123dok Petunjuk+Teknis+Cara+Distribusi+Alat+Kesehatan+yang+Baik+ (CDAKB)
123dok Petunjuk+Teknis+Cara+Distribusi+Alat+Kesehatan+yang+Baik+ (CDAKB)
123dok Petunjuk+Teknis+Cara+Distribusi+Alat+Kesehatan+yang+Baik+ (CDAKB)
Chapter 6
Using Behavioral Observation Audiometry to Evaluate
Hearing in Infants from Birth to 6 Months
Jane R. Madell
54
c06.qxd 2/13/08 8:55 PM Page 55
occur before the age of 6 months, and Gerber (1977) reported identified using these methods, many with less than severe
the average age of head turn to be at 7 1/2 months. to profound hearing losses were missed. In spite of all at-
tempts to improve test protocols, BOA continued to be con-
sidered “unreliable.”
Noisemakers
Noisemakers were the most common sound source em-
Infant Thresholds
ployed for early hearing tests. They were selected for testing
because they were readily available, simple and inexpensive, Because behavioral test protocols frequently did not reveal
and could be used in any setting (a sound room was not re- threshold responses, some audiologists proposed that re-
quired) and it was believed that infants would respond more sponses at 60 to 70 dB SPL be interpreted as normal hearing
reliably to noisemakers than to pure tone stimuli. The diffi- for very young infants (McConnell and Ward, 1967, Northern
culty with noisemakers is that they usually have very broad and Downs, 1984). However, others demonstrated that in-
frequency responses. Furthermore, their intensity is not easy fants hear at essentially adult levels (Berg and Smith, 1983;
to control even with practice exerting the pressure neces- Eisele, Berry, and Shriner 1975; Madell, 1995a, 1998; Olsho,
sary to make the sound, and stabilizing the distance from 1984; Olsho et al 1988; Spetner, and Olsho 1990; Werner and
the infant’s ear. Bove and Flugrath (1973), and Poblano et al. Gillenwater, 1990). Olsho et al (1984) Olsho et al, (1987b,
(2000) analyzed different noisemakers to determine their 1988), and Nozza (2006), reported that average behavioral
frequency responses so that responses to noisemakers could thresholds of 3-month-olds were worse than thresholds for
provide more useful information. Even if noisemakers can- young adults by 15 to 20 dB between 250 and 4000 Hz, and
not provide sufficient information to be used to assess hear- by about 30 dB at 8000 Hz. By 6 months of age, hearing sen-
ing, they can provide some gross information about how an sitivity in the high frequencies improves but thresholds at
infant responds to sound. Specifically, noisemakers can pro- 250 Hz remain elevated by about 15 dB. Thresholds improved
vide some evidence of an infant’s ability to alert to sound by 20 dB between 3 and 6 months. Olsho et al (1988) dis-
and localize to the source (Northern and Downs, 2002). cussed that the audibility curve of younger infants may differ
Before using any noisemakers, information should be ob- in shape compared with the curve of older infants and adults.
tained about the auditory signals they emit, including their It was assumed that this audibility curve difference was, at
frequency response and intensity. Results of noisemaker tests least in part, due to the characteristics of the external and
must be viewed with caution. For example, a noisemaker middle ears in infants. Arlington (2000) and Olsho et al
may have the bulk of its energy in the 2000 to 4000 Hz range, (1988) postulated that some of the threshold differences may
but also have energy at 500 Hz at 30 to 40 dB less intensity be related to sensory immaturity.
than the high frequencies. What can be surmised about the Gravel (2000); Hicks, Tharpe, and Ashmead (2000); and
infant’s response to this stimulus? It is possible that the in- Olsho et al (1987a,b, 1988,) use an observer-based procedure
fant hears the high frequency component of the stimulus, but developed by Olsho to reduce tester bias in evaluating hear-
it is also possible that the infant has a high frequency hearing ing in infants as young as 2 to 5 weeks. In this method, a trial
loss, does not hear the high frequency part of the signal, and consists of a sound or a no-sound interval. One or two
is responding to the low frequency component. trained observers watch the infant and make a determination
as to whether the interval contained a sound, or no sound,
based on the infant’s response. The observer receives feed-
Early Infant Hearing Screening Programs
back as to whether a sound is present. Once the observer
The first large-scale infant hearing screening program in the demonstrates a false-positive rate of less than 25% reliable,
United States was a citywide hearing screening project in testing begins. Hicks, Tharpe, and Ashmead (2000) used this
Denver, conducted by Marion Downs and Graham Sterritt in technique with two observers testing 2 - and 4-month-old
1964. They used a handheld noise generator that emitted a infants. They successfully obtained thresholds for 4-month-
90 dB SPL noise centered at 3000 Hz. Downs and Sterritt olds, but were not successful in obtaining thresholds for
(1964, 1967), Northern and Downs (1991), and Werner and 2-month-olds. Several authors evaluating hearing in infants
Gillenwater (1990) attempted to develop a standardized pro- report that results could be optimized by enhancing the test
cedure to assess an infant’s behavioral arousal, but a signifi- conditions. This enhancement included reducing visual dis-
cant number of false-positive test results made the testing tractions (Muir, Clifton, and Clarkson, 1989), using a salient
unreliable. Several authors have described techniques for as- auditory stimulus (Thompson and Thompson, 1972), rein-
sessing behavioral responses in infants, including observa- forcing desired behaviors (Olsho et al, 1987a, 1988), and
tion of eye widening, quieting, eye shifting, head orienting, using changes in sucking as the response criteria (Delaroche
limb movement, and changes in respiration. Attempts have Thiebaut, and Dauman, 2004; Madell, 1995a, 1998). Because
been made to calibrate the observer (Mencher et al, 1977; of the critical need to obtain reliable test results on infants,
Weber, 1969), to assess the state of the infant (Eisenberg, research in this area will need to continue.
1969), and to precisely calibrate the signal (Thompson and
Thompson, 1972.) A major problem with using the auro-
The Need for Behavioral Testing of Infants
palpebral reflex, the Moro reflex, or changes in limb move-
ment or respiration is that these behaviors are not elicited to Over time, the demand for infant hearing screening has in-
threshold stimuli, but rather are responses to suprathresh- creased significantly, so that many states have mandated
old stimuli. Although some infants with hearing loss were newborn hearing screening requirements. (See Chapter 4 for
c06.qxd 2/13/08 8:55 PM Page 56
a complete discussion of newborn hearing screening.) As neonates include immittance testing with a high-frequency
more infants survive and as hearing screening becomes more probe tone, auditory brainstem response testing (ABR),
universal, audiologists are being asked to assess hearing in auditory steady state evoked potential (ASSEP) and/or OAE.
very young infants who have failed newborn screening and Immittance testing assesses middle ear status, but does
to manage hearing loss when it is identified. One of the first not provide information about hearing. ABR and ASSEP pro-
steps in hearing loss management is the selection and fitting vide information about the auditory system’s ability to
of appropriate amplification. Hearing aid fitting requires an receive sound, but are not direct measures of hearing. OAEs
accurate assessment of the degree and type of hearing loss, assess function of the outer hair cells of the cochlea but,
with both ear and frequency specific information obtained by again, are not a direct measure of hearing. Information
air and bone conduction. about an infant’s ability to hear and attend to auditory stim-
Many audiologists feel comfortable testing hearing in uli can be obtained only with behavioral testing. For that
infants older than 6 months using visual reinforcement au- reason, no at-risk infant or child should be released from
diometry (VRA), but do not feel comfortable testing younger audiologic follow-up until behavioral test results are ob-
infants, developmentally delayed infants, or critically ill in- tained. (See Chapter 13 for a discussion of immittance test-
fants. If an infant fails a hearing screening at birth, hearing ing, Chapter 14 for a discussion of OAE testing, and Chapter
aids should be fit within a few weeks. Work by Apuzzo and 15 for a discussion of ABR and ASSEP testing.)
Yoshinaga-Itana (1995); Yoshinaga-Itana, Couter, and
Thomson (2001), and others have demonstrated that in-
Pearl
fants who are fit with appropriate technology before they
are 6 months old can develop speech and language skills
• Although ABR, ASSR, and OAE testing provides important
commensurate with their normal hearing peers, and that
information about the status of the auditory system, only
infants fit with technology older than 6 months, do not
behavioral testing directly tests hearing. For this reason,
catch up to those fit earlier. Sharma, Dorman, and Spahr
it is critical that audiologists have a behavioral technique
(2002) have demonstrated that infants who receive audi-
that is accurate for assessing hearing in infants younger
tory stimulation at a sufficiently early age have evoked
than 6 months.
potential latencies similar to normal hearing peers, but
infants who do not have sufficiently early access do not.
Behavioral testing allows the parents to participate in test-
ing by allowing them to assist in determining when the infant The Basics of Behavioral Observation Audiometry
is responding to a sound. If parents are provided with infor-
What Is Being Observed?
mation about what to observe, they can be active participants
in testing, facilitating acceptance, and understanding of hear- Historically, many behaviors have been used to assess hear-
ing loss (Gravel and McCaughey, 2004). Electrophysiologic ing in infants (arousal, limb movement, respiration changes,
testing, on the other hand, provides little for a family to ob- eye blink), but these behaviors have not proven to be suffi-
serve. It is clear that we must develop test techniques for ciently repeatable, and more importantly, they have not
evaluating very young infants that will provide the ear and been good indicators of threshold. The behavior most likely
frequency specific information necessary for the evaluation, to provide threshold responses is a change in sucking
selection, and fitting of amplification. Real-ear measures pro- (Delaroche, Thiebaut, and Dauman, 2004; Madell, 1988, 1995a,
vide good information about how much sound is reaching the 1998; Widen and Keener, 2003). Arousal responses, limb
eardrum, but this information is difficult to interpret without movements, and eye blinks frequently reveal suprathresh-
good information about the status of the infant’s unaided old level responses, but rarely threshold, since these behav-
hearing. Tonal ABR and ASSR measures provide some of this iors typically are elicited to louder stimuli. Sucking responses,
information but thresholds obtained may vary by 15 dB. however, although present at suprathreshold levels, are fre-
BOA techniques can assist in obtaining ear and frequency spe- quently observed at, or close to, threshold. Either initiation
cific information and can provide confirmation of information or cessation of sucking is an acceptable response. Some in-
obtained from electrophysiologic tests. fants will start sucking when a sound is presented, others
will cease sucking, and some will do both.
Pearl
♦ Diagnostic Audiologic Evaluation
of Neonates • Cessation or initiation of sucking is the only reliable re-
sponse for obtaining behavioral thresholds in infants
younger than 6 months.
The goal of an audiologic evaluation of an infant is usually
to determine if the child has sufficient hearing to develop
speech and language. A complete diagnostic evaluation of
Maximizing Observation of the Sucking Response
infants should include immittance testing to assess middle
ear status, and a test technique that will provide frequency Sucking can be observed with a bottle, nursing at the breast,
and ear specific information, ideally for both air and bone or with a pacifier. The family should be instructed to bring
conduction. The most common test protocols for evaluating the infant to the evaluation session hungry so that he will be
c06.qxd 2/13/08 8:55 PM Page 57
ready to suck. The infant needs to be as comfortable as pos- the validity of the child’s response if it comes a long time af-
sible during testing, so, if the infant normally drinks from a ter presentation of the stimulus. With any test protocol, (be-
bottle, the family should bring one. If the infant normally havioral or electrophysiologic) responses can be accepted
nurses, it would be best if the infant is nursed during testing. only if they fall within a reasonable time window after pres-
For this test procedure to succeed, the mother has to be entation of the stimulus. Infants are fairly consistent, inter-
comfortable being observed nursing, and some women are nally. Some respond to the “on” of the stimulus and others
not. If the mother understands the reason for the intrusion respond to the “off.” The timing of the response is also usu-
on her privacy, she usually acquiesces. If the infant uses a ally consistent. Infants respond at about the same number of
pacifier, the family should bring one along. After the infant is seconds after presentation of the stimulus each time, with
finished eating, testing can frequently continue by observing the response time slightly shorter for louder stimuli (Madell,
sucking with a pacifier. If an infant is very hungry, it is best 1998; Northern and Downs, 2002; Thompson and Weber,
to allow him a little time to eat to enable him to get over 1974; Widen and Keener, 2003).
that initial extreme hunger before beginning testing.
As soon as the baby settles down, testing can begin. The
best way to observe the sucking response is to be able to see Positioning the Infant
the infant’s mouth close-up. A good view of the mouth can The necessity of appropriately positioning the infant cannot be
easily be obtained by having a video camera in the test overstated. Positioning may, in fact, be the most important
room that can be adjusted from the control room. By using factor in obtaining accurate test results with behavioral observa-
the zoom on the camera, it is possible to focus directly on tion audiometry. To obtain reliable test results, the infant needs
the infant’s mouth, which will enable the audiologist to to be resting in a comfortable position with full support of the
have an excellent view of sucking. If a camera is not avail- head and torso, and must be visible to the testers. If the child is
able, the audiologist needs to be certain that she can clearly nursing, the mother will be holding the child in her arms. If the
see changes in sucking to use this technique. child is using a bottle or a pacifier, the child may be held in
someone’s arms or placed in an infant seat (see Fig. 6–1A–C).
How Does One Know That the Sucking Is a Response The advantage of an infant seat is that the infant will not be re-
ceiving any “signals” from the mother when he hears the sound.
to a Sound Stimulus?
Involuntary movements such as stiffening by the mother in re-
As with all other behavioral responses, timing is the key fac- sponse to sound or movement of the breast or bottle can be
tor. When using play audiometry with a child, we question transmitted to the infant; therefore, changes in sucking may
Figure 6–1 Positioning the infant for testing: (A) using a bottle, (B) using a pacifier,
and (C) nursing at the breast.
c06.qxd 2/13/08 8:55 PM Page 58
occur that are not related to the auditory stimuli. If the infant is Testing Protocol of Behavioral Observation
being held, the mother or other person holding the infant Audiometry
should be very carefully instructed about the need to remain
silent and still throughout testing to eliminate interfering with Soundfield versus Earphone Testing
test results. It is sometimes useful to have the mother wear ear- A complete audiogram includes air and bone conduction
phones to prevent her from hearing and being influenced by the thresholds in each ear at frequencies of 250 to 8000 Hz.
sound; however, many mothers prefer not to wear earphones However, infants will provide only a limited number of re-
because they want to hear what their baby is hearing. sponses in one test session, so testing protocols need to be
designed to obtain the most information with the fewest re-
The Role of the Test Assistant sponses. The goal of the initial audiologic evaluation of an in-
fant is usually to be certain that the infant has sufficient
BOA is best accomplished by using two or more observers. hearing to develop speech and language. It may not be neces-
One is the audiologist controlling the test equipment, sary to obtain ear specific information at the first visit. (Occa-
usually outside of the room where the infant is placed. The sionally, a child is referred to a pediatric audiologist because
second observer typically is sitting next to the infant. Posi- of a medical condition that requires ear-specific information
tioning of all players needs to be carefully orchestrated to immediately, but this is more frequently the exception rather
be certain that both testers can easily see the infant. than the rule. When detailed information is required during
The test assistant has several responsibilities. He must the first test session, the test protocol will obviously have to
constantly be monitoring the infant to be certain that the change.) Ear specific information is important and must be
baby’s head and torso are comfortably balanced to minimize obtained prior to releasing an infant from audiologic follow-
or preclude fussing and straining. If the infant becomes up, but the more important question at the time of the initial
fussy, testing will need to stop until the infant can be made evaluation is, Does the infant hear enough to learn language?
comfortable (Madell, 1998). For older infants, or infants Should the initial audiologic evaluation indicate that hearing
using a bottle or a pacifier, the test assistant must keep the is normal in at least one ear utilizing soundfield testing, it
infant focused at the midline, again so that the infant is may not be critical to obtain information about each ear sep-
comfortable and not distracted. It is sometimes helpful to arately at that visit. However, if the initial testing indicates
hold a colorful toy (Madell, 1998) or an LED (light emitting that hearing is not within normal limits in the soundfield,
diode, usually a small red light) (Hicks, Tharpe, and Ashmead, then ear-specific information is critical so that management
2000; Olsho, 1987a) in front of the infant in a position that can proceed. No infant should be released from audiologic
allows the infant’s head to be centered. The toy should not follow-up until ear-specific information is obtained.
be held above the infant’s head so he needs to move his neck Under most conditions, testing should begin in soundfield.
to see it. Visual distractions need to be kept to a minimum Soundfield testing is less stressful for the infant and allows
(Muir, Clifton, and Clarkson, 1989) to be certain that extra- two ears to be stimulated at the same time. This ensures test-
neous stimuli are not interfering with observation of re- ing of the best hearing ear. It also permits parents to hear the
sponses. It is important that the person holding the toy or sounds; this can be very useful in their understanding of the
LED make no change in the movement of the toy when the test results. Earphone testing can follow later in the initial
sound is presented. Any change in movement can confound test session, or in a subsequent test session. When earphone
the interpretation of whether the infant is responding to the testing is being attempted, insert earphones are the ear-
sound or to the change in the distracter. If the infant is in an phones of choice for infants. Insert earphones (Fig. 6–2) will
infant seat, the test assistant may be the one holding the remain appropriately seated in the ear canal and will provide
bottle or pacifier and holding the visual distracters. Finally, the most accurate results in tiny ears. Circumaural earphones
the test assistant will be one of the observers who judges are frequently too large and are very difficult to keep in place.
whether or not the infant responded to the sound presenta- If testing indicates thresholds at lower than normal hear-
tion by changing his or her sucking behavior. ing levels, bone conduction testing is essential. The bone vi-
brator should be held in place with either a pediatric sized
The Role of the Parents headband, or a fabric one that goes around the head and
across the forehead using Velcro to secure it in place. If a
The parents cannot be relied on as observers. Their stakes are metal headband is used, soft material such as foam or other
too high, they are not experienced in the task, and they may padding should be used for comfort and to keep the head-
not understand exactly what constitutes an acceptable re- band from moving. If a hearing loss is confirmed, the same
sponse. Parents are, however, very valuable in helping the test protocols can be used to assess functional gain with
testers to understand the baby and assisting in making the amplification in soundfield.
baby comfortable. At least one parent needs to be in the test
room to assist in understanding the test protocols and test
results. If both parents are present, the other parent can ob-
Test Stimuli
serve from the control room. The audiologist in the control
room can point out responses during testing to assist in the When planning the test session, it is important to keep in
parent’s understanding of the tests. Their observation of how mind that infants will provide only a limited number of
the baby does or does not respond will be helpful when in- responses; and so each stimulus presentation must be consid-
terpreting the final test results and presenting subsequent ered carefully. The goal of the testing is to obtain frequency-
follow-up recommendations (Flasher and Fogel, 2004). specific test results. Warble tones or narrow bands of noise
c06.qxd 2/13/08 8:55 PM Page 59
References
Apuzzo, M. L., and Yoshinaga-Itana, C. (1995). Early identification of infants Hicks, C. B., Tharpe, A. M., and Ashmead, D. H. (2000). Behavioral auditory
with significant hearing loss and the Minnesota Child Development In- assessment of young infants: methodological limitations or natural lack
ventory. Seminars in Hearing, 16, 124–139. of auditory responsiveness? American Journal of Audiology, 9, 124–130.
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2004). Guidelines for Jerger, J. F., and Hayes, D. (1976). The cross-check principle in pediatric au-
the Audiologic Assessment of Children from Birth to 5 Years of Age. diometry. Archives of Otolaryngology, 102, 614–620.
www.asha.org/members/deskref-journals/deskref/default.
Ling, D. (2002). Speech and the hearing impaired child. (2nd ed.) Washington,
Berg, K. M., and Smith, M. C. (1983). Behavioral thresholds of tones during DC: Alexander Graham Bell Association of the Deaf and Hard of Hearing.
infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 409–425.
Madell, J. R. (1988). Identification and treatment of very young children
Bess, F. H., and Humes, L. E. (2003). Audiology: the fundamentals. 3rd ed. with hearing loss. Infants and Young Children, 1, 20–30.
Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Madell, J. R. (1995a). Behavioral evaluation of infants after hearing screen-
Bove, C., and Flugrath, J. M. (1973). Frequency components of noisemakers ing: Can it be done? Hearing Instruments, 12, 4–8.
for use in pediatric audiological evaluations. Volta Review 75, 551–556.
Madell, J. R. (1995b). Speech audiometry for children. In S.E. Gerber (Ed.) Pedi-
Delaroche, M., Thiebaut, R., and Dauman, R. (2004). Behavioral audiometry: atric audiology (pp. 84–103). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
protocols for measuring hearing thresholds in babies aged 4–18 months.
Madell, J. R. (1998). Behavioral evaluation of hearing in infants and young
International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 68, 1233–1243.
children. New York: Thieme.
Downs, M. P., and Sterritt, G. M. (1964). Identification audiometry for
McConnell, F., and Ward, P. (1967). Deafness in childhood. Nashville: Van-
neonates: a preliminary report. Journal of Auditory Research, 4, 69–80.
derbilt University Press.
Downs, M. P., and Sterritt, G. M. (1967). A guide to newborn and infants
Mencher, G. T., McCullouch, B., Derbyshire, A.J ., and Dethlefs, R. (1977).
hearing screening. Archives of Otolaryngology, 85, 15–22.
Observer bias as a factor in neonatal hearing screening. Journal of
Eisele, W. A., Berry, R. C., and Shriner, T. H. (1975). Infant sucking response Speech and Hearing Research, 20, 27–34.
patterns as a conjugate function of change in the sound pressure level of Muir, D. W., Clifton, R. K, and Clarkson, M. G. (1989). The development of a
auditory stimuli. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 18, 296–307. human auditory localization response: a U-shaped function. Canadian
Eisenberg, R. B. (1969). Auditory behavior in the human neonate: func- Journal of Psychology, 43, 199–216.
tional properties of sound and their ontogenetic implications. Interna- Northern, J., and Downs, M. (1974). Hearing in children. Baltimore:
tional Audiology, 8, 34–45. Williams and Wilkins.
Ewing, J. R., and Ewing, A. W. G. (1940). Discussion on audiometric tests Northern J., and Downs, M. (1984). Hearing in children. Baltimore:
and the capacity to hear speech. Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 55, Williams and Wilkins.
339–355.
Northern, J., and Downs, M. (1991). Hearing in children. 4th ed. Baltimore:
Ewing, J. R., and Ewing, A. W. G. (1944). The ascertainment of deafness in Williams and Wilkins.
infancy and early childhood. Journal of Laryngology and Otology, 54,
309–333. Northern, J. L., and Downs, M. P. (2002). Hearing in children. 5th ed. Balti-
more: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Flasher, L. V., and Fogel, P. T. (2004). Counseling skills for speech-language
pathologists and audiologists. Clifton Park, NY: Delmar Learning. Nozza, R. (2006). Developmental psychoacoustics: auditory function in in-
fants and children. Paper presented at the 4th Widex Congress of Paedi-
Flexer, C., and Gans, D. P. (1986). Distribution of auditory response behav- atric Audiology, Ottawa, Canada, May 19–21, 2006.
iors in normal infants and profoundly multihandicapped children. Jour-
nal of Speech and Hearing Research, 29, 425–429. Olsho, L. W. (1984). Infant frequency discrimination. Infant Behavior and
Development, 7, 27–35.
Frisina, R. (1963), Measurement of hearing in children. In J.F. Jerger, (Ed.),
Modern developments in audiology, New York: Academic. Olsho, L. W., Koch, E. G, Halpin, C. F., and Carter, E.A. (1987a). An observer-
based psychoacoustic procedure for use with young infants. Develop-
Froeschels, E., and Beebe, H. (1946). Testing the hearing of the newborn. mental Psychology, 23, 627–640.
Archives of Otolaryngology, 44, 710–714.
Olsho, L. W., Koch, E. G., and Halpin, C. F. (1987b). Level and age effects in
Gerber, S. E. (1977). Audiometry in Infancy. New York: Grune and Stratton. infant frequency discrimination. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 82, 454–464.
Gravel, J. (2000) Audiologic assessment for the fitting of hearing instruments:
big challenges from tiny ears. In R. Seewald (Ed). A sound foundation Olsho, L. W., Koch, E. G., Carter, E. A., Halpin, C. F., and Spetner, N. B. (1988).
through early amplification. Proceedings of an International Conference, Pure tone sensitivity of human infants. Journal of the Acoustical Society
Vanderbilt-Bill Wilkerson Press, Phonak, AG, Nashville, TN, 2000, pp. 33–46. of America, 84, 1316–1324.
Gravel, J. S., and McCaughey, C. C. (2004). Family-centered audiologic as- Poblano, A., Chayo, I., Ibarra, J., and Reuda, E. (2000). Electrophysiological
sessment for infants and young children with hearing loss. Seminars in and behavioral methods in early detection of hearing impairment.
Hearing, 25, 309–317. Archives of Medical Research, 31, 75–80.
c06.qxd 2/13/08 8:55 PM Page 64
Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., and Spahr, A. J. (2002). A sensitive period for White, K. R., Maxon, A. B., Behrens, T. B., Blackwell, P. M., and Vohr, B. R.
the development of the central auditory system in children with (1992). Neonatal screening using evoked otoacoustic emissions: the
cochlear implants: implications for age of implantation. Ear and Hear- Rhode Island hearing assessment project. In F. H. Bess and J. W. Hall III
ing, 23, 532–539. (Eds). Screening children for auditory function. Nashville: Bill Wilkerson
Center Press.
Sininger, Y. S. (1993). Evaluation of hearing in the neonate using the audi-
tory brainstem response. Consensus Development Conference on Early White, K. R.,Vohr, B. R., and Behrens, T. B. (1993). Universal newborn
Identification of Hearing impairment in Infants and Young Children, (pp. screening using transient evoked otoacoustic emissions: results of the
95–97). Bethesda: National Institutes of Health. Rhode Island hearing assessment project. Seminars in Hearing, 14,
18–29.
Thompson, M., and Thompson, F. (1972). Response of infants and young
children as a function of auditory stimuli and test method. Journal of Widen, J. E. (1993). Adding objectivity to infant behavioral audiometry. Ear
Speech and Hearing Research, 15, 699-707. and Hearing, 14, 49–57.
Weber, B. A. (1969). Validation of observer judgments in behavioral obser- Widen, J. E., and Keener, S. (2003) Diagnostic testing for hearing loss in in-
vation audiometry. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, fants and young children. Mental Retardation and Developmental Dis-
350–355. abilities Research Reviews, 9, 220–224.
Wedenberg, E. (1956). Auditory tests on newborn infants. Acta Otolaryngo- Yoshinaga-Itana, C, Couter, D., and Thomson, V. (2001) Developmental out-
logica, 46, 446–461. comes of children with hearing loss born in Colorado hospitals with and
without universal newborn hearing screening programs. Seminars in
Werner, L., and Gillenwater, J. (1990). Pure tone sensitivity of 2-5 week old Neonatology, 6, 521–529.
infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 355–375.