Guided Imagery and Recovered Memory Therapy - Considerations and Cautions 2006
Guided Imagery and Recovered Memory Therapy - Considerations and Cautions 2006
Guided Imagery and Recovered Memory Therapy - Considerations and Cautions 2006
To cite this article: Amanda Thayer BA & Dr. Steven Jay Lynn PhD (2006) Guided Imagery and Recovered Memory Therapy:
Considerations and Cautions, Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 6:3, 63-73, DOI: 10.1300/J158v06n03_04
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
COMMENTARY SECTION
Downloaded by [Temple University Libraries] at 14:23 18 November 2014
Guided Imagery
and Recovered Memory Therapy:
Considerations and Cautions
Amanda Thayer, BA
Steven Jay Lynn, PhD
In the last half century, one of the most contentious issues that has
emerged in the field of psychology is whether psychotherapists create
rather then simply “recover” ostensibly repressed or dissociated memo-
ries of traumatic experiences, including early childhood sexual abuse.
Numerous researchers and practitioners (e.g., Lindsay & Read, 1994;
Loftus & Ketcham, 1994; Ofshe & Watters, 1994; Lynn, Lock, Loftus,
Krackow, & Lilienfeld, 2003) have articulated manifold risks inherent
in the proliferation of memory recovery techniques. However, propo-
nents of the use of such techniques have marshalled a vigorous defense
based on three contentions. First, the mere fact that a patient fails to re-
call sexual abuse should not deter a therapist from implementing mem-
ory recovery techniques insofar as traumatic memories are commonly
represssed (e.g., see Williams, 1994). Second, memory research does
not, in the main, apply to traumatic memories because there are salient
differences in the recall of traumatic and mundane events (Alpert,
Brown, & Courtois, 1998a,b; van der Kolk, 1994). Finally, therapists
are duty-bound to help their clients, which necessitates taking a non-
judgmental stance toward remembrances that arise in treatment (Fred-
rickson, 1992; Herman, 1992).
Given that the general topic of memory recovery techniques has been
debated in multiple forums, we will limit our discussion to four points:
(1) memory work in general, and guided imagery in particular, is by no
Commentary Section 65
means a rare event; (2) extant research has failed to document any thera-
peutic benefit for revisiting early memories of traumatic events; (3) at-
tempts to help clients recall early abuse, including the use of guided
imagery, can engender false memories; and (4) studies of source moni-
toring and imagination inflation provide compelling reasons to be wary
of the use of guided imagery techniques. We will address each of these
issues in turn.
Downloaded by [Temple University Libraries] at 14:23 18 November 2014
Ornstein, Ceci, and Loftus (1998) point out that it is a logical fallacy to
assume that if a certain manifestation of psychopathology follows sex-
ual abuse, then a history of sexual abuse must exist when this particular
symptom is present. This error in logic is what is commonly referred to
as “affirming the consequent.” As Ornstein et al. (1998) have noted,
“we cannot argue from current symptoms back to a history of presumed
abuse”(p. 1008).
Downloaded by [Temple University Libraries] at 14:23 18 November 2014
FINAL CONSIDERATIONS
REFERENCES
Alpert, J.L., Brown, L.S., & Courtois, C.A. (1998a). Symptomatic clients and memo-
ries of childhood abuse: What the trauma and child sexual abuse literature tells us.
Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 4, 941-995.
Alpert, J.L., Brown, L.S., & Courtois, C.A. (1998b). Comment on Ornstein, Ceci, and
Loftus (1998): Adult recollections of childhood abuse. Psychology, Public Policy,
and Law, 4, 1052-1067.
Anderson, R.E. (1984). Did I do it or did I only imagine doing it? Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: General, 113, 594-613.
Bass, E., & Davis, L. (1988). The courage to heal. New York: Harper & Row.
Bass, E., & Davis L. (1993). Beginning to heal: A first book for survivors of childhood
sexual abuse. New York: Harper Collins.
Brown, D., Scheflin, A.W., & Hammond, D.C. (1998). Memory, trauma treatment, and
the law. New York, NY: Norton & Co., Inc.
Ceci, S.J., Loftus, E.F., Leichtman, M.D., & Bruck, M. (1995). The possible role of
source misattributions in the creation of false beliefs among preschoolers. Interna-
tional Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 304-320.
Commentary Section 71
Dobson, M., & Markham, R. (1993). Imagery ability and source monitoring: Implica-
tions for eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology, 84(1), 111-118.
Frederickson, R. (1992). Repressed memories. New York: Fireside/Parkside.
Garry, M., Manning, C.G., Loftus, E.F., & Sherman, S.J. (1996). Imagination inflation:
Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bul-
letin & Review, 3, 208-214.
Goff, L.M., & Roediger, H.L. (1998). Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated
imaginings lead to illusory recollections. Memory & Cognition, 26, 20-33.
Herman, J.L. (1992). Trauma and recovery: The aftermath of violence. New York: Basic
Downloaded by [Temple University Libraries] at 14:23 18 November 2014
Books.
Horselenberg, R., Merkelbach, H., Muris, P., Rassin, E., Sijsenaar, M., & Spaan, V.
(2000). Imagining hypothetical childhood events: The role of individual differences
in imagination inflation. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 7, 128-137.
Hyman, I., & Billings, F. (1998). Individual differences and the creation of false child-
hood memories. Memory, 6(1), 1-20.
Hyman, I., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false
childhood memories. Journal of Memory & Language, 35, 101-117.
Johnson, M.K., Foley, M.A., Suengas, A.G., & Raye, C.L. (1989). Pnenomenal charac-
teristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 371-376.
Johnson, M.K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D.S. (1993). Source Monitoring. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 114, 3-28.
Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88,
67-85.
Kazdin, A.E. (1994). Behavior modification in applied settings. Pacific Grove, CA:
Brooks Cole.
Laurence, J.-R., & Perry, C. (1983). Hypnotically created memory among highly hyp-
notizable subjects. Science, 222, 523-524.
Lindsay, D.S. (1998). Depolarizing views on recovered memory experiences. In S.J.
Lynn & K. McConkey (Eds.), Truth in memory (pp. 481-494). New York: Guilford.
Lindsay, D. (1993). Eyewitness suggestibility. Current Directions in Psychological Sci-
ence, 2, 86-89.
Lindsay, D., & Read, J. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of childhood sexual
abuse: A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281-338.
Littrell, J. (1998). Is the experience of painful emotion therapeutic? Clinical Psychology
Review, 18, 71-102.
Loftus, E.F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory. New York: St. Mar-
tin’s Press.
Lynn, S.J., Lock, T., Loftus, E.F., Krackow, E., & Lilienfeld, S.O. (2003). The remem-
brance of things past: Problematic memory recovery techniques in psychotherapy.
In S.O. Lilienfeld, S.J. Lynn, & J.M. Lohr (Eds.), Science and pseudoscience in clini-
cal psychology. New York: The Guilford Press.
Lynn, S.J., Weekes, J.R., & Milano, M.J. (1989). Reality versus suggestion: Pseudo-
memory in hypnotizable and simulating subjects. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
98, 137-144.
72 JOURNAL OF FORENSIC PSYCHOLOGY PRACTICE
Maki, R.H., & Syman, E.M. (1997). Teaching of controversial and empirically vali-
dated treatments in APA-accredited clinical and counseling psychology programs.
Psychotherapy, 34, 44-57.
Mazzoni, G.A., Loftus, E.F., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible
autobiographical events: A little plausibility goes a long way. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Applied, 7, 51-59.
Mazzoni, G.A., Loftus, E.F., Seitz, A., & Lynn, S.J. (1999). Creating a new childhood:
Changing beliefs and memories through dream interpretation. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 13, 125-144.
Downloaded by [Temple University Libraries] at 14:23 18 November 2014
Mazzoni, G.A., Lombardo, P., Malvagia, S., & Loftus, E.F. (1997). Dream interpreta-
tion and false beliefs. Unpublished manuscript, University of Florence and University
of Washington.
Ofshe, R., & Watters, E. (1994). Making monsters: False memories, psychotherapy, and
sexual hysteria. New York: Scribner’s.
Ornstein, P.A., Ceci, S.J., & Loftus, E.F. (1998). Comment on Alpert, Brown, and
Courtois (1998): The science of memory and the practice of psychotherapy. Psy-
chology, Public Policy, and Law, 4, 996-1010.
Paddock, J.R., Joseph, A.L., Chan, F.M., Terranova, S., Manning, C., & Loftus, E.F.
(1998). When guided visualization procedures may backfire: Imagination inflation
and predicting individual differences in suggestibility. Applied Cognitive Psychol-
ogy, 12, 63-75.
Paddock, J.R., Noel, M., Terranova, S., Eber, H.W., Manning, C., & Loftus, E.F.
(1999). Imagination inflation and the perils of guided visualization. The Journal of
Psychology, 133, 581-595.
Polusny, M.A., & Follete, V.M. (1996). Remembering childhood sexual abuse: A na-
tional survey of psychologists’ clinical practices, beliefs, and personal experiences.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 27, 41-52.
Poole, D.A., Lindsay, D.S., Memon, A., & Bull, R. (1995). Psychotherapy and the re-
covery of memories of childhood sexual abuse: U.S. and British practitioner’s opin-
ions, practices, and experiences. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 63,
426-437.
Porter, S., Yuille, J.C., & Lehman, D.R. (1999). The nature of real, implanted, and fab-
ricated memories for emotional childhood events: implications for the recovered
memory debate. Law and Human Behavior, 23(5), 517-537.
Shobe, K.S., & Kihlstrom, J.F. (1997). Is traumatic memory special? Current Direc-
tions in Psychological Science, 6, 70-74.
Shobe, K.K., & Kihlstrom, J.F. (2002). Interrogative suggestibility and “memory
work.” In M.L. Eisen (Ed.), Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview
(pp. 309-327). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Spanos, N.P., & McLean, J. (1986). Hypnotically created pseudomemories: Memory
distortions or reporting biases? British Journal of Experimental Hypnosis, 3, 155-159.
Tousignant, J.P. (1984). Individual differences in response bias and recall: A character-
ization of the effects of misleading postevent information. Dissertation Abstracts In-
ternational, 48 (5-B), 1069.
van der Kolk, B.A. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving
psychobiology of posttraumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1, 253-265.
Commentary Section 73
doi:10.1300/J158v06n03_04