NIH Public Access: Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention For Substance Craving
NIH Public Access: Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention For Substance Craving
NIH Public Access: Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention For Substance Craving
Author Manuscript
Addict Behav. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 February 01.
Published in final edited form as:
Addict Behav. 2013 February ; 38(2): 1563–1571. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.04.001.
bCenterfor the Study of Health and Risk Behaviors, Department of Psychiatry, University of
Washington, Box 354944, 1100 NE 45th St, Suite 300, Seattle WA 98105 USA.
swbowen@uw.edu
cDepartment
of Psychology, University of Washington, Box 351525, Seattle WA 98195 USA.
hacdougl@uw.edu and shsinhsu@gmail.com
Abstract
Craving, defined as the subjective experience of an urge or desire to use substances, has been
identified in clinical, laboratory, and preclinical studies as a significant predictor of substance use,
substance use disorder, and relapse following treatment for a substance use disorder. Various
models of craving have been proposed from biological, cognitive, and/or affective perspectives,
$watermark-text
and, collectively, these models of craving have informed the research and treatment of addictive
behaviors. In this article we discuss craving from a mindfulness perspective, and specifically how
mindfulness-based relapse prevention (MBRP) may be effective in reducing substance craving.
We present secondary analyses of data from a randomized controlled trial that examined MBRP as
an aftercare treatment for substance use disorders. In the primary analyses of the data from this
trial, Bowen and colleagues (2009) found that individuals who received MBRP reported
significantly lower levels of craving following treatment, in comparison to a treatment-as-usual
control group, which mediated subsequent substance use outcomes. In the current study, we
extend these findings to examine potential mechanisms by which MBRP might be associated with
lower levels of craving. Results indicated that a latent factor representing scores on measures of
acceptance, awareness, and nonjudgment significantly mediated the relation between receiving
MBRP and self-reported levels of craving immediately following treatment. The mediation
findings are consistent with the goals of MBRP and highlight the importance of interventions that
$watermark-text
increase acceptance and awareness, and help clients foster a nonjudgmental attitude toward their
experience. Attending to these processes may target both the experience of and response to
craving.
Keywords
craving; substance use disorder; mindfulness; relapse prevention; MBRP
1. Introduction1
Over the past decade, substance use disorder has been conceptualized as a chronic relapsing
condition (McLellan, 2002; McLellan, McKay, Forman, Cacciola, & Kemp, 2005), where
relapse has been variously defined as either the return to problematic substance use
following treatment or as a process of behavior change (Brownell, Marlatt, Lichtenstein, &
$watermark-text
Wilson, 1986; Maisto, Pollock, Cornelius, Lynch, & Martin, 2003; Witkiewitz & Marlatt,
2004). A substantial amount of research over the past 20 years has focused on identifying
predictors of relapse and developing treatments (including pharmacological and
psychological) that may help prevent relapse. One of the strongest predictors of relapse to
emerge in both pre-clinical and clinical research studies is craving (Anton, 1999; Breese,
Sinha, & Heilig, 2011; Drummond, 2001; Marlatt, 1978; Shadel et al., 2011; Sinha &
O'Malley, 1999), and many of the promising pharmacotherapies and most effective
psychotherapies for addiction have focused on reducing or managing substance craving. In
the current paper, we review the efficacy of mindfulness-based relapse prevention as a
treatment for substance use disorders and empirically examine mechanisms of action for
reduction of substance craving.
(Tiffany & Wray, 2009), a physical sensation (Paulus, 2007), a stress response (Sinha & Li,
2007), or any other manifestation that is salient for an individual who endorses experiencing
“craving” or an “urge” to use substances.
1Abbreviations. MBRP = mindfulness-based relapse prevention, TAU = treatment as usual, PACS = Penn Alcohol Craving Scale,
AAQ = Acceptance and Action Questionnaire, FFMQ = Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire, ACT = Acting with Awareness
subscale of the FFMQ, NONJ = Nonjudgmental awareness subscale of FFMQ, CFI = Comparative Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean
Square Error of Approximation, SE = standard error
associated with reported craving (Volkow et al., 2006), and GABA dysregulation has been
associated with a craving drive described as a relief of tension (Addolorato, Abenavoli,
Leggio, & Gasbarrini, 2005). Other biological models of craving focus on physiological
withdrawal states, wherein craving can occur as interoceptive dysregulation (Goldstein et
al., 2009; Paulus & Stein, 2006).
Affective models suggest that craving is an emotion that can be elicited by affective
expectancies, negative affect or stress (Baker, Morse, & Sherman, 1986; Wikler, 1948). In
terms of positive expectancy, craving for drug use is elicited with positive associations with
the effects of drug use. With negative affect, craving is suggested to be a state elicited by the
avoidance of negative affect or stress associated with withdrawal such that craving can be
$watermark-text
both the result and cause of stress (Sinha & Li, 2007). Thus, the core motivation to avoid
negative affective states is the cause of craving (Baker, Piper, Fiore, McCarthy, & Majeskie,
2004). In support, stress- and negative-affect-induced states have been shown to increase
craving in the laboratory (Sinha & O'Malley, 1999). Further, negative affect is one of the
most frequently endorsed reasons for relapse (Brownell et al., 1986; Marlatt & Gordon,
1985). Within an affective model of craving, affective states can elicit craving or prevent
individuals from inhibiting craving.
an event can trigger craving, even in a laboratory setting (Sinha & Li, 2007). Additional
evidence suggests that self-efficacy is a critical factor in the relation between craving and
substance use (Marlatt & Witkiewitz, 2005). Hence, cognitive models of craving clearly
outline craving as a psychological process, separate from drug use, whereby craving can
occur without substance use, and substance use can occur without craving (Skinner &
Aubin, 2010).
While these perspectives provide unique explanations of the causes of craving, many
specific models of craving are a complex amalgam of biological, affective and cognitive
constructs. For example, the withdrawal model (Wikler, 1948) describes craving, or the
drive to use, as a result of both a biological conditioned response to drug related stimuli, and
an attempt to escape negative affective states. Additionally, the theory of neural opponent
motivation identifies craving as a biological deviation from the homeostatic regulation of
$watermark-text
neurotransmitters that can be elicited by change in affective states (Koob & Le Moal, 2001,
2008).
Mindfulness has been described as, “the awareness that emerges through paying attention,
on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally to the unfolding of experience”
(pg. 145; Kabat-Zinn, 2003). While secularized in most Western treatment contexts,
mindfulness meditation has roots in the Buddhist tradition. From a Buddhist perspective,
craving is considered a core component of human existence, and craving and attachment are
viewed as the root cause of human suffering (Bodhi, 2005). From a mindfulness perspective,
we might view addiction as an effort to either hold on to or avoid cognitive, affective or
physical experiences. In an effort to avoid suffering, an individual either clings onto positive
states (e.g., craving the next high) or avoids negative states (e.g., seeking an escape from
sadness). Mindfulness practice includes observing craving, which is considered to be a
transient cognitive and affective phenomenon, just like any other experience. Thus, the
$watermark-text
intention of the practice is to bring awareness to the experience of craving and to learn to
observe it without reacting and without judgment.
smoking images, as compared to looking at the smoking images without mindful attention.
Furthermore, during mindful attention, there was significantly reduced functional
connectivity between the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and other regions associated
with craving, including the ventral striatum and the bilateral insula. Taken together, there is
evidence to suggest that mindfulness-based treatment has the potential in addressing
neurobiological, cognitive, and affective aspects of craving.
training for addiction, Mindfulness-Based Relapse Prevention (MBRP; Bowen, Chawla, &
Marlatt, 2010; Witkiewitz, Marlatt, & Walker, 2005), was developed as an aftercare
treatment program that was designed to reduce the risk and severity of relapse following
intensive substance abuse treatment.
components of their experience while slowly increasing exposure and intensity to the
craving response. They practice approaching the reactions with a gentle curiosity, and are
given instructions to guide them through “staying with” the experience without exacerbating
it, giving into it, or attempting to suppress it. The exercise allows clients to practice imaginal
exposure and nonreactivity to substance use triggers. They learn skills to stay in contact with
the internal reactions to external triggers (i.e., craving in response to substance use cues) that
put them at high risk for relapse. Additionally, they learn an alternative, competing response
to craving by approaching the experience with curious awareness, deescalating the process
by not engaging in habitual cognitive or behavioral patterns that tend to intensify the craving
reaction.
$watermark-text
In order to increase ability to tolerate the discomfort often associated with craving and other
reactions to triggers clients maintain an ongoing practice of both formal meditation and of
exercises designed to increase awareness of triggers and reactions. They begin to increase
their ability to endure the affective and physical discomfort without reacting in ways that
may temporarily relieve distress, but lead to problematic longer-term outcomes. The final
two sessions of the course focus on social and environmental factors that either support or
detract from the maintenance of treatment gains and an ongoing mindfulness practice.
either inpatient or intensive outpatient treatment, and be medically stable to progress into
aftercare. As such, all participants had completed initial treatment immediately prior to
entering the trial, and were thus in early stages of abstinence. Clients with psychotic
disorders or acute suicidality were excluded from participating.
Following the 8-week treatment period, participants randomized to the MBRP condition
returned to their regular agency aftercare programs. As described in more detail in Section 2,
assessments were given at baseline, immediately following the 8-week treatment period, and
2 and 4 months following the treatment. Individual characteristics, psychosocial factors and
substance use in the 60day period prior to entering initial inpatient or intensive outpatient
treatment were assessed. MBRP and TAU participants reported using substances on 27 (SD
= 24) and 28.9 (SD =24.8) days, respectively (Bowen et al., 2009). This difference was not
statistically significant.
With respect to substance use outcomes, participants in both groups had a low base rate of
substance use during and following treatment, with average days of use over the follow-up
of 9.33 days for TAU (SD = 20.80) and 5.62 days for MBRP (SD = 14.33). Across both
groups, fewer than 30% of participants (29.1% in TAU, 28.6% in MBRP) had any days of
use. Of those who used, 28.6% and 33.3% of TAU and MBRP participants, respectively,
only used substances on one day during the follow-up period. A curvilinear effect of
treatment on substance use outcomes suggested that treatment gains made by MBRP
participants, compared to TAU participants, decayed by 4 months post-treatment. Analyses
of craving showed a significantly greater decrease over the 4-month follow-up period in
MBRP participants as compared to those in TAU. Additionally, there were significant
increases in acceptance, as measured by the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (Hayes et
al., 2004), in MBRP versus TAU participants. Secondary analyses of data from the study by
Bowen and colleagues (2009) found individuals who received MBRP were less likely to
experience craving in response to depressed mood and the attenuated reactivity to depressed
mood and reduced craving also predicted fewer days of substance use for those who
received MBRP (Witkiewitz & Bowen, 2010). Based on these findings, we hypothesized
$watermark-text
that MBRP may extinguish the habitual response of subjective craving during periods of
negative mood. Yet, previous studies have not examined mechanisms by which MBRP
might reduce craving or alter the response of craving during negative mood states.
Given the basic tenets of MBRP, we propose numerous factors may predict levels of self-
reported craving and changes in craving over time following MBRP. As noted above, one of
the primary goals of MBRP is to target both the experience of and response to craving.
Through several exercises and practices, clients increase their awareness of triggers that
elicit craving and of the “automatic” craving reaction in response to these triggers. They
practice acceptance of the discomfort often associated with triggers that may have, in the
past, led to craving for escape relief, such as a desire for a substance to decrease the intensity
of the negative affective, cognitive, or physical state. Finally, clients practice relating to their
experiences and reactions with a nonjudgmental attitude, decreasing the distress often
$watermark-text
psychosis, dementia, imminent suicide risk, significant withdrawal risk, need for more
intensive treatment, or not completing inpatient or intensive outpatient treatment. Out of 260
individuals screened, 29% (n = 73) failed to meet eligibility criteria, with primary exclusions
being not completing the treatment program prior to the study enrollment (n = 58), active
psychosis (n = 10), scheduling conflicts (n = 4), and active suicidality (n = 1). Of those
eligible to participate (n = 187), eighteen declined participation or failed to attend the
baseline appointment and one individuals refused to be randomized. In anticipation of higher
attrition in a novel treatment, we oversampled for the MBRP condition by 10%.
Randomization was conducted using a web-based random number sequencer (http://
www.randomizer.org). For the final total sample size of 168, of which 93 (55%) were
randomized to MBRP and 75 (45%) were randomized to TAU, reflecting the oversampling
for the MBRP treatment group.
$watermark-text
The majority of participants (63.7%) were male, with an average age of 40.5 (10.3) years.
Approximately half identified as Caucasian (51.8%), followed by African American
(28.6%), Multiracial (15.3%), and Native American (7.7%). Approximately 41.3% reported
being unemployed, with 32.9% receiving some form of public assistance, and 62.3% earning
less than $4999 per year. Approximately 45.2% of the sample reported alcohol as their
primary substance of abuse, followed by cocaine/crack (36.2%), methamphetamines
(13.7%), opiates/heroin (7.1%), marijuana (5.4%), and other (1.9%). Approximately 19.1%
reported polysubstance use. Over 40% of the sample was in treatment for legal reasons (e.g.,
treatment was court ordered) and for many individuals substance use was prohibited during
and following treatment.
2.2. Procedures
$watermark-text
All study procedures were approved by the University of Washington Institutional Review
Board. No side effects of treatment or adverse events during the course of the study were
detected or reported. Participants were recruited near the end of their inpatient or outpatient
treatment through flyers and referrals from agency or research staff. Potential participants
contacted research staff by telephone, provided verbal consent for screening, and completed
a 30–45 minute telephone eligibility screen. Following informed consent procedures,
eligible participants completed a web-based baseline assessment in a private room at the
treatment agency, with research staff available to assist or answer participants' questions.
Each participant was assigned a unique study ID number, which also serves as the ID
number for accessing the subsequent follow-up web-based assessments, which participants
were free to complete at a location of their choosing. Following completion of the
assessment, participants were randomly assigned (using a computerized random number
generator) to either 8-weeks of MBRP or continuation of their existing treatment (treatment
as usual, TAU). Participants randomized to MBRP agreed to discontinue TAU for the 8-
weeks of the course, and to resume TAU following completion of MBRP. MBRP
participants were scheduled to complete a web-based follow-up assessment immediately
following the 8-week course, and 2-months and 4-months following the intervention. TAU
participants followed the same schedule. Reminder calls for follow-up assessments were
made to each cohort according to assessment schedule. Given that the assessment was web-
based, participants were given the option to complete the assessment on their own or
schedule an appointment at the treatment agency. Participants who did not complete their
scheduled follow-up assessments were contacted via telephone to document their substance
use. All participants received $45 gift cards for completion of baseline and post-intervention
assessments, and a $50 gift card for completion of 2- and 4-month assessments. All
participants, regardless of assignment, were encouraged to continue attending any additional
community 12-step or other self-help meetings as recommended by the treatment agency.
$watermark-text
MBRP was delivered by two therapists to groups of 6–10 participants. Sessions were
conducted in the group therapy room at the treatment agency. Closed cohorts met weekly for
eight two-hour sessions. Sessions included guided meditations, experiential exercises, and
discussion. Participants were assigned daily exercises to practice between sessions, and were
given CDs for daily meditation practice. Relapse prevention practices (Daley & Marlatt,
1992) were integrated into the mindfulness-based skills. MBRP therapists held master's
degrees in psychology or social work, and all had a background in cognitive-behavioral
interventions and mindfulness meditation practice. All sessions were audio recorded, and
treatment fidelity was assessed by a team of coders who were trained to identify key
content- and style-related components of MBRP, using The Mindfulness-Based Relapse
Prevention Adherence and Competence Scale (Chawla et al., 2010).
Participants in the TAU condition continued in their standard, rolling admission outpatient
$watermark-text
aftercare, which included work in the 12-step model, process-oriented groups, and
psychoeducation. Relapse prevention skills, based on the disease model of addiction
(Gorski, 1990), were included in some of the groups. Therapists facilitating the TAU groups
were licensed Chemical Dependency Counselors, with diverse clinical training and
experience.
2.3. Measures
All measures were self-report, and were administered via a web-based assessment program
with staff available to assist participants in using the assessment interface. Research has
found no significant differences between paper-and-pencil and web administration of
commonly utilized measures (Miller et al., 2002). The measures used in the current analyses
are described below and interested readers are referred to prior publications from this study
$watermark-text
2.3.1. Alcohol and Drug Craving—The Penn Alcohol Craving Scale (PACS; Flannery,
Volpicelli, & Pettinati, 1999), a five-item self-report measure, was adapted to include
craving for both alcohol and other drugs. The PACS measures frequency, intensity, and
duration of craving, as well as an overall rating of craving for the previous week. The PACS
has shown excellent internal consistency and predictive validity for alcohol relapse. The
internal consistency of the PACS in the current sample was 0.87.
Sample items from the acting with awareness subscale include: “I find it difficult to stay
focused on what's happening in the present” and, “It seems I am “running on automatic”
without much awareness of what I'm doing.” Sample items from the non-judgment subscale
include: “I criticize myself for having irrational or inappropriate emotions” and, “When I
have distressing thoughts or images, I judge myself as good or bad, depending on what the
thought/image is about.” Items are rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (always true to never
true) with higher scores indicating greater awareness and non-judgment.
$watermark-text
estimated using Mplus version 6.11(Muthén & Muthén, 2010). First, unconditional models
of self-reported craving were estimated separately without covariates using a systematic
process of model testing whereby an intercept-only model was compared to increasingly
complex functional forms (e.g., linear slope, linear + quadratic slope, nonlinear slope).
Model fit of all models was evaluated by χ2 values, the Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA) (Browne & Cudeck, 1993), and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI)
(Bentler, 1990). Models with non-significant χ2, RMSEA less than 0.06, and CFI greater
than 0.95 were considered a good fit to the observed data (Hu & Bentler, 1999).
After establishing the unconditional model of craving, we examined the association between
treatment group and craving changes over time by including treatment condition (dummy
coded as “treatment as usual = 0” and “MBRP = 1”) as a covariate predictor of the craving
growth factors. Race and treatment hours (total hours of treatment received including MBRP
$watermark-text
groups as recorded in agency records), which have been shown to influence outcomes in the
current sample (Bowen et al., 2009; Witkiewitz & Bowen, 2010), were also included as
covariate predictors of the craving growth factors in all analyses.
estimated in Mplus using 1000 bootstrap draws to obtain confidence intervals for the
indirect effect.
All models were estimated using full information maximum likelihood, which provides
estimates of the variance-covariance matrix for all available data, including those
individuals who have incomplete data on some measures. Maximum likelihood estimation is
considered to be superior to other methods of handling missing data (e.g., listwise deletion),
when the reason for the missing data is completely random or if the variables that explain
the missing data are included in the model (i.e., the data are “missing at random;” Schafer &
Graham, 2002). Analyses indicated that individuals with missing craving assessments at 2-
and 4-months following treatment had significantly higher craving at posttreatment (t =
$watermark-text
−3.22, p = 0.002), thus one of the reasons for missing data was known and we assumed that
data were missing at random with posttreatment craving included in the model.
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analyses
Descriptive statistics for craving scores and covariates for the total sample and by treatment
groups are included in Table 1. First, it is important to note that the MBRP group had
significantly greater treatment exposure (as measured by total number of treatment hours),
on average. Craving scores had a possible range of 0 (no thoughts of craving) to 6 (constant
thoughts of craving). Individuals who were assigned to MBRP had lower craving scores
during and following treatment, with significantly lower craving scores during treatment
(midtreatment; t (125) = 2.43, p = 0.02) and immediately following treatment
$watermark-text
PACS were low at baseline (range = 0.00 to 5.60) and decreased over time before leveling
out at the 2- to 4-month follow-ups. The average variances of all growth factors were
significant, indicating significant individual variation around the mean growth trajectory of
self-reported craving.
The latent growth model of craving with growth factors regressed on treatment, race, and
treatment hours also provided a reasonable fit to the data (χ2 (13) = 19.95, p = 0.10; CFI =
0.98; RMSEA = 0.06 (90% CI: 0.00 – 0.10). The intercept of craving was significantly
associated with race (β = 0.43; B (SE) = 0.94 (0.18), p < 0.005), such that non-Hispanic
white participants had higher initial craving. Treatment condition was significantly
associated with both the linear slope (β = −0.38; B (SE) = −0.42 (0.16), p = 0.01) and the
quadratic slope (β = 0.39; B (SE) = 0.06 (0.03), p = 0.01) of craving scores. The significant
difference between treatment conditions in the changes in craving over time was further
examined using a multiple groups design whereby treatment condition was used as a
grouping variable with the means and the variances of the growth factors freed to vary
across treatment conditions. As seen in Figure 1, individuals assigned to MBRP had a
greater decrease in craving scores during treatment, and their craving scores leveled out
following treatment, whereas individuals in TAU reported a slight increase in craving scores
during treatment.
variances, and time-varying item intercepts and factor means provided the best fit to the data
(χ2 (8) = 4.69, p = 0.79; CFI = 1.00; RMSEA = 0.00 (90% CI: 0.00 – 0.06). Standardized
factor loadings were greater than 0.7 for all indicators (AAQ: β = 0.72; ACT: β = 0.78;
NONJ: β = 0.77) and the post-treatment latent factor was significantly associated with the
baseline latent factor (B (SE) = 0.64 (0.11), p < 0.001). For sake of brevity we will refer to
this latent factor as “mindfulness” throughout the remainder of this paper, however it is
important to note we do not imply that we have identified a mindfulness construct that is
distinct from previous conceptualizations of mindfulness (Baer et al., 2006). “Mindfulness”
is merely a descriptive label for the latent factor.
end of treatment so that we could examine the predictors of the level of craving immediately
following treatment, as well as the change in craving over time (i.e., slope). Treatment
hours, treatment condition, race and the mindfulness latent factor were included as
predictors of the craving growth factors.
The latent growth model with main effects of the covariate predictors and the interaction
between the covariates and treatment condition provided an adequate fit to the observed data
(χ2 (64) = 84.35, p = 0.05; CFI = 0.97; RMSEA = 0.044 (90% CI: 0.00 – 0.07). As seen in
Table 2, there was a significant main effect of treatment group, the mindfulness latent factor,
and race in predicting the craving intercept. Random assignment to MBRP, higher scores on
the latent factor (indicating greater acceptance, awareness, and nonjudgment), and being a
minority were significantly associated with lower levels of craving. There were also
significant main effects of treatment hours and race in predicting linear slope, such that more
$watermark-text
treatment hours and being non-Hispanic white were associated with a greater linear decrease
in craving over time.
Follow-up mediation analyses were conducted with each indicator of the mindfulness latent
factor incorporated into the model as an observed mediator of the association between
treatment group and the craving growth factors. These mediation analyses, which were
conducted separately for each indicator of the mindfulness latent factor (AAQ, ACT,
NONJ), provided a test of whether the mediating effect of acceptance, awareness, and
nonjudgment in the association between treatment and the craving growth factors was
specific to one of the three indicators of the latent factor. Results indicated no significant
mediating effects of any of the individual observed indicators (AAQ indirect: B (SE) =
−0.18 (0.10), p = 0.07; ACT indirect: B (SE) = −0.10 (0.06), p = 0.10; NONJ indirect: B
(SE) = −0.04 (0.05), p = 0.39).
$watermark-text
4. Discussion
In line with our previously reported results (Bowen et al., 2009), the current analyses
support that participating in MBRP was associated with significant reductions in self-
reported craving during and following treatment. Race and treatment hours were
significantly associated with the level of craving following treatment and changes in craving
over time, such that being non-Hispanic white was associated with higher levels of craving
and a greater decrease in craving over time. Treatment hours were also associated with a
greater decrease in craving.
Extending our prior research, the results from the mediation analyses supported the
hypothesis that a latent factor representing interdependent processes of acceptance,
awareness and nonjudgment would significantly mediate the relation between receiving
$watermark-text
MBRP and self-reported levels of craving immediately following the intervention. Thus,
higher levels of this factor could be a potential mechanism by which MBRP may influence
craving. Importantly, acceptance, awareness, or nonjudgment did not independently mediate
the association between MBRP and the level of craving following treatment, suggesting that
the combination of the processes is necessary to predict changes in craving.
Importantly, there was not a significant effect of MBRP or the mindfulness latent factor on
the linear or quadratic rate of change in craving over time; subsequently the mindfulness
latent factor was not a significant mediator of the association between MBRP and craving
changes. As seen in Figure 1, the greatest changes in craving for the MBRP group emerged
between the baseline and midtreatment assessment, after which the rate of change in craving
was similar across the MBRP and TAU groups. The first assessment of acceptance,
awareness, and nonjudgment after treatment initiation also occurred at midtreatment. We did
$watermark-text
find a significant association between the mindfulness latent factor and observed craving
scores at midtreatment (β = −0.37), after controlling for baseline levels, however we did not
have temporal precedence to test whether initial changes in the mindfulness latent factor
mediated the initial changes in craving observed between baseline and the midtreatment
assessment.
The finding that the mindfulness latent factor, but not the independent subscales of
acceptance, awareness, or nonjudgment mediated the association between MBRP and self-
reported craving is intriguing. On one hand, this might reflect enhanced reliability and
reduced measurement error in latent variable models. Although, as noted above in Section
1.3, this finding is also consistent with the goals of MBRP to specifically target both the
experience of and response to internal stimuli by increasing acceptance and awareness,
while maintaining a nonjudgmental attitude. Heightened acceptance, without true awareness,
could be referred to as “naïve acceptance” and could result in an individual being blindsided
by triggers that were not in conscious awareness (Andrade, May, & Kavanagh, 2009;
Ingjaldsson, Thayer, & Laberg, 2003). Improved awareness without acceptance might lead
to attempts to avoid or suppress the experience of craving, which may actually increase the
experience of craving (Berry et al., 2010). Finally, greater acceptance and awareness with a
judgmental attitude could also be counterproductive in that an individual may be highly
aware and accepting of his or her craving, while also judging him or herself and
experiencing shame (Luoma, Kohlenberg, Hayes, & Fletcher, 2011). Small sample sizes in
the current study prevented us from directly examining these speculations. For example,
none of the MBRP participants and only two of the TAU participants were more than one
standard deviation above the mean on awareness and more than one standard deviation
below the mean on acceptance.
4.1. Limitations
$watermark-text
The current study had numerous limitations and future work is needed to replicate and
extend the analyses conducted in the current study. The most significant limitation was the
subjective, self-reported measurement of craving, acceptance, awareness, and nonjudgment.
Numerous authors have questioned self-reported craving measures (Drummond, Litten,
Lowman, & Hunt, 2000) and, in general, there is not an agreed upon operational definition
of craving (Skinner & Aubin, 2010). Similar measurement issues have been raised in the
study of mindfulness and mindfulness-based treatments (Diclemente, 2010; Grossman,
2011). Furthermore, the measures of acceptance, awareness, and nonjudgment used in the
current study were not designed to assess the types of responses and experiences targeted in
MBRP, and a more specifically tailored assessment measure (e.g., a behavioral task) might
more accurately reflect the true underlying mechanisms.
$watermark-text
Other major design limitations included the brevity of the follow-up window, the lack of a
no-treatment or waitlist control group, and the amount of missing data. Finally, as noted in
prior analyses of these data (Bowen et al., 2009; Witkiewitz & Bowen, 2010), there were
also noteworthy differences between the treatment as usual and MBRP groups, with respect
to therapist training, group composition (closed cohort vs. rolling admission), and group
content (primarily educational versus highly interactive), that could also explain the current
findings.
Future research should attempt to replicate and extend the current findings by including a
longer follow-up following treatment, and adding additional and possibly multimodal
assessments of acceptance, awareness, nonjudgment and craving during treatment. Ideally,
future studies would include implicit, physiological and/or neurobiological measures of
craving, as well as behavioral, objective measures of acceptance, awareness, and
nonjudgment. Having individuals complete a behavioral task in which they practice
acceptance and awareness without judgment during a cue reactivity task could provide
further information about whether MBRP is truly changing the experiences of and responses
to substance use triggers (Westbrook et al., 2011). Furthermore, it would be useful to
combine all of the constructs identified in the current study into a single self-report
questionnaire that could measure acceptance, awareness and nonjudgment of craving
Acknowledgments
Role of Funding Sources: Funding for this study was provided by NIDA Grant R21-DA019562. NIDA had no role
in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to
submit the paper for publication.
Acknowledgements: The authors are indebted to Dr. G. Alan Marlatt who was the principal investigator of the
$watermark-text
grant that funded this study and who provided the impetus for the development of mindfulness-based relapse
prevention.
References
Addolorato G, Abenavoli L, Leggio L, Gasbarrini G. How many cravings? Pharmacological aspects of
craving treatment in alcohol addiction: a review. Neuropsychobiology. 2005; 51:59–66. doi:
10.1159/000084161. [PubMed: 15741745]
Addolorato G, Leggio L, Abenavoli L, Gasbarrini G. Neurobiochemical and clinical aspects of craving
in alcohol addiction: a review. Addict Behav. 2005; 30:1209–1224. doi:10.1016/j.physletb.
2003.10.071. [PubMed: 15925129]
Andrade J, May J, Kavanagh D. Conscious and unconscious processes in human desire. Psyche: An
Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Consciousness. 2009; 15:83–91. doi: 10.1016/
$watermark-text
s0166-2236(03)00233-9.
Anton RF. What is craving? Models and implications for treatment. Alcohol Res Health. 1999;
23:165–173. [PubMed: 10890811]
Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using Self-Report Assessment Methods to
Explore Facets of Mindfulness. Assessment. 2006; 13:27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191104268029.
[PubMed: 16443717]
Baker T, Morse E, Sherman J. The motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analysis of urges.
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 1986; 34:257–323. [PubMed: 3627296]
Baker TB, Piper ME, Fiore MC, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR. Addiction motivation reformulated: An
affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychological Review. 2004; 111:33–51. doi:
10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33. [PubMed: 14756584]
Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990; 107:238–246. doi:
10.1037//0033-2909.107.2.238. [PubMed: 2320703]
$watermark-text
Berry LM, May J, Andrade J, Kavanagh D. Emotional and behavioral reaction to intrusive thoughts.
Assessment. 2010; 17:126–137. doi: 10.1177/1073191109344694. [PubMed: 19797324]
Bodhi, B. In the Buddha's Words: An Anthology of Discourses from the Pali Canon. Somerville, MA:
2005.
Bowen; Chawla, N.; Marlatt, GA. Mindfulness-based relapse prevention for addictive behaviors: A
clinician's guide. Guilford Press; New York, NY, US: 2010.
Bowen S, Chawla N, Collins SE, Witkiewitz K, Hsu S, Grow J, Marlatt A. Mindfulness-based relapse
prevention for substance use disorders: a pilot efficacy trial. Subst Abus. 2009; 30:295–305. doi:
10.1080/08897070903250084. [PubMed: 19904665]
Breese GR, Sinha R, Heilig M. Chronic alcohol neuroadaptation and stress contribute to susceptibility
for alcohol craving and relapse. Pharmacol Ther. 2011; 129:149–171. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.
2010.09.007. [PubMed: 20951730]
Breslin FC, Zack M, McMain S. An information-processing analysis of mindfulness: Implications for
relapse prevention in the treatment of substance abuse. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice.
2002; 9:275–299. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.96.4.305.
Browne, MW.; Cudeck, R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen, KA.; Long, JS., editors.
Testing structural equation models. Sage; Beverly Hills, CA: 1993. p. 136-162.
Brownell KD, Marlatt GA, Lichtenstein E, Wilson GT. Understanding and preventing relapse. Am
Psychol. 1986; 41:765–782. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.41.7.765. [PubMed: 3527003]
Chawla N, Collin S, Bowen S, Hsu S, Grow J, Douglass A, Marlatt GA. The mindfulness-based
relapse prevention adherence and competence scale: development, interrater reliability, and
validity. Psychotherapy Resesarch. 2010; 20:388–397. doi:10.1080/10503300903544257.
Cox WM, Klinger E. Motivational structure. Relationships with substance use and processes of
change. Addict Behav. 2002; 27(6):925–940. [PubMed: 12369476]
Daley, DC.; Marlatt, GA. Relapse prevention: Cognitive and behavioral interventions. In: Lowinson,
JH.; Ruiz, P.; Millman, RB.; Langrod, JG., editors. Substance abuse: A comprehensive textbook.
2nd ed.. Williams & Wilkins; Baltimore, MD: 1992. p. 533-542.
Diclemente CC. Mindfulness specific or generic mechanisms of action. Addiction. 2010; 105:1707–
$watermark-text
Gorski TT. The Cenaps model of relapse prevention: basic principles and procedures. J Psychoactive
Drugs. 1990; 22:125–133. [PubMed: 2197389]
Grossman P. Defining mindfulness by how poorly I think I pay attention during everyday awareness
and other intractable problems for psychology's (re)invention of mindfulness: Comment on Brown
et al. Psychological Assessment. 2011; 23:1034–1040. 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0022713. [PubMed:
22122674]
Hayes SC, Strosahl K, Wilson KG, Bissett RT, Pistorello J, Toarmino D, McCurry SM. Measuring
experiential avoidance: A preliminary test of a working model. The Psychological Record. 2004;
54:553–578. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.56.6.893.
Houben K, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Getting a grip on drinking behavior: Training working memory to
reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol Sci. 2011; 22:968–975. doi: 10.1177/0956797611412392.
[PubMed: 21685380]
Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria
$watermark-text
Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008; 59:29–
53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548. [PubMed: 18154498]
Luoma JB, Kohlenberg BS, Hayes SC, Fletcher L. Slow and steady wins the race: A randomized
clinical trial of acceptance and commitment therapy targeting shame in substance use disorders. J
Consult Clin Psychol (0022-006X). 2011 doi: 10.1037/a0026070.
Mackinnon DP, Fairchild AJ. Current directions in mediation analysis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2009;
18:16–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01598.x. [PubMed: 20157637]
MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V. A comparison of methods to test
mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychol Methods. 2002; 7:83–104. doi:
10.1037//1082-989X.7.1.83. [PubMed: 11928892]
Maisto SA, Pollock NK, Cornelius JR, Lynch KG, Martin CS. Alcohol relapse as a function of relapse
$watermark-text
Sinha R, O'Malley SS. Craving for alcohol: findings from the clinic and the laboratory. Alcohol
Alcohol. 1999; 34:223–230. doi:10.1093/alcalc/34.2.223. [PubMed: 10344782]
Skinner MD, Aubin HJ. Craving's place in addiction theory: contributions of the major models.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010; 34:606–623. [PubMed: 19961872]
Tiffany ST. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and
nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev. 1990; 97:147–168. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.108.3.356.
[PubMed: 2186423]
Tiffany ST. Cognitive concepts of craving. Alcohol Res Health. 1999; 23(3):215–224. [PubMed:
10890817]
Tiffany ST, Carter BL, Singleton EG. Challenges in the manipulation, assessment and interpretation of
craving relevant variables. Addiction. 2000; 95(Suppl 2):S177–187. doi:10.1046/j.
$watermark-text
Highlights
factor.
• The mindfulness latent factor mediated the association between MBRP and
craving.
$watermark-text
$watermark-text
Figure 1.
Changes in self-reported craving scores over time by treatment group. MBRP =
mindfulness-based relapse prevention.
$watermark-text
$watermark-text
Figure 2.
Model of the “mindfulness” latent factor mediating the association between treatment and
the craving growth factors with thicker black lines marking the significant mediated effect.
As described in section 3.4 the craving intercept was centered at posttreatment by setting the
$watermark-text
Table 1
Descriptive Statistics (Means (Standard Deviations) for Total Sample and By Treatment Groups
Treatment hours 167 11.44 (6.71) 74 9.75 (8.17) 93 12.79 (4.91) .45*
Craving baseline 166 1.63 (1.26) 75 1.73 (1.42) 91 1.55 (1.13) .14
Craving midtreatment 127 1.38 (1.06) 47 1.67 (1.26) 80 1.21 (0.89) .45*
Craving posttreatment 103 1.35 (1.24) 41 1.70 (1.42) 62 1.12 (1.05) .47*
Craving 2-months post 95 1.17 (1.24) 42 1.42 (1.49) 53 0.98 (0.98) .36
Craving 4-months post 122 1.17 (1.37) 52 1.28 (1.50) 70 1.09 (1.26) .14
Acceptance baseline 148 47.14 (8.52) 72 47.18 (9.55) 76 47.09 (7.47) .01
Acceptance posttreatment 96 49.72 (8.91) 40 47.60 (10.03) 56 51.23 (7.76) .41*
Nonjudgment baseline 148 26.27 (6.47) 68 26.99 (6.68) 80 25.66 (6.27) .24
Nonjudgment posttreatment 97 27.18 (6.74) 38 25.34 (6.12) 59 28.36 (6.90) .45*
Acting with awareness baseline 156 26.87 (6.50) 72 27.69 (6.85) 84 26.15 (6.23) .24
Acting with awareness posttreatment 95 26.84 (7.04) 40 26.50 (7.21) 55 27.09 (6.97) .09
Note. MBRP = mindfulness based relapse prevention d = Cohen's d measure of effect size calculated at each time point using the formula:
*
p < 0.05, differences between treatment groups based on independent samples t-test.
Table 2
Covariate Predictors of the Change in Craving Over Time (from Baseline to 4-Months Post-Treatment)
Main Effects
Treatment hours −0.009 (0.01) −0.06 −0.02 (0.01)* −0.26 0.004 (0.01) 0.09
Race (1 = Non-Hispanic white) 0.41 (0.18)* 0.21 −0.27 (0.12)* −0.24 0.14 (.08) 0.23
Treatment group (1 = MBRP) −0.57 (0.17)** −0.30 −0.06 (0.12) −0.05 0.11 (.08) 0.18
Acceptance & awareness latent factor −0.06 (0.02)** −0.41 0.01 (0.01) 0.09 0.003 (0.01) 0.06
Mediation Model
Treatment hours −0.01 (0.01) −0.08 −0.02 (0.01)* −0.26 0.004 (0.01) 0.09
Race (1 = Non-Hispanic white) 0.38 (0.18)* 0.20 −0.27 (0.12)* −0.24 0.14 (.07)* 0.23
Treatment group (1 = MBRP) −0.46 (0.20)* −0.24 −0.08 (0.13) −0.07 0.11 (.09) 0.18
Acceptance & awareness latent factor −0.09 (0.03)** −0.43 0.01 (0.02) 0.08 0.004 (0.01) 0.06
Indirect effect −0.19 (0.10)* −0.20 0.02 (0.05) 0.04 0.009 (0.02) 0.03
ote.
*
p < 0.05
**
p < 0.01, B (SE) = unstandardized regression coefficient (standard error), β = standardized regression coefficient.