Location via proxy:   [ UP ]  
[Report a bug]   [Manage cookies]                

NIH Public Access: Cognitive Control Deficits Associated With Antisocial Personality Disorder and Psychopathy

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 14

NIH Public Access

Author Manuscript
Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.
Published in final edited form as:
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Personal Disord. 2012 July ; 3(3): 283–293. doi:10.1037/a0023137.

Cognitive Control Deficits Associated with Antisocial


Personality Disorder and Psychopathy
Joshua D. Zeier, Arielle R. Baskin-Sommers, and Joseph P. Newman
Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison
Kristina Hiatt Racer
Child and Family Center, University of Oregon

Abstract
Antisociality has been linked to a variety of executive functioning deficits, including poor
cognitive control. Surprisingly, cognitive control deficits are rarely found in psychopathic
individuals, despite their notoriously severe and persistent antisocial behavior. In fact, primary
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

(low-anxious) psychopathic individuals display superior performance on cognitive control-type


tasks under certain circumstances. To clarify these seemingly contradictory findings, we
administered a response competition (i.e. flanker) task to incarcerated offenders, who were
assessed for Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD) symptoms and psychopathy. As hypothesized,
APD related to poorer accuracy, especially on incongruent trials. Contrary to expectation,
however, the same pattern of results was found in psychopathy. Additional analyses indicated that
these effects of APD and psychopathy were associated with overlapping variance. The findings
suggest that psychopathy and APD symptoms are both associated with deficits in cognitive
control, and that this deficit relates to general antisociality as opposed to a specific antisocial
syndrome.

Keywords
Antisociality; Psychopathy; Antisocial Personality Disorder; Cognitive Control; Executive
Functioning

Identifying and exploring the risk factors and deficits that predispose individuals to
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

antisocial behavior represents a critical step towards locating causal and potentially
protective factors that relate to the expression or inhibition of inappropriate or illegal
behavior. Investigations of antisociality frequently focus on two related constructs:
Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD) and psychopathy. Antisociality Personality Disorder
comprises a pattern of antisocial attitudes and behaviors (e.g. irresponsibility, impulsivity,
irritability) that begin before the age of 15 (e.g. getting into fights, bullying, lying) and
persist in adulthood (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Psychopathy relates to a
number of the same traits; however, individuals with psychopathy are also characterized by
an arrogant and deceitful interpersonal style, callousness and lack of emotionality (Hare,

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joshua D. Zeier, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin
—Madison, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, WI 53706. jdzeier@wisc.edu.
Analyses with IQ included as a covariate were also performed, to see if the aforementioned effects remained. There was no significant
relationship of IQ and overall reaction time, F(1, 93)=0.36, p=.55, and no interaction of IQ and the incongruency (F(1, 93)<0.01, p=.
96) or facilitation (F(1, 93)=0.66, p=.42) contrasts. There was a significant main effect of IQ on overall accuracy, F(1, 93)=4.34, p=.
04. There was no significant interaction of IQ and the incongruency (F(1, 93)=0.81, p=.37) or facilitation (F(1, 93)=0.05, p=.83)
contrasts. Including these as covariates, for both the accuracy and reaction time analyses, did not substantially change any of the other
main effects or interactions presented in the main analyses.
Zeier et al. Page 2

2003). Psychopathy appears to have unique features that differentiate it from general
criminality and Antisocial Personality Disorder, substance abuse, and other forms of
disinhibitory psychopathology (Kosson, Lorenz, & Newman, 2006; Patterson & Newman,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

1993). Psychopathy is generally considered a more severe syndrome of antisociality than


APD; thus, while about 75% of prison inmates meet criteria for APD, only about 15-25%
meet criteria for psychopathy (Hare, 1996).

A primary focus of research on the causes of antisocial behavior relates to impaired


executive functioning (EF), which Morgan and Lilienfeld (2000) define as an “umbrella
term that refers to the cognitive processes that allow for future, goal-oriented behavior” (p.
114). According to this model, people with executive functioning deficits are less able to
override maladaptive response inclinations in order to maintain more appropriate and
personally beneficial behavior. Consequently, they are at high risk for persistent rule
breaking and committing acts of violence. Consistent with this model, the antisocial
syndromes examined by the authors displayed EF deficits in multiple domains.

Although the association between EF deficits and antisociality is promising, both EF and
antisociality are general terms that encompass diverse processes (e.g., cognitive control,
planning) and subtypes (e.g., psychopathy and APD), respectively. Thus, the overall
association between antisociality and EF functioning may mask the fact that different
antisocial subtypes are characterized by distinct EF deficits. Moreover, this concern may be
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

especially important for the cognitive control component of EF. Cognitive control refers to
the ability to persist in goal-directed behavior in the face of competing cognitive and
behavioral demands and is a crucial component of self-regulation (MacCoon, Wallace, &
Newman, 2004). Despite the general association between antisocial syndromes and EF
deficits (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000; see also Blair, 2001), psychopathic individuals
generally do not display deficits on cognitive control tasks (Blair et al., 2006; Brinkley,
Schmitt, & Newman, 2005; Dvorak-Bertsch, Sadeh, Glass, Thornton, & Newman, 2007;
Hart, Forth, & Hare, 1990; Hiatt, Schmitt, & Newman, 2004; Munro, Dywan, Harris,
McKee, Unsal, & Segalowitz, 2007; Smith, Arnett, & Newman, 1992; Sutker, Moan, &
Allain, 1983). Thus, despite the high level of antisocial behavior displayed by psychopathic
individuals, they do not appear to manifest deficits in cognitive control.

Furthermore, there is evidence that, under certain circumstances, psychopathic individuals


display superior performance on cognitive control tasks (Hiatt et al., 2004). This does not
reflect the presence of more efficient basic cognitive control processes in psychopathy.
According to Hiatt et al. (2004), the reduced interference displayed by psychopathic
individuals on such tasks reflects the fact that they are generally insensitive to peripheral
information once they establish a goal-directed focus of attention. Newman and colleagues
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

(e.g., Newman & Lorenz, 2003) have proposed a response modulation theory to account for
the insensitivity to peripheral information associated with psychopathy. Moreover, this
pattern appears specific to “primary psychopathy”--individuals who, in keeping with the
classic clinical description of the disorder, have low levels of anxiety combined with high
levels of psychopathy (Cleckley, 1976). Across diverse paradigms, individuals with primary
psychopathy display an abnormality in early selective attention that often precludes the
conscious representation of information that conflicts with their goal-directed behavior
(Hiatt & Newman, 2006). Of specific importance to this study, to the extent that this
abnormality in early selection reduces perception of peripheral distractions (e.g., response
conflict), it would obviate the need to use late selection or cognitive control to inhibit the
distracting information (Lavie, 2005).

The goal of the current study is to clarify the apparent inconsistencies in responsivity to
response incongruent distracters associated with different antisocial syndromes. More

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 3

specifically, this study seeks to distinguish the cognitive control deficit associated with APD
from the early selective attention abnormality apparent in psychopathy (and primary
psychopathy). An examination of these different individual difference variables, within the
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

same sample, will allow us to explore the hypothesis that these constructs, while related to
similar patterns of behavior (e.g. criminality), reflect unique cognitive underpinnings.
Further, given that antisociality is inherent to both psychopathy and APD, this strategy
affords the opportunity to examine the unique versus shared (i.e., overlapping) contributions
of psychopathy and APD to executive control.

To this end, we administered a flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974), in which centrally
presented target stimuli were flanked on either side by distracter stimuli. In a flanker task,
the distracter stimuli may be associated with the same response as the target stimuli, a
different response than the target stimuli, or with no particular response. When distracter
stimuli are incongruent with target stimuli, participants reliably respond more slowly and
less accurately (i.e., display greater interference) than when distracters are congruent or
unrelated to the target stimuli. Owing to these features, the flanker task, like the Stroop task,
is regarded as a classic test of cognitive control (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen,
2001).

To evaluate the differential association between cognitive control and antisociality versus
psychopathy, we propose a series of analytic steps. After analyzing the effects of the task
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

independent of personality variables, we first focus on APD-related effects. Given the


association between APD and deficient cognitive control (Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000), we
predict that APD symptoms will be positively associated with interference (i.e., poorer
cognitive control) on the flanker task. Interference is assessed using an “incongruency
contrast”, comparing the reaction time or accuracy for trials with incongruent distracters to
trials without incongruent information.

Next, we investigate psychopathy-related effects on task performance. Because psychopathy


is generally unrelated to cognitive control deficits, including those associated with flanker
task performance (e.g., Munro et al., 2007), we predict that psychopathy will be unrelated to
interference in this study. In order to replicate the analytic strategies of our prior studies of
selective attention anomalies in psychopathy, our analyses compare primary (low-anxious)
psychopathic individuals to non-psychopathic individuals at a comparable anxiety level and
we treat psychopathy and anxiety as dichotomous variables. Nevertheless, because
psychopathy is commonly conceptualized as a continuous variable, we also report results for
psychopathy and its psychopathy sub-factors using continuous analyses. Finally, we
investigate the unique and overlapping variance of psychopathy and APD. These analyses
allow us to explore whether our findings for the study are unique to a specific form of
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

externalizing psychopathology (APD or psychopathy) or, instead, reflect a more general


deficit that is common to these disorders.

Method
Participants
Participants consisted of 126 incarcerated Caucasian males in a maximum security
correctional institution in Wisconsin, who met our basic inclusion criteria for participation;
that is, participants had to be 45 years old or younger, free of any history of psychosis or
bipolar disorder, and not currently taking psychotropic medication. Also, only participants
scoring 70 or higher on a brief intelligence questionnaire (Zachary, 1986) were included the
study. In addition, one outlier (identified using studentized residuals of the incongruency
contrast for both reaction time and accuracy, with Bonferroni corrected p-values <.05) was
excluded from analyses. For this experiment, participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 45, with a

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 4

mean of 28.25 (SD=7.68). Estimated IQ scores ranged from 70.59 to 122.59, with a mean of
98.48 (SD=11.47). Participants’ scores on the Welsh Anxiety Scale (Welsh, 1956) ranged
from 0 to 34, with a mean of 11.91 (SD=9.25).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

All participants were assessed for psychopathy using the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised
(PCL-R: Hare, 2003). This measure uses information gleaned from an interview and a
review of institutional files to score the participant on the presence of 20 different items.
Offenders were paid $8 for their participation in this interview. Scores on this measure
ranged from 4.40 to 37.90, with a mean of 24.50 (SD =7.68). In this sample, Cronbach’s
alpha for the PCL-R total score was .82 (for 45 participants with no items omitted). Seven
participants had two PCL-R raters; interrater reliability for this subset of scores was .95.

Participants were also assessed for APD symptoms during the same interview and file
review that was used for the PCL-R assessment with the addition of specific questions about
the presence of a variety of antisocial behaviors (e.g. burglary, physical cruelty to animals,
vandalism, etc.) before age 15 to assess Conduct Disorder symptoms. The average number
of APD symptoms in this sample was 9.06 (SD=4.27) and scores on this measure (i.e.,
number of symptoms met) ranged from 1 to 20. Cronbach’s alpha for the APD total score
was .82. Seven participants had two APD raters; interrater reliability for this subset of scores
was .95.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

In addition, participants completed the Welsh Anxiety Scale (Welsh, 1956) to distinguish
between primary and secondary psychopathy (see Newman & Brinkley, 1997; Newman,
MacCoon, Vaughn, & Sadeh, 2005), as previous research suggests that the response
modulation deficits of psychopathic offenders may be specific to primary psychopathic
individuals (i.e., those with low versus high levels of anxiety).

As in previous research, participants scoring 20 or lower on the PCL-R were considered


non-psychopathic (n=44), and participants scoring 30 or higher were considered to be
psychopathic (n=54). Participants were divided into low-anxious and high-anxious group
using a median split (9) on the Welsh Anxiety Scale (Welsh, 1956). This resulted in 52
participants considered to be low-anxious, and 46 participants considered to be high-
anxious. Ultimately, then, the sample can be considered as being divided into four groups:
low-anxious psychopathic (n=29), low-anxious non-psychopathic (n=23), high-anxious
psychopathic (n=25), and high-anxious non-psychopathic (n=29) participants. There was no
difference in anxiety scores between the psychopathic and non-psychopathic groups, and no
difference in PCL-R scores between the low and high anxious groups.

Apparatus
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

The flanker task was presented on a PC with a 16 inch monitor, and was programmed in
Micro-Experimental Laboratory (Schneider, 1988) software. Participants’ eyes were roughly
30 centimeters from the screen. Responses were entered on a keyboard; no feedback was
given during the task.

Procedure
Each participant completed one testing session of 600 test trials, divided into 5 blocks of 120
trials each. For each trial, a fixation point (+) appeared for 500 milliseconds. After this, the
target and flankers appeared and remained on the screen until the participant entered a
response. The target appeared at fixation and the two distracters appeared to the left and
right, equidistant from the target (approximately one degree of visual angle). The target
stimulus was always a 5, 8, G, or M. The distracters were always one of these stimuli or a
pound sign (#). Participants entered a response indicating whether the target was a letter or a
number. This was followed by a 1000-1500 millisecond variable intertrial interval.

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 5

There were three main types of trials in this task. Congruent trials were those in which the
target and distracters were of the same type (e.g., 8 5 8), incongruent trials were those in
which the target and distractors were of different types (e.g., G 5 G), and control trials were
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

those in which the distracters were pound signs (e.g., # 5 #). The primary dependent
measures were accuracy and reaction time scores for incongruent, congruent, and control
trials. Trials in which participants took less than 100 milliseconds or more than 1500
milliseconds to respond were not included in the reaction time analyses. Participants were
paid $3 for completing this particular task.

Results
Preliminary/Task Effect Analyses
Prior to analyses of individual differences, analyses were performed to examine the effect of
trial type (i.e., congruent, control, incongruent) on accuracy and reaction time. Reaction time
and accuracy measures were analyzed separately in General Linear Models with Trial Type
as a categorical repeated measures factor. Condition effects were parsed into two planned
contrasts, an incongruency effect contrast (incongruent vs. congruent/control) and a
facilitation contrast (congruent vs. control). For reaction time, there was a significant effect
of trial type, F(2, 250)= 283.15, p<.01, ηp2=.69. The incongruency contrast revealed that
participants were significantly faster on the control/congruent trials (M=579, SD=92) than
incongruent trials (M=610, SD=93), F(1,125)=455.21, p<.01, ηp2=.79. The facilitation
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

contrast was not significant, F(1,125)=1.08, p=.30, ηp2=.01. For accuracy, again there was a
significant effect of trial type, F(2, 250)=31.97, p<.01, ηp2=.20. The incongruency contrast
revealed that participants were significantly more accurate on the control/congruent trials
(M=97.9%, SD=1.8%) than incongruent trials (M=97.04%, SD=2.5%), F(1,125)=44.30, p<.
01, ηp2=.26. The facilitation contrast revealed that participants were significantly more
accurate on the congruent trials (M=98.1%, SD=1.86%) than the control trials (M=97.8%,
SD=1.9%), F(1,125)=7.84, p=.006, ηp2=.059.

Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD) Symptoms Analyses


These analyses focused on the prediction that antisocial behavior would be related to greater
interference from inhibitory information, as reflected by lower accuracy and/or slower
response times for trials with incongruent distracters compared to the other trial types.
Reaction time and accuracy measures were analyzed separately in General Linear Models
with Trial Type (incongruent, congruent, or control) as a categorical repeated measures
factor and Antisocial Personality Disorder symptoms as both a continuous variable and a
dichotomous variable (in separate analyses). As above, condition effects were parsed into
two planned contrasts, an incongruency effect contrast and a facilitation contrast.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

For reaction time, there were no significant main or contrast effects for APD symptoms
continuously or dichotomously. However, analysis of the accuracy data revealed a
significant main effect for APD symptoms continuously, F(1,124)=8.64, p=.004. This main
effect was qualified by a significant interaction between APD symptoms and trial type, F(2,
248)=5.50, p=.006. The nature of this interaction is clarified by the significant APD
symptoms by incongruency contrast interaction, F(1,124)=7.92, p=.006. Specifically, APD
symptoms were related to significantly greater interference (i.e. worse performance) in
incongruent trials relative to other trials (see Figure 1). When analyzed dichotomously, there
was a trend level main effect for APD, F(1, 124)=3.62, p=.06. This main effect was
qualified by a significant interaction between APD symptoms and trial type, F(2, 248)=4.41,
p=.01. The nature of this interaction is clarified by the significant APD symptoms by
incongruency contrast interaction, F(1, 124)=6.54, p=.01. As with the continuous analyses,
these results reflect that antisociality was related to lower accuracy on incongruent trials.

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 6

Psychopathy Analyses
The following analyses were focused on testing the predictions for psychopathy (no
differences in interference), as well as for primary (i.e., low-anxious) psychopathic
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

individuals (no difference or less interference). Reaction time and accuracy measures were
analyzed separately in General Linear Models with Trial Type (incongruent, congruent, or
control) as a categorical repeated measures factor and Psychopathy Group (Non-
Psychopathic or Psychopathic) and Anxiety Group (Low-Anxious or High-Anxious) as
categorical between-subjects factors. As above, condition effects were parsed into two
planned contrasts, an incongruency effect contrast and a facilitation contrast.

There were no differences due to psychopathy, anxiety, or their interaction detected in the
reaction time analysis. In addition, we conducted a planned comparison that focused on low-
anxious offenders to examine the effects of primary psychopathy on performance. However,
consistent with the overall analyses, there were no significant effects associated with
primary psychopathy.

For accuracy there was a significant interaction between psychopathy group and the
incongruency contrast, F(1, 94)=5.23, p=.02, with psychopathic participants displaying
greater interference (i.e., 1.2% lower accuracy) than non-psychopathic participants (i.e., .4%
lower accuracy) on incongruent vs. other trials. There was no difference in accuracy
between the congruent and control trial types. Mean reaction time and accuracy data for the
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

different subject groups and experimental conditions are presented in Table 1.

The planned comparisons involving low-anxious psychopathic and non-psychopathic


participants revealed a significant main effect for psychopathy, F(1,50)=6.96, p=.01, with
psychopathic participants (M=97.5%, SD=2.3%) displaying significantly lower accuracy
than non-psychopathic participants (M=98.2%, SD=1.2%) across all trial types. However,
there were no significant psychopathy-related effects for either contrast within low-anxious
participants.

The above analyses were performed to replicate the analytic strategies of previous studies on
selective attention deficits in psychopathy. However, we also re-examined the psychopathy
X anxiety analysis, using both variables continuously as opposed to categorically. As before,
there were no main effects of interactions of psychopathy or anxiety for any of the reaction
time measures. For accuracy, there was a significant interaction between psychopathy and
the incongruency contrast, F(1, 122)=3.89, p=.05, similar to the group effect above. There
were also significant main effects for psychopathy, F(1, 122)=10.97, p=.001, and anxiety,
F(1, 122)=6.02, p=.02, with higher scores related to lower accuracy for both variables. There
was also a significant psychopathy by anxiety interaction for overall accuracy
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

F(1,122)=7.37, p=.01, indicating that the negative association between psychopathy and
accuracy was strongest when anxiety levels are relatively low.

Subsequent analyses were performed to investigate the relationship of the two, three, and
four factor models of psychopathy with performance on the current task (limited to
accuracy, as no reaction time effects were found for this task). For the two-factor model,
there was a significant main effect of Factor 2 (n=116), such that high Factor 2 scores were
related to lower overall accuracy, F(1, 114)=7.27, p=.01. There was also a trend for a
significant interaction between psychopathy Factor 2 and the incongruency contrast, F(1,
114)=3.48, p=.07; this suggests that, while Factor 2 scores were related to poorer accuracy
overall, this effect was most prominent for the incongruent trials.

For the three factor model (n=110), there was a trend for a main effect of facet 3, F(1,
106)=3.50, p=.06, such that facet 3 appears to have a modest negative relationship with

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 7

overall accuracy. None of the other main effects or interactions with the facets approached
significance. There were no unique main effects or interactions of the facets in the four facet
model.1
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Unique Versus Overlapping Effects of Psychopathy and APD Symptoms


Additional analyses examined the differential effects of PCL-R scores and APD symptoms
on accuracy. An analysis like the primary analyses presented above was performed,
including both PCL-R total score and number of APD symptoms as continuous variables
(mean centered and standardized. When APD symptoms and PCL-R scores were entered
simultaneously to examine the unique effects of these constructs, only the relationship
between APD symptoms and overall accuracy (i.e. main effect) remained significant, F(1,
123)=5.11, p=.025. In addition, there was a trend-level effect for the APD symptoms by
incongruency contrast interaction (p=.08). APD was not significantly related to the
facilitation contrast. Finally, none of the unique effects of PCL-R score on performance
approached significance.

Discussion
The results of this study provided support for our primary hypothesis that a deficit in
cognitive control, as measured by the Eriksen flanker task, would be positively and
significantly associated with symptoms of antisocial behavior. More specifically, we found
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

that increasing levels of APD symptoms were associated with a greater number of incorrect
responses, particularly on those trials containing response incongruent information. In light
of the fact that these trials require inhibiting the incorrect response activated by the flanker
stimuli, this finding provides good support for Morgan and Lilienfeld’s (2000) conclusions
regarding the importance of inhibitory deficits in syndromes characterized by antisocial
behavior. This finding is also consistent with the proposal that deficits in cognitive and
inhibitory control are important risk factors predisposing individuals to a chronic antisocial
lifestyle (Hughes, White, Sharpen, & Dunn, 2000; Kooijmans, Scheres, & Oosterlaan, 2000;
Muris, Meesters, & Blijlevens, 2007; Raaijmakers et al, 2008). Likewise, the ability to
inhibit pre-potent responses to contextual cues in order to maintain one’s goal-directed
behavior is commonly regarded as a core skill needed to inhibit punished responses (Geier
& Luna, 2009), delay gratification (Olson, Schilling, & Bates, 1999; Rueda, Acosta, &
Santonja, 2007), tolerate frustration (McDonald, 2008), abstain from drug use (Fillmore &
Rush, 2002; Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Ivanov, Schulz, Londo, & Newcorn, 2008; Jentsch
& Taylor, 1999; Kaufman, Ross, Stein, & Garavan, 2003; Volkow, Fowler, & Wang, 2004),
and overcome aggressive urges (Albert & Chew, 1980; Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1996;
Raaijmakers et al., 2008; Sterzer & Stadler, 2009; Valzelli, 1984).
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Although psychopathy is significantly associated with antisocial behavior, previous research


using cognitive control tasks with psychopathic offenders (Dvorak-Bertsch et al., 2007;
Munro et al., 2008) led us to predict that psychopathy would not be associated with poor
performance in the current study. Contrary to expectation, psychopathic participants were
also significantly more likely than controls to commit errors on incongruent trials regardless
of level of anxiety (i.e., primary versus secondary psychopathy). This finding suggests that
the inhibitory deficit observed in this study is associated with a general “antisociality
dimension” that cuts across psychopathy and other antisocial syndromes. This conclusion
was further substantiated by statistical analyses which found that errors on incongruent trials
were related to the variance shared by psychopathy scores and APD symptoms.

These unexpected findings for psychopathy appear inconsistent with other published reports
that found no evidence of cognitive control deficits in psychopathic offenders (Dvorak-
Bertsch et al, 2007; Hiatt et al., 2004; Munro et al., 2007; Smith et al., 1992). Such results

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 8

are particularly surprising as psychopathy is often associated with reduced rather than hyper-
sensitivity to contextual cues that contraindicate their ongoing goal-directed behavior (see
MacCoon et al., 2004 for review). Understanding the discrepancies between the findings of
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

the current study and prior research is necessary to develop a more nuanced and
comprehensive picture of the common and unique cognitive deficits related to psychopathy
and APD.

Recent research with psychopathic offenders has clarified the circumstances that modulate
sensitivity to incongruent contextual stimuli among psychopathic offenders. Using three
variants of the Stroop task, Hiatt et al. (2004) found that offenders with primary
psychopathy displayed less interference than controls when incongruent stimuli were
spatially or temporally separated from the target stimuli, but they did not differ from
controls when the target and incongruent stimuli were spatially integrated. Following
MacLeod (1998), the authors proposed that when incongruent and target stimuli are spatially
integrated, participants first process both stimuli and must subsequently inhibit the
distracting information in order to produce the correct response. Conversely, when targets
and incongruent stimuli have different physical characteristics (pictures versus words, e.g.
Newman, Schmitt, & Voss, 1997), appear in different locations (color words versus
surrounding box color, e.g. Hiatt et al., 2004) or at different points in time (e.g., Mitchell,
Richell, Leonard, & Blair, 2006), or if the demands of the task focus attention on target
stimuli and away from inhibitory cognitive (e.g. Zeier, Maxwell, & Newman, 2009) or
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

emotional stimuli (e.g. Baskin-Sommers, Curtin, & Newman, 2010), it is easier to set an
early selection filter that facilitates focusing on the target and ignoring incongruent stimuli.
Psychopathic participants appear to be particularly adept at employing such early attention
filters; thus, in each of these circumstances, they are relatively impervious to salient
distracters. In other words, psychopathic individuals display minimal interference when
early selection allows them to filter out distracters, but normal or, as in the current study,
greater interference when proper performance requires late selection. Conversely,
antisociality appears to be unrelated to differences in early selection but is associated with
less efficient late attention systems.

Assuming that psychopathy is associated with an early selection bias, their performance in
this study suggests that they were unable to employ early selective attention to gate out
distracters. In light of the fact that targets were consistently presented in the central location,
it is somewhat surprising that they did not focus on these stimuli to the exclusion of the
flanker stimuli and display less interference. However, this fact may reflect the close spatial
proximity of the targets and flankers (i.e., both the target and distracters appeared within the
scope of visual attention) or other physical aspects of the stimulus display that made it more
difficult to separate targets and incongruent distracts (e.g., both were characters presented in
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

the same font). Alternatively, it may not have been possible for them to establish a goal-
relevant focus of attention that excluded distracters because the target set overlapped with
the incongruent distracter set (i.e., both were numbers and letters). Further research is
needed to evaluate these alternatives.

The results also suggest that when early selection is not possible, psychopathy may in fact
be associated with poorer cognitive control (i.e. late selection). Psychopathic offenders
committed more errors than controls on incongruent trials—a finding that appears to reflect
antisociality more generally. In light of the fact that early selection may eliminate the need
for late selection (Lavie, 2005), an interesting possibility in this regard is that the poor
cognitive control of psychopathic offenders will tend to be expressed only when early
selection is not possible. Conversely, when early selection is possible, it will tend to
obfuscate their late selection deficit. It should be noted that the evidence on cognitive
control in psychopathy have been inconsistent, and further investigation is necessary to

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 9

clarify the circumstances and underlying mechanisms that determine when psychopathy is
associated with poor cognitive control. For example, the association between antisociality
and cognitive control deficits, and thus the association between psychopathy and cognitive
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

control deficits, may be stronger under conditions involving affectively significant stimuli
(e.g., high stress or salient reward cues)..

The findings are also informative with respect to the self-control problems of psychopathic
and other antisocial individuals. Self-regulation has been defined as the “context appropriate
balance of attention” to top-down (i.e., goal-relevant) and bottom-up (i.e., peripheral) stimuli
(MacCoon et al., 2004). The former is highly dependent on the ability to ignore irrelevant
distracters (late selection) in order to maintain a goal-relevant focus (e.g., maintaining
abstinence or controlling aggressive reactions). However, as noted by MacCoon et al.
(2004), self-regulation may also be undermined by abnormalities in early selection that
interfere with a person’s ability to accommodate unexpected information indicating that
ongoing goal-directed behavior is non-optimal or inappropriate. For example, whereas an
early selection deficit may prevent someone from noticing that their goal-directed behavior
is upsetting someone else (e.g., unwanted sexual advances), their late selection deficit may
hamper their ability to inhibit such advances in order to avoid re-incarceration. Consistent
with claims set out by Morgan and Lilienfeld (2000), the present results suggest that
antisociality is associated with a diminished capacity to maintain socially appropriate goal-
directed behavior in the face of salient stimuli that activate competing responses. Further,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

the combined early and late selection deficits of psychopathic individuals reflect a “double-
hit” of sorts, representing two different cognitive pathways that may impede effective self-
regulation. This likely relates to the elevated severity and frequency of criminal activity
found in psychopathic offenders.

Before concluding, we consider potential limitations of the current study. First, it is


important to acknowledge that support for the predicted deficits in cognitive control was
limited to analyses involving response accuracy as opposed to response times. Moreover,
although statistically significant, the differences in response accuracy were small and further
research is needed to determine the extent to which such differences may account for
meaningful differences in self-regulation outside of the laboratory context. Further
investigation is needed to determine whether reaction time and accuracy provide equally
valid measures of cognitive control with the accuracy variable providing a more sensitive
measure of performance or whether the two variables are tapping different psychological
processes. In addition, while psychopathy has not been associated with significantly worse
performance in previous investigations of cognitive control (e.g., Dvorak-Bertsch et al.,
2007, Hiatt et al., 2004; Munro et al., 2007; Smith et al., 1992; Zeier et al., 2009), the
current study had considerable power to identify group differences in cognitive control and
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

found only a small, albeit significant, difference in response accuracy (versus reaction time).
Further research will be necessary to address these limitations, and to develop greater
specificity in understanding the extent to which the association between antisociality and
cognitive control is moderated by the combination of situational factors that allow for early
selection and the presence of psychopathy.

In conclusion, the results of this study provide evidence of a significant association between
antisociality, a general construct associated with both APD symptoms and psychopathy
scores, and poor executive functioning as measured by responsivity to distracting, inhibitory
information. Such findings are consistent with proposals (e.g., Morgan & Lilienfeld, 2000)
that the self-regulation problems of antisocial individuals relate to problems with executive
control, across different antisocial subtypes. By undermining effective self-regulation, this
deficit may result in increased risk for a variety of antisocial behavior. Further research is
needed to clarify the necessary and sufficient conditions for revealing early versus late

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 10

selective attention anomalies in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. A more


complete understanding of the common and unique deficits in these antisocial subtypes will
allow for greater precision in the early identification of at-risk individuals, facilitate
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

differential diagnosis, and inform the development of therapeutic interventions that are
tailored to the specific strengths and weaknesses of these groups.

Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants MH53041 and T32-MH018931. We
acknowledge the assistance and support of the staff at the Columbia Correctional Institution, as well as the
cooperation of the Wisconsin Department of Corrections. We thank Leah Vaughn, Bill Schmitt, Chad Brinkley,
Donal MacCoon, and Jeremy Bertsch for interviewing and diagnosing participants.

References
Albert D, Chew G. The septal forebrain and the inhibitory modulation of attack and defense in the rat:
A review. Behavioral & Neural Biology. 1980; 30:357–388. [PubMed: 7013753]
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed..
Author; Washington, DC: 2000. text revision
Baskin-Sommers AR, Curtin JJ, Newman JP. Specifying the attentional selection that moderates the
fearlessness of psychopathic offenders. Psychological Science. 2010 Manuscript committed for
publication.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Blair RJR. Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and
psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, & Psychiatry. 2001; 71:727–731.
Blair KS, Newman C, Mitchell DGV, Richell RA, Leonard A, Morton J, Blair RJR. Differentiating
among prefrontal substrates in psychopathy: Neuropsychological test findings. Neuropsychology.
2006; 20:153–165. [PubMed: 16594776]
Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Carter CS, Barch DM, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring and cognitive
control. Psychological Review. 2001; 108:624–652. [PubMed: 11488380]
Brinkley CA, Schmitt WA, Newman JP. Semantic processing in psychopathic offenders. Personality
and Individual Differences. 2005; 38:1047–1056.
Cleckley, H. The Mask of Sanity. 5th ed.. Mosby; St. Louis, MO: 1976.
Dvorak-Bertsch JD, Sadeh N, Glass SJ, Thornton D, Newman JP. Stroop tasks associated with
differential activation of anterior cingulate do not differentiate psychopathic and non-psychopathic
offenders. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007; 42:585–595. [PubMed: 21765572]
Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a
nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics. 1974; 16:143–149.
Fillmore M, Rush C. Impaired inhibitory control of behavior in chronic cocaine users. Drug and
Alcohol Dependence. 2002; 66:265–273. [PubMed: 12062461]
Geier C, Luna B. The maturation of incentive processing and cognitive control. Pharmacology,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Biochemistry and Behavior. 2009; 93:212–221.


Goldstein R, Volkow N. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: Neuroimaging
evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;
159:1642–1652. [PubMed: 12359667]
Hare RD. Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal Justice and Behavior.
1996; 23:25–54.
Hare, RD. Manual for the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. 2nd ed.. Multi-Health Systems;
Toronto, Ontario, Canada: 2003.
Hart SD, Forth AE, Hare RD. Performance of criminal psychopaths on selected neuropsychological
tests. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1990; 99:374–379. [PubMed: 2266211]
Hiatt, KD.; Newman, JP.; Patrick, CJ. Handbook of Psychopathy. Guilford Press; New York, NY:
2006. Understanding psychopathy: The cognitive side; p. 334-352.
Hiatt KD, Schmitt WA, Newman JP. Stroop Tasks Reveal Abnormal Selective Attention Among
Psychopathic Offenders. Neuropsychology. 2004; 18:50–59. [PubMed: 14744187]

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 11

Hughes C, White A, Sharpen J, Dunn J. Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: ‘Hard-to-manage’


preschoolers’ peer problems and possible cognitive influences. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry. 2000; 41:169–179. [PubMed: 10750543]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Ivanov I, Schulz K, Londo E, Newcorn J. Inhibitory control deficits in childhood and risk for
substance use disorders: A review. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2008;
34:239–258. [PubMed: 18428067]
Jentsch J, Taylor J. Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications
for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 1999; 146:373–390.
[PubMed: 10550488]
Kaufman J, Ross T, Stein E, Garavan H. Cingulate hypoactivity in cocaine users during a GO-NOGO
task as revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of
Neuroscience. 2003; 23:7839–7843. [PubMed: 12944513]
Kooijmans R, Scheres A, Oosterlaan J. Response inhibition and measures of psychopathology: A
dimensional analysis. Child Neuropsychology. 2000; 6:175–184. [PubMed: 11402395]
Kosson DS, Lorenz AR, Newman JP. Effects of co-morbid psychopathy on criminal offending and
emotion processing in male offenders with antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology. 2006; 115:798–806. [PubMed: 17100537]
Lavie N. Distracted and confused?: Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2005;
9:75–82. [PubMed: 15668100]
MacCoon, DG.; Wallace, JF.; Newman, JP. Self-regulation: The context-appropriate allocation of
attentional capacity to dominant and non-dominant cues. In: Baumeister, RF.; Vohs, KD., editors.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications. Guilford; New York: 2004. p.
422-446.
MacLeod CM. Training on integrated versus separated Stroop tasks: The progression of interference
and facilitation. Memory & Cognition. 1998; 26:201–211.
McDonald, S. Executive Functions and the Frontal Lobes: A Lifespan Perspective. Taylor & Francis;
Philadelphia, PA US: 2008. Social information processing difficulties in adults and implications
for treatment; p. 471-499.
Mitchell DGV, Richell RA, Leonard A, Blair RJR. Emotion at the expense of cognition: Psychopathic
individuals outperform controls on an operant response task. Journal of Abnormal Psychology.
2006; 115:559–566. [PubMed: 16866596]
Morgan AB, Lilienfeld SO. A meta-analytic review of the relation between antisocial behavior and
neuropsychological measures of executive function. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000; 20:113–
136. [PubMed: 10660831]
Munro GES, Dywan J, Harris GT, McKee S, Unsal A, Segalowitz SJ. ERN varies with degree of
psychopathy in an emotion discrimination task. Biological Psychology. 2007; 76:31–42. [PubMed:
17604898]
Muris P, Meesters C, Blijlevens P. Self-reported reactive and regulative temperament in early
adolescence: Relations to internalizing and externalizing problem behavior and ‘Big Three’
personality factors. Journal of Adolescence. 2007; 30:1035–1049. [PubMed: 17467051]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Newman JP, Brinkley CA. Reconsidering the low-fear explanation for primary psychopathy.
Psychological Inquiry. 1997; 8:236–244.
Newman, JP.; Lorenz, AR. Response modulation and emotion processing: Implications for
psychopathy and other dysregulatory psychopathology. In: Davidson, RJ.; Scherer, K.; Goldsmith,
HH., editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. Oxford University Press; 2003. p. 904-929.
Newman JP, MacCoon DG, Vaughan LJ, Sadeh N. Validating a distinction between primary and
secondary psychopathy with measures of Gray’s BIS and BAS constructs. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology. 2005; 114:319–323. [PubMed: 15869363]
Newman JP, Schmitt W, Voss W. The impact of motivationally neutral cues on psychopathic
individuals: Assessing the generality of the response modulation hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology. 1997; 106:563–575. [PubMed: 9358687]
Olson S, Schilling E, Bates J. Measurement of impulsivity: Construct coherence, longitudinal stability,
and relationship with externalizing problems in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 12

Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for Research in
Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. 1999; 27:151–165.
Oosterlaan J, Sergeant J. Inhibition in ADHD, aggressive, and anxious children: A biologically based
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

model of child psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication


of the International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. 1996; 24:19–
36.
Patterson CM, Newman JP. Reflectivity and learning from aversive events: Toward a psychological
mechanism for the syndromes of disinhibition. Psychological Review. 1993; 100:716–736.
[PubMed: 8255955]
Raaijmakers M, Smidts D, Sergeant J, Maassen G, Posthumus J, van Engeland H, et al. Executive
functions in preschool children with aggressive behavior: Impairments in inhibitory control.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An official publication of the International Society for
Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology. 2008; 36:1097–1107.
Rueda M, Acosta A, Santonja M. Attention and motivation interdependence in self-regulation. A
neurocognitive approach. Psychology of Motivation. 2007:29–45.
Schneider W. Micro-Experimental Laboratory: An integrated system for IBM-PC compatibles.
Behavior Research Methods, Instrumentation, and Computers. 1988; 20:206–217.
Smith SS, Arnett PA, Newman JP. Neuropsychological differentiation of psychopathic and
nonpsychopathic criminal offenders. Personality and Individual Differences. 1992; 13:1233–1245.
Sterzer P, Stadler C. Neuroimaging of aggressive and violent behaviour in children and adolescents.
Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009:3. [PubMed: 19503748]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Sutker PB, Moan CE, Allain AN. Assessment of cognitive control in psychopathic and normal
prisoners. Journal of Behavioral Assessment. 1983; 5:275–287.
Valzelli L. Reflections on experimental and human pathology of aggression. Progress in Neuro-
Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 1984; 8:311–325. [PubMed: 6385139]
Volkow N, Fowler J, Wang G. The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: Brain
circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology. 2004; 47(Suppl1):3–13. [PubMed:
15464121]
Welsh, G. Factor dimensions A and R. In: Welsh, GS.; Dahlstrom, WG., editors. Basic readings on the
MMPI in psychology and medicine. University of Minnesota Press; Minneapolis: 1956. p.
264-281.
Zachary, RA. Shipley Institute of Living Scale: Revised Manual. Western Psychological Services; Los
Angeles: 1986.
Zeier JD, Maxwell JS, Newman JP. Attention moderates the processing of inhibitory information in
primary psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009; 118:554–563. [PubMed: 19685952]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Zeier et al. Page 13
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Figure 1.
Incongruency effect by APD symptoms. The relationship between APD symptoms and
accuracy was moderated by Trial Type. APD symptoms were significantly inversely related
to accuracy in all conditions, but individuals high on APD symptoms had significantly
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

worse accuracy on the incongruent trials as compared to the other trials. Accuracy means
displayed for low and high APD sypmtoms were calculated at 1.5 standard deviations below
and above the sample mean on APD symptoms, respectively. Error bars represent the
standard error for the point estimate.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Table 1
Mean reaction times, accuracy scores, and standard deviations for the flanker task by level of Psychopathy and Anxiety.

Low-Anxious High-Anxious
Zeier et al.

Non- Non-
Psychopathic Psychopathic
Psychopathic Psychopathic
n=23 n=29 n=21 n=25

Trial Type Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD


Reaction
Time
Congruent 579 96 575 87 580 74 563 72
Control 580 97 576 87 577 70 563 78
Incongruent 612 105 607 84 603 68 595 74
Accuracy
Congruent 98.9% 1.0% 97.5% 2.4% 98.0% 1.5% 98.6% 1.8%
Control 98.5% 1.3% 97.4% 2.0% 97.9% 1.7% 98.3% 1.8%
Incongruent 98.1% 1.6% 96.3% 3.1% 97.5% 1.1% 97.3% 3.1%

Personal Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 July 25.


Page 14

You might also like