Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

Sports Motivation - Mental Toughness Grit and Motivational Differences in Single-Sp

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 72

University of Arkansas, Fayetteville

ScholarWorks@UARK
Theses and Dissertations

8-2016

Mental Toughness, Grit, and Motivational


Differences in Single-sport and Multi-sport
Athletes
Cole Justin Shewmake
University of Arkansas, Fayetteville

Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarworks.uark.edu/etd


Part of the Exercise Science Commons, and the Health and Physical Education Commons

Recommended Citation
Shewmake, Cole Justin, "Mental Toughness, Grit, and Motivational Differences in Single-sport and Multi-sport Athletes" (2016).
Theses and Dissertations. 1672.
http://scholarworks.uark.edu/etd/1672

This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UARK. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by
an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UARK. For more information, please contact scholar@uark.edu.
Mental Toughness, Grit, and Motivational Differences in Single-sport and Multi-sport Athletes

A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of


the requirements for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy in Kinesiology

by

Cole Shewmake
Pittsburg State University
Bachelor of Science in Physical Education, 2004
Pittsburg State University
Master of Science in Health, Human Performance and Recreation, 2008

August 2016
University of Arkansas

This dissertation is approved for recommendation to the Graduate Council.

____________________________________
Dr. Dean Gorman
Dissertation Director

____________________________________ ____________________________________
Dr. Sharon Hunt Dr. Cathy Lirgg
Committee Member Committee Member

____________________________________
Dr. Wen-Juo Lo
Committee Member
Abstract

Whether it is at work, school or sports, mental toughness (MT), grit and motivation are

essential to accomplishing goals. Sports can be an integral platform for developing MT, grit, and

factors that increase motivation levels, and it is during childhood and adolescence that these

characteristics may develop. All of this could prove beneficial into adulthood. Therefore, the

purpose of this study was to examine MT, grit and motivational orientations of single-sport and

multi-sport athletes at different levels of competition. Considering MT, a 2 x 2 ANOVA

indicated no significant interaction effect between athlete type and competition level, F(1, 387) =

.06, p = .812, partial η2 < .001, but significant main effects for athlete type, F(1, 387) = 4.87, p =

.028, partial η2 = .012, and competition level, F(1, 387) = 17.33, p < .001, partial η2 = .043.

Furthermore, regarding grit, a 2 x 2 ANOVA indicated no significant interaction effect between

athlete type and competition level, F(1, 382) = .32, p = .571, partial η2 = .001, and no

significance for the athlete type main effect, F(1, 382) = 1.22, p = .270, partial η2 = .003.

However, a significant main effect was found for competition level, F(1, 382) = 5.42, p = .020,

partial η2 = .014. As for motivation, a 2 x 2 ANOVA indicated a significant interaction effect

between athlete type and competition level F(1, 373) = 4.11, p = .043, partial η2 = .011 and a

significant main effect for competition level F(1, 373) = 9.50, p = .002, partial η2 = .025.

However, there was no significant finding for the type of athlete, F(1, 373) = .76, p = .385,

partial η2 = .002. Examining the simple main effects for competition level, it was discovered that

single-sport athletes only achieving high school level playing experience scored significantly

lower than single-sport high school athletes that went on to play collegiately, F(1, 373) = 13.61,

p < .001. Moreover, multi-sport high school athletes scored significantly higher than single-sport

high school athletes, F(1, 373) = 5.42, p = .020.


©2016 by Cole Shewmake
All Rights Reserved
Acknowledgements

This doctoral dissertation would not have been possible without the help and support of

several individuals around me, of a few I would like to acknowledge below.

To begin with, I feel extremely blessed by God with a tremendously supportive family.

Without the Peace of my Savior and the steadfast encouragement from my family, mentors,

colleagues, and friends along the way, I would not have been able to accomplish this notable

task.

Second, I would like to specifically thank my advisor and dissertation chair, Dr. Dean

Gorman. From the very beginning you have been extremely supportive and encouraging. I

always knew you were on my side! Next, I want to thank the rest of my dissertation committee.

Dr. Sharon Hunt, you are such a sweet and encouraging individual. Thank you for always

boosting my confidence with your positive words. Dr. Cathy Lirgg, thank you for the direction

you provided throughout the whole doctoral process. Your insight was always valuable. Dr. Lo,

thank you for your patience, time, and energy in giving me statistical advice for not just my

dissertation but in class and on other projects as well.

Additionally, I would like to thank Dr. Paul Calleja, Dr. Jack Kern, and Dr. Amanda

Sullivan for your wisdom and support while completing my graduate work. You all treated me

like a colleague and not just a lowly student. Dr. John Oppliger, thank you for your wisdom as I

started this process and for your on-going mentoring as a full-time professor. Without your

encouragement and guidance, I would not be where I am today!

Lastly, I want to thank Mike Merrie. My family and I moved to Fayetteville not knowing

anyone, and you made the transition so much easier as a new friend. I can’t tell you how much it
meant to have someone going through this process with me, encouraging me along the way. The

end is near and you were right, “I got this Bro!”


Dedication

First and foremost, I would like to dedicate my dissertation work to my wife, Jennifer.

Without your constancy, encouragement, and support, I would not have started this venture, let

alone complete it! Thank you! Additionally, I would like to dedicate this dissertation my kids,

Selah, Noa, and Creed. Often, you had to be without dad while I worked. During those times, I

missed you as much as you may have missed Daddy! And I would like to add a dedication

mention to baby Shewmake, I can’t wait to meet you! Lastly, I would like to dedicate this to my

parents, Buck and Elaine Shewmake. Thanks for your continual support and for instilling in me a

work ethic to persistently pursue my aspirations. Thank you all!


Table of Contents

I. INTRODUCTION ………………………………………………………………………. 1

Significance of the Study ………………………………………………………………... 4

Purpose of the Study ………………………..…………………………………………… 5

Research Questions .……………………………………………………………………... 7

Definitions of Terms …………………………………………………………………….. 7

II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ………………………………………………………..… 11

Youth Sports …………………………………………………………………………… 11

Sport Specialization ……………………………………………………………………. 14

Sport Diversification …………………………………………………………………… 16

Mental Toughness ……………………………………………………………………… 17

Grit ……………………………………………………………………………………... 21

Motivation ……………………………………………………………………………… 22

Mental Toughness, Grit, and Motivation Questions …………………………………… 25

III. METHODS …………………………………………………………………………….. 28

Participants ……………………………………………………………………………... 28

Procedure …………………………………………………………………………….… 28

Measures ...……………………………………………………………………………... 28

Demographics ………………………………………………………………………….. 29

Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ) …………………………………….. 29

Grit-S …………………………………………………………………………………… 30

Sports Motivation Scale-6 (SMS-6) ………………………………………………….… 31

Statistical Analysis ……………………………………………………………………... 31


IV. RESULTS ……………………………………………………………………………… 33

Mental Toughness …………………………………………………………………….... 33

Grit …………………………………………………………………………………...… 33

Motivation ……………………………………………………………………………… 35

V. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS ……………………………………………...… 38

Discussion and Research Findings …………………………………………………….. 38

Mental Toughness ……………………………………………………………………… 38

Grit ………………………………………………………………………………...…… 39

Motivation ……………………………………………………………………………… 40

Delimitations …………………………………………………………………………… 42

Limitations ……………………………………………………………………………... 42

Assumptions ……………………………………………………………………………. 43

Recommendations for Future Research ……………………………………………...… 43

VI. REFERENCES ………………………………………………………………………… 46

VII. APPENDICES …………………………………………………………………………. 54

Appendix A: Research Study Consent Form …………………………………………... 54

Appendix B: IRB Approval Letter ……………………………………………………... 55

Appendix C: Figure C1: Pie chart for age of participants …………………………...… 56

Appendix C: Figure C2: Pie chart of sports played by participants ……………............ 57

Appendix C: Figure C3: Pie chart of competition level achieved by participants …….. 58

Appendix D: Demographics and sports history questionnaire …………………............ 59

Appendix E: Figure E1: Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ) ……..……. 60

Appendix E: Figure E2: Short Grit Scale (Grit-S) Questionnaire ………..… ………… 61
Appendix E: Figure E3: Sport Motivation Scale (SMS-6) ………………..…………… 62

VIII. TABLES AND FIGURES

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Mental Toughness, Grit, and Motivation ……......... 34

Figure 1. Error Bars for Externalized and Internalized motivation for athlete type …… 36
CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Sports have become a vital part of the lives of youth and their families, sometimes to an

obsession. Significant time is poured into youth sports starting at young ages. Children, as young

as 10 years old, will sometimes travel nationally to compete, playing in tournaments multiple

weekends throughout the year (Butler, 2011; Keown, 2011). The financial commitment is intense

as well, with some households spending as much as 10 percent of the family’s gross income on

travel, private coaching, and equipment (Sullivan, 2015).

One phenomenon that is seen today is youth choosing to play a single sport at early ages

(Ferguson & Stern, 2014). Many times, the adolescent, or his or her parent, has decided that it is

in the adolescent’s best interest to focus on one sport in order to maximize success in that

particular sport and attain an athletic scholarship (Malina, 2010; Wojtys, 2013). Armour (2015)

provides an example of a 12 year old that has his eyes set on being an NFL quarterback. He

attends football camps, works with a private quarterback coach, and even has an online athletic

profile touting his talents. The potential for achieving expert status at the earliest possible age is

very attractive to those that choose to specialize in a sport (Ferguson & Stern, 2014). However,

while focusing on one sport, youth may be missing out on valuable experiences that can be

gained from participating in multiple sports.

The disadvantages, as well as the advantages, of youth specialization in sports are well

known. Mental and physical disadvantages cited are participant burnout and overuse injury

(DiFiori et al., 2014; Gould, 2010). According to DiFiori et al. (2014), sport specialization puts

an individual at “high risk” to develop overuse injuries at various parts of the body like the

shoulder, elbow, hip/pelvis, lower back, knee, lower leg, ankle, and foot. Likewise, when

1
specializing, youth experience added pressure to win, mental stress from longer practice sessions

and seasons, and boredom of a sport after playing for several years (Woods, 2016).

While many youth choose to specialize in a single sport, others choose to diversify their

sport experiences. Choosing to play multiple sports as a child has few disadvantages, but a

couple of the perceived disadvantages include falling behind peers in skill development and

becoming over-involved in activities having negative implications like decreased academic

performance (American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Dance

[AAHPERD], 2013; Gould & Carson, 2004). The advantages to participating in multiple sports

throughout adolescence has numerous benefits (National Association of Youth Sports [NASPE],

2010). These benefits include greater overall skill development, more opportunity for social

development, skill transferability to other sports, and a longer sports career (Bailey, 2006; Baker,

Cobley, Fraser-Thomas, 2009; Gould, 2010; NASPE, 2010). With the idea that skills transfer

from sport to sport, several elite and professional figures have promoted the multi-sport track.

For example, a survey by ESPN found that 95% of NFL quarterbacks were at least two sport

athletes in high school (Seifert, 2015). Furthermore, Abby Waumbach, arguably one of the best

women’s soccer players today, credits basketball with some of her success in soccer (Rogers,

2015). Urban Myer, head football coach of The Ohio State Buckeyes, has expressed his favor of

multi-sport athletes (O’Sullivan, 2015). Finally, in 2014, 88 percent of Olympic athletes

surveyed felt participating in multiple sports helped them become better at their main sport

(Riewald & Snyder, 2014). Additionally, 71 percent of the Olympians considered themselves

multi-sport athletes through high school before specializing.

Affective benefits to diversifying in sports during adolescence have been examined

(AAHPERD, 2013). However, little is known concerning the constructs of mental toughness, grit

2
and motivational orientations in single-sport and multi-sport athletes. Mental toughness (MT)

may mean various things to different people (see Gucciardi, Hanton, Gordon, Mallett, & Temby,

2015 for review). When an individual talks about someone being mentally tough they could be

referring to one’s ability to address pain effectively (Bull, Shambrook, James, & Brooks, 2005),

to not give up (Jaeschke & Sachs, 2012), or to perform successfully in stressful situations (Jones,

Hanton, & Connaughton, 2002). Like MT, grit is connected to adversity (Duckworth & Gross,

2014). For instance, there is a popular billboard with a picture of John Wayne (i.e. the actor that

played Rooster Cogburn in the classic movie True Grit) that states, “Don’t much like quitters,

son”, with the phrase “GRIT Pass It On” printed at the bottom (“Grit,” n.d.). Being gritty is about

not quitting, however there is more to it. Grittiness can also be used to describe someone that is

diligent and determined over an extended period of time (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews &

Kelly, 2007).

Being mentally tough, as well as being gritty, may explain why some individuals succeed

while taking on a task when most cannot. For example, as discussed in a 2013 news article by

Sloane, Hanna, & Ford, Diana Nyad had a dream of swimming from Cuba to Florida. On her

fifth attempt at the age of 64, she successfully completed her goal having to overcome numerous

environmental and psychological obstacles. In a similar fashion, motivation answers the “why”

question, but instead of explaining why one is successful, it answers why one would participate.

In Nyad’s case, she set her aims high early in her life. Once she aged and her mother passed

away, she decided that she wanted to commit to her aspiration of the long swim. In her mind, she

was motivated to complete it before she got any older.

Finally, according to Ryan and Deci’s (2000a) Self-Determination Theory, an individual

can be intrinsically motivated, extrinsically motivated or amotivated. Individuals that participate

3
in activities for the pleasure of doing so, or for a sense of accomplishment, are said to be

intrinsically motivated. Those that are seeking an external reward are extrinsically motivated,

and sometimes individuals are not motivated at all – amotivated.

Significance of the Study

People encounter multiple situations that require high levels of MT and grit every day.

Likewise, motivation plays a significant role in everyday life. Experiences requiring adolescents

to be mentally tough or gritty could prove beneficial to them as they mature into adulthood.

Obviously, the quality of our experiences plays a huge role in developing MT and grit

(Connaughton, Hanton, & Jones, 2010). To an extent, motivation levels can be affected by the

quality of our experiences as well (Álvarez, Balaguer, Castillo, & Duda, 2009). But, what about

the quantity of our experiences? One would think the greater the variety of stressful situations to

which one is exposed, the greater the opportunity to learn how to best cope with that stressful

condition. It stands to reason, the more exposure a person has to stressful experiences, the better

equipped that person will be in effectively facing troublesome incidents as they arise

(Connaughton, Wadey, Hanton, & Jones, 2008). As the expression goes, “there’s nothing like

experience.” Simply put, exposure to a variety of demanding situations may provide an

opportunity to build confidence in consistently controlling stressful circumstances.

Stressful conditions in sports occur abundantly. The opportunity to play multiple sports

provides greater exposure to a variety of situations than playing just one sport (NASPE, 2010).

Thus, the individual may be allotted greater opportunity to develop MT and grit (Thelwell,

Weston, & Greenless, 2005). This could prove beneficial to them later in life. However, too

much stress can create an unpleasant setting that is exhaustive (DiFiori et al., 2014). Thus, when

4
one experiences burnout, it may result in amotivation and the individual giving up the activity

(DiFiori et al., 2014).

Purpose of the Study

Being extremely popular today, sports have become a tremendous avenue for adolescents

to develop skills they can carry on throughout their lifetime (AAHPERD, 2013; National Council

of Youth Sports [NCYS], 2008). Teamwork, persistence, resiliency, character and leadership are

all life skills that can be enhanced through participation in youth sports (Theokas, 2009). Thus,

sports provides a pathway to develop these life skills (AAHPERD, 2013)

Hardiness and resiliency have both been linked to MT and grit (Clough, Earl, & Sewell,

2002; Kelly, Matthews, & Bartone, 2014). In addition, perseverance and hard work, qualities

needed to complete goals despite setbacks, are attributes connected to mentally tough and gritty

individuals (Duckworth & Gross, 2014; Jaeschke & Sachs, 2012; Winerman, 2013). Motivation

has been associated with MT and grit as well. According to Jones, Hanton, & Connaughton

(2002), individuals that are mentally tough are highly motivated from deep within to succeed.

Additionally, Middleton, Marsh, Martin, Richards, and Perry (2004) maintained that MT motives

originate internally. Similarly, grit has been tied to motivation (Duckworth & Gross, 2014).

Moreover, Hochanadel and Finamore (2015) suggest that grit can be developed through

internalizing the motivation for participation. Lastly, in a study by J. Reed (2104), it was noted

that gritty individuals look for activities with internal rewards when selecting physical activity.

MT characteristics such as hardiness, resiliency, and perseverance can be related to sport

(Bawa, 2010; Ramzi & Besharat, 2010; Theokas, 2009). These MT attributes can be observed

when sport offers adverse conditions allowing one the possibility to learn how to cope with

stressful situations (Hedstrom & Gould, 2004; Russell & Limle, 2013). Also, sports provide the

5
opportunity to understand and develop work ethic. According to Duckworth and Gross (2014),

spending years or more dedicated to a task may develop grit within an individual. Likewise,

developing a gritty mentality allows one to be resilient in difficult situations. Finally, motivation

plays a huge role in sports. Children first start playing sports because they are interested in them

and enjoy them (Sage & Eitzen, 2013; Watts, 2002). This intrinsic motivation leads to other

advantages from sport like psychological and skill development (AAHPERD, 2013; Côté, Lidor,

& Hackfort, 2009). In addition to intrinsic motivation, individuals may become externally

motivated as well, by striving to maintain a healthy lifestyle for the benefits of overall health

(Fraser-Thomas & Côté, 2006).

Specialization versus diversification in sport is frequently discussed in today’s sporting

world, as can be seen by several professional organizations issuing position statements regarding

the topic (Ferguson & Stern, 2014). Likewise, MT, grit and motivation are highly discussed

topics within physical activity and sport (Álvarez, Balaguer, Castillo, & Duda, 2009; Butt,

Weinberg, & Culp, 2010; Connaughton, Hanton, & Jones, 2010; Crust, 2007; Fraser-Thomas &

Côté, 2006; Jalili, Hosseini, Jalili, & Salehian, 2011; Kelly, Matthews, & Bartone, 2014; Li,

Wang, Pyun, & Kee, 2013; Mahoney, Gucciardi, Ntoumanis, & Mallett, 2014; Middleton et al.,

2004; Readdy, Raabe, & Harding, 2014; J. Reed 2014; Thelwell, Weston, & Greenless, 2005).

During an investigation of elite sport performers, Jones, Hanton, and Connaughton (2002)

highlighted a connection between MT and motivation. Similarly, Hochanadel & Finamore

(2015), as well as Duckworth and Gross (2014), associated grit with motivation. However, there

has been no past research investigating the relationships between MT, grit, or motivation

between single-sport and multi-sport athletes specifically. Therefore, the purpose of this study is

6
to examine MT, grit and motivational orientations of single-sport athletes and multi-sport

athletes at different levels of competition.

Research Questions

In order to examine MT, grit and motivation in athletes at different levels of competition,

the following research questions were developed to guide the research: 1) Do single-sport

athletes or athletes participating in multiple sports throughout high school score higher in MT as

measured by the Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ)? 2) Do single-sport athletes or

athletes participating in multiple sports throughout high school score higher in grit as measured

by the Short Grit Scale (Grit-S)? 3) Are there motivational differences between single-sport and

multi-sport athletes as measured by the Sport Motivation Scale-6 (SMS-6)? 4) Is there a

difference in MT between those that played a sport collegiately or above and those that did not

play sports past high school? 5) Is there a difference in grit between those that played a sport

collegiately or above and those that did not play sports past high school? 6) Are there

motivational differences between those that played a sport collegiately or above and those that

did not play sports past high school?

Definition of Terms

There are a handful of terms that need to be clarified. For the purpose of this study, the

following definitions will be used for the terms below.

Amotivation: lack of motivation; the state of lacking an intention to act (Ryan & Deci,

2000a).

Autonomy: having a personal “will” to act. Autonomy within Self-Determination Theory

does not mean being selfish or independent, but instead refers to an individual volition to act

(Ryan & Deci, 2000b).

7
Diversification (sport): sampling or participating in multiple sports throughout

childhood and adolescence (Wiersma, 2000). Sport diversification often leads to a more well-

rounded individual athlete.

External regulation: behaviors performed to satisfy an external demand or obtain an

externally imposed reward contingency. External regulation is classified as coming from an

external perceived locus of causality (Ryan & Deci, 2000a).

Externalized motivation: motivation that is said to be from an external perceived locus

of causality. Associated process of externalized motivation include external regulation and

introjected regulation (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Externalized motivation is not to be confused with

extrinsic motivation.

Extrinsic motivation: attempting a task in order to attain a separable outcome (Ryan &

Deci, 2000a). Extrinsically motivated individuals pursue an activity, not because it is inherently

interesting, but in order to gain something from it (Ryan, Williams, Patrick, & Deci, 2009).

Extrinsic motivation is not to be confused with externalized motivation.

Grit: perseverance and passion for long-term goals (Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews &

Kelly, 2007).

Gritty: displaying perseverance and passion for long-term goals.

Identified regulation: motivated by the personal importance seen in the behavior.

Identified regulation is classified as coming from a somewhat internal perceived locus of

causality (Ryan & Deci, 2000a).

Integrated regulation: the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation. It occurs

when an individual has assimilated extrinsically motivating factors with one’s own personal

values. Many qualities of integrated regulation are shared with intrinsic motivation. Integrated

8
regulation is classified as coming from an internal perceived locus of causality (Ryan & Deci,

2000a).

Internalized motivation: motivation that is said to be from an internal perceived locus

of causality. Associated processes of internalized motivation include identified regulation,

integrated regulation, and intrinsic motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Internalized motivation is

not to be confused with intrinsic motivation.

Intrinsic motivation: doing something because it is inherently interesting or enjoyable

(Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Factors associated with intrinsic motivation include conscious valuing of

activity, hierarchical synthesis of goals, interest, enjoyment, and inherent satisfaction. Intrinsic

motivation is not to be confused with internalized motivation.

Introjected regulation: motivation stemming from ego or pride; can be caused by the

pressure to perform in order to avoid guilt or anxiety. Introjected regulation is classified as

coming from a somewhat external perceived locus of causality (Ryan & Deci, 2000a).

Mental toughness: the emotional and psychological ability to withstand, or recover

from, setbacks by staying focused, controlled, and committed during an adverse task or

condition. Mentally tough individuals are thought to be emotionally resilient, confident, focused

and able to cope with pressure (Jones et al., 2002; Loehr, 1995; Clough, Earl & Sewell, 2002;

Jones & Moorhouse, 2008).

Motivation: to be moved to do something (Ryan & Deci, 2000a).

Multi-sport athlete: An athlete that finished high school playing two or more sports

(Definition utilized by the researcher.)

Perceived locus of causality: According to Self-Determination Theory, one’s perceived

locus of causality can be attributed to one’s sense of autonomy (Ryan & Deci, 2000b).

9
Single-sport athlete: An athlete that finished high school playing only one sport

(Definition utilized by the researcher.)

Specialization (sport): year round training in a specific sport with the exclusion of other

sports at a young age (Ferguson & Stern, 2014).

10
CHAPTER 2

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

From the professional ranks to the pee-wee leagues, sports in American culture are

extremely popular. A little over 60 million youth participate in sports every year (NCYS, 2008).

Adolescents grow up admiring their favorite teams and players. They see them performing on

television or in person, and they believe with a little dedication, they too can reach the elite level.

So, just like their heroes, they start at an early age playing the games they love in hopes of one

day being the next Peyton Manning, LeBron James, or Bryce Harper.

Youth Sports

There are several benefits to adolescents participating in youth sports. These benefits

include learning new skills and improving on existing skills to become better at something,

having the chance to be around friends and make new ones, and having the opportunity to

discover one’s own strengths and weaknesses (AAHPERD, 2013; Watts, 2002). But, most

importantly, kids just want to have fun (Sage & Eitzen, 2013; Watts, 2002)!

It is obvious that participating in a sport provides physical benefits to the individual

(AAHPERD, 2013). Adolescents playing sports can see improvements in both health-related and

performance-related fitness measures (National Strength and Conditioning Association [NSCA],

2009). This fitness during youth lays the foundation for their adult life. In fact, adolescents

participating in sports are more likely to carry on their positive physical activity habits into

adulthood (Bailey, 2006). Additionally, adolescents who participate in youth sports are reported

to have a higher metabolic rate as adults (Yang, Telama, Hirvensalo, Viikari, & Raitakari, 2009).

The expansion of physical development attributes have also been found to be beneficial to

adolescents playing sports (NASPE, 2010).

11
While participating in sports, adolescents can learn social skills that can be useful to them

later in life, and these social skills allow them to learn sportsmanship, cooperation skills, respect

for others and how to build strong relationships with their peers (Bailey, 2006). Moreover,

Cranmer and Myers (2015) indicated sport allows adolescents to develop communication skills.

Effective communication with a teammate or coach, and accountability to those individuals, are

skills similar to interacting with colleagues in a professional work place. Understanding that one

is accountable to others may lead some to rise to the occasion and set higher expectations for

themselves, thus uniting everyone in an overall purpose (Gould & Carson, 2004). Learning to

strive towards a common goal with other individuals is of the upmost importance in preparing

for a professional working career (Messmer, 2003). Overall, sports may provide a setting where

kids can learn to be good teammates (Theokas, 2009).

Along with physical and social benefits, maybe more importantly, affective benefits may

be realized while participating in sports (AAHPERD, 2013; Wiersma, 2000). Sports can teach

things such as responsibility, persistence, dedication, and discipline (Theokas, 2009). Learning

responsibility in fulfilling one’s duties is a similar characteristic to being accountable to others.

The responsibility of being on time, caring for your equipment, and generally, fulfilling your

duties are essential skills to being an effective employee (Robles, 2012). It takes discipline and

dedication to learn these valuable skills. With the development of the specific skills, increased

confidence in oneself can be realized (DiFiori et al., 2014; AAHPERD, 2013; Watts, 2002). The

more responsible an individual becomes, the greater the confidence he or she will have in his or

her abilities (AAHPERD, 2013). This, in turn, can boost one’s self-esteem. However, failures are

sure to happen, but this isn’t necessarily a bad thing. Experiencing failure provides the

opportunity to persevere through difficult situations (Thelwell et al., 2005). Learning from one’s

12
mistakes by positively bouncing back is a skill that can prove beneficial later in life (Bailey,

2006). Finally, we can learn humility in sports (Austin, 2014). Success will come and go.

Sometimes it is during our failures that humility understood, which helps an individual to better

realize how to be humble when one wins (AAHPERD, 2013). Being humble is a characteristic

that can foster success, and in general, it makes us more enjoyable to be around. Learning these

characteristics about oneself aids in developing one’s personality (Bailey, 2006).

Participating in sports provides opportunity for cognitive development as well. In Prosser

and Jiang’s (2008) review of literature investigating the relationship between physical activity

and academic performance, it was noted that, while some studies found no significant

correlations between physical activity and academic performance, there are multiple research

studies that have found a positive relationship between the two (see Prosser and Jiang’s article

“Relationship between school physical activity and academic performance of children” for full

list of suggested studies). Because of this, Prosser and Jiang (2008) were cautious in their

conclusion pointing out the weak correlation in some studies. However, their literature review

produced more findings suggesting a relationship between physical activity and academic

performance than against. Further review of literature produced more research positively

connecting physical activity and athletic performance. When comparing athletes to non-athletes,

athletes tend to have better grades, test scores, and attendance (AAHPERD, 2013; Watts, 2002).

It has also been noted that problem solving skills can be positively influenced by sport activity.

For example, youth participating in sport are able to increase their strategic skills, generating

plans and applications for successful completion of objectives (Baker, 2003). An increase in

mental alertness, attention and working memory have been attributed to sports as well

(AAHPERD, 2013).

13
Sport Specialization

A popular phenomenon within sport culture today is focusing on one sport during

adolescence (Ferguson & Stern, 2014; Hedstrom & Gould, 2004). This is happening at younger

and younger ages. Children can be seen specializing in one sport as early as age nine (K. Reed,

2014). The reasons for specialization vary. The most obvious explanation of an individual

choosing to specialize in a sport is for mastery of skills (Ferguson & Stern, 2014; Gould, 2010;

Wiersma, 2000). Back in the early 1980’s, Newell and Rosenbloom (1981) discovered a positive

relationship between achievement and practice time. In fact, research by Andres Ericsson and

colleagues (1993) concluded that in order to achieve expert status, individuals must dedicate

themselves to 10,000 hours of deliberate practice of a skill for a minimum of 10 years. This

allows individuals greater early skill acquisition, allowing them to gain a competitive edge

(Ferguson & Stern, 2014; Wiersma, 2000).

Sometimes individuals are not as worried about achieving elite status as they are about

falling behind their peers (Gould, 2010). Particular demands of sports influence whether youth

decide to specialize or not (DiFiori et al., 2014). Some activities, like soccer and baseball, are

very sport specific. For example, the specificity of foot-eye coordination involved in soccer is

very unique (Neto, Barbieri, Barbieri, & Gobbi, 2009). Likewise, the timing involved in hitting a

baseball or softball is exclusive to those sports and requires great time and dedication to master

(Epstein, 2013). Even if mastery isn’t the ultimate goal, individuals may feel they will fall behind

their peers if they choose not to specialize in a sport at a young age (Gould, 2010).

As children get older, many start dreaming of playing for their high school, collegiately,

or even professionally (Gould, 2010). By choosing to specialize, they believe they will have a

better chance of realizing those dreams (Hill, 1991; Watts, 2002). Sometimes it may not even be

14
the child who is choosing to specialize, but it is the parents who have made that decision (Gould,

2010). Some parents decide it is in the best interest of their child to focus on one sport in hopes

of realizing the prized college athletic scholarship (Hill, 1991). Coaches, too, see the benefits of

kids specializing. Many coaches today will encourage youngsters to focus on one sport, because

they do not want to share time with another sport (Watts, 2002). They believe the more

exclusively they have their individual athletes, the better the team will become and, likewise,

will have a better chance of winning (Watts, 2002).

While there are advantages to specializing in one sport at an early age, there has been

numerous literature discussing the disadvantages to it (Branta, 2010; DiFiori et al., 2014; Hill,

1991; NASPE, 2010; Watts, 2002; Wiersma, 2000). The two most cited reasons against sport

specialization are athlete burnout and overuse injury (DiFiori et al., 2014; Watts, 2002).

Mentally, adolescents are burned out and no longer wish to participate. An example of this

comes from a study by Shibko (2015), which found adolescents who specialize in a single sport

before the age of 10 are more likely to drop out than those who wait to specialize. Physically,

adolescents wear out as well. Repetitive stress on the same muscles, ligaments, tendons, and

joints causes the body to become vulnerable, and thus, overuse injuries occur (DiFiori et al.,

2014). As someone plays the same sport year round, the same body tissues and structures are

getting overloaded with stress without an opportunity to recuperate (Hollander, Meyers, &

LeUnes, 1995). Therefore, experts suggest that adolescents need an offseason to recuperate

(NASPE, 2010). Even with this advice, sometimes both parents and coaches will encourage

young athletes to focus on one sport year round (Hedstrom & Gould, 2004; Malina, 2010). With

parents and coaches asking young athletes to specialize, it places tremendous stress on the

adolescent to not disappoint (Wiersma, 2000).

15
Sport Diversification

The benefits to specialization that were previously discussed naturally can be seen as

disadvantages to sport diversification. It is believed by some that youth choosing to play multiple

sports throughout the year are at risk of falling behind their peers in specific skill development

due to not devoting all of one’s time to a given sport (Ferguson & Stern, 2014). This purported

“lack of expertise” hinders those diversifying from keeping up; the time spent playing a second

or third sport is thought to diminish the development of specific skills of the first sport

(Wiersma, 2000).

While specialization in sport has become more common (Ferguson & Stern, 2014), there

are still some youth who choose to diversify themselves in their sporting endeavors, and the

reasons for doing so are numerous. Sport diversification allows one to develop as a well-rounded

athlete (Griffin, 2008). It provides the opportunity to experience multiple skill demands,

coaching personalities, and teammate interactions (NASPE, 2010).

There is a lot that goes into becoming a well-rounded athlete. There are physical, social,

and cognitive benefits to playing multiple sports (NASPE, 2010). Athletes diversifying in sports

have the opportunity to balance out activities throughout the year, reducing the chance of both

burnout and overuse injury (NASPE, 2010). Physically, humans need rest. As an adolescent

dabbles in different sports, he or she is able to stay active, while not repetitively putting stress on

the same body parts (DiFiori et al., 2014). Similarly, the body benefits physically from

expanding the possible movements encountered while playing multiple sports (Branta, 2010).

According to Gallahue, Ozmun, and Goodway (2012), it is very important that young people are

provided a variety of experiences when it comes to motor skill development. These experiences

allow for development of fundamental motor skills (Branta, 2010); specialized skills needed for

16
sport are dependent on proficiency of fundamental movement skills (Gallahue et al., 2012).

Finally, adolescents, and adults as well, need time to let their bodies rest and recover (DiFiori et

al., 2014). Participating in multiple sports throughout the year limits overuse injury, since a

variety of sports require different sport specific muscles and body parts like those involved in the

overhand throw in baseball (NASPE, 2010).

Participating in multiple sports provides an opportunity for social benefits as well

(NASPE, 2010). Being involved in multiple sports, provides the opportunity to develop

relationships with a variety of individuals, such as peers and authority figures (NASPE, 2010).

As an adult, one will interact with a variety of individuals, especially in the workplace. While

playing multiple sports, there is the possibility of experiencing varying coaching styles and

personalities (Theokas, 2009). Being given the chance to develop a variety of relationships

provides for greater social development (Bailey, 2006).

Cognitive benefits can be found from playing multiple sports as well. Exposure to an

assortment of strategies from differing sports could allow for more creativity from sport to sport

due to the increased ability to process incoming information from a variety of situations (Baker

et al., 2009). Strategies and conceptual skills may carry over and be useful in another sport (Hill,

1991).

Mental Toughness (MT)

An often reported study by Gould, Hodge, Peterson, and Petlichkoff (1987) found that 82

percent of wrestling coaches stated MT as the most important characteristic in their athletes’

success. In his book, “Mental Toughness Training for Sports: Achieving Athletic Excellence”,

Loehr (1986) states characteristics making up MT as being responsible for 50 percent of the

success found in athletics. Those who have been around sports have probably heard the term MT

17
being used. It is a term that is often applied in connection with sports, but when it is used, there

are frequently different meanings in mind.

The definition of MT has been highly discussed over the past decade or two (see Gucciardi,

et al., 2015 for review). With varying definitions being used by coaches and athletes, researchers

have spent the last decade attempting to nail down a definition. In literature addressing MT in

sport, the most utilized definition is by Jones et al. (2002). In this definition, MT is both innate

(natural) and developed within an individual. These researchers define mental toughness as the

natural or developed psychological edge that enables one to:

 Generally, cope better than your opponent with many demands (competition, training,

lifestyle) that sport places on a performer.

 Specifically, be more consistent and better than your opponents in remaining determined,

focused, confident, and in control under pressure.

Research by Thelwell et al. (2005) employed the same definition as Jones and colleagues (2002)

with one exception. Thelwell et al. (2005) concluded that mentally tough individuals always

cope better than their opponents with demands that sport places on a performer. Words and

concepts highlighted in both of these definitions include coping, consistency, and taking on

pressure. Moreover, Gucciardi et al. (2015) defined MT as the personal capacity to produce

consistently high levels of subjective or objective performance despite everyday challenges and

stressors as well as significant adversities. Like Jones et al. (2002) and Thelwell et al. (2005),

consistency is used in the definition of MT. Additionally, Gucciardi et al. (2015) mention in their

definition that mentally tough performers can overcome adversity. One last definition presents a

few additional characteristics of a mentally tough person. Middleton et al. (2004) also discuss

facing adversity and taking on pressure, and these researchers have the only definition that

18
mentions perseverance; perseverance is often referenced when describing someone that is

mentally tough (Jaeschke & Sachs, 2012).

Along with defining MT, several models have been created featuring characteristics of

individuals that exhibit MT. In his book “The New Toughness Training for Sports”, Dr. Loehr

(1995) outlined his four markers of toughness: emotional flexibility, emotional responsiveness,

emotional strength, and emotional resiliency. Clough, Earl and Sewell (2002) highlighted

commitment, control, challenge, and confidence as the 4 C’s model of MT. Motivation, self-

confidence, attentional focus, and coping with pressure have been tagged the four pillars of MT

by Jones and Moorhouse (2008). Connaughton et al. (2010) created a framework that described

which of the 30 total attributes they claim to affect MT should fall within four dimensions (i.e.

attitude/mindset, training, competition, and post-competition). Bull et al. (2005) generated a MT

pyramid. With this pyramid, upbringing establishes the base which is titled “environmental

influences”. It is this base that influences the next three levels of the pyramid – tough character,

tough attitudes, and tough thinking. Lastly, Middleton and colleagues (2004), after interviewing

25 elite-level athletes, discovered 12 characteristics of MT. These characteristics include self-

efficacy, mental self-concept, potential, task-specific attention, perseverance, task familiarity,

personal bests, task value, goal commitment, positivity, stress minimization, and positive

comparisons. This final model tackles that which has been the deficiency in past MT research:

the complete nature of MT. Specifically, it addresses the notion that MT can be influenced by

stable personality characteristics (i.e. self-belief, goal commitment) and more impressionable

MT actions (i.e. task-specific attention, stress minimization, potential).

Common themes emerging from the definitions and models of MT should be noted.

Emotional strength, tough character, tough attitude, commitment and the ability to minimize

19
stress were highlighted above (Bull et al., 2005; Clough et al., 2002; Loehr, 1995; Middleton et

al., 2004). This suggests that mentally tough individuals will display a strong disposition. Self-

confidence, self-efficacy, mental self-concept, and confidence are terms inferring that a mentally

tough person is self-aware (Clough et al., 2002; Jones & Moorhouse, 2007; Middleton et al.,

2004). To be mentally tough, one needs to be attentive to how an individual best functions in

completing tasks. Therefore, MT requires task-specific attention, attentional focus, and control

(Clough et al., 2002; Jones & Moorhouse, 2007; Middleton et al., 2004). Tough thinking,

emotional resiliency, emotional flexibility, coping, and perseverance are said to be important

(Bull et al., 2005; Jones & Moorhouse, 2007; Loehr, 1995; Middleton et al., 2004). Adapting to,

and fighting through, difficult situations are characteristics of someone with great MT.

Additional characteristics of MT throughout the literature include persistence and motivation

(Jaeschke & Sachs, 2012; Ryan & Deci, 2000a). In summary, literature suggests mentally tough

individuals consistently cope better than their opponents by continuing to be determined,

focused, confident, and controlled during stressful situations.

The nature vs nurture debate can apply to MT (Crust, 2007). Meaning, you are either

born mentally tough or you learn to become mentally tough. While some research findings state

the development of MT to be mostly shaped by a person’s experiences, others acknowledge that

it can be “trait-like” in nature (Bull et al., 2005; Jones et al., 2002; Thelwell et al., 2005).

Currently, the general thought process is that MT is guided by both experiences (learned) and

nature (innate) (Crust, 2007; Jones et al., 2002).

Externally, MT is swayed by the environmental conditions afforded to the individual

(Crust & Swann, 2011). Aspects of the environment that play a role include coaching style,

competition level, and the amount of time in the setting. According to Bull et al. (2005), the type

20
of attitude displayed by the coach may affect a player’s level of MT. It has been reported that an

enjoyable environment that affords the opportunity to display one’s skills aids in MT

development (Connaughton et al., 2008). Additionally, a competitive setting and a tough

physical environment can contribute to the development of MT (Butt et al., 2010; Connaughton

et al., 2008). According to Jones and Parker (2013), accumulating of years of experience

positively contributes to becoming MT. This supports Middleton et al. (2004) which found that

being accustomed to a specific type of environment leads to greater MT, as well as the finding

that when expending effort over time, MT increases (Mahoney et al., 2014).

Internally, MT is thought to be “trait-like” (Jones et al., 2002). It has been noted that MT

can be the result of an “internalized motive” within an individual (Jones et al., 2002; Ryan &

Deci, 2000a). Desire is another term that has been used, and as previously mentioned, MT comes

naturally to some (Jones et al., 2002). Lastly, MT is said to come from having “tough character”

(Bull et al., 2005). This is another example of MT being considered a personality trait that

someone acquires at birth.

Grit

The concept of grit has been around for over a century (James, 1907). Long ago

researchers identified passion and hard work as distinguishing factors of successful individuals

(Galton, 1892). Recently, grit has been defined as perseverance and passion for long-term goals

(Duckworth et al., 2007). Furthermore, “grit entails working strenuously toward challenges,

maintaining effort and interest over years despite failure, adversity, and plateaus in progress”

(Duckworth et al., 2007, pp. 1087-1088).

Several characteristics of gritty individuals have been discussed recently. Kelly,

Matthews, and Bartone (2014) have associated grit with hardiness, relating hardiness to a strong

21
feeling of commitment to an activity. Gritty individuals are said to display passion and

perseverance over time (Winerman, 2013). Perseverance for extended amounts of time requires

stamina to pursue those activities into the future (Duckworth et al., 2007). To maintain this level

of perseverance commands self-control. Thus, individuals that are said to be gritty display great

self-control and are diligent (Duckworth & Gross, 2014; Winerman, 2013). Additionally,

individuals with grit follow through with plans (Duckworth et al., 2007). This is done by

maintaining interest in an endeavor over time even when obstacles are present (Kelly et al.,

2014; Duckworth & Gross, 2014). In summary, gritty individuals are committed and can

persevere in tackling tasks that extend over a longer period of time.

Motivation

The term used to address the question of why people choose to participate in activities is

known as motivation. According to Merriam-Webster Collegiate Dictionary (2005), motivation

is a force or influence that causes someone to do something. The fact is, people experience

various motivational orientations. Sometimes, people engage in activities because they are

interested in them, and other times, individuals are looking for a separate outcome from

completing the activity (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Occasionally, it is a combination of both interest

in the activity and seeking a separate outcome that motivates an individual (Ryan & Deci,

2000a).

Dr. Richard Ryan and Dr. Edward Deci (2000b) are well-known in the psychology field

for the development of their theory on motivation, known as Self-Determination Theory (SDT).

According to SDT, humans have three basic psychological needs that are inherent in all

individuals and are considered essential for ideal development (Deci & Vansteenkiste, 2004).

These three needs are the need for autonomy, relatedness (connectedness), and competence

22
(Ryan & Deci, 2000a; Deci & Vansteenkiste, 2004). Autonomy in SDT does not necessarily

represent the need for independence, but rather, represents one’s craving to have control over

one’s will (Ryan & Deci, 2000c). This attitude of volition can be accomplished individually or

collectively (Van den Broeck, Vansteenkiste, De Witte, & Lens, 2008). The need for relatedness

states that humans innately want to feel connected to others, and it also suggests that individuals

need to feel like they belong in the world around them (Deci & Vansteenkiste, 2004). Lastly, the

need for competence signifies a desire to effectively master the world around them (Deci &

Vansteenkiste, 2004).

The central tenet of SDT is that humans are both extrinsically and intrinsically motivated

(Ryan et al., 2009) to pursue the three basic needs of autonomy, relatedness and competence.

Extrinsic motivation relates to action that is done to fulfill the need to accomplish a “separable

outcome” from the activity itself, whereas intrinsic motivation is action not to achieve external

rewards but that which is done for the shear enjoyment of participating in the activity (Ryan &

Deci, 2000a). SDT also defines a lack of motivation for an activity as amotivation (Ryan & Deci,

2000a).

In SDT, motivation falls along a continuum of no motivation (amotivation) to full

inherent satisfaction and joy of participating in an activity (intrinsic motivation) (Ryan & Deci,

2000a). In the middle of this are four types of extrinsic motivation: external regulation,

introjection, identification, and integration (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Levels of autonomy can differ

greatly within extrinsic motivation. This is recognized by the perceived locus of causality

(PLOC) for the behavior. PLOC was a concept introduced by Fritz Heider (1958) in a book

titled, “The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations.” A PLOC refers to how one internalizes a

motive (DeCharms, 1968). Sometimes an individual will play strictly for a tangible reward or to

23
avoid a consequence (i.e. external regulation), and other times an individual will participate to

accomplish a personal goal (i.e. integration). Therefore, identifying the level of extrinsic

motivation is done by considering the PLOC for the behavior.

According to SDT, extrinsic motivation can stem from the desire to obtain solely external

rewards (i.e. external regulation) to understanding the “value” of accomplishing a task (i.e.

integrated regulation) (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). External regulation is seen as the least autonomous

level of extrinsic motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). External demands drive externally regulated

actions. That is, an externally regulated behavior is performed to accomplish an external reward

(Ryan & Deci, 2000b). Introjected regulation is the next type of extrinsic motivation. Self-esteem

plays a large role in this type of extrinsic motivation. Actions to avoid guilt or anxiety, or to

boost pride, are seen as being introjected (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). This manner of egotistical

behavior is not fully internalized and as a result, is considered being “somewhat” external in

regards to one’s PLOC (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). The next type of extrinsic motivation is identified

regulation. This type of extrinsic motivation is considered to have a somewhat internal PLOC.

Here an individual values a behavior as personally important or satisfying (Ryan & Deci, 2000b).

The most autonomous type of extrinsic motivation is integrated regulation. It is often seen as

very similar to intrinsic motivation, but it is still a behavior that is performed for a separable

outcome. However, this separable outcome is seen as being assimilated with oneself (Ryan &

Deci, 2000b). For example, participating in a sport because that individual identifies oneself with

that activity. The sport is not performed for the enjoyment it brings, but rather, it is because one

feels it is a part of them as a person.

Lastly, according to Ryan et al. (2009), intrinsic motivation is closely associated with

feelings of autonomy. Individuals naturally seek out sporting activities that are enjoyable,

24
interesting, and challenging to them (Frederick & Ryan, 1995). As noted by Ryan and Deci

(2000a), being intrinsically motivated means one acts for reasons other than an external prompt,

pressure, or reward. This natural curiosity of acting on one’s interests allows for overall personal

growth. In short, intrinsically motivated individuals engage in an activity to fulfill the inherent

gratification that it brings (Ryan et al., 2009).

Mental Toughness, Grit & Motivation

MT, grit and motivation all play a significant role in the world of youth sports.

Motivation is important in the development of young athletes. Having an internal desire to

persevere is a characteristic of gritty individuals (Hochanadel & Finamore, 2015). Additionally,

Csikszentmihalyi, Rathunde, & Whalen, (1993) found that talent development is dependent on

an athlete being motivated to participate in his or her activity. However, past research has shown

that the personal characteristic of grit can predict success as much as talent does, if not more so

(Duckworth et al., 2007). Finally, according to J. Reed (2014), gritty individuals seek out

difficult activities that are also intrinsically rewarding.

In regards to psychological concepts such as MT and motivation, a wide range of

experiences and relationships has been beneficial (Gould, 2010). In contrast, specializing in one

sport has been shown to reduce self-determination, which stems from an environment that stifles

intrinsic motivation (Gould, 2010). Similarly, what starts out as internal motives for participation

can be transformed into external motives due to reduced enjoyment levels from the activity. In

other words, burnout from specializing in a single sport can change internal reasons for

participation into external motives (Fraser-Thomas & Côté, 2006). The same is true in the

opposite direction. What starts out as external motives for participation can be changed to

internal motives as the individual begins to take ownership of the activity. This “making it their

25
own” attitude stems from a sense of autonomy (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). As mentioned earlier, it is

often suggested that athletes narrow down their playing experiences (at the influence of their

parents or coaches) to focus on mastery of one sport, in order to attain an athletic scholarship

(Malina, 2010). This external pressure to do so can stifle autonomy, which in turn suppresses

motivations that started out as internal (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). Consequently, it would be

interesting to see if single-sport or multi-sport athletes, at different levels of sport, have varying

levels of motivation.

Often times, deliberate practice is linked to specialization in a single sport (Ericsson,

Krampe, & Tesch-Romer 1993). However, deliberate practice is not naturally motivating

(Ericsson et al., 1993). Conversely, unsystematic play establishes intrinsic motivation for an

activity (Côté, et al., 2009). To take it a step further, Côté (1999) suggested that multiple sports

settings were needed to develop the intrinsic motivation that would propel an athlete to the “next

level” of his or her sporting career. According to SDT, extrinsic motivation lies on a continuum

from external rewards to the internal value that is obtained from the activity (Ryan & Deci,

2000b). Therefore, this research will further investigate differences between motivational

perceived locus of causalities between multi-sport and single-sport athletes different competition

levels.

Researchers have asserted that MT characteristics are essential from sport to sport (Jones,

Hanton, & Connaughton, 2007), and Bull et al. (2005) raised the question of whether MT is

specific to certain sports, be it individual versus team sports or contact versus non-contact sports.

However, exploration concerning MT needs to be considered for single-sport and multi-sport

athletes and their attained level of competition.

26
Lastly, little research has been conducted examining grit and sports. Furthermore, no

research has examined grit and specialization or diversification in sport. Therefore, the same

questions for MT, regarding single-sport and multi-sport athletes and competition level, apply to

grit.

27
CHAPTER 3

METHODS

Participants

The participants for this study included 392 former high school athletes (236 males and

156 females) from individual and team sports. The sample came from students enrolled in

courses within a physical education department from three Midwestern universities. The ages of

participants ranged from 18 to 35, with an average age of 21.6 years (SD = 2.59). The

participants self-reported their former high school enrollments, which ranged from 50 to 6600,

with a median enrollment of 800. They participated in sports such as baseball, basketball, cross

country, football, golf, gymnastics, softball, soccer, tennis, track and field, and volleyball while

in high school. A consent form (see Appendix A for a copy of the consent to participate form) to

participate was approved by the researcher’s university institutional review board (IRB#15-05-

721) (see Appendix B for a copy of the IRB approval letter).

Procedure

The researcher contacted professors from three Midwestern Kinesiology Departments to

assist in administering the surveys. Consent forms, sports history surveys, and questionnaires

were hand delivered to a representative from the kinesiology departments from each university

that distributed the surveys to their colleagues. The questionnaires were administered at the end

of the class period. Prior to completion of the survey, participants were informed of the general

purpose of the research study and of their right to withdraw at any time with no penalty. The data

were stored in a manila envelope during travel and in a secure file cabinet upon return to the

researcher’s office.

Measures

28
Demographics. The survey instrument consisted of demographics questions (i.e. gender,

age, race) and questions designed to gather information on participants’ sports history. Sports

history questions included topics such as: sports played throughout the participants’ lifetime, the

number of years the participant played each sport, which sports were played competitively

during high school, and the highest level of competition played (i.e. freshman team, JV, varsity,

college) (see Appendix C, Figure C1 for age, Figure C2 for sports, and Figure C3 for

competition level breakdown of participants. Also see Appendix D for specific demographics

questions and sports history questions). Along with the demographics questions, the survey

instrument included three questionnaires: Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ)

(Sheard, Golby & van Wersch, 2009), Short Grit Scale (Grit-S) (Duckworth & Quinn, 2009), and

Sport Motivation Scale-6 (SMS-6) (Mallet, Kawabata, Newcombe, Otero-Forero, & Jackson,

2007) (see Appendix E for questionnaires).

Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ). The SMTQ, developed by Sheard et

al. (2009), is a fourteen-item questionnaire arranged on a four-point Likert scale (Not at all true =

1 to Very true = 4), that is designed to measure global MT in sports settings (maximum MT

score = 56; minimum score = 14). The SMTQ contains three MT subscales: confidence (e.g. “I

have an unshakeable confidence in my ability,” I have what it takes to perform well while under

pressure,” and “I have qualities that set me apart from other competitors”), constancy (e.g. “I am

committed to completing the tasks I have to do” and “I give up in difficult situations” [reverse

scored]) and control (e.g. “I worry about performing poorly” [reverse scored] and “I am

overcome by self-doubt [reverse scored]). The three MT subscales utilized in the development of

the SMTQ were consistent with review of MT literature. According to Crust (2007), confidence

is seen as having an unwavering belief in oneself. An individual that displays constancy is one

29
that is consistently persistent over time and control refers to one’s ability to remain influential

during adversity.

Utilizing 1,142 participants ranging in age from 16 to 63, the SMTQ was found to be a

reliable and valid measure of MT (Sheard et al., 2009). Internal consistency was found to be high

with Cronbach’s alphas of .80, .74, and .71 reported found for confidence, constancy, and

control, respectively. Acceptable divergent validity was acknowledged with low-to-moderate

correlations with optimism, positive affect, and negative affect. Likewise, a good model fit was

indicated (RMSEA = .05). (see Appendix E for SMTQ questionnaire)

Short Grit Scale (Grit-S). The Grit-S is an eight-item questionnaire designed by

Duckworth & Quinn (2009) to measure “trait-level perseverance and passion for long-term

goals” (p. 166). Question responses for the Grit-S follow a five-point Likert scale (“Very much

like me” = 1 to “Not much like me at all” = 5). The maximum score of 40 signifies an extremely

gritty individual and a minimum score of 8 represents an individual that is not at all gritty. The

Grit-S has 2 subscales: Consistency of Interest (e.g. “I often set a goal but later choose to pursue

a different one” and “New ideas and projects sometimes distract me from previous ones”) and

Perseverance of Effort (e.g. “I am diligent” [reverse scored] and “Setbacks don’t discourage me”

[reverse scored]). Consistency of Interest statements are designed to identify individuals who are

persistent over time by sticking with a goal, while Perseverance of Effort statements recognize

the passion with which individuals pursue that goal (Duckworth et al., 2007).

Collecting a final total of 2,526 participants from two United States Military Academy,

West Point classes (2008 & 2010), Duckworth & Quinn (2009) found acceptable reliability and

validity within the Grit-S. Overall Grit-S internal consistency alphas ranged from .73 to .76, with

Consistency of Interest subscale alphas ranging from .73 to .74. and Perseverance of Effort

30
ranging from .60 to .65. Fit indexes revealed a good fit for both the 2008 and 2010 West Point

classes (RMSEA = .061 and .068, respectively) (see Appendix E for Grit-S questionnaire).

Sport Motivation Scale-6 (SMS-6). The SMS-6 is a twenty-four item questionnaire

designed along a motivation continuum within the framework of SDT (Mallett et al., 2007). The

intention of the SMS-6 is to explore why athletes participate in sport. The seven-point Likert

scale responses range from “Does not correspond at all” to “Corresponds exactly” and a

maximum score of 168 and minimum of 24 can be achieved, with higher scores signifying

greater levels of motivation. There are six subscales from which to respond when presented with

the question “Why do you practice your sport?”: amotivation (e.g. “I don’t seem to be enjoying

my sport as much as I previously did”), external regulation (e.g. “Because it allows me to be well

regarded by people that I know”), introjected regulation (e.g. “Because I must do sports to feel

good about myself”), identified regulation (e.g. “Because training hard will improve my

performance”), integrated regulation (e.g. “Because it is an extension of me”), and intrinsic

motivation (e.g. “For the satisfaction I experience while I am perfecting my abilities”).

A two stage confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) study conducted by Mallet et al. (2007)

confirms the reliability and validity of the SMS-6. Internal consistency was found to be high with

Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .70 to .86 for the six subscale items with a mean of .78. The

CFA revealed acceptable root mean square error of approximation values (RMSEA = .05) (see

Appendix E for SMS-6 questionnaire).

Statistical Analysis

A 2 x 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to assess MT among single-sport and

multi-sport athletes with differing levels of competition achieved (α = .05). Likewise, a 2 x 2

ANOVA was used to evaluate differences in grit between the type of high school athlete one was

31
to the level of competition attained (α = .05). A third 2 x 2 ANOVA was used to measure

motivation differences among single-sport and multi-sport high school athletes and their level of

competition reached (α = .05). And finally, two independent samples t tests were conducted to

explore differences in internalized (intrinsic regulation, integrated regulation, and identified

regulation) and externalized (introjected regulation and external regulation) motivation in regards

to single-sport and multi-sport athletes.

32
CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

Mental Toughness

A 2 x 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to investigate the research

question regarding the type of athlete (single-sport and multi-sport high school athletes), their

level of attained competition (high school playing experience or college or above playing

experience), and their MT scores. The ANOVA indicated no significant interaction effect

between athlete type and competition level, F(1, 387) = .06, p = .812, partial η2 < .001, but

significant main effects for athlete type, F(1, 387) = 4.87, p = .028, partial η2 = .012, and

competition level, F(1, 387) = 17.33, p < .001, partial η2 = .043. A post hoc test was not

performed for competition level or athlete type because there are fewer than three levels for each

variable. The athlete type main effects revealed that multi-sport athletes scored higher on MT

than did single-sport athletes. Additionally, the competition level main effects showed that

college level athletes scored higher on MT than did athletes that only played at the high school

level. Means and standard deviations for motivation scores can be found in Table 1.

Grit

A 2 x 2 ANOVA was conducted to investigate the research question regarding the type of

athlete (single-sport and multi-sport high school athletes), their level of attained competition

(high school playing experience or college or above playing experience), and their grit scores.

The ANOVA indicated no significant interaction effect between athlete type and competition

level, F(1, 382) = .32, p = .571, partial η2 = .001, and no significance for the athlete type main

effect, F(1, 382) = 1.22, p = .270, partial η2 = .003. However, a significant main effect was found

for competition level, F(1, 382) = 5.42, p = .020, partial η2 = .014. The competition level main

33
effects showed that college level athletes scored higher on grit than did athletes that only played

at the high school level. A post hoc test was not performed for competition level because there

are fewer than three levels for that variable. Means and standard deviations for grit scores can be

found in Table 1.

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics for Mental Toughness, Grit, and Motivation

Variable Athlete Classification N M SD


Mental Single-Sport High School 147 42.12 5.39
Toughness Single-Sport College 68 44.57 5.32
Multi-Sport High School 97 43.48 5.65
Multi-Sport College 79 45.67 4.54
Total 391 43.60 5.44
Grit Single-Sport High School 147 28.47 3.96
Single-Sport College 66 29.29 4.72
Multi-Sport High School 96 28.72 4.64
Multi-Sport College 77 30.06 4.45
Total 386 28.99 4.39
Motivation Single-Sport High School 143 102.69 0.918
Single-Sport College 66 112.52 0.785
Multi-Sport High School 92 108.26 0.906
Multi-Sport College 76 110.29 0.781
Total 377 107.30 18.23
Note: Single-sport high school athlete = An athlete that finished high school playing
only one sport and did not play in college. Single-sport college athlete = An athlete
that finished high school playing only one sport and later played collegiately or above.
Multi-sport high school athlete =An athlete that finished high school playing two or more
sports and did not play in college. Multi-sport college athlete = An athlete that finished
high school playing two or more sports and later played collegiately or above.

34
Motivation

A 2 x 2 ANOVA was conducted to evaluate the research question regarding the type of

athlete (single-sport and multi-sport high school athletes), their level of attained competition

(high school playing experience or college or above playing experience), and their motivation

scores. The ANOVA indicated a significant interaction effect between athlete type and

competition level F(1, 373) = 4.11, p = .043, partial η2 = .011 and a significant main effect for

competition level F(1, 373) = 9.50, p = .002, partial η2 = .025. However, there was no significant

finding for the type of athlete, F(1, 373) = .76, p = .385, partial η2 = .002. Group means show

college level athletes to have higher levels of motivation than high school level athletes.

Since the interaction between competition level and athlete type was significant, the

competition level main effect was ignored and instead the level of competition simple main

effects were examined. In doing so, the difference in competition level between single-sport and

multi-sport high school athletes was evaluated separately. To control for Type I errors across the

two simple main effects, the alpha level was set at .025. Regarding motivation, there were no

significant differences between attained level of competition (high school vs. college) and multi-

sport high school athletes, F(1, 373) = .54, p = .465, but there was a significant difference in

single-sport athletes and attained level of competition, with college level athletes that played

only one sport in high school scoring higher than single-sport high school athletes that did not go

on to play in college, F(1, 373) = 13.61, p < .001. In addition, there were no significant

differences between single-sport and multi-sport high school athletes that went to play in college,

F(1, 373) = .55, p = .460. However, for those that played only at the high school level, multi-

sport athletes score significantly higher than single-sport athletes, F(1, 373) = 5.42, p = .020.

Means and standard deviations for motivation scores can be found in Table 1.

35
To further investigate motivational orientations, an independent-samples t test was

conducted to evaluate whether single-sport or multi-sport athletes have more of an internalized

motive for participation or externalized motive. The t test was significant, t(388) = -2.49, p =

.013, with multi-sport athletes (M = 5.66, SD = .852) scoring higher in internalized motivation

than single-sport athletes (M = 5.42, SD = .964). The 95% confidence interval for the difference

in means ranged from -.415 to -.048. The eta square index indicated that 2% of the variance of

the internalized motivation variable was accounted for by whether or not the athlete was a single-

sport or multi-sport athlete. Figure 1 shows the distributions for the two groups.

Figure 1. Error bars (two standard deviations above and below the mean) for externalized and
internalized motivation for athlete type.

Exploring externalized motivational orientations for participation regarding single-sport

and multi-sport athletes, an independent-samples t test was conducted. Multi-sport athletes (M =


36
4.20, SD = 1.20) scored higher in externalized motivation than did single-sport athletes (M =

3.99, SD = 1.21), but the t test was found to be non-significant, t(388) = -1.78, p = .076. Figure 1

shows the distributions for the two groups.

37
CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

Discussion of Research Findings

This study was conducted to investigate differences in MT, grit and motivation among

different types of athletes. The results of this study revealed data indicating that multi-sport

athletes score higher on MT than do single sport athletes. However, there were no significant

differences in athlete type for grit or motivation. For all three dependent variables assessed, MT,

grit, and motivation, significant differences were found in competition level scores with single-

sport high school athletes having the lowest scores of the various group combinations.

Additionally, multi-sport athletes appear to display higher levels of internalized motivation than

do single-sport athletes.

Mental Toughness

A review of literature indicates mentally tough individuals demonstrate strong coping

behaviors, resiliency, confidence, and commitment (Clough et al., 2002; Thelwell et al., 2005;

Theokas, 2009). Both single-sport and multi-sport athletes will exhibit these characteristics at

some time. Sports are obviously physically demanding, but they can also be emotionally

exhausting (DiFiori et al., 2014; Gould, 2010). When asked “why did you quit playing that

sport”, the single-sport athletes from this study gave several responses, but the most common

replies were: “I did not enjoy the sport anymore”, “An injury wouldn’t allow me to play

anymore”, “I didn’t have time to play multiple sports”, and “I wasn’t good enough”. The

findings of this study may support these responses. Of course, talent is a huge factor, but when

looking at single-sport athletes, one would have to consider MT as one variable that would

separate those that made it to play collegiately and those that played in high school only. Single-

38
sport high school athletes that only played at the high school level recorded the lowest MT scores

of the four levels of athlete classifications. See Table 1 for means. Furthermore, multi-sport high

school athletes scored significantly higher in MT than the single-sport athletes. This may support

previous research that fewer experiences limit the opportunity to develop MT (Thelwell et al.,

2005).

Both single-sport and multi-sport high school athletes that played collegiately scored

higher than those that only played at the high school level. Furthermore, both types of athletes

that played collegiately or above did not score significantly different from each other for MT.

With multi-sport and single-sport high school athletes that made it to the college level scoring

significantly higher in MT than non-college athletes, it begs the question what comes first? Does

MT come from playing sports, or is it the mentally tough individual that succeeds in sport? This

specific question was not of primary importance and therefore, cannot be answered from this

study. However, literature may suggest that MT comes first. Jones et al. (2002), Bull et al.

(2005), and Ryan & Deci (2000a), have all indicated that MT may be a “trait-like” characteristic

that is phylogenetic in nature. If this is true, adolescents should focus more on activities that

develop MT while young, instead of being concerned about specializing in a single sport.

Grit

Grit has been defined as perseverance and passion for long-term goals (Duckworth,

Peterson, Matthews & Kelly, 2007). Both single-sport and multi-sport athletes can display grit.

However, one would think that single-sport athletes would display more grit. If one sets their

goal to excel at a task, then an individual that selects one specific task and pours themselves into

it could be described as gritty. On the other hand, the same can be said for multi-sport athletes

that grew up playing multiple sports. Gritty individuals, having started a sport early in life, will

39
tend to stick with it. However, neither single-sport nor multi-sport high school athletes scored

significantly higher than the other in grit. Though multi-sport high school athletes that made it to

the college level or above recorded the highest levels of grit, they did not score significantly

higher than any of the three other groups.

One distinguishing characteristic of gritty individuals is the long-term aspect of achieving

a goal (Duckworth & Quinn, 2009). Physically, one must have talent to succeed at high levels of

sport. However, Duckworth and Quinn (2009) have noted that grit can be as much of a predictor

of success as talent. With this in mind, it stands to reason that college athletes would score higher

in grit than high school athletes, which is what the results of this study indicate. However, there

was no significant interaction between the two independent variables for grit, suggesting no

considerable advantage for any combination of athlete type or competition level. While being

gritty is important to completion of long-term goals, there are other factors, both physical and

mental, that might play a role in whether or not an athlete reaches college level or higher

competition.

Motivation

Motivation is a concept that reaches many aspects of life. There are many reasons

athletes choose to compete in sports. Some athletes compete in order to receive a prize at the

end, and some athletes compete for the satisfaction of accomplishing a task or goal. According to

Gould & Carson (2004), “motivation is a critical component [to sport] and is greatly influenced

by support and encouragement from those in the field and one’s family members” (p. 20). Again,

single-sport athletes that only played at the high school level recorded lower scores for

motivation than all other groups. SDT states that individuals have three basic needs: autonomy,

relatedness and competence (Ryan & Deci, 2000a; Deci & Vansteenkiste, 2004). With this in

40
mind, it seems reasonable that those individuals that are good enough to play beyond high school

will feel more competent, while athletes only playing at the high school level feel less

competent, and therefore, less motivated. The same can be true of autonomy. Autonomy in SDT

is related to volition. Individuals that have made their sport(s) their own will exhibit higher levels

of motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000c). Due to the popularity of specialization in sport today,

adolescents are sometimes encouraged to play a single sport by parents and coaches (Hill, 1991;

Watts, 2002). This pressure from authority can suppress an individual’s motivation. One

common reason given by parents or adolescents choosing to specialize in one sport is that

college recruiters encourage it (Hedstrom & Gould, 2004). This would infer that adolescents and

parents have a college athletic scholarship in mind for their future. However, internally

motivated behavior is more sustaining than externally motivated behavior. If one’s main motive

for participating in a sport is to achieve an athletic scholarship, then over time motivation is more

than likely to dwindle. To counter this, an individual should look for ways to transform what

starts out as extrinsic motivation for an activity into intrinsic motivation by fully assimilating the

behavior with oneself (Ryan & Deci, 2000a). Additionally, one could assume that athletes

playing multiple sports would have more opportunity to become connected to others and thus

increase their sense of belonging. The findings of this study supports these notions as single-

sport high school athletes scored significantly lower than both multi-sport high school athletes

and single-sport college athletes in motivation.

Furthermore, a review of the literature indicates that individuals involved in sports are

more intrinsically motivated (AAHPERD, 2013). This would suggest that multi-sport athletes

have a more internalized motive for participation. Additionally, Gould (2010) suggests that

specializing in a single sport may weaken intrinsic motivation. The results of this study

41
reinforces these ideas since multi-sport athletes scored higher on internalized motivation than did

single-sport athletes. Similarly, multi-sport athletes also reported higher externalized motives

than single-sport athletes. Consequently, paralleling the discussion concerning MT, one has to

ask if sports nurture motivation within an individual or individuals have natural levels of

motivation prior to getting involved. If motivation is more phylogenetic, then this study would

support that notion, since individuals that have made it to more advanced levels of sport reported

higher levels of motivation.

Delimitations

 A convenient sample of college-aged students enrolled in various Health, Human

Performance, & Recreation (HHPR) courses was used for this study.

 Adolescents currently participating in interscholastic sports were not part of the sample.

 Non-athletes were excluded from this study by removing all questionnaires from

participants that did not participate in at least one interscholastic sport in high school.

 There was no interest to control for gender in this study.

 High school population of participants was not considered.

Limitations

 Former athletes not enrolled in HHPR courses did not have a chance to be a part of this

study.

 All questionnaires were found to be reliable for the ages of the participants. However,

participants that were not currently participating in a sport were asked to recollect their

playing days to honestly answer the questionnaires.

 Motivations and personalities change over time and may play a role in how surveys were

answered.

42
 With this study, there was no way to predict how much of the participant’s responses

were influenced by their environment. Some coaches may create a climate that is

conducive to fostering certain types of motivation while others generate an environment

that stifles motivation.

 Occasionally, participants from small interscholastic schools are multi-sport athletes out

of necessity, while sometimes, larger schools have more single-sport athletes (Hill,

1991).

Assumptions

 The researcher assumed that students enrolled in HHPR courses participated in sports in

high school.

 The researcher assumes that the participants were able to accurately recall their sports

history and did so honestly.

 The researcher assumes that the participants honestly completed the surveys.

Recommendation for Future Research

MT has been explored by a handful of researchers recently; many have attempted to

define it since there are a wide variety of definitions out there (see Gucciardi, et al., 2015 for

review). Likewise, grit is a popular construct that has recently re-surfaced (Duckworth et al.

2007; Kelly et al., 2014; Winerman, 2013). Motivation has been investigated by numerous

academics for several years (DeCharms, 1968; Deci & Vansteenkiste, 2004; Heider, 1958; Ryan

& Deci, 2000b; Ryan et al. 2009). It can be a difficult construct to understand and is undoubtedly

an important one, especially in the academic world (Ryan & Deci, 2000a).

There are many paths that one could follow from the results of this study. First, younger

populations need to be considered. If MT, grit and motivation are nurtured at a young age by

43
participating in sports, then younger populations need to be assessed. Second, school populations

should be considered. With the promotion of sport specialization of young athletes today, fewer

and fewer adolescents are participating in multiple sports (Hedstrom & Gould, 2004). This is

especially true of larger schools where there is more competitiveness for playing time due to the

large number of athletes from which to choose; sometimes at smaller schools, adolescents may

play multiple sports out of necessity (Bell, Post, Trigsted, Hetzel, McGuine, & Brooks, 2016).

Viewing the specialization phenomenon with respect to grit, future research should

dissect the effects of the number of years specializing. Since gritty individuals pursue long-term

goals for extended periods, do athletes that have specialized longer than athletes that have

decided to specialize later in adolescence display more grit? Likewise, does the number of years

participating affect MT levels?

Next, it would be interesting to see if certain sports are correlated with higher or lower

levels of MT, grit or motivation. Prior research suggests that there are no significant differences

in MT between team and individual sport athletes (Jalili et al., 2011). But, what about grit and

motivation? Do individual sport or team sport athletes exhibit varying levels of motivation or

grit? Finally, again with the phenomenon of sports specialization, it would be intriguing to

investigate college athletes to see which athletes, from what sports and how many, chose to

specialize versus diversify in sports while growing up.

In general, MT, grit and motivation should be further explored, as all three can be found

useful in everyday life. MT, a construct that includes resiliency, emotional strength, and the

ability to overcome adversity, is important for success in almost any endeavor in life. Similarly,

gritty individuals are able to preserve over time to accomplish goals, which is also crucial in

everyday life. Lastly, without motivation, one will likely not be successful. We all need a reason

44
“why” to do something. If all three of these psychological constructs can be fostered in sports

participation, then it is important to continually explore that connection.

45
References

Álvarez, M. S., Balaguer, I., Castillo, I., & Duda, J. L. (2009). Coach autonomy support and
quality of sport engagement in young soccer players. The Spanish Journal of Psychology,
12(1), p. 138-148.

American Alliance for Health, Physical Education, Recreation and Dance [AAHPERD]. (2013).
Maximizing the benefits of youth sport (Position Statement). Retrieved July 15, 2014,
from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download;jsessionid=3A6E6143CCAC2DBD7
6C2146F72ACCB67?doi=10.1.1.391.9036&rep=rep1&type=pdf

Armour, N. (2015, February 25). He’s 12 years old, not the next Tom Brady. USA Today Sports.
Retrieved from http://usatoday.com

Austin, M. W. (2014). Is humility a virtue in the context of sport? Journal of Applied


Philosophy, 31(2), 203-214. doi: 10.1111/japp.12049

Baker, J. (2003). Early specialization in youth sport: A requirement for adult expertise? High
Ability Studies 14(1), p. 85-94.

Baker, J., Cobley, S., & Fraser-Thomas, J. (2009). What do we know about early sport
specialization? Not much! High Ability Studies, 20(1), 77-89.

Bailey, R. (2006). Physical education and sport in schools: A review of benefits and
outcomes. Journal of School Health, 76(8), 398.

Bawa, H. S. (2010). Personality hardiness, burnout and sport competition anxiety among
athletics and wrestling coaches. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 44, I57-I58.
doi:10.1136/bjsm.2010.078725.193

Bell, D. R., Post, E. G., Trigsted, S. M., Hetzel, S., McGuine, T. A., & Brooks, M. A. (2016).
Prevalence of sport specialization in high school athletics: A 1-year observational study.
The American Journal of Sports Medicine, 44(6), 1469-1474. doi: 10.1177/0363546516
629943

Branta, C. F. (2010). Sport specialization: Developmental and learning issues. Journal of


Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 81(8), p. 19-28.

Bull, S., Shambrook, C., James, W., & Brooks, J. (2005). Towards an understanding of mental
toughness in elite English cricketers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 209-227.
doi:10.1080/10413200591010085

Butler, S. (2011, April 29). $4,000 for youth baseball: Kids’ sports costs are out of control. CBS
News. Retrieved from http://www.cbsnews.com

Butt, J., Weinberg, R., & Culp, B. (2010). Exploring Mental Toughness in NCAA Athletes.

46
Journal of Intercollegiate Sport, 3, 316-332.

Clough, P., Earle, K., & Sewell, D. (2002). Mental toughness: The concept and its measurement.
In I. Cockerill (Ed.), Solutions in sport psychology (pp. 32–45). London: Thomson

Connaughton, D., R., Hanton, S., & Jones, G. (2010). The development and maintenance of
mental toughness in the world’s best performers. The Sport Psychologist, 24, 168-193.

Connaughton, D., Wadey, R., Hanton, S., & Jones, G. (2008). The development and maintenance
of mental toughness: Perceptions of elite performers. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26(1),
83-95. doi:10.1080/02640410701310958

Côté, J. (1999). The influence of the family in the development of talent in sport. The Sport
Psychologist, 13, 395-417.

Côté, J., Lidor, R., & Hackfort, D. (2009). ISSP Position Stand: To sample or to specialize?
Seven postulates about youth sport activities that lead to continued participation and elite
performance. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 9, 7-17.

Cranmer, G. A., & Myers, S. A. (2015). Sports teams as organizations: A leader-member


exchange perspective of player communication with coaches and teammates.
Communication & Sport, 3(1). 100-118. doi: 10.1177/2167479513520487

Crust, L. (2007). Mental toughness in sport: A review. International Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 5, 270-290.

Crust, L., & Swann, C. (2011). Comparing two measures of mental toughness. Personality and
Individual Differences, 50(2), 1-18. Retrieved May 5, 2015, from http://eprints.lincoln.
ac.uk/3702/1/MT_Measures_pdf.pdf

Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1993). Talented teenagers: The roots of success and
failure. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. New
York: Academic Press.

Deci, E. & Vansteenkiste, M. (2004). Self-Determination Theory and basic need satisfaction:
Understanding human development in positive psychology. Ricerche di Psicologia, 27,
23-40.

DiFiori, J., Benjamin, H., Brenner, J., Gregory, A., Jayanthi, N., Landry, G., & Luke, A. (2014).
Overuse injuries and burnout in youth sports: A position statement from the American
Medical Society for Sports Medicine. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 24(1), 3-20.

Duckworth, A., & Gross, J. (2014). Self-Control and Grit: Related but Separable Determinants of

47
Success. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 319-325.
doi:10.1177/096372141541462.

Duckworth, A., & Quinn, D. (2009). Development and validation of the Short Grit Scale (Grit-
S). Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(2), 166-174. doi:10.1080/00223890802634290

Duckworth, A., Peterson, C., Matthews, M., & Kelly, D. (2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion
for long-term goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 1087-1101.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1087

Epstein, D. J. (2013). The sports gene: Inside the science of extraordinary athletic performance.
New York, NY: Penguin Group (USA) Inc.

Ericsson, A., Krampe, R., Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the
acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 3. 363-406.

Ferguson, B., & Stern, P. (2014). A case of early sports specialization in an adolescent athlete.
The Journal of the Canadian Chiropractor Association, 58(4), 377-383.

Fraser-Thomas, J., & Côté, J. (2006). Youth sports: Implementing findings and moving forward
with research. Athletic Insight, 8(3), 12-27.

Frederick, C. M. & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Self-determination in sport: A review using Cognitive


Evaluation Theory. International Journal of Sport Pschology, 26(1), 5-23.

Gallahue, D. L., Ozmun, J. C., & Goodway, J. D., (2012). Understanding motor development:
infants, children, adolescents, adults (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Galton, F. (1892). Hereditary Genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London:
Macmillan.

Grit featuring John Wayne. (n.d.). Retrieved March 16, 2016, from http://www.values.com/
inspirational-sayings-billboards/72-grit

Griffin, J. (2008). Sport psychology: Myths in sport education and physical education. Journal of
Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 79(8), p. 11-13.

Gould, D. (2010). Early sport specialization: A psychological perspective. Journal of Physical


Education, Recreation & Dance, 81(8), 33-37.

Gould, D. & Carson, S. (2004). Myths surrounding the role of youth sports in developing
Olympic champions. Youth Studies Austrailia, 23(1), 19-26.

Gould, D., Hodge, K., Peterson, K., & Petlichkoff, L. (1987). Psychological foundations of
coaching: Similarities and differences among intercollegiate wrestling coaches. The Sport
Psychologist, 1(4), 293-308.

48
Gucciardi, D., Hanton, S., Gordon, S., Mallett, C., & Temby, P. (2015). The Concept of Mental
Toughness: Tests of Dimensionality, Nomological Network and Traitness. Journal of
Personality, 83, 26-44.

Hedstrom, R. & Gould, D. (2004). Research in Youth Sports: Critical Issues Status (Institute for
the Study of Youth Sports Research Report). Retrieved January 27, 2015, from
http://www.pysc.org/projects/documents/ResearchinYouthSports-CriticalIssuesStatus.pdf

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

Hill, G. (1991). One-sport high school athletes. Education Digest, 56(6), 70-72.

Hochanadel, A. & Finamore, D. (2015). Fixed and growth mindset in education and how grit
helps students persist in the face of adversity. Journal of International Education
Research, 11(1), 47-50.

Hollander, D., Meyers, M., & LeUnes, A. (1995). Psychological factors associated with
overtraining: Implications for youth sport coaches. Journal of Sport Behavior, 18(1), 3-
20.

Jaeschke, A. & Sachs, M. (2012). 100,000 miles closer to a definition of mental toughness: The
farther you run, the more the mind dominates. Marathon & Beyond, 16(5), 44-67.

Jalili, F., Hosseini, S. A., Jalili, F., & Salehian, M. H. (2011). Comparison of personality
dimensions, mental toughness, and social skills of female students athletes (team-
individual) and non-athletes. Annals of Biological Research, 2(6), 554-560.

James, W. (1907, March 1). The energies of men. Science, 25, 321–332.

Jones, G., Hanton, S., & Connaughton, D. (2002). What is this thing called mental
toughness? An investigation of elite sport performers. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 14, 205-218.

Jones, G., Hanton, S., & Connaughton, D. (2007). A framework of mental toughness in the
world’s best performers. The Sport Psychologist, 21, 243-264.

Jones, J., & Moorhouse, A. (2008). Developing mental toughness: Gold medal strategies for
transforming your business performance (2nd ed.). Oxford: Spring Hill.

Jones, M. I., & Parker, J.K. (2013). What is the size of the relationship between global mental
toughness and youth experience? Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 519-523.

Kelly, D., Matthews, M., & Bartone, P. (2014). Grit and hardiness as predictors of performance
among West Point cadets. Military Psychology, 26(4), 327-342. doi:10.1037/ml0000050.

49
Keown, T. (2011, August 24). Elite travel baseball and basketball teams make for a youth sports
industrial complex. ESPN. Retrieved from http://www.espn.go.com

Li, C., Wang, C. K., Pyun, D. Y., & Kee, Y. H. (2013). Burnout and its relations with basic
psychological needs and motivation among athletes: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14, 602-700. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.
2013.04.009.

Loehr, J. (1986). Mental toughness training for sports: Achieving athletic excellence. Lexington,
MA: S. Greene Press.

Loehr, J. (1995). The new toughness training for sports: Mental, emotional, and physical
conditioning from one of the world's premier sports psychologists. New York, NY:
PLUME.

Mahoney, J., Gucciardi, D., Ntoumanis, N., & Mallet, C. (2014). Mental toughness in sport:
Motivational antecedents and associations with performance and psychological health.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 36, 281-292. doi:10.1123/jsep.2013-0260

Malina, R. M. (2010). Early sport specialization: Roots, effectiveness, risks. 9(6), 364-371. doi:
10.1249/JSR.0b013e3181fe3166.

Mallett, C., Kawabata, M., Newcombe, P., Otero-Forero, A., & Jackson, S. (2007). Sport
motivation scale-6 (SMS-6): A revised six-factor sport motivation scale. Psychology of
Sport and Exercise, 8, 600-614. doi:10.10016/j.psychsport.2006.12.005

Messmer, M. (2003). Encouraging teamwork in the workplace. The National Public Accountant,
p. 32-33.

Middleton, S. C., Marsh, H. W., Martin, A. J., Richards, G. E., & Perry, C. (2004). Discovering
mental toughness: A qualitative study of mental toughness in elite athletes. Self Research
Centre Biannual Conference, Berlin. Available from http://www.sectionone
wrestling.com/discovering_mental_toughness.pdf

National Association of Youth Sports [NASPE]. (2010). Guidelines for participation in youth
sport programs: Specialization versus multiple-sport participation (Position Statement).
Retrieved May 6, 2015, from http://www.shapeamerica.org/advocacy/positionstatements/
pe/loader.cfm?csModule=security/getfile&pageid=4651

National Strength and Conditioning Association [NSCA]. (2009). Youth resistance training:
Updated position statement from the National Strength and Conditioning Association
(Position Statement). Retrieved June 13, 2016, from http://journals.lww.com/nsca-
jscr/Fulltext/2009/08005/Youth_Resistance_Training__Updated_Position.2.aspx#

National Council of Youth Sports [NCYS]. (2008). Report on trends and participation in

50
organized youth sports (Market Research Report). Retrieved March 27, 2015, from
www.ncys.org/ publications/2008-sports-participation-study.php

Neto, O. B., Barbieri, F. A., Barbieri, R. A., Gobbi, L. T. (2009). Agility, speed and motor skill
performance of practitioners and non-practitioners of soccer. Fitness & Performance
Journal, 8(2), p. 110-114.

Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P.S. (1981). Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice.
In J.R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 1–55). Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum

O’Sullivan, J. (2015, January 25). The perils of single-sport participation. Changing the Game
Project. Retrieved from http://www.changingthegameproject.com

Prosser, L., & Jiang, X. (2008). Relationship between school physical activity and academic
performance of children. The International Journal of Learning, 15(3), 11-15.

Ramzi, S., & Besharat, M. A. (2010). The impact of hardiness on sport achievement and mental
health. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 823-826.
doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.07.192

Readdy, T., Raabe, J., & Harding, J. S. (2014). Student-athletes’ perceptions of an extrinsic
reward program: A mixed-methods exploration of Self-Determination Theory in the
context of college football. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 26, 157-171.

Reed, J. (2014). A Survey of Grit and Exercise Behavior. Journal of Sport Behavior, 37(4), 390-
406.

Reed, K. (2014, October 31). Youth Sports Specialization Defies Logic. The Huffington Post.
Retrieved May 12, 2015, from http://www.huffingtonpost.com/ken-reed/youth-sports-
specializati_b_6084732.html?utm_hp_ref=sports&ir=Sports

Riewald, S. & Snyder, C. (2014). The path to excellence: A view on the athletic development of
U.S. Olympians who competed from 2000-2012. Initial report: Results of the Talent
Identification and Development Questionnaire to U.S. Olympians. Retrieved from
http://www.teamusa.org

Robles, M. M. (2012). Executive perceptions of the top 10 soft skills needed in today’s
workplace. Business and Professional Communication Quarterly, 75(4), p. 453-465.

Rogers, M. (2015, July 3). U.S. women were multi-sport athletes before focusing on soccer.
USA Today. Retrieved from http://www.usatoday.com

Russell, W. D. & Limle, A. N. (2013). The relationship between youth sport specialization and
involvement in sport and physical activity in young adulthood. Journal of Sport
Behavior, 36(1), 82-98.

51
Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new
directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54-67. doi:10.1006/ceps.1999
.1020

Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2000b). Self-Determination Theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68-78.

Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (2000c). The darker and brighter sides of human existence: Basic
psychological needs as a unifying concept. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 319-338.

Ryan, R., Williams, G., Patrick, H., & Deci, E. (2009). Self-determination theory and physical
activity: The dynamics of motivation in development and wellness. Hellenic Journal of
Psychology, 6, 107-124.

Sage, G., & Eitzen, D. (2013). Youth and Sport. In Sociology of North American Sport (9th ed.,
p. 70). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Seifert, K. (2015, February 5). QB survey: Cross-training only helps. ESPN. Retrieved from
http://www.espn.go.com

Sheard, M., Golby, J., & van Wersch, A. (2009). Progress toward construct validation of the
sports mental toughness questionnaire (SMTQ). European Journal of Psychological
Assessment, 25(3), 186-193. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.25.3.186

Shibko, J. R. (2015). Factors associated with youth athletes discountinuing vs. staying in a sport.
(Distinction Paper, Otterbein University). Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.
otterbein.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1003&context=stu_dist

Sloane, M., Hanna, J., & Ford, D. (2013). ‘Never, ever give up’: Diana Nyad completes historic
Cuba-to-Florida swim. CNN. Retrieved from http://www.cnn.com

Sullivan, P. (2015, January 16). The rising costs of youth sports, in money and emotions. The
New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com

Thelwell, R., Weston, N., Greenless, I. (2005). Defining and understanding mental toughness
within soccer. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 326-332.
doi:10.1080/10413200500313636

Theokas, C. (2009). Youth sport participation-A view of the issuse: Introduction to the special
section. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 303-306. doi:10.1037/a0015042

Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., De Witte, H., & Lens, W. (2008). Explaining the
relationships between job characteristics, burnout, and engagement: The role of basic
psychological need satisfaction. Work & Stress: An International Journal of Work,
Health & Organisations, 22(3), p. 277-294.

52
Watts, J. (2002). Perspectives on sport specialization. Journal of Physical Education,
Recreation & Dance, 73(8), 32-37.

Webster Inc. (2005). Merriam-Webster's collegiate dictionary (11th ed.). Springfield, MA:
Merriam-Webster.

Wiersma, L. (2000). Risks and benefits of youth sport specialization: Perspectives and
recommendations. Pediatric Exercise Science, 12, 13-22.

Winerman, L. (2013). What sets high achievers apart? Monitor on Psychology, 44(11), 28-31.

Wojtys, Edward, M., (2013). Sports Specialization vs Diversification. Sports Health, 5(3), 212-
213. doi:10.1177/1941738113484130

Woods, R. B. (2016). Social issues in sport (3rd ed.). Champaign, Ill.: Human Kinetics.

Yang, X., Telama, R., Hirvensalo, M., Viikari, J., & Raitakari, O. (2009). Sustained participation
in youth sport decreases metabolic syndrome in adulthood. International Journal of
Obesity, 33, 1219-1226. doi:10.1038/ijo.2009.171

53
Appendix A

Research Study Consent Form

54
Appendix B

IRB Approval Letter

55
Appendix C

Demographics of Participants

Figure C1. Pie chart of age of participants.

56
Appendix C

Demographics of Participants

Figure C2. Pie chart of sports played by participants.

57
Appendix C

Demographics of Participants

Figure C3. Pie chart of competition level achieved by participants.

58
Appendix D

Demographic and Sports History Questions

59
Appendix E

Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire (SMTQ)

Figure E1. Sports Mental Toughness Questionnaire. Sheard, M., Golby, J., & van Wersch, A.
(2009). Progress toward construct validation of the sports mental toughness questionnaire
(SMTQ). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(3), 186-193. doi:10.1027/1015-
5759.25.3.186.
60
Appendix E

Short Grit Scale (Grit-S)

Figure E2. Short Grit Scale (Grit-S). Duckworth, A., & Quinn, D. (2009). Development and
validation of the Short Grit Scale (Grit-S). Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(2), 166-174.
doi:10.1080/00223890802634290.

61
Appendix E

Sport Motivation Scale-6 (SMS-6)

Figure E3. Sport Motivation Scale-6 (SMS-6). Mallett, C., Kawabata, M., Newcombe, P., Otero-
Forero, A., & Jackson, S. (2007). Sport motivation scale-6 (SMS-6): A revised six-factor sport
motivation scale. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 600-614. doi:10.10016/j.psychsport.2006.
12.005.

62

You might also like