An Independent Research Study Into The Effect of Beatboxing On The Skill and Enjoyment of Young People in Phonics
An Independent Research Study Into The Effect of Beatboxing On The Skill and Enjoyment of Young People in Phonics
An Independent Research Study Into The Effect of Beatboxing On The Skill and Enjoyment of Young People in Phonics
Dedications
Firstly, I give my utmost gratitude to my ever supporting family for providing resources,
materials and much needed love to enable me to pursue freely the things that inspire me.
Secondly, to Rochelle for consistently providing her exponential intelligence and invaluable
belief to propel me onwards and upwards, thank you.
Finally, to Chris Robson (my inspiring tutor) and every creative, artistic individual who has
instilled their wisdom upon me, I hope you continue to do Props.
Contents
1. Introduction
2. Methodology
3. Literature Review
4. Analysis
5. Conclusion
6. References
1. Introduction
“An Independent Research Study into the Effect of Beatboxing on the Skill and Enjoyment of
Young People in Phonics”
It is first integral to note, that the genesis of this study has been one which has grown organically
throughout my university practice; that of the role and importance that beatboxing may hold for
the linguistic development of young people. Using my own small scale research in conjunction
with relevant literature, this study aims to discover the strengths and legitimacy of this theory.
The linguistic development and musical expression of young people have been two areas that I
have had a deep interest in for several years. In terms of linguistic development I had the
opportunity to read and reflect upon a variety of language acquisition theories by pioneers such
as Aitchison (1987) and Chomsky (1968) in A-level English Language. In addition, as a newly
qualified teacher and a practicing beatboxer, I have a genuine interest in exploring how the
musical voice can be used as a means of education and expression. By bringing together these
two areas I began to wonder whether using the vocal apparatus as an instrument for language and
as an instrument for music, could greatly benefit learners in a plethora of educational contexts.
During my fourth year school placement I was offered what became an enlightening opportunity
when I was asked to use beatboxing to teach a series of successive phonics sessions. I believe
this held significance, as for the first time my passion for beatboxing had been recognised as a
potential tool for delivering an area of the curriculum to young people. For this reason I believe it
gave greater necessity for this area of research to be undertaken. Thus, I combined my own
theories surrounding beatboxing with existing educational literature (Phase 6 of the Letters and
Sounds document (D of E, 2007) and the All Our Futures document (NACCCE, 1999) to plan a
somewhat ‘different’ educational experience.
The focus group for these lessons were a small group of pupils who lacked the desired phonic
skill level for their age. What was also intriguing was that from conversations with the class
teacher, the children were also experiencing issues showing enthusiasm and enjoyment for the
synthetic phonic approach that the school implemented. Unfortunately, due to the demands of the
school timetable and timing issues, I was unable to teach the full series of sessions. As a result, a
strong desire to continue exploring these ideas remained.
This pursuit for advocating the link between music and language learning was re-instigated
during my university sessions when, as a part of the assessment for the course, we received an
opportunity to engage in educational research. Immediately, I felt that I must take this
opportunity to continue my investigations that were previously cut short. In order to present an
educationally viable idea, it was fundamental that I began to read current initiatives and research
key concepts that I might address.
Through’ (Watkins, C.L., 1997:7), in which it states it was ‘the largest educational study ever
conducted in the history of education research’.
Similarly, there has been a recent academic zeitgeist highlighting the importance of a
‘systematic, comprehensive, phonics-based approach’ (ibid). This initial research was enough to
suggest that using a musical approach to teaching phonics as opposed to other reading methods,
would be the most relevant area to pursue. More so, initial reading suggested that the
fundamental ideas behind beatboxing could lend to the teaching of phonics. Hallam, S. (2010:63)
reciprocates this notion by stating that the plosive, rhythmic sounds that are the foundation of
beatboxing, could affirm the ‘phonemic awareness’ and rhythm of language taught to children at
an early stage in their education (Stanovich,1993:16).
Due to the parameters (length, time, ethical considerations etc) of this particular project, I am
aware that there may or may not be any conclusive evidence to prove or disprove my theory. Yet
I contend that the research project has academic wealth and value due to the fact that I will attain
a better understanding of how educational research can have impact on in class teaching. As a
newly qualified teacher I believe it is important to reflect on current practices and use new
research to raise educational standards by re-engaging in current practices and allowing for
change.
2. Methodology
Due to the ‘discovery’ nature of this study, my research will be ‘small scale’. Similarly, due to
the “insight, rather than statistical analysis” (Bell, 1993:45) approach to findings, I aim to
utilise varied and neutral methods to ensure optimum objectivity. The process in which I have
devised my methodology has been meticulous and well planned. To ensure my methods were
sufficiently creative, I based my devising around the idea that the research is looking at ‘socio-
cultural understandings and practices of a group of people’. As a result, the study could be
described as following an ‘ethnographic’ approach:
“Rather than offering a particular method for data collection, ethnography may be conducted
using a wide range of methodologies…Ethnographies typically aim to provide holistic accounts
that include the views and perspectives, beliefs and values of all those involved in the particular
socio-cultural practice or institutional context being studies” (Siraj-Blatchford et al. 2001:193)
One may conceptualise this study as a piece of ‘action research’, in which one is potentially
concerned with ‘finding a solution to a problem’ (Cohen et al. 2000:92). Thus, I must strike
balance and remain inquisitive in order to stay in line with the ‘discovery’ based nature of this
ethnographic study. Similarly, I must acknowledge there will be a degree of bias (ibid) and to
ensure there is minimal compromise of validity, data must be collected effectively and concepts
addressed should be well informed by literature.
To ensure ethical considerations are complied with, I will use the official university forms and
comply with the protocols of social research and gather the relevant ethics forms (Bell, 1987),
ensuring that I avoid the dilemmas outlined by Cohen and Mannion (1994:272) of ‘privacy,
anonymity, confidentiality, betrayal and deception’ (C.F. Appendix 1)
The implementation of my research will consist of three prominent phases, of which after each
phase, there will be reconnaissance, review and action. To ensure ‘triangulation’ (Deanscombe,
2010:65) and therefore optimise validity, I will use a variety of data collection devices that I will
mention throughout each phase. However, the notion of validity will be a questionable concept
as with a discovery based study, validity is compromised with high levels of researcher bias.
This is acceptable but must be acknowledged as ‘true’ (Siraj-Blatchford, 2001).
Phase 1
Selection of participants will be based around a lack of ‘enjoyment’ and ability in phonics. This
will have been observed and identified by myself and the class teacher.
I will then conduct an interview with each child. Bell (1993), outlines that the advantages of
interviews is their ‘adaptability, in that the interviewer can probe responses and investigate
motive and feelings which a questionnaire cannot do. This is conducive when asking about to the
extent a participant ‘enjoys’ something. However, Robson (1993) warns that interviews can
contain bias and can be time consuming. Despite this, I feel using a semi-structured interview
method, in that it allows for some ‘purposeful conversation’ (Maykut and Morehouse, 1994:81)
which will work well with the age range being worked with in my research (year two pupils).
For the interview, I will use an audio recording device and ensure collected data is securely
stored. This data will be used as a reference point to gauge progression for the final phase of the
study. Questions are based around the participant’s self-definitions of enjoyment, current ability
to vocalise graphemes and their perceptions of beatboxing (C.F. Appendix 2)
Phase 2
The purpose of phase 2 is to deliver an informed phonic session using beatboxing as a vehicle for
teaching. These sessions will be based around the phonemes used in the interview from phase
one. The structure of the session is concisely and explicitly presented in the lesson plans (C.F.
Appendix 3). The participants will engage in several activities outlined in the lesson plan which
have been devised to provide an alternative means of vocalising phonemes, using beatboxing as
a context to do so. These phonemes are entitled the ‘Focus Phonemes’ and are highlighted in the
relevant lesson plan
(C.F. Appendix 4b). Said implementation will then culminate in an evaluative activity, assessing
primarily the progression of the children in terms of vocalising the ‘focus phonemes’ and the
enjoyment of the children. This assessment will be in the form of a ‘beatbox nonsense’ poem,
outlined in the relevant lesson plan (C.F. Appendix 3). Resources used in the sessions can be
found in the appendices section (C.F. Appendix 4a)
However, there is a ‘research paradox’ (Robson, 1997:43), where I myself am delivering the
session but am also required to document findings. Thus, to ensure objectivity, I will prepare
a visual recording of the session which I can review afterward.
Completed work by participants will also be analysed to discover any pertinence toward to study.
The work will then be recorded using both visual and audio equipment to discover if the
participants exhibit visual ‘enjoyment’ and to hear the vocalisation of key phonemes.
Phase 3
In order to effectively review progress from the initial interview, a second semi-structured
interview will be implemented (C.F Appendix 2). This will be similar to the original but slightly
modified. I will use audio recording software to capture responses. Accompanying this will be a
short debrief for participants.
Following the interviews, data will be gathered and suitably analysed in relation to the
hypotheses arrived at during the literature review (Deanscombe, 2010:227) for relevance and
pertinence to the study, ensuring that data is kept confidential.
3. Literature Review
Over the past decade, ‘high levels’ of research have been propagated in regards to a
governmental push on phonic teaching (Goswami, 2008:124). This educational zeitgeist has
intensified the requirements for a plethora of non-governmental and governmental initiatives.
This literature review attempts to discuss the quality of such initiatives and the relevance of their
position in current educational contexts. It also attempts to look at research and ideas behind the
significance of music, in particular the contemporary genre of ‘beatboxing’, in providing a
potential context for phonic delivery.
"the diligent, concentrated and systematic teaching of phonics is central to the success of all the
schools that achieve high reading standards in Key Stage 1" (OFSTED, 2010)
In recent times, reading initiatives have derived directly from ‘guidance which accompanies the
PNS literacy framework’ outlining that schools and early years settings should ‘put in place a
systematic, discrete programme as the key means for teaching high-quality phonic work’ and that
‘shared and guided reading sessions should not be used to replace discrete phonics teaching but
they can provide opportunities to reinforce children’s developing phonic knowledge and skills
(DfES & PNS, 2006:7). This research outlines that a systematic approach is in fact a certifiable
means of teaching reading in schools effectively and is currently a governmental
recommendation.
An enquiry into the teaching of early reading prompted a recommendation for the approach to
phonic teaching that schools should adopt. Such a recommendation came from The Rose Report
(Rose, 2006) and outlined that ‘the systematic phonics approach selected by schools should be
synthetic phonics’. Systematic phonics delivery can be defined as;
Further research was implemented into the relative educational benefits of both analytic and
synthetic phonics, (Watson & Johnston, 2004). Control groups were used and a comparison
between analytic and synthetic phonics was conducted. Researchers found that:
“At the end of the experimental period, the synthetic group children read more words and letters
correctly than the children in the other two groups and they still performed better ten weeks and
nine months later.”
This research suggested that a synthetic approach to teaching phonics was more effective.
Similarly, in its White Paper ‘The Importance of Teaching’
(DfE, 2010), the government acknowledged the value of a synthetic phonics approach; “The
evidence is clear that the teaching of systematic synthetic phonics is the most effective way of
teaching young children to read, particularly those at risk of having problems with reading... We
will provide the resources to support the teaching of systematic synthetic phonics in primary
schools”. (para 4.16:43)
However, despite the research outlining clear benefits of a synthetic approach to phonics, critics
argue that it does not ‘develop comprehension skills which are the essence of reading’ (Watson
& Johnston, 2004: 339).
Evidently, there is a clear disrepute in regards to the most effective approach to a systematic
phonic delivery. Despite this however, research has shown ‘specific systematic phonics
programs are all significantly more effective than non-phonics programs.’ (National Institute of
Child Health and
Human Development, 2000:93). Supporting this, literature released after the Clarkmannanshire
research states that it is not simply one systematic approach that should flourish, but instead
‘components should not be developed in isolation. Best practice integrates skills teaching with
more authentic, contextually-grounded literacy activities, responding to the interests of the
learner and the literacy contexts of their homes and communities’’
(United Kingdom Literacy Association (UKLA), 2005:3). Stannard (2005:21) supports this
notion by arguing ‘against the promotion of a single best method’.
Considering this exploration into appropriated and systematic methods of phonic teaching; one
could deduce that adopting methods relevant to the context that early phonic education is being
implemented is a far more conducive means of promoting effective literacy:
‘‘The most common comment, cited by a third (32 per cent) of respondents, [was] that a variety
of teaching/learning methods needs to be used alongside phonics, including contextual
understanding’’ (QCA, 2006:13).
From this informed discussion, one could deduce that indeed a systematic approach is the most
apt for teaching phonics but whether one specific style should be used is not so clear. Rather
multiple approaches can only serve to enhance aptitude. Thus, there will now be a review of
relevant literature with a focus on how using music as a context for learning could form an
aspect of this multi-faceted approaches.
Music has, for many years, been identified as an ‘essential element of early childhood education’
(Pound & Harrison, 2003:23). All humans are born with the potential to be musical and
according to Glover, (2000:4) ‘music has a powerful motivating effect on adults and children’.
This motivational effect can be seen to have a positive and beneficial effect on general learning.
There seems to be little existing research in regards to the affect of music in an early literacy
setting, however, Youth Music (2006) identified that where ‘music specialists were engaged on a
regular basis with a setting the children’s music was more productive and had a higher impact
upon their learning’. In a study of the enjoyment of music, Dittermore (1968:12) finds that music
can have the power to "increase our power to understand what it is to truly enjoy and to achieve
more control over our activities which bring enjoyment”.
This notion of music increasing one’s enjoyment is an idea outline in my proposal. I will now
discuss increasing enjoyment in a learning context could increase one’s ability to learn.
Primarily, one must define educational enjoyment; Epstein (1977:83) outlined that:
‘enjoyment is a concept that is self-defined’ by students and when students describe enjoyment
they ‘describe actual, real-time emotional experiences, they refer to state emotions (e.g., “I am
enjoying class at this moment”). An emotional trait can be conceptualized as a long-enduring
disposition to experience a particular emotional state’.
Goetz (2004:9) also outlines that ‘high levels of enjoyment should coincide with flexible
learning styles’. Goetz et al. (2003:24), states that ‘emotional experiences should both influence
and be influenced by their learning behaviour’. These theoretical outlines of educational
enjoyment seem to suggest that it is through the process of the enjoyment of learning that
students can become conscious of specific learning areas and in turn, become aware of the
learning style that is most effective for them. Thus, synthesising these discussions of informed
literature; one could hypothesis that the idea of using music as an enjoyable context could be
positively conducive to student’s learning behaviour.
However, due to the tentativeness of defining ‘enjoyment’; one requires a more educationally
supported and research when considering if, on a more specific level, music could be a vehicle,
as mentioned previously, for teaching systematic synthetic phonics. Thus, I will move to discuss
the surrounding literature in regards to how music could facilitate such a theory.
Palmer (2004:25) suggests that ‘the ability to move to a beat also forms part of a child’s musical
experiences’, This notion of rhythm being a fundamental part of language is synonymised by
Kolb (1996:76) who proposes that rhythm is ‘essential for literacy learning’ (ibid), and goes on
to put forward that: “due to the fact that children have a natural and innate disposition towards
rhythm it is an appropriate and effective tool for developing listening, speaking and writing skills
as it enables children to become aware of the rhythm and patterns in spoken and written
language.”
Towell (1999:284) reciprocates this theory of elements of music being conducive to language
learning by stating that “combining rhyme rhythm rap and repetition exposes children to the best
of worlds, the world of music and the world of literature”.
Ergo, the idea of music existing as a context in which learning can thrive is an area that is
seemingly one which is supported by valid research. More specifically however, the various
aspects of music could act as a vehicle for language learning. Returning to the earlier discussion
on a multi-faceted synthetic phonics approach, Susannah et al. (1993:21) highlight how: “Music
training can be of immense benefit to a child's language development…pitch, rhythm, duration
and timbre can be helpful in distinguishing phonemes and morphemes, recognising similar
sounds in words and gaining fluency in reading through an awareness of the rhythmic structure
of language”.
It is with this idea of music promoting the ability to ‘distinguish phonemes and morphemes using
the elements of music’, where the review adopts a more personal perspective. The discussion
moves from the overarching benefits of music to a more specific musical approach.
Beatboxing (also known as ‘vocal percussion’) is a relatively new musical genre and one which
has “developed well outside academia…so there is (to our knowledge) very little scholarly work
on the topic, either its history or its current practice.” (Stowell et al,2008: [online]). According
to Noise et al. (2006: [online]) in an in depth article on the history of vocal percussion from the
past 1000 years, they suggest that ‘The history of Beatboxing is blurry', however they do agree
that beatboxing is “an art form…part of the music and urban scene.” The roots of beatboxing are
evidently ‘urban’. This notion of the urbanity of beatboxing being of educational merit is an area
I will pursue after a discussion of the key concepts of beatboxing.
Essentially the beatboxer uses ‘vocal organs to emulate the beats and rhythms of a drum machine
(Matela, 2002: [online]). With this idea of the promotion of ‘short and plosive’ sounds that allow
a beatboxer to replicate percussive sounds, one can readily draw links to the encouraged
techniques taught when delivering synthetic phonics. Synthetic phonics is built on the premise
that there is “synthesis of phonic skills from their smallest unit (graphemes)” (Hempenstall,
2001:26). It is this promotion of using the smallest unit of sound that facilitates effective
blending of words. However, research has found that in the early stages of phonic learning,
students often pronounce phonemes with an ‘uh’ suffix, for example, research by the Department
for Educational Excellence (1998: [online]) found that Pupils may read them as sight words or
by the “cuh-a-tuh spells cat” approach in which a simple “rhyme” is learned by rote. Thus, for
pupils to eventually blend words effectively in a synthetic phonic approach, it is key they learn
how to pronounce the graphemes correctly. Specifically these are to be short and infinitive
sounds, for example ‘c’ as opposed to ‘cuh’.
Beatboxing promotes the use of said short sounds ‘The produced vocal percussive sounds have
short duration (average 0.25 seconds)’, (Scheirer,
1980:588). One could hypothesise that not only are the fundamental elements of beatboxing
conducive to promoting the skills required for the delivery of synthetic phonics, but also the art
and background of beatboxing could readily apply to those who benefit highly from a synthetic
approach. According to the NRP & NICHD (1997) the synthetic phonic approach was: ‘most
effective in aiding children from low-socioeconomic backgrounds’ and ‘urban areas’. The NCES
(1998) reciprocate this notion by outlining that:
‘Urban learners are often disproportionately represented at the lower levels of phonemic
awareness and alphabetic knowledge because of lack of exposure to these skills at home’
From my own experience of teaching beatboxing as an art form, it has been young people from
the more socio-economically deprived areas that are both a) more aware of beatboxing as a
musical genre and b) already capable of performing elements of beatboxing. After further
research The NCES (1998), also found that it was ‘Their inability to generate sounds is usually a
result of being unable to segment a word into distinctive sounds’.
Thus, synthesising the previous discussions around the concepts addressed in the proposal; one
has discovered that it seems a systematic synthetic approach to phonics is currently viewed as the
most effective. It also seems from literature and research surrounding the notions of music and
enjoyment, an enjoyable music approach could be conceptualised as a means of delivering such a
phonic approach. However, after further research in conjunction with my own personal
experience and belief, I have found that research has alluded to the relatively new and
academically untapped genre of beatboxing, being an educationally viable vehicle for teaching
the fundamental aspects of synthetic phonics. Specifically, beatboxing could be a means of
facilitating the early reading skills of students from low socio-economical backgrounds.
Due to the small scale nature of this research project I feel it is logistically conducive to explore
the notion of beatboxing as an enjoyable vehicle for teaching synthetic phonics.
4. Analysis
This section groups the findings of the interviews, audio and visual evidence gathered from the
research, and includes comparisons with other studies, research and a discussion. Throughout I
will triangulate my results by contrasting and comparing data collected from teacher, pupil and
observer perspectives. This is to ensure optimum objectivity and validity. Throughout analysis, a
statement put forward in the methodology must be kept in mind;
‘the notion of validity will be a questionable concept as with a discovery based study, validity is
compromised with high levels of researcher bias. This is acceptable but must be ‘acknowledged’
as true.’
Throughout this analysis one must be aware that this small scale research study came from
personal origins and therefore the purpose is to gain an insight into a relatively unreported
theory.
This study aimed to assess the effect of two in depth interactive sessions (C.F. Appendix 3) on
the following hypotheses highlighted from the literature review:
(Stake, 2005) of the key findings. I will explore these hypotheses using evidence and data to
discover emerging themes and trends.
The structure of the analysis will contain three distinct parts. Due to the discovery nature of the
study, the data analysis will adopt a narrative structure in order to tell the ‘story’ of the research.
Through each phase, a brief summary will be provided of how the information was obtained,
how the data was subsequently analysed (Deanscombe, 2010) and finally a comparison and
consequent interpretation will be put forward with regards to the original research proposal:
‘to discover the effect of beatboxing on the enjoyment and skill of young people in phonics’.
For the purposes of this small scale study, one participant will be used as a representative
example for the focus group of six participants. The reasons for this are threefold:
1. The child who will act as a representative example, upon completion of the literature
review, was seen to be the most relevant participant due to their apparent socio-economic
background and existing skill level in phonics.
2. Due to the ethnographic nature of the study, telling the narrative of one child’s ‘journey’
throughout is the most conducive means of presenting information in this ‘discovery’ and
‘insight’ based study.
3. Using information for one child is more favourable when considering the logistical
parameters of this study (time, ethics, word limit etc).
However, interviews with other participants will be included in the appendices (C.F. Appendix
11) for the reader to ‘self-triangulate’ results if and when required (Siraj-Blatchford, 2001).
To gather information relating to hypothesis (3), it was important to first ensure relevant
background information was obtained for each child. This enabled the researcher to deduce the
current socio-economic context of both the school and participants. A ‘child profile’ (C.F.
Appendix 6) was devised for NB with support from the class teacher. This aimed to acquire basic
information regarding the child’s background and current phonic level. As can be seen from the
child profile, NB lived in close proximity to the school, an area which has a high Index of
Multiple Deprivation (C.F. Appendix 12). In addition, NB lives in a single parent household.
Prior to embarking on phase two, a review was taken in order to reflect and take action for the
second phase of research. This can be viewed in the form of a research diary (C.F. Appendix 7).
To obtain information regarding hypotheses (1), a semi-structured interview was conducted to
gauge a starting point for the participants. The interview gauged the children’s perceptions of
enjoyment and subsequently their enjoyment of phonics. This extract of the interview aimed to
gain an insight into NB’s self-perception of enjoyment:
Researcher: Ok so you don’t think you’re really good but you don’t think you’re bad?
NB’s response when asked for his definition of enjoyment was that it is
“when you like things what you’re doing”. When compared to Epstein’s theory of student’s self
perception of enjoyment one could suggest he is describing ‘actual, real-time emotional
experiences’ (Epstein, 1977). NB uses the continuous verb ‘doing’ and is suggesting that when
one enjoys something they are experiencing it in the moment. When asked if he enjoyed phonics,
the reply was ‘an enthusiastic nod’ and more specifically, it was ‘reading new books’ that he
enjoyed the most. If one were to analyse the structure of this sentence, it is not the act of reading
that NB enjoys, but the idea of receiving ‘new’ books.
These insights suggest NB does have some self-perception of enjoyment. However, one must be
aware that NB is six years old, therefore his restricted vocabulary combined with potentially
leading interview questions could limit the validity of his description.
A subsidiary aim of the initial interview was also to determine what level of understanding the
participants had regarding the concept of beatboxing. In order to do so, they were asked what
they thought it was and more specifically if they knew that beatboxing uses phonemes. When NB
was asked, this was the response:
Researcher: Beatboxing?
(Researcher goes on to reference a brief session that was implemented with NB involved from
around three months ago based around beatboxing)
NB: (Nods) When you do lots of sounds and…singing with your mouth.
Researcher: Ok and did you know that beatboxing uses phonemes?
At this stage NB was not aware that beatboxing uses phonemes. Therefore as the analysis stage
progresses, a comparison will be made to view any change in NB’s perceptions of beatboxing
from phase one to phase three.
In order to obtain information regarding hypothesis (1), interview questions were devised to
assess the vocalisation of phonemes that are both recognised by the Letters and Sounds
document (2007) and are also stated as they key sounds used in beatboxing from Stowell’s
(2008) research.
Phase two was designed to address the ‘Focus Phonemes’ which are traditionally ‘over-
vocalised’ (i.e. ‘cuh’instead of ‘c’), outlined by the Department for Educational Excellence
(1998). The pronunciation of the ‘focus phonemes' (C.F. Appendix 4b), were deliberately
assessed prior to the implementation stage, to gauge how each participant vocalised the
phonemes. These vocalisations were recorded using a voice recorder which acted as a neutral
observational device to maximise objectivity and promote triangulation. This is an extract from
NB’s assessment:
Researcher: OK, I’m going to show you some phonemes here and I just want you to read them
out loud so I’ll just point to them and you read them ok?
NB: buh
NB: tuh
NB: kuh
In relation to the research implemented by the DfEE (1998), NB can be heard ‘over-vocalising’
the phonemes. As this research is concerned with a synthetic phonic approach, in order to
effectively blend words and read with fluency, one must vocalise phonemes in a short and
succinct manner (Hempenstall, 2001).
Following the assessment of the vocalisation of phonemes, participants were asked to read aloud
words containing the focus phonemes. This was in order to assess the fluency and speed in which
they were able to blend the phonemes to read aloud the words. These were the findings from
NB’s interview:
NB interview extract (C.F. Appendix 8)
NB: ticker
(NB can be heard vocalising ‘tuh – i – kuh’ and again ‘tuh – i – kuh’ …’tuh –i…)
NB: tick!
From this, we can see that NB does manage to vocalise and read aloud the words ‘tick’ and
ticker’. However, there are clearly some issues with the fluency and speed of which he does so.
This implies that NB is lacking the ability to effectively blend words. In relation to hypothesis
(1), at this stage
NB’s over-vocalisation of the focus phonemes was impeding his ability to blend and
subsequently read whole words with fluency and speed.
At this stage, it was important that reconnaissance was made in order to tailor the forthcoming
implementation stage shown in the research diary (C.F. Appendix 8).
The aims of phase two were to directly address the methods in which participants vocalised the
focus phonemes. This would be done through two enjoyable sessions which used beatboxing as a
medium to facilitate the vocalisation of the phonemes. In order to condense these aims into more
‘child-friendly’ language; a suitable phrase was designed which was both memorable and
contained the desirable success criteria. This phrase was ‘short and sweet and on the beat’: one
which aimed to encourage the plosive, rhythmic sounds that are desirable in a synthetic blending
method of reading and that are also the ‘foundation of beatboxing’ (Stanovich, 1993).
To ensure research triangulation, video footage was recorded for triangulation purposes and to
ensure maximum objectivity. The structure of these sessions can be seen here (C.F. Appendix 3).
Despite difficulties being experienced in terms of logistic and technological faults outlined in the
evaluation of the lesson (C.F. Appendix 3), the sessions were successful and the aims achieved.
The next stage was to use my own observations with reference to the video, to gather pertinent
information and analyse it in relation to hypotheses
(1) & (2). This next extract is audio from the video footage taken from session one (C.F.
Appendix 9):
4 minutes and 55 second (note: - some class teachers were acquiring resources at the time so
there was some background noise)
Researcher: When the screen goes to the focus phoneme I’d like you to say it for me ok?
Analysis of this footage shows the researcher did not acknowledge that NB was over-vocalising
the ‘ck’ or ‘k’ phoneme. The session progressed and NB engaged in the activities designed to
encourage participants to vocalise the phonemes in the ‘short and sweet’ manner desired. As the
researcher and one who was directly involved in the research process, the requirement for the
need of multiple data collection instruments became apparent. It is only upon analysis of the
video footage that one can view an early progression in relation to hypothesis (1). At 28 minutes
and 47 seconds into the session, when the children were asked to participate in a task requiring
them to think of more words containing the focus phonemes, a pertinent observation was made:
(NB can be seen and heard in the far left of the footage)
NB: ‘Chuh’
This scenario that was previously overlooked suggests that NB has taken note that the aim of the
sessions was to ensure the phonemes stay ‘short and sweet’. He clearly begins to apply this
concept by self-correcting his vocalisation of the ‘ch’ phoneme.
At 29 minutes and 22 seconds, NB again self-corrects his vocalisation of the focus phonemes:
Researcher: so can you think of some more words with our focus phonemes?
NB: a bat
NB: ‘buh’
NB: ‘b’
NB: ‘t’
By this stage it seems that NB had already begun to utilise the concept of keeping the phonemes
‘short and sweet’ and applied it to his own vocalisation of the focus phonemes. One could
suggest that thus far, utilising the fundamental aspects under the heading of ‘beatboxing with
phonics’ is facilitating the application of synthetic phonic skills.
Session two of the implementation stage was designed to build upon the concept of using
phonemes that are ‘short and sweet and on the beat’. The participants would create a nonsense
poem (C.F. Appendix 10), allowing for assessment of the skills used regarding vocalisation of
phonemes and the awareness of rhythm. The nonsense aspect meant that the participants did not
need to worry over the content of the poem; it was simply the sounds and rhythm of the words
that were being assessed.
By putting the focus phoneme words into a ‘rhythm frame’ (C.F. Appendix 4a), the participants
would be pulling together their ability to keep the phonemes ‘short and sweet and on the beat’.
In order to observe the participants outputs, close up video footage of children reciting their
‘beatbox poems’ (C.F. Appendix 11) was captured, to both view any visual signs of enjoyment
(relating to hypothesis 2) and to gauge a clearer view of how they vocalised the phonemes
(hypothesis 1). Analysis of NB’s close up video will be used as a representative example.
Footage of NB’s close up nonsense poem (C.F. Appendix 11), showed that no obvious signs of
visual enjoyment appear. However, there are valid reasons why this may have been the case: due
to the nature of this close up, one may deduce that NB felt nervous; aware that he was being
filmed and thus interfered with any intrinsic or extrinsic levels of enjoyment. In order to
triangulate and measure the representativeness of this observation; I analysed two other close up
videos of participant’s poems. These participants will be referred to as JA and FK. In JA’s video
(C.F. Appendix 11), we can see a similar result as there is a clear ‘nervous’ disposition as she
recites her poem. Similarly, in FK’s this feeling is seemingly reciprocated. In terms of hypothesis
(2), it is difficult to make any definitive observations as the participants may have been
experiencing a ‘Hawthorne effect’; a ‘modification of behaviour based on the knowledge of
being observed’ (Parsons, 1974:922).
In relation to hypothesis (1) however, from the close up footage one can hear that NB is
whispering the nonsense poem. It seems NB is attempting to lower the dynamics of his voice in
order to make the vocalisation of the focus phonemes more clear. As a result he was fulfilling the
‘short and sweet’ aspect of the success criteria. (C.F. Appendix 11):
Another relevant observation is that during the poem, NB engages in a clear ‘head-nodding’
motion. This is performed on each beat of the 4/4 pulse that the poem adopts. From this, it
implies that NB is also fulfilling the ‘on the beat’ aspect of the success criteria. This recognition
of the rhythm in language seemingly supports Kolb’s (1996) research which outlines that:
“due to the fact that children have a natural and innate disposition towards rhythm it is an
appropriate and effective tool for developing listening, speaking and writing skills as it enables
children to become aware of the rhythm and patterns in spoken and written language.”
It seems NB had recognised the value of the ‘short and sweet’ element when vocalising the focus
phonemes. In conjunction with it being ‘on the beat’, NB has combined the two essential
elements of the success criteria, creating the desired ‘beatbox’.
To truly assess the value of NB’s progression through this research, analysis of the outcomes of
phase three is integral. The purpose of this final semi-structured interview was to view how far
NB had progressed in relation to hypotheses (1) & (2). NB’s interview was transcribed and
analysed. Reflecting the structure of phase one, NB’s perceptions of enjoyment as a standalone
concept and subsequently his enjoyment of beatboxing were noted (C.F. Appendix 8):
Researcher: Do you know what I mean when I say ‘you enjoy something’. What does
‘enjoying’ something mean?
NB: Like when you do something and its good and you like it. Like when you play a game and
you like it.
On the surface, NB’s response in phase three was similar to phase one. However, at this stage
NB does explain in more depth and gives an example of when one may enjoy something. When
considering Dittermore’s theory that music increases “our power to understand what it is to truly
enjoy and to achieve more control over our activities which bring enjoyment”
(Dittermore: 1968:12). One may deduce that NB, through engagement in focused musical
activity (beatboxing), has become more aware of enjoyment as a concept.
More specifically, when asked to specify what it was about working with the idea of beatboxing
that NB enjoyed, if we compare his response in phase one to phase three (C.F. Appendix 3), a
clear change in NB’s knowledge is evident. He has moved from not knowing that beatboxing
uses phonemes, to knowing that it does. However, it is important to note the ‘leading’ nature of
such questions and this fact must be acknowledged when considering the validity of such an
observation.
Analysing the phase three interview in relation to hypothesis (1), information was gathered in an
identical manner to phase one. This aimed to assess how the vocalisation of the ‘focus
phonemes’ had progressed as a result of the implementation stage. These were the results:
Researcher: Ok could you read these phonemes for me?
By comparing the vocalisation of the phonemes from the interview in phase one to that of phase
three, NB has clearly positively progressed his vocalisation of the phonemes ‘b’
from ‘buh’ to ‘b’ and ‘t’ from ‘tuh’to ‘t’.
Furthermore, when asked to read aloud words containing the focus phonemes. These were the
results:
(NB can be heard vocalising ‘tuh’ then self corrects… ‘t’ ‘i’ ‘ck’ ‘er’)
NB: ticker
(NB can be heard vocalising ‘tuh’, then self corrects ‘t’ ‘i’ ‘ck’)
NB: tick!
When asked to use the focus phonemes to blend, it is clear that NB still began by over-vocalising
the phonemes however he, without prompt, self-corrected and continued with the ‘short and
sweet’ versions of the phonemes that had been covered in phase two. As a result, by directly
relating this discovery to hypothesis (1), the information gathered implies the implementation
stage has had a positive effect on NB’s vocalisation. It must be made clear however, that these
are not definitive conclusions and due to the nature of the study, these are simply insights and
implications. The relevance and validity of which will be discussed in the conclusion.
Validity
When considering the validity of this study, one could propose a significant flaw is the lack of an
impartial researcher. If a positivist approach had been adopted, this proposal would have been
acknowledged. Although, due to the small scale and ‘insight’ nature of this study, from the outset
there were clear vested interests from the researcher and thus validity may have been
compromised. However, the definition of validity as described by Siraj-Blatchford (2001),
provides an alternative viewpoint:
‘ethnographic researchers are more interested in what ‘seems’ to be true than in any objectively
defined truth and ‘our assessments of validity are based upoin our perception of ‘worth’ and
hence our value systems. Thus in ‘clarifying our rationale we are delegating this responsibility to
the reader, who may accept or reject our explicit values’.
Upon reflection, it is integral to note that my own position as researcher has will affect the
influence and interpretation of the results. Thus ‘coming clean’ with my own vested interests and
allowing the reader to make their own judgements is where the validity of this study becomes
evident.
However, my aim is not to cease here. If I were to re-implement this independent research study,
I would undoubtedly choose a larger sample size and more utilise more effective and well
researched data collection instruments, in order to triangulate and obtain optimum validity.
Therefore, this small scale research study has inspired a desire to take this research further and
into a larger context. The next step is to assess the requirements to facilitate such an educational
study.
5. Conclusion
This research began as a study to discover the extent to which beatboxing could increase the skill
and enjoyment of phonics for young people. Developing on my personal and professional
experiences, I used available academic literature to support my hypothesis; music can be used as
a tool to educate in a non-traditional sense (for the purpose of this study: phonics). What I
realised from the initial stages of my research was that due to the nature of and general
accessibility to, the art form of beatboxing (forming out of hip-hop cultures of the 1990s), may
be particularly relatable to young people from low-socioeconomic backgrounds. Whilst aware of
the flaws of this generalisation, I believe that this connection is still incredibly important and
significantly under-researched.
It is evident that beatboxing and other “urban” practices have more or less been excluded from
academic discussion (Morrell et. Al., 2002:85). For a variety of reasons (of which I lack the
structural space to explore now, see: Gilborn 2006, Baratz and Baratz 2012) these non-traditional
and alternative practices have been perceived to lack the necessary value to warrant significant
academic research. It is for this reason that I felt apprehensive embarking upon such research as
my views and opinions would appear in relative isolation. Yet, I believe it has strong academic
grounding from which this research project aims to prove. By bringing the link between phonics
and beatboxing to the proverbial academic table, I aim to highlight the necessity and importance
of giving weight to alternative methods of research. Everywhere in current educational practices,
we can see examples of ‘lending’ from other cultures and I believe beatboxing is an
educationally viable context for phonic instruction.
The most poignant outcome of this study has been the amount of progress made by the focus
participant in such a short space of time. I believe that with more time and access to better
resources this area should warrant greater research, as it may potentially hold alternative teaching
methods that would serve to enrich the curriculum. I hope this paper will spark useful debate
regarding how the use of non-traditional educational methods can benefit early education.
6. References
Bell, J. (1993) Doing Your Research Project (2nd Ed), Buckingham: Open University Press
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2000). When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing
biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26
Cohen, L. and Manion, L. (1994) Research Methods in Education. London, Routeledge and
Kegan Paul
Denscombe, M. (2010) (4th ed). The good research guide for small scale social research
projects. Maidenhead: Open University Press
Department for Education (2010) The Importance of Teaching: Schools White Paper 2010.
http://www.education.gov.uk/ [accessed 10 January 2012].
Department for Education and Skills (DfES) & Primary National Strategy (PNS) (2006). Phonics
and early reading: an overview for headteachers, literacy leaders and teachers in schools, and
managers and practitioners in early years settings, Guidance Papers. Available online at: http://
www.standards.dfes.gov.uk/primaryframeworks/literacy/Papers/ [accessed 2 February, 2012]
Goetz, A. (2004). Promoting self-regulatory competences in students. Final report to the first
wave of the project. Department of Psychology, University of Munich
Goetz, A et al. (2003) Hall Emotions, learning and achievement from an educational–
psychological perspective P. Mayring, C. v. Rhoeneck (Eds.), Learning emotions. The influence
of affective factors on classroom learning, Peter Lang, Frankfurt am Main, pp. 9–28
Goswami, U. (2008) Learning to read across languages: The role of phonics and synthetic
phonics, in: K. Goouch & A. Lambirth (Eds) Understanding phonics and the teaching of
reading: critical perspectives. UK, Open University Press, pp. 124–143.
Hallam, S. (2010) 'Music Education' In P.Juslin and J. Sloboda (eds) , Handbook of Music and
Emotion: theory, research, applications. Oxford (Oxford University Press).
Hempenstall, K. (2001). School-based reading assessment: Looking for vital signs. Australian
Journal of Learning Disabilities, pp. 26-35.
Kolb, G. (1996). Read with a beat: Developing literacy through music and song. The Reading
Teacher 50, no. 1: 76–9. Lederer, K. (2004) Part 1: An Introduction to beatboxing. [online]
Available at: http://www.humanbeatbox.com/article/the-phonetics-of-beatboxing-part-
1/ [Accessed: 15/01/2012].
Maykut, P., & Morehouse, R. (1994). Beginning qualitative research: A philosophic and
practical guide. London: Falmer Press.
Morrell, E and Duncan, J. (2002) Promoting Academic Literacy with Urban Youth through
Engaging Hip-Hop Culture. The English Journal. Vol. 91, No. 6. Article Stable. pp. 88-92Noise,
W. (2006) The Real History of Beatboxing: Part 2. [online] Available
at: http://www.humanbeatbox.com/article/history-of-beatboxing-part-2/ [Accessed: 20/01/2012].
National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education. (1999). All Our
Futures: Creativity, Culture and Education.
Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: an evidence-based assessment
of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading
instruction: Reports of the subgroups. NIH publication no. 00-4754 (Washington, DC, US
Government Printing Office).
OFSTED (2009) Inspection Report 111156, London; Office for standards in Education
Pound, L. & C. Harrison (2003), Supporting Musical Development in the Early Years,
Buckingham: Open University Press, p.23.
Primary National Strategy (2007) Letters and sounds, Available online at:
http://www.standards.dfes.gov.uk/clld/las.html (Accessed 21 December 2007). Towell, J H
(2000) Motivating students through music and literature. The Reading Teacher. Vol 53. No 4.
Pp284-297
Qualification and Curriculum Authority (2004) ‘Key Skills: Communication Level One’ Key
Skills Qualifications Standards and Guidance. QCA: London. http://www.qca.org.uk/
Robson, C. (2002) Real World Research (2nd Ed.) Ocford, Blackwell Publishers
Rose, J. (2009) Independent Review of the Primary Curriculum: Final Report (No. 00499-
2009DOM-EN). DCSF.
Scheirer E. (1998), “Tempo and beat analysis of acoustic musical signals,” Journal of the
.Acoustical Society of America, vol. 103, no. 1, pp. 588,601
Stannard J. (2005) `Give Them Literacy with All the Lights On', The Times Educational
Supplement (9 December): 21.
Stanovich, K. E. (1993). Does reading make you smarter? Literacy and the development of
verbal intelligence. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 24, pp.
133-180). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Susannah, J. et al. (1993) The Relationship between Music and Reading in Beginning
Readers. Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational
Psychology, 13 (1), p.21.
Torgeson, J.K. (1994) Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading, Journal of
Learning Disabilities, 27, pp. 276–286.
UKLA (2005) Submission to the Rose Review. (Accessed online at: http://www.ukla.org January
2012).
Watkins, C.L. (1997). Project Follow Through: A case study of contingencies influencing
instructional practices of the educational establishment. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge Center for
Behavioral Studies.
Watson, J. & Johnston, R. (1998) Accelerating reading attainment: the effectiveness of synthetic
phonics (The Scottish Office: Education and Industry Department). Available online at:
www.standards.dfes.gov.uk/pdf/literacy/rjohnston-phonics.pdf
Youth Music. (2006). Turning their ears on . . . keeping their ears open - Exploring the impact of
musical activities on the development of pre-school age children. Northumberland: Northumbria
University.