Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Journal of Adolescence: Catherine Rothon, Jenny Head, Emily Klineberg, Stephen Stansfeld

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Adolescence
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jado

Can social support protect bullied adolescents from adverse outcomes?


A prospective study on the effects of bullying on the educational
achievement and mental health of adolescents at secondary schools
in East London
Catherine Rothon a, *, Jenny Head b, Emily Klineberg c, Stephen Stansfeld a
a
Centre for Psychiatry, Wolfson Institute of Preventive Medicine, Old Anatomy Building, Barts and the London School of Medicine and Dentistry, Queen Mary
University of London, Charterhouse Square, London EC1M 6BQ, UK
b
Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, University College London, Gower Street Campus, 1-19 Torrington Place, London WC1E 6BT, UK
c
Department of Social Medicine, University of Bristol, Canynge Hall, 39 Whatley Road, Bristol BS8 2PS, UK

a b s t r a c t

Keywords: This paper investigates the extent to which social support can have a buffering effect
Achievement against the potentially adverse consequences of bullying on school achievement and
Adolescents mental health. It uses a representative multiethnic sample of adolescents attending East
Bullying
London secondary schools in three boroughs. Bullied adolescents were less likely to ach-
Depression
ieve the appropriate academic achievement benchmark for their age group and bullied
Mental health
School boys (but not girls) were more likely to exhibit depressive symptoms compared to those
Social support not bullied. High levels of social support from family were important in promoting good
mental health. There was evidence that high levels of support from friends and moderate
(but not high) family support was able to protect bullied adolescents from poor academic
achievement. Support from friends and family was not sufficient to protect adolescents
against mental health difficulties that they might face as a result of being bullied. More
active intervention from schools is recommended.
Ó 2010 The Foundation for Professionals in Services for Adolescents. Published by Elsevier
Ltd. Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license.

Introduction

Bullying is broadly defined as a desire to hurt and the execution of a harmful action; it is characterised by repetition and
either a physical or a psychological power imbalance (Farrington, 1993; Rigby, 2002; Smith & Brain, 2000; Smith & Sharp,
1994). It may come in the form of verbal abuse, physical aggression or relational victimisation. The first two forms of
bullying have sometimes been called “direct bullying” as they include directly aggressive behaviour. Relational victimisation
is the manipulation of peer relationships in order to exclude someone (Wolke, Woods, Bloomfield, & Karstadt, 2000).
Numbers bullied vary by location and definition. Around 30% of children are bullied at some point whilst at school and
between 5% and 10% are regularly bullied (Newman, Holden, & Delville, 2005).
Research has shown that boys are more likely to be involved in bullying, either as bullies or as victims, than girls (Forero,
McLellan, Rissel, & Bauman, 1999; Kaltiala-Heino, Rimpela, Rantanen, & Rimpela, 2000; Nansel et al., 2001; Rigby, 2000; Seals

* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ44 20 7882 2030; fax: þ44 20 7882 5728.
E-mail addresses: c.rothon@qmul.ac.uk (C. Rothon), j.head@ucl.ac.uk (J. Head), e.klineberg@qmul.ac.uk (E. Klineberg), s.a.stansfeld@qmul.ac.uk
(S. Stansfeld).

0140-1971/Ó 2010 The Foundation for Professionals in Services for Adolescents. Published by Elsevier Ltd. Open access under CC BY-NC-ND license.
doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.02.007
580 C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588

& Young, 2003). Rigby (2000) found that boys and girls were similarly likely to be bullied through name calling, teasing and
deliberate exclusion. Boys are more likely to receive threats of harm and be physically bullied (Baldry, 2004; Owens, Shute, &
Slee, 2000; Rigby, 2000; Roland & Idsoe, 2000). There is evidence that prevalence of bullying declines with age (Newman
et al., 2005; Smith, Madsen, & Moody, 1999).
Cross-sectional research on the effects of bullying on psychological health has consistently found that those that are
bullied exhibit poorer emotional adjustment in early to late adolescence, indicating an association but no direct causality
(Alikasifoglu, Erginoz, Ercan, Uysal, & Albayrak-Kaymak, 2007; Baldry, 2004; Brunstein Klomak, Marrocco, Kleinman,
Schonfeld, & Gould, 2007; Espelage & Holt, 2001; Fekkes, Pijpers, & Verloove-Vanhorick, 2004; Forero et al., 1999; Greco,
Freeman, & Dufton, 2007; Kaltiala-Heino, Rimpela, Marttunen, Rimpela, & Rantanen, 1999; Kaltiala-Heino et al., 2000;
Kumpulainen, Rasanen, & Puura, 2001; Saluja et al., 2004; Seals & Young, 2003; Smith, Talamelli, Cowie, Naylor, &
Chauhan, 2004; van der Wal, de Wit, & Hirasing, 2003). Few studies have prospectively examined the relationship
between bullying and the psychological health of the victim. One study found that bullied Australian secondary school
students in year 8 (age 13) had a higher risk of depression and anxiety over the following school year (Bond, Carlin, Thomas,
Rubin, & Patton, 2001). The association between bullying and poor psychological health may work in both directions; Fekkes
et al. found that victims of bullying had significantly higher chances of psychosomatic and psychosocial problems compared
with children who were not bullied; conversely, children with depressive and anxiety symptoms had a higher chance of being
bullied (Fekkes, Pijpers, Fredriks, Vogels, & Verloove-Vanhorick, 2006).
A limited amount of research has been carried out on the relationship between bullying and educational achievement in
adolescence, although there have been some studies on the potential effects of bullying (Nansel et al., 2001). A few studies
have been carried out using younger pupils. One US study found a link between bullying at elementary school and low
achievement (Glew, Fan, Katon, Rivara, & Kernic, 2005). A longitudinal study in UK primary schools found that being a victim
of relational bullying was the second most important predictor of academic achievement for year 2 (age 6–7 years); no effect
was found for direct bullying (Woods & Wolke, 2004). The study was limited by the short time lag between the measure for
bullying and that for academic achievement. Other studies have not looked specifically at bullying but have found links
between “peer rejection” and academic achievement (Ladd, 1990; Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). A prospective study found that,
amongst the youngest cohort, those that were most rejected by their peers performed worse on tests. However, they found no
direct relationship between peer rejection and later academic achievement (DeRosier, Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 1994).
Responses to peer rejection have also been found to be important; aggressive-rejected but not submissive-rejected adoles-
cents had low academic achievement (Wentzel & Asher, 1995).
Although a large number of studies have looked at bullying prevention strategies, few studies have examined processes
that may already be operating to mitigate the negative effects of bullying (Baldry & Farrington, 2005). One such process is
social support. Support from family, particularly parents, is one important form of social support. However, it has been argued
that in adolescence, individuals spend more unsupervised time with their peers and friends are increasingly relied upon for
support (Marini, Dane, Bosacki, & Ylc-Cura, 2006). Adjustment to secondary school may therefore have important links with
the ability of young people to initiate and maintain positive peer relationships (Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). This may impact on
eventual academic success.
There are a number of mechanisms whereby high levels of social support might translate into more positive outcomes for
students who are bullied. House (1981) identified 4 main types of social support: emotional, instrumental, informational and
appraisal support. Later work by House et al. places particular emphasis on “the emotionally or instrumentally sustaining
quality of social relationships” in relation to social support (House, Landis, & Umberson,1988). Both emotional and instrumental
support may be used by victims of bullying. Emotional support may be particularly salient when a young person is experiencing
bullying. Newman et al. (2005) conceptualise bullying as a chronic stressor and social support as one of the coping resources on
which bullied students can draw. Instrumental support may also be important; as well as operating to enable individuals to
cope with bullying whilst it is occurring, high levels of social support may stop bullying at an earlier stage. Bullying is a group
phenomenon; bystanders can have an important impact on the trajectory that the bullying takes by either assisting the bully,
not getting involved or by intervening. In most cases bystanders will remain uninvolved which enables the bully to continue
(Fekkes, Pijpers, & Verloove-Vanhorick, 2005). It has been shown, however, that if bystanders do try to stop the bullying, it
usually ceases (Hawkins, Pepler, & Craig, 2001). There is also a possibility of reverse causation; students with low levels of social
support may be more likely to be bullied; their isolation may draw bullies to them (Newman et al., 2005).
The pathways whereby social support may bolster academic achievement are not well understood. Again, support might
be instrumental or emotional. One way in which instrumental support might work is through students with more friends
receiving more academically-related assistance. In this way social support might have a direct impact on achievement
(Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). Alternatively, the impact of social support may be indirect; students with high levels of emotional
social support may be less likely to suffer from psychological distress which in turn predicts higher academic achievement
(Wentzel, 1994; Wentzel, Weinberger, Ford, & Feldman, 1990). Vygotsky’s augmentation of Cognitive Development Theory,
which gave prominence to social interaction in influencing language and thought development, also places emphasis on
interaction with peers as an important factor in facilitating intellectual progress (Vygotsky, 1978).
Studies examining social support as a moderator have had mixed findings. Rigby (2000) found that social support and
bullying contributed independently to poor mental health but that perceived availability of social support did not impact on
victimised adolescents more than pupils that were not victimised. Another study found evidence for a buffering effect of
social support against anxiety and depressive symptoms for victims of bullying (Davidson & Demaray, 2007). Holt and
C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588 581

Espelage (2007) found the lowest levels of anxiety and depression amongst bullies, victims and bully-victims with moderate
peer social support (Holt & Espelage, 2007). Newman et al. (2005) looked at “isolation” as a moderator in reactions to bullying
and found that being bullied does the most damage (measured by stress on reaching college) to those with high levels of
perceived isolation.
Because bullying has a negative influence on children’s health and educational outcomes it is important that healthcare
workers and teachers have a good understanding of bullying and its potential consequences. This paper studies the association
between bullying and both educational achievement and the exhibition of depressive symptoms in British secondary schools.
Based on the literature, the key hypotheses are as follows:

1. Victims of bullying will have a greater propensity to exhibit depressive symptoms


2. Victims of bullying will be less likely to reach national achievement benchmarks
3. Social support from friends will be more effective as a protective factor than social support from the family
4. Bullied students with low levels of perceived social support will exhibit poorer outcomes than bullied students with higher
levels of perceived social support.

Methods

Participants

The data come from the Research with East London Adolescents: Community Health Survey (RELACHS), a school based
epidemiological study of a representative sample of 2790 adolescents from year 7 (11–12 years) and year 9 (13–14 years)
attending 28 comprehensive schools in Hackney, Newham, and Tower Hamlets in 2001 (Stansfeld et al., 2003). Of those pupils
that did not participate, the majority were not available during due to school absence, illness or other school activities. The
second phase of data collection took place in 2003. 2093 pupils in the original sample completed the questionnaire.

Measures

Bullying
Bullying was measured at baseline with a self-report question. The item used to measure whether an adolescent had been
subjected to bullying was as follows: “how often have you been bullied in school this term?”. Possible responses were:
“I haven’t been bullied in school this term”, “once or twice”, “sometimes”, “about once a week”, “several times a week”. A
further category of “never bullied” was added based on another item: “have you ever been bullied at school”. The variable was
then recoded into a binary variable whereby being a victim constituted experiencing bullying “sometimes”, “about once
a week” or “several times a week”. Being bullied once or twice in a term was combined with the category for never being
bullied because bullying is defined as a repeated action (Baldry & Farrington, 2004; Olweus, 1993; Smith & Brain, 2000).

Educational achievement
Educational achievement was measured two years after the baseline survey. For the younger age group, the benchmark
used for educational achievement at age 13–14 was the attainment of level 5 or above in English, mathematics and science in
the Key Stage 3 examinations. These national tests are intended to indicate if a student is working at the target level for their
age. For the older group, the benchmark was the attainment of 5 or more General Certificate of Secondary Education
Examinations (GCSEs) at grades A*–C (taken at age 15–16). These benchmarks are used by the Department of Education and
Skills as an indicator of adequate performance (Department for Education and Skills, 2006). Data on educational achievement
at Key Stage 3 and GCSE was obtained from Local Education Authorities.

Depressive symptoms
Depressive symptoms were measured using the Short Moods and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) two years after the
baseline survey (Angold et al., 1987). Statements about the emotions and behaviour of the respondent over the past 2 weeks
were rated. Examples of items include: “I felt miserable or unhappy”, “I didn’t enjoy anything at all”, “I cried a lot”. There are
13 items in this scale: “true”, “sometimes true”, or “not true”. The scores for these items were summed to produce an overall
magnitude of symptoms, with a score of 8 or above indicating the presence of depressive symptoms. In the original validation
against the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children – Depressive Scale this threshold yielded a positive predictive value to
80 per cent and a negative predictive value of 68 per cent (Angold et al., 1995). The Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient for
this sample is 0.90.

Social support
The degree of social support derived from family and friends was measured at baseline using the Multidimensional Scale
of Perceived Social Support (MSPSS) (Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & Farley, 1988). This is a 12-item scale which produces scores that
measure levels of social support from three sources: family, friends and a significant other. Items included: “my family really
582 C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588

tries to help me”, “I can count on my friends when things go wrong”, “I can talk about my problems with my family”. This scale
has been found to have a high level of internal and test–retest reliability; the overall reliability coefficient is 0.88 (Zimet et al.,
1988). The scale has been found to have good concurrent, construct and discriminant validity (Zimet et al., 1988). The
Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient for this sample is 0.90. Social support scores were split into tertiles (high, moderate
and low support).

Ethnicity
Ethnicity was categorised as follows using a revised version of the 2001 census: white UK, Bangladeshi, Pakistani, Asian
Indian, black African, black Caribbean, other.

Free school meals


Pupils are eligible for free school meals if their parents receive Income Support, Income Based Jobseekers’ Allowance,
support under part VI of the Immigration and Asylum Act 1999 or (with some conditions) Child Tax Credit. Data on free school
meals was collected from school records.

Procedure

All 42 schools in the three London boroughs were stratified by borough and school type (comprehensive, voluntary, other).
Thirty schools were randomly selected. Head teachers were informed about the study and asked for permission for their school
to participate. Within the 28 schools that agreed to take part, four representative, mixed ability classes were selected (two from
year 7 and two from year 9). The overall response rate was 84% at baseline. Information sheets explaining the study were given
to teachers, parents and pupils a week before the visit to the school. Parents were given the opportunity to opt their child out of
the study and pupils gave fully informed consent on the day. A team of researchers administered the questionnaire in class-
rooms in one session of 40–50 min. One member of the team led the class, explaining the questionnaire and providing assurance
that all answers would be anonymous and confidential. Three or four additional researchers assisted, answering queries from
participants, ensuring that participants did not confer with each other and checking for missing data on completion of the
questionnaires. These methods were used at both data collection phases. Meetings were held with a community advisory
group, consisting of teachers, parents, health and social care professionals, to advise on ethical aspects of the work and on the
research process. The study protocol was approved by the East London and the City Local Research Ethics Committee.

Statistical analysis

Because the primary sampling unit for the study was the school, it was necessary to make adjustments for the clustered
survey design in the analyses. Failing to adjust for this would result in an overstatement of precision by ignoring the possible
lack of independence of observations within the same school. Adjustments were made for survey design using the svy suite of
commands in Stata. An equal number of classes were selected in each school regardless of school size, so that probability of
selection varied by school. Data were reweighted to ensure that the data were representative of all adolescents attending
comprehensive schools in the three boroughs at the time of the baseline survey.
Missing data was analysed using crosstabulation and the chi-squared option in Stata. Odds ratios and confidence intervals
were calculated using the Stata logistic regression command. These analyses were carried out for boys and girls combined.
Mantel–Haenszel analysis was carried out to identify possible confounding variables. For the achievement models, these were
gender, age group, ethnicity and eligibility for free school meals. For the SMFQ models, potential confounders were gender,
age group and ethnicity. Mantel–Haenszel analysis was carried out to identify possible interactions. Separate models were
run for achievement and SMFQ as the outcome variable. Independent variables were dropped from the model if they did not
result in a change in the odds ratio for bullying.
For the analysis examining the impact of bullying on depressive symptoms, there was the option of excluding those
depressed at baseline. This has disadvantages as a reduced number of adolescents would be included. However, if bullied
children with a specific problem at baseline were to be included, it is harder to study the causal relationship between bullying
and health outcomes. Two sets of analyses were therefore carried out firstly including those depressed at baseline (reported
here) and then excluding them. The results were in the same direction, although there was greater statistical evidence for the
associations when those depressed at baseline were included.

Results

Participants

48.6% of the participants were male; 27.0% were white, 25.1% Bangladeshi, 20.9% black, 9.1% Indian, 6.7% Pakistani and
11.2% of other ethnic origin. 47.6% were eligible for free school meals. The average age of the younger age group was 12.18
(standard deviation ¼ 0.33) and the older age group 14.22 (standard deviation ¼ 0.33); 49.5% of the sample was in the
younger age group.
C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588 583

2734 participants had complete data on bullying. Those who did not reach the academic benchmark were less likely to
have data on bullying (p ¼ 0.003). 2145 participants had complete data on achievement. Pupils who were bullied were less
likely to have data on achievement (p ¼ 0.007), as were white pupils (p < 0.0001) and pupils eligible for free school meals
(p ¼ 0.076). 1876 participants had complete data on depressive symptoms at time 2. Those that were bullied (p < 0.0001),
white pupils (p < 0.0001) and pupils eligible for free school meals (p ¼ 0.001) were less likely to have complete data on
depressive symptoms.

Bullying prevalence

9.1% of the sample reported being bullied “sometimes” or more often in the term in which the survey was taken. Table 1
shows the crude odds ratios for the association between bullying and the variables of interest. There was very strong evidence
that those in the older age group were less likely to have been bullied (OR ¼ 0.56, 95% CI 0.39, 0.80). There was some evidence
that Indian pupils were less likely to have been subject to bullying in the last term (OR ¼ 0.53, 95% CI 0.29, 0.96). There was
evidence that those with moderate (OR ¼ 0.57, 95% CI 0.46, 0.71) and high (OR ¼ 0.54, 95% CI 0.35, 0.83) levels of social
support from friends were about half as likely to have been bullied.

Bullying and academic achievement

45.6% of the sample reached the academic achievement benchmark. Table 2 shows the crude odds ratios for the associ-
ation between achievement and the main exposure variables. Those that had been bullied in the last term were approxi-
mately half as likely to achieve the benchmark (OR ¼ 0.46, 95% CI 0.29, 0.72). Those in the older age group were 1.5 times more
likely to reach the benchmark than those in the younger age group (OR ¼ 1.50, 95% CI 1.12, 2.03). Girls were more likely to
reach the benchmark than boys (OR ¼ 1.84, 95% CI 1.18, 2.86). Indian children were approximately twice as likely to reach the
achievement as white pupils (OR ¼ 1.97, 95% CI 1.37, 2.84). There was very strong evidence for lower performance by those
pupils eligible for free school meals (OR ¼ 0.55, 95% CI 0.43, 0.71). There was some evidence that high levels of social support
from family were associated with lower levels of achievement (OR ¼ 0.74, 95% CI 0.59, 0.93).
Table 3 shows the adjusted models with achievement as the outcome variable. Adjusting for age group, gender, ethnicity
and eligibility for free school meals reduced the crude odds ratio for the sample to 0.46 (95% CI 0.28, 0.76), a very modest
reduction.
There was little evidence for social support from friends as a confounder of the relationship between bullying and
achievement; the odds ratio fell to 0.45 when this was added to the model (95% CI 0.27, 0.75). Support from family also had
little effect (OR ¼ 0.46, 95% CI 0.28, 0.76). Mantel–Haenszel analysis provided some evidence for an interaction between
bullying and social support from friends (test for homogeneity of odds ratios p-value ¼ 0.013) and a very weak suggestion of
an interaction between bullying and social support from family (test for homogeneity of odds ratios p-value ¼ 0.130). Social

Table 1
Characteristics of sample and association between key variables and being bullied: univariate logistic regression analyses.

% N OR (95% CI) bullied this term p-value


Age group
Younger 49.5 1381 1
Older 50.5 1408 0.56 (0.39, 0.80) 0.003

Gender
Male 48.6 1355 1
Female 51.4 1434 0.88 (0.64, 1.22) 0.432

Ethnicity
White 27.0 742 1
Bangladeshi 25.1 690 0.69 (0.42, 1.14) 0.142
Black 20.9 575 0.84 (0.48, 1.45) 0.510
Indian 9.1 250 0.53 (0.29, 0.96) 0.036
Pakistani 6.7 184 1.12 (0.56, 2.24) 0.741
Other 11.2 308 1.36 (0.75, 2.45) 0.294

Eligible for FSM


No 52.4 1338 1
Yes 47.6 1217 0.75 (0.47, 1.19) 0.211

Family soc support


Low 30.0 805 1
Moderate 36.2 974 0.91 (0.56, 1.52) 0.735
High 33.8 909 1.13 (0.75, 1.69) 0.551

Friends soc support


Low 30.9 830 1
Moderate 33.9 912 0.57 (0.46, 0.71) <0.0001
High 35.2 946 0.54 (0.35, 0.83) 0.006
584 C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588

Table 2
Association between key variables and reaching achievement benchmark: univariate logistic regression
analyses.

OR (95% CI) reached achievement benchmark p-value


Bullied
No 1
Yes 0.46 (0.29, 0.72) 0.002

Age group
Younger 1
Older 1.50 (1.12, 2.03) 0.009

Gender
Male 1
Female 1.84 (1.18, 2.86) 0.009

Ethnicity
White 1
Bangladeshi 1.10 (0.72, 1.68) 0.635
Black 1.22 (0.79, 1.88) 0.360
Indian 1.97 (1.37, 2.84) 0.001
Pakistani 0.82 (0.49, 1.36) 0.423
Other 1.86 (1.10, 3.16) 0.022

Eligible for FSM


No 1
Yes 0.55 (0.43, 0.71) <0.0001

Family soc support


Low 1
Moderate 0.94 (0.74, 1.20) 0.624
High 0.74 (0.59, 0.93) 0.011

Friends soc support


Low 1
Moderate 1.15 (0.93, 1.41) 0.182
High 1.04 (0.80, 1.35) 0.753

support from friends was protective. After adjustment for a bullying  friends social support interaction, bullying was not
associated with educational achievement for students with high levels of support from friends. For students who did not have
high levels of support from friends, bullying was associated with lower odds of achieving the academic benchmark. Bullied
students with moderate (OR ¼ 0.27, 95% CI 0.11–0.65) or low (OR ¼ 0.38, 95% CI 0.19–0.76) levels of support had approxi-
mately a third of the odds of achieving the academic benchmark, compared to students who were not bullied.
Introducing the bullying  family social support interaction resulted in an interesting finding; for students with moderate
levels of support from family, bullying did not appear to be associated with lower odds of achieving the academic benchmark
(OR ¼ 1.00, 95% CI 0.49–2.06). However, there was evidence that bullied students with either high (OR ¼ 0.20, 95% CI 0.07–
0.51) or low (OR ¼ 0.40, 95% CI 0.21–0.76) levels of social support from family had lower odds of achieving the benchmark
compared to those students who were not bullied.

Bullying and depression

27.3% of the sample had depressive symptoms. Table 4 shows the crude odds ratios for the association between depressive
symptoms and the main exposure variables. Those bullied in the last term were approximately one and a half times more
likely to have depressive symptoms (OR ¼ 1.42, 95% CI 1.05, 1.94). There was weak evidence that those in the older age group
were more likely to be depressed (OR ¼ 1.25, 95% CI 0.99, 1.59). There was very strong evidence for a higher prevalence of
depressive symptoms amongst girls. They were more than twice as likely to show depressive symptoms as boys (OR ¼ 2.16,

Table 3
Association between bullying and reaching achievement benchmark: multivariate logistic regression analyses (N ¼ 1587).

OR (95% CI) p-value


Crude OR 0.48 (0.30, 0.75) 0.002
Adjusting for age group, gender, ethnicity, eligibility for free school meals 0.46 (0.28, 0.76) 0.004
þFriends social support 0.45 (0.27, 0.75) 0.004
þFamily social support 0.46 (0.28, 0.76) 0.004
þBullying  friends social support Low support: 0.38 (0.19, 0.76) 0.009
Moderate support: 0.27 (0.11, 0.65) 0.005
High support: 0.89 (0.40, 1.99) 0.775
þBullying  family social support Low support: 0.40 (0.21, 0.76) 0.007
Moderate support: 1.00 (0.49, 2.06) 0.999
High support: 0.20 (0.07, 0.51) 0.002
C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588 585

Table 4
Association between key variables and depressive symptoms: univariate logistic regression analyses.

OR (95% CI) depressive symptoms p-value


Bullied
No 1
Yes 1.42 (1.05, 1.94) 0.026

Age group
Younger 1
Older 1.25 (0.99, 1.59) 0.058

Gender
Male 1
Female 2.16 (1.74, 2.68) <0.0001

Ethnicity
White 1
Bangladeshi 1.37 (0.92, 2.03) 0.111
Black 1.01 (0.73, 1.38) 0.965
Indian 1.53 (0.93, 2.51) 0.091
Pakistani 0.87 (0.56, 1.36) 0.526
Other 1.36 (0.95, 1.95) 0.954

Eligible for FSM


No 1
Yes 0.91 (0.71, 1.17) 0.441

Family soc support


Low 1
Moderate 0.60 (0.49, 0.72) <0.0001
High 0.44 (0.31, 0.61) <0.0001
Friends soc support
Low 1
Moderate 0.79 (0.61, 1.02) 0.070
High 0.90 (0.69, 1.16) 0.400

95% CI 1.74, 2.68). Pupils with high levels of family social support were less than half as likely to have depressive symptoms
(OR ¼ 0.44, 95% CI 0.31, 0.61). Moderate family social support was also associated with lower odds of depressive symptoms
(OR ¼ 0.60, 95% CI 0.49, 0.72).
Table 5 shows the adjusted models with depression as the outcome variable. There was evidence from the Mantel–
Haenszel analysis for a difference in odds ratios by gender (test for homogeneity of odds ratios p-value ¼ 0.019). Adjusting for
age group, gender and ethnicity with a gender  bullying interaction led to an odds ratio for bullying for boys of 2.34 (95% CI
1.41, 3.87) and an odds ratio of 1.11 (95% CI 0.67, 1.84) for girls. Bullying thus appeared to have an effect on the mental health of
boys, but not that of girls. Adjusting for social support from friends led to a reduction in the odds ratio for boys to 2.17 (95% CI
1.31, 3.61) although bullying still had a significant impact (p ¼ 0.004). Adjusting for social support from family did not result in
the impact of bullying on depression being attenuated (OR ¼ 2.29, 95% CI 1.34, 3.93). Adding depressive symptoms at baseline
to the model resulted in a reduction in the odds ratio for bullying to 1.60 (95% CI 0.92, 2.77). Weak evidence for an association
between being bullied and depressive symptoms for boys at follow-up remained (p ¼ 0.091).

Discussion

9.1% of adolescents in our sample reported being bullied in the previous term. Bullying had a strong impact on academic
achievement for boys and girls. It also impacted negatively on boys’ mental health. Boys and girls were equally likely to be
victims of bullying. There were few differences in the percentage of students who were bullied across ethnic groups. There
was evidence that bullying decreased with age and that those with high levels of social support from their friends were less

Table 5
Association between bullying and depressive symptoms: multivariate logistic regression analyses (N ¼ 1794).

OR (95% CI) p-
value
Crude OR 1.42 (1.02, 1.95) 0.035
Adjusting for age group, gender, ethnicity, gender  bullying Boys: 2.34 (1.41, 3.87) 0.002
Girls: 1.11 (0.67, 1.84) 0.682
þFriends social support Boys: 2.17 (1.31, 3.61) 0.004
Girls: 1.08 (0.65, 1.79) 0.760
þFriends social support, family social support Boys: 2.29 (1.34, 3.93) 0.004
Girls: 1.10 (0.65, 1.88) 0.708
þFriends social support, family social support, depressive symptoms at Boys: 1.60 (0.92, 2.77) 0.091
baseline Girls: 0.84 (0.48, 1.45) 0.512
586 C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588

likely to be bullied. There was evidence that a high level of support from friends was able to protect bullied adolescents from
poor achievement at school. A moderate (but not high) level of support from the family was also protective.

Bullying prevalence

Other research has observed a decline in bullying prevalence with age, as is found here (Newman et al., 2005; Smith et al.,
1999). The importance of social support from friends (as opposed to family) in reducing an adolescent’s chance of being
bullied is of interest. This supports previous literature which has argued that adolescents spend increasing amounts of
unsupervised time with their peers rather than their family (Marini et al., 2006; Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). It has been argued
that their support may be more important than family support as it is in adolescence that independence from parents is
developed (Sebald, 1992; Youniss & Smollar, 1985).

Academic achievement

Being bullied had a strong impact on an adolescent’s chances of achieving the national academic benchmark for their age.
One possible mechanism for the finding is that those that are bullied have been found to be more likely to play truant or be
absent from school for other reasons (Smith et al., 2004). It does not support accounts that suggest that bullied pupils are able
to turn to and succeed at school work in order to escape from the problems that they are experiencing (Sharp, 1995).
Family social support protects against poor outcomes only when it is provided at a moderate level. Low or high support is
associated with decreased odds of reaching the academic benchmark for pupils who are bullied. It may be that social support
operates to produce a variety of outcomes. Whilst some argue that parental support is important for the development of
resilience in adolescents, over-protective parenting may have the opposite effect and make young people less assertive and
independent (Bowen, Smith, & Binney, 1994; Olweus, 1993; Swihart & Cotter, 1997). High levels of social support from friends,
on the other hand, do appear to be able to protect against the negative impact of bullying. It may be that a high level of social
support from friends has a particular role in limiting the extent of any bullying that does occur. Friends may intervene rather
than standing by; it has been shown that if bystanders become involved in preventing their friends being bullied, they are
usually successful (Hawkins et al., 2001).

Depression and bullying

Being bullied had a strong impact on the odds of being depressed. When a gender  bullying interaction was introduced
into the analysis, it was found that this was only the case for boys. A key question raised by the analysis, then, is why boys are
affected by the stress of bullying and girls are not. A Finnish study found that girls more often reported using the more
constructive tactic of “stress-recognition” (for example crying or screaming in addition to seeking support) as a strategy than
boys (Olafsen & Viemero, 2000). Another study in Finland found that a higher proportion of girls than boys used the effective
strategy of “nonchalance” in response to bullying, although both boys and girls frequently used less helpful responses. In the
case of girls this was more usually “helplessness” and for boys “counteraggression” (Salmivalli, Karhunen, & Lagerspetz, 1996).
Strategies that seek compromise may also be effective (Feldman & Gowan, 1998). It has been found that girls are more likely to
run for help or comfort the victim or demonstrate conflict resolution strategies (Feldman & Gowan, 1998; Osterman et al.,
1997; Salmivalli, Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, Osterman, & Kaukiainen, 1996).

Limitations and strengths

The main strengths of the study lie in its use of a representative sample of young people and the availability of stand-
ardised and previously validated measures of psychological distress. The large minority ethnic component of the sample
enabled analysis by ethnic grouping; this has often been a problem in analysis of educational differentials by ethnicity
(Demack, Drew, & Grimsley, 2000; Drew & Gray, 1990). The data is also unusual in that it provides comprehensive measures of
psychological distress alongside information on educational achievement; few British datasets provide this.
The bullying items do not provide a definition of what bullying is. However, the term “bullying” is more familiar in English
than in a number of other languages and has been in usage for a long time (Smith, Cowie, Olafsson, & Liefooghe, 2002). Smith
et al. (2002) found that fourteen year olds in their sample were able to clearly separate physical “aggression” and physical
“bullying” using cartoon depictions.
The fact that the key measures are self-report raises issues. One is the unmeasured results of an adolescent’s realisation
that they are being bullied. It may be that adolescents who recognise the bullying that they are being subjected to are better
equipped than those that do not as they are more prepared to respond in an assertive manner. It is possible, then, that the
analysis reported here underestimates the negative effects of bullying as it does not take account of those adolescents that are
bullied but are unable or unwilling to recognise the nature of the treatment that they are experiencing. There is a similar
problem with the measurement of social support. However, perceived support has been found to be protective, even when
perceptions are not accurate (Lakey & Cassady, 1990).
A final issue is that of missing data. There was evidence that those who did not reach the academic benchmark and who
had depressive symptoms were less likely to have data on bullying. This may have led to an over- or underestimation of the
C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588 587

association between bullying and the key outcomes. Sensitivity analysis was carried out whereby it was assumed in the first
set of analyses that all pupils with missing data on bullying had experienced being bulled and in the second set of analyses
that they had not. The main findings were the same.

Conclusions and future research

Although support from family and friends can protect adolescents against poor academic outcomes, the analysis here has
demonstrated that support from friends and family alone cannot mitigate against the strong negative effect that bullying has on
mental health amongst secondary school pupils in East London. It has been shown in other studies that being bullied can have
a longer term impact and contribute to both poor mental health, trouble with personal relationships and unemployment risk in
adulthood (Gladstone, Parker, & Malhi, 2006; Hugh-Jones & Smith, 1999; Varhama & Bjorkqvist, 2005). Given its substantial
impact, it seems vital that strategies to tackle the problem are developed to prevent the development of the most serious
consequences such as suicide attempts (Rigby & Slee, 1999). Early and wide-ranging intervention may avoid the need for
specialist psychiatric consultation, to which bullied children are disproportionately referred (Kumpulainen et al., 1998, 2001).

Acknowledgements

RELACHS was commissioned by the East London and City Health Authority. The data collection was funded by East London
and City Health Authority and the Department of Health. CR is funded by a Medical Research Council Special Training
Fellowship (G0601707). We would like to thank the adolescents who took part in the RELACHS study as well as the research
team for the collection of the data.

References

Alikasifoglu, M., Erginoz, E., Ercan, O., Uysal, O., & Albayrak-Kaymak, D. (2007). Bullying behaviours and psychosocial health: results from a cross-sectional
survey among high school students in Istanbul, Turkey. European Journal of Pediatrics.
Angold, A., Costello, E., Messer, C., Pickles, A., Winder, F., & Silver, D. (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of
depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 237–249.
Angold, A., Weissman, M., Merikangas, K., Prusoff, B., Wickramaratne, P., Gammon, D., et al. (1987). Parent and child reports of depressive symptoms in
children at low and high risk of depression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28, 901–915.
Baldry, A. (2004). The impact of direct and indirect bullying on the mental and physical health of Italian youngsters. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 343–355.
Baldry, A., & Farrington, D. (2004). Evaluation of an intervention program for the reduction of bullying and victimization in schools. Aggressive Behavior, 30,
1–15.
Baldry, A., & Farrington, D. (2005). Protective factors as moderators of risk factors in adolescence bullying. Social Psychology of Education, 8, 263–284.
Bond, L., Carlin, J., Thomas, L., Rubin, K., & Patton, G. (2001). Does bullying cause emotional problems? A prospective study of young teenagers. British
Medical Journal, 323, 480–484.
Bowen, L., Smith, P., & Binney, V. (1994). Cohesion and power in the families of children involved in bully-victim problems at school. Journal of Family
Therapy, 14, 371–387.
Brunstein Klomak, A., Marrocco, F., Kleinman, M., Schonfeld, I., & Gould, M. (2007). Bullying, depression, and suicidality in adolescents. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 40–49.
Davidson, L., & Demaray, M. (2007). Social support as a moderator between victimization and internalizing–externalizing distress from bullying. School
Psychology Review, 36, 383–405.
Demack, S., Drew, D., & Grimsley, M. (2000). Minding the gap: ethnic, gender and social class differences in attainment at 16, 1988–95. Race, Ethnicity and
Education, 3, 117–143.
Department for Education and Skills. (2006). Trends in education and skills.
DeRosier, M., Kupersmidt, J., & Patterson, C. (1994). Children’s academic and behavioural adjustment as a function of the chronicity and proximity of peer
rejection. Child Development, 65, 1799–1813.
Drew, D., & Gray, J. (1990). The fifth year examination results of black young people in England and Wales. Educational Research, 32, 107–117.
Espelage, D., & Holt, M. (2001). Bullying and victimization during early adolescence: peer influences and psychosocial correlates. In R. Geffner, & M. Loring
(Eds.), Bullying behavior: Current issues, research, and interventions. Binghamton, NY: Haworth Press.
Farrington, D. (1993). Understanding and preventing bullying. In M. Tonry (Ed.), Crime and justice (pp. 381–458). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Fekkes, M., Pijpers, F., Fredriks, A., Vogels, T., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. (2006). Do bullied children get ill, or do ill children get bullied? a prospective cohort
study on the relationship between bullying and health-related symptoms. Pediatrics, 117, 1568–1574.
Fekkes, M., Pijpers, F., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. (2004). Bullying behavior and associations with psychosomatic complaints and depression in victims.
Journal of Pediatrics, 144, 17–22.
Fekkes, M., Pijpers, F., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. (2005). Bullying: who does what, when and where? Involvement of children, teachers and parents in
bullying behavior. Health Education Research, 20, 81–91.
Feldman, S., & Gowan, L. (1998). Conflict negotiation tactics in romantic relationships in high school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 27, 691–706.
Forero, R., McLellan, L., Rissel, C., & Bauman, A. (1999). Bullying behaviour and psychosocial health among school students in New South Wales, Australia:
cross sectional survey. British Medical Journal, 319, 344–348.
Gladstone, G., Parker, G., & Malhi, G. (2006). Do bullied children become anxious and depressed adults? A cross-sectional investigation of the correlates of
bullying and anxious depression. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194, 201–208.
Glew, G., Fan, M.-Y., Katon, W., Rivara, F., & Kernic, M. (2005). Bullying, psychosocial adjustment, and academic performance in elementary school. Archives
of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 159, 1026–1031.
Greco, L., Freeman, K., & Dufton, L. (2007). Overt and relational victimization among children with frequent abdominal pain: links to social skills, academic
functioning, and health service use. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 319–329.
Hawkins, D., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2001). Naturalistic observations of peer interventions in bullying. Social Development, 10, 512–527.
Holt, M., & Espelage, D. (2007). Perceived social support among bullies, victims and bully-victims. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 984–994.
House, J. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
House, J., Landis, K., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241, 540–545.
Hugh-Jones, S., & Smith, P. (1999). Self-reports of short- and long-term effects of bullying on children who stammer. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
69, 141–158.
588 C. Rothon et al. / Journal of Adolescence 34 (2011) 579–588

Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpela, M., Marttunen, M., Rimpela, A., & Rantanen, P. (1999). Bullying, depression, and suicidal ideation in Finnish adolescents: school
survey. British Medical Journal, 319, 348–351.
Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpela, M., Rantanen, P., & Rimpela, A. (2000). Bullying at school – an indicator of adolescents at risk for mental disorders. Journal of
Adolescence, 23, 661–674.
Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., Henttonen, I., Almqvist, F., Kresanov, K., Linna, S., et al. (1998). Bullying and psychiatric symptoms among elementary school-
age children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 705–717.
Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., & Puura, K. (2001). Psychiatric disorders and the use of mental health services among children involved in bullying. Aggressive
Behavior, 27, 102–110.
Ladd, G. (1990). Having friends, keeping friends, making friends, and being liked by peers in the classroom: predictors of children’s early school adjust-
ment? Child Development, 61, 1081–1100.
Lakey, B., & Cassady, P. (1990). Cognitive processes in perceived social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 3–21.
Marini, Z., Dane, A., Bosacki, S., & Ylc-Cura. (2006). Direct and indirect bully-victims: differential psychosocial risk factors associated with adolescents
involved in bullying and victimization. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 551–569.
Nansel, T., Overpeck, M., Pilla, R., Ruan, W. J., Simons-Morton, B., & Scheidt, P. (2001). Bullying behaviors among US youth: prevalence and association with
psychosocial adjustment. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285, 2094–2100.
Newman, M., Holden, G., & Delville, Y. (2005). Isolation and the stress of being bullied. Journal of Adolescence, 28, 343–357.
Olafsen, R., & Viemero, V. (2000). Bully/victim problems and coping with stress in school among 10 to 12-year-old pupils in Aland, Finland. Aggressive
Behavior, 26, 57–65.
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.
Osterman, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., Landau, S., Fraczek, A., & Pastorelli, C. (1997). Sex differences in styles of conflict resolution: a developmental and
cross-cultural study with data from Finland, Israel, Italy and Poland. In D. Fry, & K. Bjorkqvist (Eds.), Cultural variation in conflict resolution. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Owens, L., Shute, R., & Slee, P. (2000). “Guess what I just heard!”: indirect aggression among teenage girls in Australia. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 67–83.
Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social support on adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 57–68.
Rigby, K. (2002). New perspectives on bullying. Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Rigby, K., & Slee, P. (1999). Suicidal ideation among adolescent school children, involvement in bully-victim problems, and perceived social support. Suicide
and Life Threatening Behavior, 29, 119–130.
Roland, E., & Idsoe, T. (2000). Bullying in school: three national innovations in Norwegian schools in 15 years. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 135–143.
Salmivalli, C., Karhunen, J., & Lagerspetz, K. (1996). How do the victims respond to bullying? Aggressive Behavior, 22, 99–109.
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: participant roles and their relations to social
status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 1–15.
Saluja, G., Iachan, R., Scheidt, P., Overpeck, M., Sun, W., & Giedd, J. (2004). Prevalence of and risk factors for depressive symptoms among young adolescents.
Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 158, 760–765.
Seals, D., & Young, J. (2003). Bullying and victimization: prevalence and relationship to gender, grade level, ethnicity, self-esteem, and depression.
Adolescence, 38, 735–757.
Sebald, H. (1992). Adolescence. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Sharp, S. (1995). How much does bullying hurt? The effects of bullying on the personal well-being and educational progress of secondary aged students.
Educational and Child Psychology, 12, 81–88.
Smith, P., & Brain, P. (2000). Bullying in schools: lessons from two decades of research. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 1–9.
Smith, P., Cowie, H., Olafsson, R., & Liefooghe, A. (2002). Definitions of bullying: comparison of terms used, and age and gender differences, in a fourteen-
country international comparison. Child Development, 73, 1119–1133.
Smith, P., Madsen, K., & Moody, J. (1999). What causes the age decline in reports of being bullied in school? Towards a developmental analysis of risks of
being bullied. Educational Research, 41, 267–285.
Smith, P., & Sharp, S. (1994). School bullying: Insights and perspectives. London: Routledge.
Smith, P., Talamelli, L., Cowie, H., Naylor, P., & Chauhan, P. (2004). Profiles of non-victims, escaped victims, continuing victims and new victims of school
bullying. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 565–581.
Stansfeld, S., Haines, M., Booy, R., Taylor, S., Viner, R., Head, J., et al. (2003). Health of young people in East London: The RELACHS study 2001. London: The
Stationary Office.
Swihart, E., & Cotter, P. (1997). The manipulative child: How to regain control and raise resilient, resourceful, and independent kids. New York: Macmillan.
Varhama, L., & Bjorkqvist, K. (2005). Relation between school bullying during adolescence and subsequent long-term unemployment in adulthood in
a Finnish sample. Psychological Reports, 96, 269–272.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
van der Wal, M., de Wit, C., & Hirasing, R. (2003). Psychosocial health among young victims and offenders of direct and indirect bullying. Pediatrics, 111,
1312–1317.
Wentzel, K. (1994). Family functioning and academic achievement in middle school: a social-emotional perspective. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 268–
291.
Wentzel, K., & Asher, S. (1995). The academic lives of neglected, rejected, popular, and controversial children. Child Development, 66, 754–763.
Wentzel, K., & Caldwell, K. (1997). Friendships, peer acceptance, and group membership: relations to academic achievement in middle school. Child
Development, 68, 1198–1209.
Wentzel, K., Weinberger, D., Ford, M., & Feldman, S. (1990). Academic achievement in preadolescence: the role of motivational, affective and self-regulatory
processes. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11, 179–193.
Wolke, D., Woods, S., Bloomfield, L., & Karstadt, L. (2000). The association between direct and relational bullying and behaviour problems among primary
school children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 989–1002.
Woods, S., & Wolke, D. (2004). Direct and relational bullying among primary school children and academic achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 42,
135–155.
Youniss, J., & Smollar, J. (1985). Adolescent relations with mothers, fathers, and friends. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Zimet, G., Dahlem, N., Zimet, S., & Farley, G. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52, 30–41.

You might also like