Singing and Vocal Development: June 2006
Singing and Vocal Development: June 2006
Singing and Vocal Development: June 2006
net/publication/279063526
CITATIONS READS
46 16,387
1 author:
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Graham Frederick Welch on 06 July 2015.
Graham F. Welch
Abstract
The human voice has a central place in the ontogenesis of our musicality. Musical development begins pre-
birth through the foetal experiences of the melody-like contouring of our mother’s voice. These earliest
experiences form the foundation for subsequent musical (including vocal and linguistic) behaviour. The
ongoing interactions between our individual neuropsychobiological development with the sounds and
expectations of the maternal socio-cultural environment continue to shape the development of vocal skills,
including singing, throughout childhood and into adolescence. By the time that puberty is reached, self-
identity (whether tending towards the positive or negative) in relation to the art and expectations of singing
is firmly established. If the individual has been exposed to an appropriately nurturing environment,
considerable singing skills are normally evidenced. However, the experience of negative comments,
particularly from adults such as parents and teachers, can have a life-long detrimental impact on singing
behaviours and the realisation of musical potential. The onset of voice change in adolescence requires a
revision (“re-mastering”) of established singing skills and marks a fundamental transition in the creation
of adult musicality. Throughout these formative years from birth onward, individual singing development is
usually incremental and positive, but can be inhibited by socio-cultural factors.
Introduction
Despite the warmth in the room as they shook the snow off their winter coats and
gathered around the kitchen table, there was a collective sense of nervousness and, in
some cases, unease that was barely touched by the hostess’ cheerful manner and greeting.
Outside, the dark of a Newfoundland evening had already descended and the hostess
wondered if some of the wind’s icy chill was reflected in the body language. This gathering
was to be the first of several sessions for the group when things usually unspoken,
sometimes hidden for many decades, would be allowed to surface.
“You went to school, the first thing that happened, everybody had to be singing in
little concerts and things. You go to class; the nuns would say, “anybody can sing.”
You’d go and you were embarrassed to tears because you knew you couldn’t sing
and there was no help…I was told…you really can’t sing, you can go back to your
classroom.”
(Knight, S. interview with C., aged 50, private communication)
“I remember playing skipping and singing on the street. I can’t remember the
tunes now. My sister - I remember singing a little bit to her, but I don’t think I
ever really thought I couldn’t sing until Grade Seven [age twelve] and the teacher
and all my friends and I was in glee club and that was a major time. She stopped
and said, “Somebody is tone-deaf here.” She said, “It’s you, Julie, you’re tone-
deaf.” She said, “ You don’t have any notes. You just can’t sing along with the
music at all.” I said, “I really want to stay in glee club because my friends are
there.” She said, “You can stay in glee club but you’re not allowed to sing. You
just got to mouth the words. You can’t sing.” From then on, I assumed that I was
tone deaf. I never sang in any other choirs at all after that. I go to church most of
the time and I mouth the words. If we are out with a bunch of friends at a party, I
try to mouth the words. Maybe, if I had a drink or two, I might sing. And even
when I heard myself, I felt that I couldn’t sing. My voice is deep anyway…I know
a lot of people have deep voices and are beautiful singers, but I just assumed that
because my voice is deep that I couldn’t sing…that’s thirty-five years ago…I was
sitting second row back and there were kids behind me. You can imagine how
embarrassed I felt.”
(Knight, S. interview with V., aged 47, private communication)
“We always sang. We’d sit on the fence in the evening, friends and stuff like this,
and we’d sing different songs that would be on the go and, of course, you would
be playing and there would be songs with that. But it was always something that
we did. Then in Grade Six [age eleven], we had a two-room school and we had
Grade One to Three in one room and Grade Four to Six in the other room, and
the same teacher, of course, taught the three Grades. Her daughter was in school
with us and there was some kind of play or something for Christmas, and so
singing – these songs were sung. I practised at home for ages and I stood up to
sing it and she [the teacher] told me to sit down, that I couldn’t sing. Well, I was
devastated. And I thought I had done such a good job with it…I’m sure I wanted
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 4 of 26
to cry. Of course, you came home; it was no good of telling your parents at the
time that something like this had happened to you.”
(Knight, S. interview with L., aged 42, private communication)
Over the next few weeks and months, these adults shared many similar detailed
memories. Despite the passing of time, these episodes of childhood were vividly recalled.
A sense of embarrassment, shame, deep emotional upset and humiliation, usually
accompanied by reports of a lifelong sense of musical inadequacy were commonly
expressed elements. For these particular Canadians, as for many other adults around the
world in different cultural contexts, the associations between singing and childhood were
not positive. Within the local Newfoundland culture, singing competency either as an
individual or within a group has always had high status. Consequently, any perceived
singing “failure” in childhood has often led to continued self-identify as a “non-singer” (cf
Knight, 1999) and has reinforced a cultural stereotype of a community that is divided in
two: those who “can sing” and those who “cannot”.
Similar findings have been reported from other studies of adults in North
America, the UK and Scandinavia. Yet, despite such experiences, there are some adults
who never give up hope of improvement and there have been several successful examples
of specialist choirs being started for adult “non-singers” (cf Mack, 1979; Richards &
Durrant, 2003). These include a new community choir in St. John’s, Newfoundland, four
“beginners” choirs in one London college that have a twenty year history, various
“Singing from Scratch” choirs in the Midlands and South-East of England and similar
initiatives in Sweden, USA, Canada, Australia and New Zealand.
The existence of such choirs for adult “non-singers” is one of a number of
significant challenges to a bi-polar “can/cannot” categorisation of singing behaviours.
They are part of the evidence base for singing to be considered as a normal developmental
behaviour that can be enhanced or hindered, particularly by the events and experiences of
childhood. The prime source of such singing “failure” for an individual is a particular
moment in childhood and/or adolescence when there is a mismatch between developing
singing competences and a set singing task (cf Welch, 1979; 1985; 2000a; 2000b; 2005a;
Cooksey & Welch, 1998). Erroneous adult expectation often creates the problem. This
mismatch may then become further “objectified” by continuing inappropriate comment
from adults or peers, which suggests that the singing problem is evidence of an underlying
disability in music. Arguably, the number of singing “failures” that are socially generated
in our communities would be reduced radically if there was a greater awareness of (a) how
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 5 of 26
singing mastery develops, (b) how children of the same age can be in different phases of
development (as is considered normal with other forms of culturally-biased behaviour,
such as reading) and (c) how best to provide suitable “developmentally sensitive” singing
activities. The narrative that follows reviews the nature of singing development from early
childhood through to (and including) adolescence. Particular features are highlighted of
how normal development may be fostered, shaped, and sometimes hindered.
for example, typically incorporate rich musical properties when interacting with infants:
they speak and sing at higher pitch levels, use a wider pitch range, longer pauses, often at a
slower rate, and use smooth, simple, but highly modulated intonation contours (see
Thurman & Welch, 2000; Welch, 2005b; Chapter 2, this volume). At birth, neonates
continue to be particularly sensitive to the sound of the human voice, whilst
demonstrating a certain initial perceptual plasticity towards any language (Eimas, 1985).
Two-day-old neonates, for example, listen longer to women singing in a maternal style
(Masataka, 1999). Adult singing (both male and female) appears to be especially
significant, as demonstrated in its beneficial effects on premature infants’ physiological
functioning through changes in heart rate and oxygen saturation, alongside a reduction in
stressful behaviours (Coleman et al, 1997).
The earliest vocal behaviour is crying. It contains all of the ingredients of
subsequent vocalisation, including singing, with variations in intensity and pitch, as well as
rhythmic patterning and phrasing (Vihman, 1996). At the age of two months, cooing and
vowel-like sounds are already evidenced and being shaped by the maternal culture (Ruzza
et al, 2003). Aspects of “musical babbling” that contain definite musical features, such as
pitch and rhythmic patterns, are also evidenced from two months onwards (Tafuri &
Villa, 2002). Their incidence and quality appear to be related positively to the amount of
time devoted to daily singing behaviours by the mother; the greater the amount of
maternal singing, the increased likelihood of earlier musical babbling. By the age of three
to four months, the infant is able to imitate their mother’s exaggerated prosodic contours
that characterise infant-mother interaction (Masataka, 1992). Vocal play emerges around
the ages of four to six months (Papousek, 1996). By the age of one year, infants are
sufficiently cued into the language of the maternal culture for elements to be reflected in
their own vocalisations. As examples, French infants babble using French speech units,
Russian infants babble using Russian and Japanese infants using Japanese (Meltzoff,
2002).
In general, the first year of life is characterised by increasingly diverse vocal
activity. The first vocalisations of infancy, with their communication of affective state
(discomfort and distress, then also comfort and eustress), are expanded to include quasi-
melodic features (2-4 months), developing vocal control (4-7 months), with vocal pitch
behaviours that are directly linked to the prosodic features of the mother tongue.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 7 of 26
4
Phrases
3 Songs
2
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28
Figure 1: Accuracy ratings of Italian children (n=28) aged 2.6 to 3.3 years in imitating
song phrases and complete songs modelled by their mothers (Tafuri & Welch,
unpublished data). Ratings are based on a 7-point scale of perceived accuracy.
For the youngest children, the boundaries between singing and speaking may be
blurred, or at least ambiguous to the adult listener, and are related to the dominance of a
particular contour schema (Davidson, 1994) as well as to the influence of the mother
tongue. For example, a longitudinal study in Canada of young girls aged 18 to 38 months
from monolingual and bilingual backgrounds reported that “intermediate vocalisations” (a
type of vocal behaviour at the boundary between speech and song) were more prevalent
in Mandarin and Cantonese-speaking children than in English-speaking children (Mang,
2000/1). A follow-up study in Hong Kong with mono- and bilingual three- and four-year-
olds confirmed these findings and revealed that, regardless of age, the manipulation of
vocal pitch was used to distinguish between singing and speaking (Mang, 2002). The mean
fundamental frequencies (F0) for songs were reported to be consistently higher than
speech, but “own choice” songs were performed at a slightly lower pitch than a criterion
song. In addition, the older English monolingual children demonstrated a wider mean F0
differentiation between their singing and speaking behaviours compared to their
Cantonese mono- and bilingual peers. Taken together, such examples from these diverse
cultural settings remind us that singing behaviour is subject to developmental processes,
whilst also being sensitive to socio-cultural context (including task). In the above
examples, context also includes the presence or absence of a pitch-based language as the
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 9 of 26
1
The conceptualisation of development as occurring in ‘phases’ is a common outcome of research that is
undertaken over a long period with time for researcher reflection and the evaluation of new data. For
example, the current author has developed and reviewed a particular model of vocal pitch matching over the
past two decades (1986; 2002), which reconceptualises the evidence and reduces the number of
developmental ‘phases’ (rather than the originally labelled ‘stages’) from five to four.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 11 of 26
and assessed each year) (see Figure 2). However, this singing behaviour was in marked
contrast to their ability to learn the words of the songs, which was extremely good, even
in their first term of compulsory schooling at age 5 (Figure 2: Year 1 data). Furthermore,
when the pitch elements of the target songs were deconstructed into simpler musical tasks
in which the children were required to match individual pitches, echo melodic contours,
or copy small melodic fragments, the children were significantly more pitch accurate, as
demonstrated by year-on-year improvements. There were no sex differences in their
singing of these three types of deconstructed tasks. Boys and girls were equally successful
and demonstrated similar improvements over time. In contrast, when the same boys were
faced with the challenge of singing a complete song, their vocal pitch became less accurate
and, as a group, they demonstrated little or no improvement in song singing across the
three years. Overall, singing competency appeared to be closely related to the nature of
the task, with many boys negatively affected in the task of singing a “school” song. In line
with these longitudinal findings, two recent studies suggest that gender stereotyping may
be a factor in the lack of singing development in some young boys (Hall, 2005; Joyce,
2005). Australian research into five-year-old boys’ singing (Hall, op.cit.) indicates that
singing may be perceived as a “female” activity. UK research of nine- and ten-year-olds
(Joyce, op.cit.) across three primary schools found that only one-third of boys enjoyed
singing (compared with two-thirds of girls) and that boys believed that girls were better
singers.
In addition to age, sex/gender and task, there are also contextual factors that can
affect children’s singing behaviours. For example, the UK longitudinal study data
demonstrated a clear “school effect” (Welch, 2000a). When comparing individual school
data, all the children in one inner-city school improved their singing skills over the three
years, notwithstanding their poor socio-economic environment and generally low
academic attainment in other areas of the curriculum, whereas relatively few children
made progress in another school, despite them having much higher socio-economic status
and attainment levels. A major factor in these differences appears to have been teacher
expectation. Progress was most marked where the class teacher expected and worked
consistently for singing improvement with all their pupils over a sustained period. Similar
findings concerning school effects on singing motivation, perceived self-identity as a
singer and overall enjoyment of singing as a school activity are also reported by Joyce
(2005).
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 12 of 26
7
6
5
4 Age 5 (year 1)
Age 6 (year 2)
3 Age 7 (year 3)
2
1
0
Words Single Simple Fragments Songs
pitches glides (pitch)
Age
Socio-cultural differences have been exampled also in the more advanced singing
skills demonstrated by a large class of first-grade Chinese (Hong Kong) children
compared with their USA peers (Rutkowski & Chen-Haftek, 2000). Similarly, an
assessment of the singing behaviours of 120 Hong Kong children aged seven to nine years
from various language groups (Mang, 2003), using both the Rutkowski and Welch
developmental profiles, reported statistically significant effects for sex (favouring girls) as
well as mother-tongue. Chinese monolingual children performed consistently better than
English bilingual children, even though the criterion song was in English. This was seen as
a further indication (following Mang, 2001; Rutkowski & Chen-Haftek op. cit.) that
Cantonese-speaking children achieve singing mastery earlier than their English
counterparts, perhaps because the pitch centres for speech and singing of the former are
more closely aligned.
Both the USA- and UK-based developmental models agree that different “phases”
of singing competency are likely to be exampled within any group of children entering
their first school class. Some children already will be extremely competent performers of
complete songs from the experienced maternal culture (both words and music), whilst
others will be less advanced and will be in one of the “earlier phases” of singing
development. This does not mean that the latter group of “developing” singers will not
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 13 of 26
gain singing mastery, particularly if they are provided with an appropriately nurturing
environment in which singing tasks are designed to match, then to extend, current vocal
behaviours. For such children, it is likely that their pre-school interactions have provided
fewer opportunities to fulfil their singing potential (as outlined in the Early Childhood and
Pre-school section earlier).
The effects of singing alone or with a group are equivocal in the research
literature. Some research evidence suggests that children may become more accurate in
reproducing the musical features of a criterion song when singing in a group compared to
singing alone (e.g. Buckton; 1982; Greene, 1993). Other research (e.g. Goetze, 1985;
Smale, 1988) reports the opposite in favour of increased reproductive accuracy if the
young child is assessed when singing alone. It may be possible to reconcile these two
positions by assuming that individual singing behaviour is likely to be framed by an
interaction between current singing competency, the nature of the singing task, the
competency of other singers in the group and an individual’s current ability to make sense
of the available feedback. There is an internal psychological feedback monitoring system
that is essentially outside conscious awareness which is used for a moment-by-moment
self-monitoring of the singing behaviour. This system draws on information from internal
sense receptors, as well as internal and external auditory information concerning the
relative matching of vocal behaviour with an external model (see Welch, 1985, 2005a).
Where the individual is able to make sense of and use these different feedback channels in
combination, then singing as a member of a skilled group may promote more competent
behaviour. Where the individual is less able to make sense of and use this feedback, such
as when surrounded by a less skilled group of singers and/or when it is difficult to “hear”
their own voice, then performing in a group context may be more disadvantageous. Data
from studies of choral acoustics, for example, indicates that auditory feedback for one’s
own vocal output is reduced when (i) other singers are in close proximity (self-to-other
ratio) and (ii) when nearby singers are singing, or attempting to sing, the same pitches
(Daugherty, 2000; Ternström, 1994).
Nevertheless, it is likely that singing competency will be nurtured through
exposure to frequent opportunities for vocal play within an environment that encourages
vocal exploration and accurate imitation (Young, 2002; Mang, 2003; Welch, 2005a).
The data from various studies on early singing development were collated into a
theoretical protocol “baseline assessment of singing” for use with children on entry to
school (Welch & Elsley, 1999). This was evaluated subsequently with a small class of
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 14 of 26
children (n=19) aged from three years eight months to five years ten months (King, 2000).
In general, the data supported key features of the model, namely that singing competence
is likely to vary at an individual level with musical task, such as in the sung reproduction of
melodic contour, pitch intervals and song text. Any assessment of singing abilities in
young children, therefore, should provide a mixture of tasks (such as pitch glides and
pitch patterns as well as song melodies) as a basis for diagnosis and curriculum planning.
Furthermore, recent neuropsychobiological data on pitch processing modules in the brain
(Peretz & Coltheart, 2003) supports a hierarchical model in which melodic contour (pace
Davidson, 1994; Rutkowski, 1997; Welch, 1998) is analysed before the processing of
intervals and tonality (see Welch, 2005a for a review).
With regard to children’s ability to invent songs, a series of studies (Davies, 1986;
1992; 1994) indicate that five to seven-year-olds have a range of song-making strategies.
These include narrative songs (chant-like in nature, often with repeated figures), as well as
songs that have more conventional features, such as an opening idea and a clear sense of
closure, four-phrase structures, repetition, phrases that both “borrow” from the
immediate musical culture and which also may be transformed (sequenced, inverted,
augmented) in some way. Overall, children in the first years of schooling demonstrate a
clear sense of musical form and of emotional expression in their invented songs.
Older Childhood
The latter years of childhood are characterised by a general singing competency
for the majority. Relatively few children are reported as singing “out-of-tune” at the age of
eleven years (Howard et al, 1994; Welch, 1979; 2000b). For example, evidence from a wide
range of studies indicates that approximately 30% of pupils aged seven years are reported
as being relatively “inaccurate” when vocally matching a melody within a Western cultural
tradition. However, this proportion drops to around 4% of the same pupil population by
the age of eleven. Within each of these and the intervening age groups, “out-of-tune”
boys outnumber girls by a ratio of 2 or 3:1 (Welch, 1979). Culture, however, continues to
be significant. Anthropological and ethnomusicological studies, for example, have
suggested that young children from the Anang in Nigeria can sing “hundreds of songs,
both individually and in choral groups” by the age of 5 (Messinger, 1958: 20), Venda
children in South Africa were reported as both learning special children’s songs and
composing new songs for themselves (Blacking, 1967), whereas Herati children in
Afghanistan tended to focus on the imitation of adult models, with the children
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 15 of 26
2
The data for 2004 on the numbers of cathedrals with female choristers in UK cathedrals has been collated
by Claire Stewart as part of her ongoing doctoral studies at the Institute of Education into their impact on
the all-male choral tradition.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 16 of 26
Welch & Howard, 2002; Sergeant et al, 2005). With regard to perceived singer gender, a
summary of recent research data (Figure 3) indicates that, whilst it is possible for an
untrained solo singer’s sex to be identified relatively accurately from around the age of
eight onwards, it is also equally possible for trained female choristers from the age of 8 to
be systematically mistaken as male, depending on the particular piece of music being
performed. However, once the female chorister moves into her mid-teens, the voice
quality becomes more characteristically identifiable as “female” (“womanly”)3. In general,
children's voices tend to be higher in pitch and have a less complex acoustic make-up than
those of adults. Nevertheless, children are able to achieve similar loudness levels as adults
by using relatively more breath until the age of twelve, when adult-like breathing patterns
are observed (Stathopoulos, 2000).
Figure 3: Confusability by age and gender of children and adolescents aged four to
sixteen years. The figure is extrapolated from measured data of perceived confusability for
untrained singers (Sergeant et al, 2005) and measured data of perceived confusability for
trained singers (Welch & Howard, 2002). Initially, untrained young boys are confused as
girls. Then, the sexes become more readily distinguishable from the age of 8/9 years.
However, singing training can enable girls from 8/9 years to 14 years to sound ‘boy-like’
in certain pieces from the repertoire. From 14 years onwards, singer sex becomes more
readily identifiable.
3
For a detailed review of the literature on gender and chorister voice, including similarities and differences
in the underlying anatomy and physiology for singing, see Welch & Howard (2002). For data on the
perceived gender of untrained children’s voices, see Sergeant et al (2005).
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 17 of 26
distinct “stages” in female adolescent voice change (see in Figure 4a). In the first stage
(termed “pre-pubertal”) the voice has a “light, flute-like quality” with no apparent register
changes. The comfortable singing range is between D4 to D5, within a wider singing range
of Bb3 to F5 (and up to A5). The next stage (“pre-menarchial” – Stage IIA) is characteristic
of the beginnings of female voice mutation around the ages of eleven to twelve. The
comfortable range is approximately the same as previously (D4 to D5), within a slightly
expanded overall range (A3 to G5). However, there is often breathiness in the tone due to
inadequate closure of the vocal folds as a result of growth occurring in the laryngeal area.
A singing register transition typically appears between G4 and B4 and some girls may have
difficulties in singing lower pitches. Singing often becomes uncomfortable and effortful
and a breathy voice quality is characteristic across the range. The next stage is the peak of
female voice mutation (“post-menarchial” – Stage IIb). Singing is characterised by a
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 18 of 26
limited comfortable range (B3 to C5), discomfort (particularly at upper pitches), distinct
voice qualities for each sung register and with the lower part of the voice often taking on a
more “alto” and often husky quality. The final stage (“young adult female” – Stage III) has
a much-expanded comfortable singing range (A3 to G5), less breathiness, greater
consistency in tone quality and greater singing agility. Vibrato often appears at this stage
and the voice has a more adult, womanly quality. Ongoing research (cf Welch & Howard,
2002; Welch, 2004) indicates that adolescent voice change is the same for relatively
untrained female singers as for those who have been involved in sustained vocal
performance, such as through membership of a female cathedral choir. However, as with
adult female singers (Lã & Davidson, 2005), there is always some individual variation in
the impact of puberty on the singer’s voice related to slight differences in the underlying
endocrinological metabolism and physiological functioning.
Male adolescent voice change has a more extensive literature, both in Europe and
the USA. One major longitudinal study was conducted by Cooksey (2000), initially based
on fieldwork in California in the late 1970s, then drawing on further studies in the USA
during the following decade, as well as a London-based cross-cultural study in the 1990s
(Cooksey & Welch, 1998). Overall, he reports six “stages” of adolescent male singing
voice change (see in Figure 4a) that are characterised by an overall lowering of the
sung pitch range. Whilst the rate of voice change is unpredictable for any given individual,
it is reliably sequential for all.
In the first male adolescent stage (“unchanged”), the mean sung vocal pitch range
is A3 to F5, with the tessitura pitch boundaries C#4 to A#4. The voice quality is perceived
as “clear”, with relatively little evidence of breathiness in the tone. The beginnings of
voice change (termed by Cooksey as Stage I, “Midvoice I”) are marked by a reduced vocal
range (Ab3 to C5) and instability of sung pitch, particularly for the upper frequencies,
which tend to be produced with increased effort, as well as tone quality that is perceived
as more effortful, strained and breathy. The sung range then descends approximately in
thirds across the next three stages (see Figure 4a), with each stage being characterised by a
reduced mean range and relative continuing instability in the production of upper pitches,
but contrasted by relative stability for the lower pitches. The pitch ranges are: Stage II
(“Midvoice II”), F3 to A4; Stage III (“Midvoice IIa”), D3 to F#4; followed by Stage IV
(“New Baritone”, also termed “New Voice”), B2 to D#4. Within these, Stage II may be
regarded as the mid-point of voice change and this is when a falsetto register (C5 to B5)
first appears and (for some) a whistle register (C6 to C7). Stage III (“Midvoice IIa”) is
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 19 of 26
characterised by the greatest vocal instability and the least clear vocal quality. It is only in
the final stage of voice change (Stage V, “Settling Baritone”, also termed “Emerging Adult
Voice” G2 to D4) that the mean sung pitch range opens out again and the voice timbre
begins to adopt a clearer, less breathy quality. However, the number and intensity of
harmonics do not yet approximate normal adult characteristics. Nevertheless, for each
stage of voice change the adolescent male has a (limited) number of pitches that can be
produced comfortably and musically (see the darker shaded elements in the ranges for
male voices in Figure 4a) and it has been possible in recent years to find a greater
awareness by publishers to produce repertoire that is specially written as being suitable for
these changing voices.
In general, age is a poor predictor for establishing voice change stages, with any
given age group likely to encompass several stages. It is possible for an individual to pass
through all stages of adolescent voice change in twelve months, but is it also possible for
this process to be much slower and to last several years. Nevertheless, a summation of
selected UK and Japanese data for over three thousand males, aged nine to fourteen years,
provides some indication of the possible proportions of different categories of voice
change by age group (Figure 4b). As can be seen, the ages of twelve to fourteen have
significant proportions of males whose voices are perceived to have already “changed”, or
in the process of “changing”, whilst embracing a reducing number that are still
“unchanged”. Ideally, choral groups of adolescent male singers in this age range are best
suited, therefore, to music that has been arranged specifically for them in three parts,
using the Cooksey classification guidelines (Unchanged and Stage I on a top line, Stages II
and III on a middle line, and Stages IV and V on the bottom line), rather than to attempt
traditional four-part music in which the tessiturae often are likely to be mismatched with
current singing abilities.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 20 of 26
Figure 4a: Stages of singing voice change for females (based on Gackle, 2000) and males
(based on Cooksey, 2000)
Figure 4b: Extrapolated model of adolescent male voice change by age, based on UK
(Geddye, personal communication) and Japanese data (Norioka, 1994), total n=3,188
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 21 of 26
References
Adachi, M., & Trehub, S.E. (1999). Children’s communication of emotion in song. In S.
W. Yi (Ed.), Music, Mind and Science (pp.454-465). Seoul: Seoul National University
Press.
Adachi, M., & Trehub, S.E. (2000, April). Preschoolers’ Expression of Emotion through Invented
Songs. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in the Psychology
of Music and Music Education, University of Leicester, England.
Blacking, J. (1967). Venda children’s songs. Johannesburg: University of Witwatersrand Press.
British Educational Research Association Music Education Review Group. (2001).
Mapping Music Education Research in the UK. Southwell, UK: British Educational
Research Association.
Buckton, R. (1982). Sing a song of six-year-olds. Wellington, New Zealand: Council for
Educational Research.
Coleman, J. M., Pratt, R. R., Stoddard, R. A., Gerstmann, D. R., & Abel, H.-H. (1997).
The effects of the male and female singing behaviours and speaking voices on
selected physiological and behavioural measures of premature infants in the
intensive care unit. International Journal of Arts Medicine, 5(2), 4-11.
Cooksey, J. (2000). Voice transformation in male adolescents. In L. Thurman, & G. F. Welch (Eds.),
Bodymind and Voice: Foundations of Voice Education. Revised Edition (pp. 718-738). Iowa City,
Iowa: National Center for Voice and Speech.
Cooksey, J., & Welch, G.F. (1998). Adolescence, singing development and National
Curricula design. British Journal of Music Education, 15(1), 99-119.
Daugherty, J.F. (2000). Choir spacing and choral sound: Physical, pedagogical, and
philosophical dimensions. In B.A. Roberts & A. Rose (Eds.), Conference Proceedings of
the International Symposium, Sharing the Voices: The Phenomenon of Singing II (pp. 77-88).
St. Johns, Newfoundland, Canada: Memorial University of Newfoundland Press.
Davidson, L. (1994). Songsinging by young and old: a developmental approach to music.
In R. Aiello with J. Sloboda (Eds.), Musical Perceptions (pp. 99-130). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Davies, C. (1986). Say it till a song comes: reflections on songs invented by children 3-13.
British Journal of Music Education, 3(3), 279-293.
Davies, C. (1992). Listen to my song: a study of songs invented by children aged 5 to 7
years. British Journal of Music Education, 9(1), 19-48.
Davies, C. (1994). The listening teacher: An approach to the collection and study of
invented songs of children aged 5 to 7. In H. Lees (Ed.), Musical connections:
Tradition and change (pp. 120-127). Auckland, NZ: International Society for Music
Education.
Doubleday, V., & Baily, J. (1995). Patterns of musical development among children in
Afghanistan. In E.J. Fernea (Ed.), Children in the Muslim Middle East (pp. 431-444).
Austin: University of Texas Press.
Dowling, W. J. (1988). Tonal structure and children’s early learning of music. In J.
Sloboda (Ed.), Generative Processes in Music (pp. 113-128). Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Dowling, W. J. (1999). The development of music perception and cognition. In D.
Deutsch (Ed.), The Psychology of Music, 2nd Edition (pp. 603-625). London:
Academic Press.
Eimas, P.D. (1985). The perception of speech in early infancy. Scientific American, 252(1),
34-40.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 23 of 26
Gabrielsson, A., & Örnkloo, H. (2002, August). Children’s Perception and Performance of Emotion in Singing
and Speech. Paper presented at the ISME Early Childhood Conference, Copenhagen,
Denmark.
Gackle, L. (2000). Understanding voice transformation in female adolescents. In L. Thurman, &
G.F. Welch (Eds.), Bodymind and Voice: Foundations of Voice Education. Revised Edition (pp.
739-744). Iowa City, Iowa: National Center for Voice and Speech.
Goetze, M. (1985). Factors affecting accuracy in children’s singing. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University
of Colorado.
Greene, G.A. (1993). The effects of unison singing versus individual singing on the vocal pitch accuracy of elementary
school children. Paper presented at the Southern Division of the Music Educators National
Conference.
Hall, C. (2005). Gender and boys’ singing in early childhood. British Journal of Music Education, 22(1),
5-20.
Hargreaves, D.J. (1996). The development of artistic and musical competence. In I. Deliege & J.
Sloboda (Eds.), Musical Beginnings (pp. 145-170). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Harries, M.L.L., Griffin, M., Walker, J. & Hawkins, S. (1996). Changes in the male voice during
puberty: Speaking and singing voice parameters. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 21(2), 95-100.
Heidelbach, U. (1996). Die entwicklung der knabenstimme zur mannerstimme bei chorsangern dargestellt am
singstimmfeld. Unpublished MD thesis, Technischen Universität Dresden.
Herman-Giddens, M. E., Slora, E. J. Wasserman, R. C., Bourdony, C. J., Bhapkar, M. V., Kock, G.
G., & Hasemeier, C. M. (1997). Secondary sexual characteristics and menses in young girls
seen in office practice: A study from the Pediatric Research in Office Settings Network.
Pediatrics, 99(4), 505–512.
Howard, D.M., Angus, J.A., & Welch, G.F. (1994). Singing pitching accuracy from years 3
to 6 in a primary school. Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics, 16(5), 223- 230.
Johnstone, T., & Scherer, K.R. (2000). Vocal communication of emotion. In M. Lewis &
J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 220-235). New York:
Guildford Press.
Joyce, H. (2005). The effects of sex, age and environment on attitudes to singing in Key Stage 2.
Unpublished master’s dissertation, Institute of Education, University of London.
King, R. (2000). An investigation into the effectiveness of a baseline assessment in singing and some
influential home-environmental factors. Unpublished master’s dissertation, Roehampton
Institute London, London.
Knight, S. (1999). Exploring a cultural myth: what adult non-singers may reveal about the nature of
singing. In B.A. Roberts & A. Rose (Eds.), The Phenomenon of Singing II (pp. 144-154). St.
John’s, NF: Memorial University Press.
Lã, F., & Davidson, J. (2005). Investigating the relationship between sexual hormones and female
Western classical singing. Research Studies in Music Education, 25, 75-87.
Levinowitz, L. (1989). An investigation of preschool children’s comparative capability to sing songs
with and without words. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 100, 14-19.
Mack, L. (1979). A descriptive study of a community chorus made up of 'non-singers.' Unpublished EdD
dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.
Mang, E. (2000/2001). Intermediate vocalisations: an investigation of the boundary between speech
and songs in young children’s vocalisations. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education,
147, 116-121.
Mang, E. (2002). An investigation of vocal pitch behaviours of Hong Kong children. Bulletin of the
Council for Research in Music Education, 153/4, 128-134.
Mang, E. (2003). Singing competency of monolingual and bilingual children in Hong Kong. In
L.C.R. Yip, C.C. Leung, & W.T. Lau (Eds.), Curriculum Innovation in Music (pp. 237-242). Hong
Kong: Hong Kong Institute of Education.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 24 of 26
Masataka, N. (1992). Pitch characteristics of Japanese maternal speech to infants. Journal of Child
Language, 19, 213-223.
Masataka, N. (1999). Preference for infant-directed singing in 2-day old hearing infants of deaf
parents. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1001-1005.
Meltzoff, A.N. (2002). Elements of a developmental theory of imitation. In A.N. Meltzoff & W.
Prinz, (Eds.), The Imitative Mind. (pp. 19-41). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Messinger, J. (1958). Overseas report. Basic College Quarterly, Fall, 20-24.
Moog, H. (1976). The musical experience of the pre-school child. (trans. C. Clarke). London: Schott.
Nawrot, E.S. (2003). The perception of emotional expression in music: evidence from infants,
children and adults. Psychology of Music, 31(1), 75-92.
Norioka, Y. (1994). A survey of Japanese school aged poor pitch singers. In G. F. Welch, & T.
Murao (Eds.), Onchi and singing development (pp. 49-62). London: David Fulton Publishers.
Papousek, M. (1996). Intuitive parenting: a hidden source of musical stimulation in
infancy. In I. Deliege & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Musical Beginnings (pp. 88-112). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Peretz, I., & Coltheart, M. (2003). Modularity and music processing. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 688-
691.
Richards, H., & Durrant, C. (2003). To sing or not to sing: A study on the development of ‘non-
singers’ in choral activity. Research Studies in Music Education, 20, 78-89.
Rutkowski, J. (1990). The measurement and evaluation of children’s singing voice development. The
Quarterly, 1(1-2), 81-95.
Rutkowski, J. (1997). The nature of children’s singing voices: Characteristics and assessment. In B.A.
Roberts (Ed.), The Phenomenon of Singing (pp. 201-209). St. John’s, NF: Memorial University
Press.
Rutkowski, J. & Chen-Haftek, L. (2000, July). The singing voice within every child: A cross-cultural comparison
of first graders’ use of singing voice. Paper presented to the ISME Early Childhood Conference,
Kingston, Canada.
Ruzza, B., Rocca, F., Boero, D.L., & Lenti, C. (2003). Investigating the musical qualities of
early infant sounds. In G. Avanzini, C. Faienza, D. Minciacchi, L. Lopez, & M.
Majno (Eds.), The neurosciences and music (Vol. 999, pp. 527-529). New York: Annals
of the New York Academy of Sciences.
Sergeant, D. C., Sjölander, P., & Welch, G. F. (2005). Listeners’ identification of gender
differences in children’s singing. Research Studies in Music Education, 25, 28-39.
Smale, M.J. (1988). An investigation of pitch accuracy of four- and five-year-old singers. Dissertation
Abstracts International, AAT – 8723851.
Stathopoulos, E.T. (2000). A review of the development of the child voice: an anatomical and
functional perspective. In P.J. White (Ed.), Child Voice (pp. 1-12). Stockholm: Royal Institute
of Technology, Voice Research Centre.
Story, B. H., Titze, I. R., & Hoffman, E. A. (1997). Volumetric image-based comparison of male and
female vocal tract shapes. National Center for Voice and Speech Status and Progress Report 11, 153–
161.
Sundberg, J. (2000). Emotive transforms. Phonetica, 57, 95-112.
Sundin, B. (1997). Musical creativity in childhood: A research project in retrospect.
Research Studies in Music Education, 9, 48-57.
Tafuri, J., & Villa, D. (2002). Musical elements in the vocalisations of infants aged 2 to 8 months.
British Journal of Music Education, 19(1), 73-88.
Ternström, S. (1994). Hearing myself with others: Sound levels in choral performance measured
with separation of one’s own voice from the rest of the choir. Journal of Voice, 8(4), 293-302.
Titze, I. (1994). Principles of voice production. Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice-Hall.
Thurman, L., & Klitzke, C. (2000). Highlights of physical growth and function of voices from
prebirth to age 21. In L. Thurman, & G.F. Welch (Eds.), Bodymind and Voice: Foundations of
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 25 of 26
Voice Education. Revised Edition (pp. 696-703). Iowa City, Iowa: National Center for Voice
and Speech.
Thurman, L., & Welch, G.F. (Eds.), (2000). Bodymind and Voice: Foundations of Voice Education. Revised
Edition. Iowa City, Iowa: National Center for Voice and Speech.
Trehub, S.E. (2001). Musical predispositions in infancy. In R. J. Zatorre & I. Peretz (Eds.), The
Biological Foundations of Music (Vol. 930, pp.1-16). New York: Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences.
Vihman, M.M. (1996). Phonological development. Oxford: Blackwell.
Welch, G. F. (1979). Poor pitch singing: A review of the literature. Psychology of Music, 7(1), 50-58.
Welch, G. F. (1985). A schema theory of how children learn to sing in tune. Psychology of Music, 13(1),
3-18.
Welch, G. F. (1986). A developmental view of children’s singing. British Journal of Music
Education, 3(3), 295-303.
Welch, G. F. (1998). Early childhood musical development. Research Studies in Music
Education, 11, 27-41
Welch, G. F. (2000a). Singing development in early childhood: the effects of culture and education
on the realisation of potential. In P.J. White (Ed.), Child Voice (pp. 27-44). Stockholm: Royal
Institute of Technology.
Welch, G. F. (2000b). The developing voice. In L. Thurman, & G.F. Welch (Eds.), Bodymind and
Voice: Foundations of Voice Education (pp. 704-717). Iowa: National Center for Voice and
Speech.
Welch, G. F. (2002). Early Childhood Musical Development. In L. Bresler & C.
Thompson (Eds.), The Arts in Children’s Lives: Context, Culture and Curriculum. (pp.
113-128). Dordrecht, NL: Kluwer.
Welch, G. F. (2004). Developing young professional singers in UK cathedrals. Proceedings,
2nd International Physiology and Acoustics of Singing Conference, Denver, USA. Retrieved 3
June, 2005 from http://www.ncvs.org/pas/2004/pres/welch/welch.htm
Welch, G. F. (2005a). Singing as communication. In D. Miell, R. MacDonald & D. J. Hargreaves
(Eds.), Musical Communication. (pp.239-259). New York: Oxford University Press.
Welch, G. F. (2005b). The musical development and education of young children. In B. Spodek &
O. Saracho (Eds.), Handbook of Research on the Education of Young Children. (pp. 251-267).
Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Welch, G. F. & Elsley, J. (1999). Baseline assessment in singing. Australian Voice, 5, 60-66.
Welch, G. F., & Howard, D. (2002). Gendered voice in the cathedral choir. Psychology of Music, 30(1),
102-120.
Welch, G. F., Sergeant, D. C., & White, P. (1996). The singing competences of five-year-
old developing singers. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 127, 155-
162.
Welch, G. F., Sergeant, D. C., & White, P. (1997). Age, sex and vocal task as factors in
singing ‘in-tune’ during the first years of schooling. Bulletin of the Council for Research in
Music Education, 133, 153-160.
Welch, G. F., Sergeant, D. C., & White, P. (1998). The role of linguistic dominance in the
acquisition of song. Research Studies in Music Education, 10, 67-74.
Williams, B., Larson, G., & Price, D. (1996). An investigation of selected female singing-
and speaking-voice characteristics through comparison of a group of pre-menarchial
girls to a group of post-menarchial girls. Journal of Singing, 52(3), 33-40.
Young, S. (2002). Young children’s spontaneous vocalizations in free play: Observations
of two- to three-year-olds in a day care setting. Bulletin of the Council for Research in
Music Education, 152, 43-53.
Welch, G.F. (2006). Singing and Vocal Development.
In: G.McPherson (Ed.), The Child as Musician, (pp.311-329). New York: Oxford University Press. page 26 of 26
i
Rutkowski (1997) Singing Voice Development Measure (SVDM)
Phase 1 The words of the song appear to be the initial centre of interest rather
than the melody, singing is often described as ‘chant-like’, employing a
restricted pitch range and melodic phrases. In infant vocal pitch
exploration, descending patterns predominate.
Phase 2 There is a growing awareness that vocal pitch can be a conscious
process and that changes in vocal pitch are controllable. Sung melodic
outline begins to follow the general (macro) contours of the target
melody or key constituent phrases. Tonality is essentially phrase based.
Self-invented and ‘schematic’ songs ‘borrow’ elements from the child’s
musical culture. Vocal pitch range used in ‘song’ singing expands.
Phrase 3 Melodic shape and intervals are mostly accurate, but some changes in
tonality may occur, perhaps linked to inappropriate register usage.
Overall, however, the number of different reference pitches is much
reduced.
Phase 4 No significant melodic or pitch errors in relation to relatively simple
songs from the singer’s musical culture.