Bruner: Bruner's Three Modes of Representation
Bruner: Bruner's Three Modes of Representation
Bruner: Bruner's Three Modes of Representation
simplypsychology.org/bruner.html
The outcome of cognitive development is thinking. The intelligent mind creates from
experience "generic coding systems that permit one to go beyond the data to new and
possibly fruitful predictions" (Bruner, 1957, p. 234).
Thus, children, as they grow, must acquire a way of representing the "recurrent regularities"
in their environment.
So, to Bruner, important outcomes of learning include not just the concepts, categories, and
problem-solving procedures invented previously by the culture, but also the ability to
"invent" these things for oneself.
Cognitive growth involves an interaction between basic human capabilities and "culturally
invented technologies that serve as amplifiers of these capabilities."
These culturally invented technologies include not just obvious things such as computers
and television, but also more abstract notions such as the way a culture categorizes
phenomena, and language itself. Bruner would likely agree with Vygotsky that language
serves to mediate between environmental stimuli and the individual's response.
The aim of education should be to create autonomous learners (i.e., learning to learn).
In his research on the cognitive development of children (1966), Jerome Bruner proposed
three modes of representation:
Rather than neat age-related stages (like Piaget), the modes of representation are
integrated and only loosely sequential as they "translate" into each other.
Enactive (0 - 1 years)
This appears first. It involves encoding action based information and storing it in our
memory. For example, in the form of movement as a muscle memory, a baby might
remember the action of shaking a rattle.
1/5
The child represents past events through motor responses, i.e., an infant will “shake a
rattle” which has just been removed or dropped, as if the movements themselves are
expected to produce the accustomed sound. And this is not just limited to children.
Many adults can perform a variety of motor tasks (typing, sewing a shirt, operating a lawn
mower) that they would find difficult to describe in iconic (picture) or symbolic (word) form.
Iconic (1 - 6 years)
This is where information is stored visually in the form of images (a mental picture in the
mind’s eye). For some, this is conscious; others say they don’t experience it. This may
explain why, when we are learning a new subject, it is often helpful to have diagrams or
illustrations to accompany the verbal information.
Symbols are flexible in that they can be manipulated, ordered, classified etc., so the user
isn’t constrained by actions or images. In the symbolic stage, knowledge is stored primarily
as words, mathematical symbols, or in other symbol systems.
Bruner's constructivist theory suggests it is effective when faced with new material to
follow a progression from enactive to iconic to symbolic representation; this holds true
even for adult learners.
Bruner's work also suggests that a learner even of a very young age is capable of learning
any material so long as the instruction is organized appropriately, in sharp contrast to the
beliefs of Piaget and other stage theorists.
The use of words can aid the development of the concepts they represent and can remove
the constraints of the “here & now” concept. Bruner views the infant as an intelligent &
active problem solver from birth, with intellectual abilities basically similar to those of the
mature adult.
Educational Implications
For Bruner (1961), the purpose of education is not to impart knowledge, but instead to
facilitate a child's thinking and problem-solving skills which can then be transferred to a
range of situations. Specifically, education should also develop symbolic thinking in
2/5
children.
In 1960 Bruner's text, The Process of Education was published. The main premise of
Bruner's text was that students are active learners who construct their own knowledge.
Bruner (1960) opposed Piaget's notion of readiness. He argued that schools waste time
trying to match the complexity of subject material to a child's cognitive stage of
development. This means students are held back by teachers as certain topics are deemed
too difficult to understand and must be taught when the teacher believes the child has
reached the appropriate state of cognitive maturity.
Bruner (1960) adopts a different view and believes a child (of any age) is capable of
understanding complex information: 'We begin with the hypothesis that any subject can be
taught effectively in some intellectually honest form to any child at any stage of
development.' (p. 33)
Bruner (1960) explained how this was possible through the concept of the spiral
curriculum. This involved information being structured so that complex ideas can be
taught at a simplified level first, and then re-visited at more complex levels later on.
Therefore, subjects would be taught at levels of gradually increasing difficultly (hence the
spiral analogy). Ideally, teaching his way should lead to children being able to solve
problems by themselves.
Bruner (1961) proposes that learners’ construct their own knowledge and do this by
organizing and categorizing information using a coding system. Bruner believed that the
most effective way to develop a coding system is to discover it rather than being told it by
the teacher. The concept of discovery learning implies that students construct their own
knowledge for themselves (also known as a constructivist approach).
The role of the teacher should not be to teach information by rote learning, but instead to
facilitate the learning process. This means that a good teacher will design lessons that
help students discover the relationship between bits of information. To do this a teacher
must give students the information they need, but without organizing for them. The use of
the spiral curriculum can aid the process of discovery learning.
Bruner, like Vygotsky, emphasized the social nature of learning, citing that other people
should help a child develop skills through the process of scaffolding. The term scaffolding
first appeared in the literature when Wood, Bruner, and Ross described how tutors'
interacted with a preschooler to help them solve a block reconstruction problem (Wood et
al., 1976).
3/5
The concept of scaffolding is very similar to Vygotsky's notion of the zone of proximal
development, and it's not uncommon for the terms to be used interchangeably. Scaffolding
involves helpful, structured interaction between an adult and a child with the aim of helping
the child achieve a specific goal.
'[Scaffolding] refers to the steps taken to reduce the degrees of freedom in carrying out
some task so that the child can concentrate on the difficult skill she is in the process of
acquiring' (Bruner, 1978, p. 19).
Bruner states that what determines the level of intellectual development is the extent to
which the child has been given appropriate instruction together with practice or experience.
So - the right way of presentation and the right explanation will enable a child to grasp a
concept usually only understood by an adult. His theory stresses the role of education and
the adult.
Bruner views symbolic representation as crucial for cognitive development, and since
language is our primary means of symbolizing the world, he attaches great importance to
language in determining cognitive development.
Children’s COGNITIVE STRUCTURES You can SPEED-UP cognitive development. You don’t have
develop over time to wait for the child to be ready
Children are ACTIVE participants in the The involvement of ADULTS and MORE KNOWLEDGEABLE
learning process PEERS makes a big difference
Cognitive development entails the The involvement of ADULTS and MORE KNOWLEDGEABLE
acquisition of SYMBOLS PEERS makes a big difference
References
Bruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given. New York: Norton.
4/5
Bruner, J. S. (1961). The act of discovery. Harvard Educational Review, 31, 21-32.
Wood, D. J., Bruner, J. S., & Ross, G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal
of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 17(2), 89-100.
Further Information
5/5