth University.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Don E.
adequate divergent validity with other measures of forgiveness
Davis, Department of Counseling and Psychological Services, Georgia (Worthington, Hook, Utsey, Williams, & Neil, 2007). There-
State University, P.O. Box 3980, Atlanta, GA 30302-3980. E-mail: fore, the purpose of the present article is to develop a measure
ddavis88@gsu.edu of one’s decision to forgive and to conceptually and empirically
280
DECISION TO FORGIVE 281
distinguish decisional forgiveness from forgiveness as an end mate decision to seek greater forgiveness, but she was now more
state. compassionate toward and patient with herself. Relieved of guilt,
From a stress-and-coping perspective (Exline, Worthington, she had greater energy to work toward forgiving while also setting
Hill, & McCullough, 2003; Worthington, 2006), offenses are in- appropriate boundaries and developing coping skills to engage in
terpersonal stressors that can lead to unforgiving emotions (e.g., gradual healing.
resentment, bitterness, anger) and motivations via appraisals of Although decisional forgiveness is considered an early step in
injustice, which are threatening. For example, consider Sarah who the prominent models of promoting forgiveness, the construct has
recently found out that her partner of 10 years has been involved a meager research base of only a handful of published studies
in a sexual affair for 6 months. Complicating her recovery, she (Holeman et al., 2011; Hook et al., 2013; Hook, Worthington,
feels pressure from her family and spiritual community to forgive. Utsey, Davis, & Burnette, 2012; Watkins et al., 2011). All used the
Feeling guilty because she believes that she should forgive, based same unpublished measure—The Decisional Forgiveness Scale
on her spiritual beliefs, she also feels afraid of being exploited (DFS; Worthington et al., 2007)—which has substantial limita-
again. If Sarah attended an intervention based on any of the leading tions. Most notably, that measure showed poor evidence of diver-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
models for promoting forgiveness, she would learn a more precise gent validity, being much too strongly related (i.e., rs as high as
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
definition of forgiveness. Forgiveness involves a reduction of .70; Hook, 2007) to the most widely used measure of forgiveness
negative (and perhaps an increase in positive) cognition, emotions, (i.e., the Transgression-Related Interpersonal Motivations Scale
and motivations toward an offender, and these changes often lead [TRIM]; McCullough et al., 1998). In addition, compared to the
to positive behavioral changes (Exline et al., 2003). Thus, forgive- TRIM, the DFS did not show a distinct pattern of relations with
ness is not excusing, justifying, exonerating, accepting, moving on, other constructs. Thus, our goal in the present article was to
seeking justice, forbearing, forgetting, or reconciliation. In addi- develop a measure of decisional forgiveness and differentiate it
tion, she would learn that, for severe betrayals for which hurt and conceptually and empirically from the TRIM.
resentment does not dissipate rapidly within a few weeks (Mc- One reason that the initial version of the DFS may have had
Cullough et al., 2003; McCullough, Luna, Berry, Tabak, & Bono, such poor evidence of divergent validity is that items were based
2010), forgiveness will likely require an effortful process that on a highly nuanced distinction between behavioral intentions and
involves two parts. forgiving motivations or emotions (Worthington et al., 2007).
The first part involves making a decision to forgive (DiBlasio, Indeed, items on the DFS (e.g., “I will try to help him or her if he
2000; Enright & Fitzgibbons, 2000; Exline et al., 2003). Making a or she needs help”; “I will try to get back at him or her” [reverse
decision to forgive involves committing to apply energy and effort scored]) appear conceptually very similar to items on the TRIM
to regulate negative emotions, thoughts, and behaviors until un- (e.g., “I’d find it difficult to act warmly toward him/her”; “I’m
forgiving emotions (e.g., hurt, resentment, bitterness) are substan- going to get even”). This fine-grained distinction between behav-
tially reduced. Indeed, for severe offenses, victims decide to for- ioral intentions and motivations did not hold up well empirically.
give without knowing how much energy, effort, or time will be In the current article, we focused on a more basic distinction: If
required, or if forgiveness is even possible. Decisions to forgive forgiveness is an endpoint that involves substantially reducing
may occur in discrete moments—like flipping a switch (e.g., negative (and in some situations increasing positive) thoughts,
making a clear and memorable decision to forgive)— especially if emotions, and motivations toward the offender, then deciding to
the decision is publicly acknowledged through a public ritual or forgive involves making a deliberate commitment to apply energy
communication (Hook et al., 2009). This step is important because and effort to move toward that endpoint. Indeed, consistent with
the remainder of the intervention assumes that the client is willing Prochaska and DiClemente’s (1982) Transtheoretical Model of
to work on forgiving the offense. Change, clients may enter treatment with a range of motivations to
The second part involves learning strategies to promote forgive- forgive: They may (a) feel vehemently opposed; (b) be considering
ness, based on the particular theoretical model. For example, in the the idea, but fear forgiveness will leave them vulnerable to exploi-
REACH Forgiveness model (Worthington, 2006), after deciding to tation; (c) have decided to forgive, but be exploring ways of
forgive, clients are taught that greater forgiveness is achieved increasing forgiveness; (d) have decided to forgive and be actively
through a process of emotional replacement, as the victim trans- employing coping strategies; or (e) feel that they have achieved
forms unforgiving emotions with positive, other-oriented emotions complete forgiveness. Forgiveness interventions usually seek to
such as empathy, sympathy, compassion, or love (Exline et al., allow clients to express this ambivalence and have their complex
2003). Thus, clients learn to alter their story about the offense in feeling validated before asking clients to move toward a decision
ways that reduce negative rumination and practice various activi- or commitment to working on the goal of forgiveness.
ties designed to promote other-oriented emotions, such as empathy This approach is informed and aligned with core themes of
or gratitude. counseling psychology such as strength-focused interventions
This distinction may be especially helpful for clients who feel (Fowers, 2005) and emphasis on common factors such as therapy
cultural pressure to forgive from their family or community (Davis alliance (e.g., Flückiger, Del Re, Wampold, Symonds, & Horvath,
et al., 2013; Hook & Worthington, 2008; Hook et al., 2009). In our 2012). For example, before moving into application of intervention
example, Sarah felt an obligation to forgive that will likely inter- activities, assessing the degree to which clients have made a
fere with healing. Although Sarah wants to forgive, it relieved decision to forgive helps the therapist or group leader negotiate
pressure when her counselor told her that forgiveness does not and clarify the tasks and goals of therapy to establish a strong
require repairing the relationship. The concept of decisional for- therapy alliance. Although forgiveness interventions have strong
giveness also helped her develop realistic expectations of herself. evidence of efficacy, there may be ways to enhance their potency
She accepted her current level of forgiveness. She made a legiti- by tailoring treatment to client’s degree of decisional forgiveness.
282 DAVIS ET AL.
For example, clients with high levels of decisional forgiveness ined whether decision to forgive at Time 1 predicted unforgiveness
may not benefit from spending several hours completing activities at Time 2.
designed to reduce ambivalence and increase alignment with the Finally, although the DTFS should generally be negatively
therapy goal of forgiving. Likewise, clients with low decisional related to the TRIM subscales, we wanted to show that it is
forgiveness may not benefit from engaging in activities or inter- conceptually important when both are high (as might occur in the
ventions designed to teach skills to promote forgiveness if they beginning of an intervention when participants have made a deci-
have not made forgiveness a goal. Furthermore, promoting deci- sion to forgive, but they have not yet applied strategies to promote
sional forgiveness may relieve pressure to forgive in communities forgiveness). Accordingly, we recruited participants who had ex-
that strongly obligate forgiveness or reconciliation (Davis et al., perienced a recent betrayal by a religious/spiritual leader. We
2013; Hook et al., 2009), which may provide avenues for adapting picked this context because we believed it might reflect a partic-
interventions to the unique challenges that clients face based on the ular situation in which individuals would feel pressure to make a
way their community understands and practices forgiveness. These decision to forgive, yet still harbor unforgiving emotions toward
are exciting and promising possibilities, but they cannot be ex- the offender. We hypothesized not only would the DTFS be
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
plored until a measure to access decisional forgiveness has been negatively related to the TRIM, but these two scales would also
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
her”; “My choice is to forgive him or her”; “My choice is to factor loadings ranged from .57 to .96. The Cronbach’s alpha
release any negative feelings I have”; and “I have chosen not to coefficient was .93. Skew and kurtosis scores for the DTFS
intentionally harbor resentment toward him or her.” Participants were less than one (i.e., ⫺.91 and .22, respectively).
were asked to rate their agreement with items using 5-point ratings After evaluating the factor structure, next we tested three hy-
ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). potheses related to construct validity of the DTFS. First, we
Forgiveness. The 12-item TRIM (McCullough et al., 1998) examined a three-factor model including items from the TRIM
was used to assess one’s experience of forgiveness. The TRIM has (i.e., five items for decision to forgive, five items for revenge
two subscales: Revenge (five items; e.g., “I’ll make him/her pay”) motivations, and seven items for avoidance motivations). Factors
and Avoidance (seven items; e.g., “I keep as much distance be- were modeled as correlated. This model also showed excellent fit,
tween us as possible”). Items are completed using 5-point ratings 2(116) ⫽ 258.71, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽ .97, RMSEA ⫽ .05 (95%
from (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Higher scores (i.e., CI ⫽ .05, .06), SRMR ⫽ .04. Second, to examine our hypothesis
higher avoidance or revenge motivations) indicate greater unfor- that the DTFS would be moderately and negatively related to the
giveness (hence less forgiveness). The TRIM is the most widely TRIM subscales, we examined the factor correlations. As pre-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
used measure of forgiveness (Fehr et al., 2010). McCullough and dicted, the DTFS showed moderate relations with the revenge,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
colleagues (1998) reported Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for r ⫽ ⫺.39, p ⬍ .001 and avoidance, r ⫽ ⫺.47, p ⬍ .001 motiva-
scores on both subscales ranging from .85 to .93 and estimates of tions, suggesting that these measures assess related yet distinct
3-week temporal stability ranging from .44 to .65. Scores on the constructs.
scale showed evidence of construct validity, with subscales being Third, we examined whether the DTFS was more strongly
negatively correlated with other measures of forgiveness, relation- related to the stage of change item than the TRIM subscales. To
ship satisfaction, and commitment (McCullough et al., 1998). For test this prediction, we examined a model with the stage item
the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was .90 for (manifest variable) regressed simultaneously on latent factors
revenge and .80 for avoidance. based on the DTFS and the TRIM, and we used the model test
Stage of change forgiveness. We adapted a single-item mea- command to compare the magnitude of these paths. The overall
sure that has been used regularly in motivational interviewing model showed excellent fit, 2(130) ⫽ 277.10, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽
research (e.g., Cook & Perri, 2004) to examine participants’ read- .97, RMSEA ⫽ .05 (95% CI ⫽ .04, .06), SRMR ⫽ .04. As
iness to work on forgiving an offense: “Please find the statement predicted, the stage of change item was more strongly related to
that best describes the way you feel right now about the person the DTFS (b ⫽ .81, p ⬍ .001) than it was to the Avoidance
who hurt you.” The item was rated using a 5-point rating from 0 (b ⫽ ⫺.45, p ⬍ .001; Wald test ⫽ 122.14, p ⬍ .001) or Revenge
(I am not considering forgiveness) to 4 (I have already forgiven). (b ⫽ ⫺.36, p ⬍ .001; Wald test ⫽ 110.27, p ⬍ .001) subscales.
We are aware of the limitations of singe items (e.g., difficulty Taken together, the results of Study 1 provided evidence for the
estimating reliability). However, given that no multiple item scales factor structure and estimated internal consistency of the DTFS.
existed to assess stage of change for forgiveness, we decided to Furthermore, in Study 1, we also provided initial evidence of the
begin by adapting this single item measure. Cook and Perri (2004) construct validity of the DTFS relative to the TRIM. These find-
found a .91 correlation between stages classifications based on a ings present initial evidence that making a decision to forgive is
single item and those based on multiple-item scales. different than the process of forgiving.
change for a small amount of course credit. They indicated consent Study 3
online and then completed a set of demographic questions. Next
they recalled a specific offense and completed the DTFS and the The primary goal of Study 3 was to examine whether the
TRIM. They were emailed a follow-up survey one week later that interaction between decisional forgiveness and forgiveness is con-
included the DTFS and the TRIM. ceptually important. Generally, making a decision to forgive and
Measures. Participants completed the DTFS (Cronbach’s al- forgiveness ought to align (i.e., negative correlation between the
pha coefficients were .93 at Time 1 and .92 at Time 2) and TRIM DTFS and TRIM subscales), but we wanted to see whether their
(Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for avoidance were .95 at Time 1 misalignment (as might occur early in an intervention when a
and .96 at Time 2; for revenge, they were .89 at Time 1 and .91 at client decides to forgive but has yet to employ strategies to work
Time 2). These measures were described in Study 1. toward this goal) would predict stress-related outcomes. Accord-
ingly, we recruited participants who had experienced a major
offense by a religious/spiritual leader within the last year. Specif-
Results and Discussion ically, we predicted that DTFS scores and TRIM subscales would
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
ⴱ ⴱⴱ
Figure 1. Cross-lagged model in Study 2. T1 ⫽ Time 1; T2 ⫽ Time 2. p ⬍ .05. p ⬍ .01.
DECISION TO FORGIVE 285
Measures. Participants completed the DTFS (␣ ⫽ .94) and was associated with greater existential distress (B ⫽ .12, p ⫽
TRIM (revenge ␣ ⫽ .81; avoidance ␣ ⫽ .90). These measures .023).
were described in Study 1. The second regression analysis examined avoidance as a mod-
Existential distress. Existential distress was assessed with the erator. The conditional main effects did not predict significant
7-item Negative Religious Coping subscale of the Religious Cop- variance in existential distress in Step 1 (R2 ⫽ .01, p ⫽ .141), and
ing Scale-Brief Version (RCOPE-B; Pargament, Koenig, Perez, the interaction term did not predict existential distress in Step 2
2000; Pargament et al., 1998). Items were rated from 0 (not at all) (⌬R2 ⫽ .00, p ⫽ .309).
to 5 (a great deal). Higher scores indicate greater struggle. An The results of Study 3 partially supported our hypothesis that
example item is, “Wondered whether God (or the Sacred) had decision to forgive would moderate the relation between forgive-
abandoned me.” The subscale demonstrated evidence of internal ness and existential distress. In addition, once again, decision to
consistency, with Cronbach’s alpha coefficients ranging from .69 forgive was only moderately related to the TRIM. Thus, this study
to .81 (Pargament et al., 1998). The RCOPE-B is one of the most provided additional evidence that the DTFS and the TRIM are
widely used measures of spiritual/existential struggle or alienation conceptually distinct constructs.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
and has been robustly linked with physical and mental health
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Table 2
Hierarchical Regression Analyses in Study 3
Our study had several limitations. First, all studies sampled to forgive among a broader array of constructs (e.g., motivations or
college students. Our samples were diverse in terms of race/ intentions associated with forgiveness).
ethnicity, but not in age or educational background. It will be Third, our studies only used self-report measures. The definition
important to establish norms for the DTFS based on samples more of forgiveness implies an intrapsychic change of cognitions, emo-
diverse in a variety of variables. In addition, the DTFS should be tions, motivations, and behaviors toward an offender. Although
normed with diverse treatment-seeking populations. self-report measures of forgiveness are useful, it may be important
Second, four of the five items on the DTFS included the word to supplement self-report measures of forgiveness with physiolog-
forgiveness, which may in some samples lead these items to be too ical measures of stress or behavioral indicators of forgiveness
strongly correlated with each other based on this common word. (Dorn, Hook, Davis, Van Tongeren, & Worthington, 2014).
The one item that did not explicitly mention forgiveness refers to Fourth, with the exception of Study 2, our studies used cross-
reduction of negative emotions, so the scale does not explicitly sectional designs. Even Study 2 was limited by the use of only two
reference increases in positive cognition, emotions, or motivations. time-points. Researchers might include the DTFS in future exper-
The scale may correlate more strongly with negative constructs imental or intervention studies. We hypothesize that successful
associated with forgiveness, such as anger or rumination, than it interventions promote rapid shifts in decisional forgiveness, which
does positive constructs such as empathy or conciliatory behaviors. might lead to subsequent increases in forgiveness over time. Fur-
The present study was focused on distinguishing the DTFS from thermore, we expect that clients who never make a decision to
the TRIM, but future work might work to situate making a decision forgive will be much less likely to benefit from forgiveness inter-
ventions. Being able to accurately identify these clients may help
researchers focus specific intervention strategies for clients who do
not want to forgive. Indeed, we are aware of no interventions that
specifically focus on clients who are highly opposed to the idea of
forgiveness, but it is very important to understand more about this
group. In some cases, perhaps these clients are reticent to forgive
for good reasons, and current interventions may have minimal
content focused on their particular reservations regarding forgive-
ness.
Conclusion
Although applied work on forgiveness has proliferated over the
last 20 years, there is still a need for more research on how to adapt
interventions to clients’ specific needs. The prominent strategies of
promoting forgiveness have focused on promoting a decision to
forgive, but very little is known about this construct. Advancing
research on different parts of the forgiveness process has important
Figure 2. Graph of interaction of commitment to forgive with revenge implications for how to promote forgiveness in clients who are
scores on spiritual struggle scores. ambivalent about whether they even want to forgive.
DECISION TO FORGIVE 287
.00012.x
mental health. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48, 447– 455. http://
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.48.4.447
covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alterna-
Cook, C. L., & Perri, M., III. (2004). Single-item vs multiple-item mea-
tives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
sures of stage of change in compliance with prescribed medications.
10705519909540118
Psychological Reports, 94, 115–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.94.1
McCullough, M. E., Fincham, F. D., & Tsang, J. A. (2003). Forgiveness,
.115-124
forbearance, and time: The temporal unfolding of transgression-related
Davis, D. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Hook, J. N., & Hill, P. C. (2013).
interpersonal motivations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Research on religion/spirituality and forgiveness: A meta-analytic re-
ogy, 84, 540 –557. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.3.540
view. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 5, 233–241. http://dx.doi
McCullough, M. E., Luna, L. R., Berry, J. W., Tabak, B. A., & Bono, G.
.org/10.1037/a0033637
(2010). On the form and function of forgiving: Modeling the time-
DiBlasio, F. A. (2000). Decision-based forgiveness treatment in cases of
forgiveness relationship and testing the valuable relationships hypothe-
marital infidelity. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
sis. Emotion, 10, 358 –376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019349
37, 149 –158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087834
McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L., Jr.,
Dorn, K., Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., Van Tongeren, D. R., & Worthington,
Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close
E. L., Jr. (2014). Behavioral methods of assessing forgiveness. The
relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement. Journal of
Journal of Positive Psychology, 9, 75– 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1586 –1603. http://dx.doi.org/
17439760.2013.844267
10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1586
Enright, R. D., & Fitzgibbons, R. P. (2000). Helping clients forgive.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. http://dx.doi McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Rachal, K. C. (1997).
.org/10.1037/10381-000 Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. Journal of Personality and
Exline, J. J., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Hill, P., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Social Psychology, 73, 321–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
Forgiveness and justice: A research agenda for social and personality .73.2.321
psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 337–348. Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. (2008). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0704_06 Muthén and Muthén.
Fehr, R., Gelfand, M. J., & Nag, M. (2010). The road to forgiveness: A Orcutt, H. K. (2006). The prospective relationship of interpersonal forgive-
meta-analytic synthesis of its situational and dispositional correlates. ness and psychological distress symptoms among college women. Jour-
Psychological Bulletin, 136, 894 –914. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ nal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 350 –361. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0019993 0022-0167.53.3.350
Fincham, F. D. (2000). The kiss of the porcupines: From attributing Pargament, K. I., Koenig, H. G., & Perez, L. M. (2000). The many methods
responsibility to forgiving. Personal Relationships, 7, 1–23. http://dx.doi of religious coping: Development and initial validation of the RCOPE.
.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00001.x Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 519 –543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
Flückiger, C., Del Re, A. C., Wampold, B. E., Symonds, D., & Horvath, (SICI)1097-4679(200004)56:4⬍519::AID-JCLP6⬎3.0.CO;2-1
A. O. (2012). How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel Pargament, K. I., Smith, B., Koenig, H. G., & Perez, L. (1998). Patterns of
longitudinal meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 10 – positive and negative religious coping with major life stressors. Journal
17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0025749 for the Scientific Study of Religion, 37, 710 –724. http://dx.doi.org/
Fowers, B. J. (2005). Virtue and psychology: Pursuing excellence in 10.2307/1388152
ordinary practices. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa- Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1982). Transtheoretical therapy:
tion. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/11219-000 Toward a more integrative model of change. Psychotherapy: Theory,
Greenberg, L. J., Warwar, S. H., & Malcolm, W. M. (2008). Differential effects of Research, & Practice, 19, 276 –288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0088437
emotion-focused therapy and psychoeducation in facilitating forgiveness and Sandage, S. J., Hill, P. C., & Vang, H. (2003). Toward a multicultural
letting go of emotional injuries. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55, 185– positive psychology: Indigenous forgiveness and Hmong culture. The
196. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.2.185 Counseling Psychologist, 31, 564 –592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Hayes, A. F. (2008). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi- 0011000003256350
tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Wade, N. G., Hoyt, W. T., Kidwell, J. E., & Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2014).
Guilford Press. Efficacy of psychotherapeutic interventions to promote forgiveness: A
Holeman, V. T., Dean, J. B., DeShea, L., & Duba, J. D. (2011). The meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 154 –
multidimensional nature of the quest construct forgiveness, spiritual 170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0035268
perception, and differentiation of self. Journal of Psychology and The- Wade, N. G., Vogel, D. L., Liao, K. Y.-H., & Goldman, D. B. (2008). Measuring
ology, 39, 31– 43. state-specific rumination: Development of the Rumination about an
288 DAVIS ET AL.
Interpersonal Offense Scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55, Worthington, E. L., Jr., Berry, J. W., Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., Scherer, M.,
419 – 426. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.419 Griffin, B. J., . . . Campana, K. L. (in press). Forgiveness-reconciliation
Watkins, D. A., Hui, E. K., Luo, W., Regmi, M., Worthington, E. L., Jr., and communication-conflict-resolution interventions versus rested con-
Hook, J. N., & Davis, D. E. (2011). Forgiveness and interpersonal trols in early married couples. Journal of Counseling Psychology.
relationships: A Nepalese investigation. The Journal of Social Psychol- Worthington, E. L., Jr., Hook, J. N., Utsey, S. O., Williams, J. K., & Neil, R. L.
ogy, 151, 150 –161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224540903368541 (2007, October 5). Decisional and emotional forgiveness. Paper presented at
Worthington, E. L., Hunter, J. L., Sharp, C. B., Hook, J. N., Van Tongeren, the International Positive Psychology Summit, Washington, DC.
D. R., Davis, D. E., . . . Monforte-Milton, M. (2010). A psychoeduca- Worthington, E. L., Jr., Witvliet, C. V. O., Pietrini, P., & Miller, A. J.
tional intervention to promote forgiveness in Christians in the Philip- (2007). Forgiveness, health, and well-being: A review of evidence for
pines. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 32, 75–93. emotional versus decisional forgiveness, dispositional forgivingness,
Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2006). Forgiveness and reconciliation: Theory and and reduced unforgiveness. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 30,
application. New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge. 291–302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-007-9105-8
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
Appendix
Norms Across Studies 1–3 by Race
Decision to forgive 381 19.71a 4.98 96 17.16b 5.96 47 19.81ab 4.94 240 18.73a 5.99
Note. Different subscripts indicate that means were different from each other using a Bonferonni post hoc test at p ⬍ .05.