Phonetics and Phonology PDF
Phonetics and Phonology PDF
Phonetics and Phonology PDF
Federico Villarreal
GUÍA DIDACTICA
INGLÉS VI CICLO
Euded
Escuela Universitaria
Educación a distancia
2
1.UNIT 1 : PHONETICS
1.1 Virtual Resources.............................................................. 3
1.2 Introduction........................................................................4
1.3 The Speech Organs...........................................................7
1.4 Consonants........................................................................8
1.5 Vowels...............................................................................15
1.6 Phonetic Transcription.......................................................22
2. UNIT 2: PHONOLOGY
2.1 Phonemes..........................................................................24
2.2 Allophones.........................................................................25
2.3 Minimal Pairs.....................................................................27
2.4 Phonotactics .....................................................................27
2.5 Teaching Pronunciation: PHONICS...................................29
3. UNIT 3: STRESS AND RHYTHM
3.1 Resources.........................................................................30
3.2 Stress ...............................................................................32
3.3 Rhythm..............................................................................40
3.4 Teaching Pronunciation: LIMERICKS AND CHANTS.......46
4. UNIT 4: INTONATION AND CONNECTED SPEECH
4.1 Resources........................................................................ 47
4.2 Intonation...........................................................................48
4.2 Connected Speech............................................................54
4.3 Teaching Pronunciation: COMMUNICATIVE STRATEGIES.....61
5. BIBLIOGRAPHY...........................................................................62
3
UNIT I: PHONETICS
VIRTUAL RESOURCES
http://www.cambridge.org/it/cambridgeenglish/catalog/teacher‐training‐development‐and‐
research/english‐phonetics‐and‐phonology‐4th‐edition/components
DOWNLOAD FREE SAMPLES Chapter 2 The production of speech sounds
http://www.cambridge.org/other_files/cms/PeterRoach/PeterRoach_Glossary.html
http://www.cambridge.org/servlet/file/EPP_PED_Glossary.pdf?ITEM_ENT_ID=2491706&ITEM_VE
RSION=1&COLLSPEC_ENT_ID=7
DOWNLOAD English Phonetics and Phonology Glossary (A little encyclopaedia of Phonetics
4
PHONETICS
Introduction.
Whereas syntax is about sentence formation, and semantics about sentence
interpretation, phonetics and phonology cover the field of sentence utterance.
Phonetics is concerned with how sounds are produced, transmitted and perceived
(we will only look at the production of sounds). Phonology is concerned with how
sounds function in relation to each other in a language. In other words, phonetics is
about sounds of language, phonology about sound systems of language. Phonetics
is a descriptive tool necessary to the study of the phonological aspects of a
language. Phonetics and phonology are worth studying for several reasons. One is
that as all study of language, the study of phonology gives us insight into how the
human mind works. Two more reasons are that the study of the phonetics of a foreign
language gives us a much better ability both to hear and to correct mistakes that we
make, and also to teach pronunciation of the foreign language (in this case English)
to others.
As phonetics and phonology both deal with sounds, and as English spelling and
English pronunciation are two very different things, it is important that you keep in
mind that we are not interested in letters here, but in sounds. For instance, English
has not 5 or 6 but 20 different vowels, even if these vowels are all written by different
combinations of 6 different letters, "a, e, i, o, u, y". The orthographic spelling of a
word will be given in italics, e.g. please, and the phonetic transcription between
square brackets [pli:z]. Thus the word please consists of three consonants, [p,l,z],
and one vowel, [i:]. And sounds considered from the phonological point of view are
put between slashes. We will use the symbols.
5
6
7
All the organs shown contribute to the production of speech. All the sounds of
English are made using air on its way out from the lungs. The lungs pull in and push
out air. The air goes out via the trachea, where the first obstruction it meets is the
larynx, which it has to pass through. Inside the larynx the air passes by the vocal
folds or chords, which, if they vibrate, make the sound voiced. Afterwards the air
goes up through the pharynx, and escapes via either the oral or the nasal cavity.
Almost all the organs involved in speech production also have other functions. The
lungs and the diaphragm are obviously involved in breathing, as is the nasal cavity,
which cleans, heats and humidifies the air that is breathed in. The teeth and the
tongue play a part in digestion, and in a way, so do the vocal folds, as they have to
be closed when swallowing, to keep the food from going down the wrong way.
8
Consonants
On the way out the air flow can be more or less obstructed, producing a consonant,
or is simply modified, giving a vowel. If you pronounce the first sound of the word
paper you close your mouth completely and that is the utmost obstruction, whereas
if you pronounce the first sound of the word after the mouth is more open than
normal, the air flows as freely as it possibly can.
Consonants are often classified by being given a so-called VPM-label. VPM stands
for Voicing, Place and Manner:
- voicing means that the vocal folds are used; if they are not, the sound is voiceless
(note that vowels always imply the use of vocal folds).
- place of articulation is the place where the air flow will be more or less obstructed.
Voicing
The larynx is in the neck, at a point commonly called Adam's apple. It is like a box,
inside which are the vocal folds, two thick flaps of muscle. In a normal position, the
vocal folds are apart and we say that the glottis is open (figure a). When the edges
of the vocal folds touch each other, air passing through the glottis will usually cause
vibration (figure b). This opening and closing is repeated regularly and gives what is
called voicing.
9
The only distinction between the first sounds of sue and zoo for example is that [s]
is voiceless, [z] is voiced. The same goes for few and view, [f] is voiceless, [v] is
voiced. If you now say [ssssszzzzzsssss] or [fffffvvvvvfffff] you can either hear the
vibrations of the [zzzzz] or [vvvvv] by sticking your fingers into your ears, or you can
feel them by touching the front of your larynx (the Adam's Apple).
This distinction is quite important in English, as there are many pairs of sounds that
differ only in voicing. In the examples below the first sound is voiceless, the other is
voiced: pie/buy, try/dry, clue/glue, chew/Jew, thigh/thy. This distinction can also be
made in between two vowels: rapid/rabid, metal/medal, or at the end of a word:
pick/pig, leaf/leave, rich/ridge.
10
Places of Articulation.
As we saw above [p,t,k] are all voiceless, so there must be another way to distinguish
between them, otherwise we would not be able to tell try apart from pry or cry, or
pick from tick or kick . Apart from the behaviour of the vocal folds, sounds can also
be distinguished as to where in the oral cavity they are articulated (i.e. where in the
mouth there is most obstruction when they are pronounced)
Bilabial sounds are produced when the lips are brought together. Examples are [p],
which is voiceless, as in pay or [b] and [m] which are voiced, as in bay, may.
Labiodental sounds are made when the lower lip is raised towards the upper front
teeth. Examples are [f] safe (voiceless) and [v] save (voiced).
Dental sounds are produced by touching the upper front teeth with the tip of the
tongue. Examples are [S] oath (voiceless) and [C] clothe (voiced).
11
Alveolar sounds are made by raising the tip of the tongue towards the ridge
that is right behind the upper front teeth, called the alveolar ridge. Examples
are [ t,s ] too,sue, both voiceless, and [d,z,n,l,r ] do, zoo, nook, look, rook,
all voiced.
Palatoalveolar sounds are made by raising the blade of the tongue towards
the part of the palate just behind the alveolar ridge. Examples [R,tR] pressure,
Palatal sounds are very similar to palatoalveolar ones, they are just produced
further back towards the velum. The only palatal sound in English is [ j ] as
Velar sounds are made by raising the back of the tongue towards the soft
palate, called the velum. Examples [k] back, voiceless, and [g, M] both voiced
Glottal sounds are produced when the air passes through the glottis as it is
Manners of Articulation.
We can now distinguish between English consonants from two points of view, that
of voicing, and that of place. We can see that [b] and [t] are different in both respects,
[b] is voiced and bilabial, and [t] is voiceless and alveolar. [p] differs from [b] only in
being voiceless, as both are bilabial, and [p] differs from [t] only in being bilabial, as
both are voiceless.
There are still pairs of sounds where we cannot yet describe the difference of one
from the other, e.g. [b,m] bend, mend as both are voiced and bilabial, and [t,s] ton,
son which both are voiceless and alveolar. As the examples show, we can however
12
tell the words apart, and this is because the sounds are different in a way we have
not yet discussed, and that is with respect to their manner of articulation.
The manner of articulation has to do with the kind of obstruction the air meets on its
way out, after it has passed the vocal folds. It may meet a complete closure
(plosives), an almost complete closure (fricatives), or a smaller degree of closure
(approximants), or the air might escape in more exceptional ways, around the sides
of the tongue (laterals), or through the nasal cavity (nasals).
Plosives are sounds in which there is a complete closure in the mouth, so that the
air is blocked for a fraction of a second and then released with a small burst of sound,
called a plosion (it sounds like a very small explosion). Plosives may be bilabial [p,b]
park, bark, alveolar [t,d] tar, dark or velar [k,g] car, guard. There is a fourth kind of
plosive, the glottal stop. The word football can be pronounced without interruption in
the middle as in [fTtbN:l] or with a complete closure of the glottis instead of [t]:
[fT>bN:l].
In English a voiceless plosive that occurs at the begining of a word and is followed
by a vowel, is rather special in the sense that at the release of a plosion one can
hear a slight puff of air (called aspiration) before the vowel is articulated. Hence in
“pen “we hear [pçen]. These aspirated voiceless plosives are not considered to be
different sounds from unaspirated voiceless plosives from the point of view of how
they function in the sound system. This difference, which can be clearly heard, is
said to be phonetic.
Fricatives have a closure which is not quite complete. This means that the air is not
blocked at any point, and therefore there is no plosion. On the other hand the
obstruction is big enough for the air to make a noise when it passes through it,
because of the friction. This effect is similar to the wind whistling around the corner
of a house. Fricatives may be labiodental [f,v] wife, wives, dental [S,C] breath,
breathe, alveolar [s,z] sink, zinc, palato-alveolar [R,Y] nation, evasion, or glottal [h]
help. [h] is a glottal fricative. As it has no closure anywhere else, and as all air passes
between the vocal folds, this means that [h] is like aspiration unaccompanied by any
obstruction.
13
Affricates are a combination of a plosive and a fricative (sometimes they are called
"affricated plosives"). They begin like a plosive, with a complete closure, but instead
of a plosion, they have a very slow release, moving backwards to a place where a
friction can be heard (palatoalveolar). The two English affricates are both
palatoalveolar, [tR] which is voiceless, chin, rich, and [dY] which is voiced, gin, ridge.
The way an affricate resembles a plosive followed by a fricative is mirrored in the
symbols. Both consist of a plosive symbol followed by a fricative one: [ t+R], [d+Y].
Nasals resemble plosives, except that there is a complete closure in the mouth, but
as the velum is lowered the air can escape through the nasal cavity. Though most
sounds are produced with the velum raised, the normal position for the velum is
lowered, as this is the position for breathing (your velum is probably lowered right
now when you are reading this). The three English nasals are all voiced, and [m] is
bilabial, ram, [n] is alveolar, ran, and [M] velar, rang. In the section on places, the
dotted line on the pictures of bilabial, alveolar, and velar articulations illustrate the
three nasals.
Laterals are sounds where the air escapes around the sides of the tongue. There is
only one lateral in English, [l], a voiced alveolar lateral. It occurs in two versions, the
socalled "clear l" before vowels, light, long, and the "dark l" in other cases, milk, ball.
Words like little, lateral have one of each type. "Dark l" may be written with the
symbol [4]. "Clear l" is pronounced with the top of the tongue raised, whereas for
"dark l " it is the back of the tongue which is raised. Here again, as with aspirated
and unaspirated voiceless plosives, even though "clear l" and "dark l" are
phonetically different, they cannot be said to be different sounds from the point of
view of how they function in the sound system. If you produce a "dark l" where usually
you have a "clear l", for example at the beginning of the word long, your
pronunciation will sound odd but nobody will understand a different word.
14
Approximants are sounds where the tongue only approaches the roof of the mouth,
so that there is not enough obstruction to create any friction. English has three
approximants, which are all voiced. [r] is alveolar, right, brown, sometimes called
post-alveolar, because it is slightly further back that the other alveolar sounds
[t,d,s,l]. [j] is a palatal approximant, use, youth, and [w] is a velar approximant, why,
twin, square. [w] always has lip-rounding as well, and therefore it is sometimes called
labio-velar.
[r] only occurs before vowels in southern British English, whereas other accents, e.g.
Scottish, Irish, and most American ones, also can have it after vowels. Therefore
those accents can make a distinction between e.g. saw and sore, which are
pronounced exactly alike in southern British English.
15
Vowels
We shall first have a closer look at the way in which vowels differ from consonants.
Then we shall analyse vowels phonetically, i.e. according to:
- tongue position: how high in the mouth is the tongue, and which part of the tongue
is the highest?
Even though all the languages of the world contain both vowels and consonants,
and although almost everybody has some idea of whether a given sound is a vowel
or a consonant in his language, there is actually more than one way to distinguish
between the two classes of sounds. From a phonetic point of view one way of
distinguishing is by considering which sounds have the highest degree of
obstruction. Although vowels have almost no obstruction, and some consonants
(obstruents, nasals, and the lateral) have a high degree of obstruction, there is a
group of consonants (the approximants) which would be classified as vowels if this
criterion was used: approximants have no more obstruction than vowels. This can
be seen by comparing the approximant [j] in yeast [ji:st]with the vowel [i:] in east [i:st].
then there is no syllable anymore:* [kt]. [ a:] is then the sound that cannot be left out.
Compare with yeast whereas [j] can be left out, giving [i:st], [i:] can’t:*[jst]. Syllabicity
seems to be the criterion to determine whether a sound is a vowel or a consonant.
The above discussion would not be complete if we didn't mention the problem of
socalled syllabic consonants. This is the case when sounds like / r,l,n / may function
as a separate syllable consisting of an only sound, as in /kPt+n/ cotton or /zp+l/
apple, where English speakers clearly hear two separate syllables. In these words,
the /n/ and /l/ seem to function as the nucleus of the second syllable of these words.
However they cannot be classified as vowels, as they can never occur alone as a
word.
Tongue Position
Tongue position is described using two criteria: the height (how high is the tongue)
and the part of the tongue involved in the production of the sound. In English the
tongue may either be high, i.e. when the speaker produces e.g. [i:, u:] in [bi:t, bu:t]
beat, boot, intermediate, e.g. [e,N:] in [bet, bN:t] bet, bought, or low, e.g. [z,a:] in [bzt,
ba:t] bat, Bart.
The part of the tongue involved in the production of a vowel can also be illustrated
with the examples above. If you say [i:] and then [u:] just after it, you almost have
the feeling that you are moving your tongue backwards. This is because [i:] is a front
vowel, and [u:] is a back vowel, or in other words, the highest point in the
pronunciation of [i:] is the front of the tongue, whereas the highest point in [u:] is the
back of the tongue. Figure (17) gives you two examples of tongue position: a) is an
example of the front of the tongue being at the highest b) it is the back of the tongue
which is nearest to the palate.
For example [D] is front and [N:] is back, and [z] front, [@:] back. There are also
vowels in between front and back, called central, namely [2:,?,U] as in [w2:d, fN:w?d,
mUd] word, forward, mud. [2:] for instance is between [e] and [N:], as can be seen
from [bed, b2:d, bN:d], bed, bird, board.
To give an accurate account of tongue position one has to combine height of the
tongue and part of the tongue involved.
18
Length
As you may have seen, there are two types of [i] sound in English placed in two
different positions. However for the purpose of description, what is relevant is not the
difference of position but that of the perceived length of the vowel. Thus it is said that
[i:] is a long vowel and [H] is a short one. The same is valid for [u:] / [T], [2:]/[?], [N:]/
[P]. Symbols for long vowels all have a colon.
Phonologically, one can establish the rule such as only long vowels may be the last
sound of a syllable, whereas short vowels are always followed by at least a
consonant. If we take away the final [t] from court, [kN:] is a possible syllable (core)
whereas [kP] could not possibly occur. (Exceptions from this are the three short
vowels that occur in completely unstressed syllables, [sHtH, HntT, swet?] city, into,
sweater).
Rounding
Vowels may also be different from each other with respect to rounding. If you
compare [i:] in [tRi:z] cheese with [u:] in [tRu:z] choose, you will see that not only is
19
[i:] a front vowel and [u:] a back vowel, but [i:] is also unrounded where [u:] is
rounded. When pronouncing [u:] your lips are rounded, but when pronouncing [i:] the
corners of the mouth are much further apart.
Nasality
There are no nasal vowels in British English, i.e. no vowels in which the air also
escapes through the nose.
Diphthongs
So far we have only been considering vowels that were constant, i.e. vowels that
were pronounced at one and the same place. Such vowels are called
monophthongs, and English has 12 of them.
English also has 8 diphthongs, which are vowels that change character during their
pronunciation, that is, they begin at one place and move towards another place.
Compare for example the monophthong in car with the diphthong in cow, or the
monophthong in girl with the diphthong in goal. The vowels of cow and goal both
begin at a given place and glide towards another one . In goal the vowel begins as
if it was [?], but then it moves towards [T]. Therefore it is written [?T], as in [g?Tl]
goal, with two symbols, one for how it starts and one for how it ends.
20
Table of Vowels
As we saw above, the best way of noting the tongue position is by using the vowel
diagrams, but as they do not contain information about length and rounding, we can
summarise the description of English vowels in the following table:
i: long high front unrounded monophthong
PHONETIC TRANSCRIPTION
VIRTUAL RESOURCES
http://www.uiowa.edu/~acadtech/phonetics/english/english.html
ADRIAN UNDERHILL
http://www.macmillanenglish.com/phonemic-chart/
Youtube Videos
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QkfBiYXqp30
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=3ue4mrFG68Y
24
UNIT 2: PHONOLOGY
Phonemes
No language has a writing system that uniquely represents each sound in the
language. That is, no alphabet has a one-to-one correspondence between sounds
and letters. Instead, one letter may represent different sounds, and one sound may
be represented by different letters or letter sequences. In English, for example, the
same sound is represented by the c in cat and the k in kite. At the same time, the
letters ea have different sounds in tea, bread, steak, and idea. Linguists wishing to
study the sound system of a language need a more consistent method to analyze
the sounds than an alphabet provides. For that reason, they use phonemic
transcription.
In phonemic transcription, each sound is represented by one and only one written
mark. Phonemic transcription makes use of many of the letters of the alphabet but
uses them in a consistent way. For instance, the first sound of cat and kite is always
written with a /k/. Phonemes are indicated by putting them between slash marks. To
25
show the first phoneme in pet , a linguist would write /p/. A linguist could also show
more details in the pronunciation of a sound by using phonetic transcription. The first
sound of pet could be written as [ pʰ ]. Phonetic transcription is written with square
brackets. The small raised h represents a puff of air that speakers produce as they
make the /p/ sound at the beginning of a word. This feature, aspiration, is phonetic,
not phonemic, because in English, aspiration is never used to signal a change in
meaning.
Allophones
Now listen to the vowels in the words cat and cad. Are they identical or different?
We hope you said “different.” Can you now say how they differ? We hope you said
that one was longer than the other. Now listen to the consonants after the vowels.
Are these the same or different? Again, we hope you said different, and that you
know that [t] is voiceless and [d] is voiced. Now, which vowel, the longer or the
shorter, precedes [d] and which precedes [t]?
We hope you said that the longer vowel precedes the voiced consonant. Are the two
vowels similar in any way? Again, we hope you said that they seem to be longer and
shorter versions of the same vowel, [{]. Let’s use [:] to indicate extra length. So, the
vowel before voiceless [t] is just [{], but the one before voiced [d] is [{:].
We have already noted that if we substitute the aspirated allophone of /t/ for its
unaspirated relative, then we create an odd pronunciation of a word. [t Il] is the
typical American English pronunciation of till, but [tIl] is not. What, if any, patterns
can we observe in where allophones of a phoneme can and cannot occur?
phoneme before a voiced consonant will be appreciably longer (up to three times
longer) than the allophone of the same vowel phoneme before a voiceless
consonant. For example, listen to the pronunciation of /E/ in bet and bed. You should
have little difficulty in hearing the difference in vowel length. We can represent the
pattern of occurrence (distribution) of these two allophones of the phoneme /E/ as
the following phonological rule: When the phoneme /E/ occurs before a voiceless
consonant it is pronounced as its allophone [E]; when it occurs before a voiced
consonant it is pronounced as its allophone [E:]. (Remember: [:] is a diacritic
indicating a lengthened sound.)
In fact, the rule is much more general than this. Because it applies to all vowels, we
can write it as: In English a vowel is longer before a voiced consonant than it is
before a voiceless one. One of our objectives in studying a language is to be able to
describe these sound patterns, i.e., to be able to specify in the most general terms
possible the phonetic environments in which each allophone occurs.
Let’s look at another very systematic set of English vowel allophones. The vowels of
cap and can differ phonetically: that of cap is a plain []; that of can is nasalized,
represented by [{~]. (If you have trouble hearing the difference, try starting to say
each word normally and then omit the final consonant.) The phoneme // thus has
the allophones, [] and [{~]. In fact, all English vowels have both nasalized and non-
nasalized allophones. We can represent this as the rule: Whenever an English vowel
occurs before a nasal consonant, it becomes nasalized; otherwise it is non-nasalized
In fact, the situation is a bit more intricate that this. Because nasals are voiced, we
should expect a vowel before them to be lengthened relative to the same vowel
before a voiceless sound. And, indeed, this is what we find. Listen to the vowels in
cat, cad, and can. You should notice that the first vowel is unlengthened, [{]; the
second one is lengthened, [{:]; and the third one is both lengthened (in fact, probably
even more than the second one) and nasalized, [{~:].
27
Phonemic distinctions in a language can be tested via pairs and sets of words. When
two words such as pat and bat are identical in form except for a contrast in one
phoneme, occurring in the same position, the two words are described as a minimal
pair. More accurately, they would be classified as a minimal pair in the phonology of
English. (Arabic, for example, does not have this contrast between /p/ and /b/.) Other
examples of English minimal pairs are fan–van, bet–bat, site–side. Such pairs have
traditionally been used in the teaching and testing of English as a second or foreign
language to help students develop the ability to understand the contrast in meaning
based on the minimal sound contrast.
When a group of words can be differentiated, each one from the others, by changing
one phoneme (always in the same position in the word), then we have a minimal set.
For example, one minimal set based on the vowel phonemes of English could
include feat, fit, fat, fate, fought, foot, and another minimal set based on consonant
phonemes could have big, pig, rig, fig, dig, wig.
Phonotactics
This type of exercise involving minimal sets also allows us to see that there are
definite patterns in the types of sound combinations permitted in a language. In
English, the minimal set we have just listed does not include forms such as lig or vig.
According to the dictionary, these are not English words, but they could be viewed
as possible English words. That is, our phonological knowledge of the pattern of
sounds in English words would allow us to treat these forms as acceptable if, at
some future time, they came into use. They might, for example, begin as invented
abbreviations (I think Bubba is one very ignorant guy. ~ Yeah, he’s a big vig!). Until
then, they represent “accidental” gaps in the vocabulary of English. It is, however,
no accident that forms such as [fsɪɡ] or [rnɪɡ] do not exist or are unlikely ever to exist.
They have been formed without obeying some constraints on the sequence or
position of English phonemes. Such constraints are called the phonotactics (i.e.
permitted arrangements of sounds) in a language and are obviously part of every
28
Teaching Pronunciation
Teaching PHONICS
http://busyteacher.org/16913-teach-phonics-esl-classroom.html
RESOURCES
Vaughan-Rees, M. (2010). Rhymes and rhythm: a poem-based course for english pronunciation
study. Garnet Education.
Freeman, D. E., & Freeman, Y. S. (2004). Essential linguistics: What you need to know to teach
reading, ESL, spelling, phonics, and grammar. Heinemann Educational Books.
Brinton, L. J. (2000). The structure of modern English: a linguistic introduction. John Benjamins
Publishing.
32
Stress
Every word spoken in isolation has at least one stressed syllable. In articulatory
terms, stress involves a rise in air pressure; an increase in the activity of the
respiratory muscles forces more air out of the lungs during the articulation of a
particular syllable. There may also be an increase in the activity of the larynx,
resulting in higher pitch. In acoustic terms, the stressed syllable is perceived as
longer, louder, and of higher pitch. The term stress is sometimes used
interchangeably with accent, but accent should not be confused here with the other
use of the term to refer to dialect features (as in “a British accent”).
Certain languages in the world have an accentual system based on pitch diVerence,
not stress diVerences. That is, syllables carry varying levels of pitch, and pitch
diVerences alone can distinguish words. These tonal languages include Chinese,
Thai, West African languages, and Amerindian languages. But English has stress
accent.
To find the stressed syllable in English, say a polysyllabic word and tap your finger
at the same time. You will naturally tap on the stressed syllable. The reason for this
is that it is easier to produce one increase in muscular activity in conjunction with
another, so you use your respiratory muscles and your hand muscles
simultaneously. If you try to tap on an unstressed syllable, you will get a distortion in
the pronunciation of the word. Try saying the following word while tapping your finger
(the stressed syllable is marked):
The rule for stress in Germanic words is very simple: words are always stressed on
the first syllable (as in ápple, fáther, húnger), except prefixed verbs, which are
stressed on the root syllable (as in forgét, belíeve, withdráw). However, English has
borrowed many words from the Romance languages, which have a diVerent stress
principle: stress falls on the penultimate syllable, as in admónish, unless there are
two consonants or a tense vowel at the end, as in adápt, exíst. The result is that the
33
stress system of Modern English is now very complex, and accent is not entirely
predictable.
For example, if you say computation in isolation or at the end of a sentence, the ta
syllable will carry the strong stress, but the com syllable will also carry a seemingly
weaker stress. What is actually happening here is superimposition of an intonational
pattern (discussed in the next section) called a tonic accent onto the last stressed
syllable. So we say that ta carries primary stress and com carries secondary stress,
thus còmputátion. Secondary stress is sometimes diYcult to hear, but generally it will
be separated by at least one syllable — either before or after — from the syllable
carrying primary stress, as follows:
That is, secondary stress will occur in words where the stressed syllable is followed
by two or more syllables or where the stressed syllable is preceded by two or more
syllables.
In transcription, the IPA system of marking stress is the use of a superscript tick
before the primary stressed syllable and a subscript tick before the secondary
stressed syllable, e.g., eligibility [À7lIJˇ6ÁbIl6ti], or only primary stress is indicated.
Unstressed syllables are not marked.
Stress is a meaningful feature of speech in respect to both words and word groups
in English. It has functions in the province of morphology, syntax, and discourse:
noun verb
próduce prodúce
áddress addréss
import impórt
ínsult insúlt
súrvey survey
íncline inclíne
éxport expórt
There are also derivationally-related pairs that show the same stress pattern:
concéive(V) and cóncept (N), procéed (V) and prócess (N), or preténd (V)
and prétense (N). But note that there are many exceptions: respéct and
rewárd are both a noun and a verb; cómment is both a noun and a verb; and
díffer and defér are different verbs; compare also belíeve (V) and belíef (N).
A word, as we shall see in the next chapter, has only one primary stress, while
a phrase has more than one.
N A
pícture picturésque
jóurnal journalese
4. Stress is not unrelated to full vowels; unstressed vowels may or may not be
reduced to [6], while stressed vowels are generally full (see above):
Another important aspect related to stress is that of the “weak forms”. There is a
number of words in English (almost all of which belong to the category called function
or grammatical words) which can be pronounced in two different ways, a strong and
a weak form. There are about forty such words and it is important to be aware of
their existence as they can provoke misunderstandings. English-speaking people
find the strong forms unnatural and learners of English can misunderstand English
speakers, who will surely use weak forms.
There are some rules to learn. The strong form will be used when:
a) they occur at the end of a sentence, as in “Chips are what I’m fond of”.
36
b) A weak-form word is being contrasted with another word, as in “The letter is from
him, not to him.”
c) A weak-form word is given stress for the purpose of emphasis, as in “You must
give me more money.”
d) A weak-form is being “cited” or “quoted”, as in “You shouldn’t put “and” at the end
of a sentence.”
A (ə)
AND (ən)
BUT (bət)
THAN (ðən)
AT (ət or æt)
OF (əv or ɒv)
TO (tə or tu)
AS (əz or æz)
MUST (m s, m st or m st)
The second example shows that we usually don’t stress verbs but must stress
an accompanying auxiliary verb.
If the old information— the topic— concerns having pizza for dinner, then the first
sentence might be used to question specifically whether the addressee wants it
(“you” as opposed to “your brother”). If the topic concerns the addressee’s wants
38
for dinner, then the second sentence might be used to question what food he or
she wants (“pizza” as opposed to “spaghetti”). If the topic concerns having pizza
for some meal, then the third sentence might be used to question which meal
pizza should be served at (“dinner” rather than “lunch”). The third sentence — in
which the last noun in the clause receives the greatest prominence — is also the
most neutral version of this question, where no particular item is being unduly
stressed .
When we speak entire sentences, we do not, in fact, stress every word. Try doing
so and see how unnatural it sounds. Instead, we stress only certain words, and
unstress others. Generally, we place stress on the major parts of speech, or
content words (the nouns, verbs, adjectives, and adverbs), and remove stress
from the minor parts of speech, or function words (the prepositions, conjunctions,
pronouns, articles, and so on). Consider the following sentence:
We would expect autumn, dry, yellow, leaves, fall, and trees to carry stress, and
in, the, and from not to. Moreover, in English these stresses fall as much as
possible at regular intervals, making English a “stress-timed” language. The
amount of time necessary for an utterance depends upon the number of stressed
syllables, with unstressed syllables occupying much less time than stressed
ones. Say the following sentences and note where the stresses fall. Observe that
certain content words which one might expect to carry stress do not do so:
39
ártificial artifícial
ábsent-minded absent-mínded
arístocratic aristocratic
Because the placement of stress in a sentence is a matter of spacing out the stresses
as evenly as possible and because it is usual to place stress near to the end of the
sentence in English, on the last major part of speech, pérson carries stress; as a
consequence, stress on the preceding adjective is placed as far from it as possible,
on the first or second syllable. In contrast, since véry carries emphatic stress, the
stress on the following adjective is placed as late as possible, on the penultimate
syllable.
40
Rhythm
Speech, as with all bodily movements such as breathing, walking, heart-beat, etc.,
is highly rhythmical; it tends to have a regular beat. But what marks the beat differs
is various languages. Pike distinguished two kinds of rhythm in languages: (i)
syllable-timed rhythm, where syllables tend to occur at regular intervals of time, and
consequently all syllables tend to have the same length (e.g. Spanish and French)
and (ii) stressed-limed rhythm, where stressed syllables tend to occur at regular
intervals. That means that the syllables might vary in length since there might be a
varying number of syllables between stresses. English is a stress-timed language.
In the following Spanish sentence syllables have the same length and occur at
regular intervals:
In the equivalent English sentence, syllables vary in length but stressed syllables
occur regularly:
In English, rhythm is organized into feet (Abercrombie 1964). The foot begins with
the stressed syllable and includes all the unstressed syllables up to the next stress
where a new foot begins. The above English sentence has four stresses and
consequently four feet. Using slashes to indícate foot boundary we could represent
feet as follows: I / 'want you to / 'come with me to the / 'doctor's to / 'morrow. The
beat at the beginning of the foot might be silent, we mark this silent beat with a caret
(„).
No language is purely stressed-timed or syllable-timed but tends to behave more
like one or the other pattern. Delattre argües that Germán, for instance, takes a
position midway between English and Spanish with respect to rhythm. Catalán
seems to be a similar case.
Rhythm is also tempo dependent. The faster the speech, the more stressed-timed
the rhythm (Angenot et al). Thus, Spanish or Portuguese, said to be syllable-timed
languages, become more stressed-timed when spoken at a fast rate, although
41
vowels keep their distinctive quality. Thus, the following Spanish and Brazilian
Portuguese sentences uttered at a fast speaking rate might result in a rhythmic
pattern basically stressed-determined:
[kek'tvaf'ze]
The basic differences between syllable-timed languages (such as Spanish) and
stressed-timed languages (such as English) are:
syllable-timed stressed-limed
3. In English, speaking rate (fast vs slow speech) does not affect the duration of
stressed and unstressed syllables proportionally. In Spanish, speaking rate effects
on the duration of stressed and unstressed vowels is roughly the same.
Rhythmic Alternation
English rhythm, apart from being conditioned by the principles of isochrony and
stress-timing, is characterized by the principle of rhythmic alternation; that is, in
English weak and strong syllables alternate with one another. How is it that regular
alternation of stressed and unstressed syllables results, if speakers do not choose
their words according to their lexical stress pattern, but to the meaning they want to
express and the form in which they want to couch this meaning? It seems that once
the lexemes of an utterance have been selected, a set of rules (syntactical,
morphological, phonetic and rhythmic) are applied to the utterance to determine its
phonetic realization. Thus, a structure which does not observe the stress alternation
principle (because too many stresses appear next to each other, or there are long
sequences of unstressed syllables) will be modified by the rhythm rules to conform
more closely the ideal rhythmic alternation.
43
1. If a series of content words appear next to each other, some stresses are
dropped. There is a stress on each of the following content words: 'John's I
'friend l'wants l'get l'home Ibe'fore l'midnight ; but some of these stresses,
usually stresses on alternate words, are dropped when these words occur in
a sentence such as: 'John's friend 'wants to get 'home before 'midnight. Tap
with your finger at each stressed syllable while saying this sentence and you
will find it quite natural to tap al the syllables marked as stressed. Similarly, if
an utterance contains a succession of three stresses, as in 'B 'B 'C ; 'big
/'blue/'eyes ; 'five l'eight l'two , the intermediate stress tends to be dropped in
order to achieve a more regular alternation: 'BB'C, 'big blue 'eyes, 'five eight
'two.
There are constraints on stress addition and deletion. Stresses cannot be deleted
just anywhere. Thus, in the above utterance John's friend wants to get home
before midnight, the stresses on John/home I midnight cannot be deleted though
others can. Similarly, stresses cannot be added just anywhere. If an extra stress
is to be added in 'leave it in the 'car, due to a very deliberate and slow speaking
rate, it cannot be added on the determiner, but must be added on the preposition:
'leave it 'in the 'car. Based on evidence of this kind Giegerich (46) suggests a
hierarchy of stressable words.
(4) x x (4) x x
(3) x x (3) x x x
(2) x x x -* (2) x x x
(1)XXXXX (1 ) X X X X X
If two stressed syllables are immediately next to each other on an upper level, with
no intervening beats at the level below, there is a «stress clash» and stress shifts
towards a leftmost strong syllable (at least at level 2). Thus, ,Japa'nese 'students
becomes 'Japanese 'students, but be'tween 'cars does not become *'between 'cars,
because the first syllable in between is not a strong syllable.
45
To sum up, the factors that contribute to maintain a regular rhythm in English are of
two kinds. (1) There are those which affect the distribution of stresses to ensure
rhythmic alternation:
a. dropping of some stresses to prevent too many stresses coming together, eg.
'nice 'oíd 'book —*• 'nice oíd 'book;
c. stress movement to a preceding strong syllable if two stresses are next to each
other: Heath'row 'airport —* 'Heathrow 'airport.
(2) There are those which affect adjustments in the length of sounds to keep a
constant time interval between stresses.
c. Weak forms.
46
Teaching Pronunciation
http://jazzchants.net/home
Find out about Carolyn Graham and her jazz chants. Discuss how you can use them
in class.
Find out what LIMERICKS are, and discuss how they can help you teach Stress and
Rhythm.
47
RESOURCES
Celce-Murcia, M., Brinton, D. M., & Goodwin, J. M. (1996). Teaching pronunciation: A reference for
teachers of English to speakers of other languages. Cambridge University Press.
Kelly, G. (2006). How To Teach Pronunciation (With Cd). Pearson Education India.
48
Intonation
In studying intonation, it has been the practice to recognize either different levels of
pitch (generally four levels) or different intonational contours, described as falling or
rising.
Using the latter approach, we identify a number of different pitch patterns, which
convey different meanings:
(continuation marker);
49
We can see the meaning of these pitch patterns even in a one-word sentence:
short falling: Yes. “The answer is ‘yes’, but I am impatient with your question or find
it unimportant”.
Here there are two tone groups, corresponding to syntactic units, both with falling
intonation. A more formal style might consist of three tone groups:
Each tone group contains a tonic syllable, which carries the major shift in intonation.
Usually, the tonic syllable is the last stressed syllable in the tone group. It expresses
50
the information which the speaker considers new (unknown) and most important, as
in the following sentences where the tonic syllable is underlined:
Did you get the job? vs. Did you lose your job?
Let’s now examine pitch patterns in different sentence types. In each example the
tonic syllable is underlined.
a. A whale is a mammal. (Here the topic of the conversation is whales. What the
2. A yes/no question has a long rising intonation pattern, since it expects an answer.
d. Do you want coffee,// or tea? (This actually represents two yes/no questions, the
e. You are giving up coffee? (Note that this is not syntactically a question, but the
f. You bought a new car,// eh? (The particle eh and the rising intonation turns this
51
An echo question, which asks for the repetition of what has been said before, also
has long rising or falling-rising intonation.
a. He said what?
3. A wh-question has a long falling intonation pattern (like a statement) since this
type of
a. Where did you put the paper? (This is the neutral emphasis.)
b. Where did you put the paper? (This focuses on “you” as new information.)
4. A tag question has two tone groups; the first half is syntactically a statement, while
the second half is syntactically a question .
a. You will help,// won’t you? (This follows the expected pattern where the first
tone unit has (short) falling intonation because it is a statement and the second tone
b. He bought a paper,// didn’t he? (This is not a real question, but merely asks
for confirmation, so unlike (a), the second tone unit has long falling intonation.)
5. An alternative question consists of two tone groups as well, the first having
question intonation and the second statement intonation:
52
a. Did you buy a paper,// or not? (The first pattern is long rising, and the second long
falling. The second tone unit acts as a statement added as a kind of afterthought.)
6. A list has a number of tone groups with short rising intonation pattern indicating
that the discourse continues:
a. I bought some apples,// oranges,// and peaches. (The last tone group is falling
because this is a statement. Long falling intonation on the first two tone groups
yields a very slow, deliberate, solemn style, while long rising intonation of these
tone units yields a highly dramatized style, often used when addressing children.)
b. Do you want an apple, an orange, or a peach? (The last tone group is rising
Complex sentences have a similar pattern— short rising followed by the appropriate
end intonation — whether the subordinate clause precedes or follows the main
clause:
a. Are you sure? (The vowel of the tonic syllable may be elongated.)
b. It’s raining?
53
b. We’re in for some hard times, I think. (In this case, there may also be short rising
Connected Speech
Before this chapter gets to the discussion of individual connected speech features,
it gives a comprehensive overview of what connected speech is made up of. Firstly,
we need to distinguish pronunciation of words in isolation (in their citation forms)
from words in connected speech. In connected speech, pronunciation of words is
highly influenced by their surrounding sounds as well as by context. From my
experience, pronunciation of words is usually only taught in isolation in their citation
forms and students are left to their own devices to figure out how they work in the
flow of speech. “Citation forms are a form of speech that is, to say the least,
somewhat idealised” (Giegerich 1994, p.249). In authentic communication, there are
no boundaries between words; instead, they flow smoothly in a stream of sound.
“The stretch of speech is continuous; we stop and pause for breath from time to time
[but] none of the units has clearly identifiable boundaries, some definite point in time
where it begins and ends” (Halliday 2004, p.5). Furthermore, in speaking, words are
sometimes swallowed or linked to make a new sound, which makes it difficult for a
learner to understand and reproduce English native speech. It is certainly reasonable
that students learn word pronunciation in isolation, but a teacher should also allocate
some of the teaching time to teaching linking, correct stressing, rhythm or intonation.
Linking
Consonant + vowel
It usually happens in English speech that word-final consonant is linked to the first
vowel sound of the following word. Hence, if we consider a sentence “These are old
eggs”, in connected speech it becomes [ðiː.zә.rәʊl.deɡz] (Giererich 1992, p.280).
What happens in this example is what Underhill calls “fully liaised speech”,
“characterized by a seamless, continuous quality, where final consonants are linked
to following vowel sounds, and initial consonants to preceding final vowel sounds”
(Underhill 2005). Giegerich (1992) sustains that such consonants are probably not
totally disassociated from the preceding syllables, but they become ambisyllabic.
55
These two phenomena are a natural way to connect two words where the first one
ends in a vowel and the second start with a vowel. When a word ends in /i:/, or a
diphthong which finishes with /i/, speakers often introduce a /j/ to ease the transition
to a following vowel sound and when a word ends in /u:/, or a diphthong which
finishes with /ʊ/, speakers often introduce a /w/ to ease the transition to a following
vowel sound (Kelly 2002, p.31).
Assimilation
“This term describes how sounds modify each other when they meet, usually across
word boundaries” (Kelly 2002, p.109). When words are pronounced separately in
their citation forms, their pronunciation differs from pronunciation in natural
connected speech in which they are joined together without any clear boundaries. In
such cases, “we employ an economy of effort, and get our articulators ready for the
next sound. Certain sounds are either absorbed or modified into others” (Kelly 2002,
p.109). It is important to note that, in its extent, assimilation is a variable
phenomenon. “It is more likely to be found in rapid, casual speech and less likely in
slow, careful speech. The cases that have most been described are assimilations
affecting consonants” (Roach 110, 2009). Kenworthy (1990) describes 7 basic rules
for assimilation as explained below. The first five are examples of regressive
assimilation, in which the first consonant of the second word influences the
articulation of the last consonant at the end of the first word, and the last two are
cases of progressive assimilation (coalescence), where the two consonants merge
to create a sound that shares the characteristics of both of the consonants:
56
- /t/, /d/ and /n/ often become bilabial before bilabial consonants /p/, /b/,
/m/:
- /t/ assimilates into /k/ before /k/ or /g/. /d/ assimilates into /g/ before /k/
or /g/:
All the above mentioned rules concern assimilation of place. Another type of
assimilation is assimilation of manner. “It is much less noticeable, and is only found
in the most rapid and casual speech” (Roach 2009, p.112). This type of assimilation
will not be discussed here in more detail as it was not included in the course syllabus
so as to adjust it to the learners‟ level of English. Assimilation of voicing on the other
hand is an important aspect of English pronunciation and therefore, it was included
in the syllabus.
- Final /v/ and /d/ becomes voiceless /f/ and /t/ because of the following
voiceless /t/
„have to‟
„used to‟
„supposed to
A closely related topic is progressive assimilation of voice with the suffix “s” (third
person singular suffix, noun plural suffix or possessive suffix). “[It] will be pronounced
as /s/ if the preceding consonant is fortis (“voiceless”) and as /z/ if the preceding
consonant is lenis (“voiced”)” (Roach 2009, p.113), thus:
The same way the preceding consonant affects the articulation of “s”, it does with
the “-ed” verb and adjective suffix. Hence:
„kissed‟ [kɪst]
„played‟ [pleɪd]
„washed‟ [wɒʃt]
58
„bored‟ [bɔːrd]
„asked‟ [æskt]
„explained‟ [ɪkˈspleɪnd]
In both cases, the distinction is most tangible when such a word is followed by a
vowel sound, because the consonant is linked to the vowel creating a more
resounding sound:
“Much more could be said about assimilation, but from the point of view of learning
or teaching English pronunciation, to do so would not be very useful” (Roach 2009,
p.113).Roach (2009) argues that it is essentially a natural phenomenon and the only
important matter is to remember the restriction, specific to English, on voicing
assimilation mentioned above. A great deal of attention was also given to the study
of coalescence in phrases such as “did you”, “would you” or “don‟t you”
Elision
“The nature of elision can be stated quite simply: under certain circumstances
sounds disappear” (Roach 2009, p.113). It is, again, characteristic of rapid, casual
speech. Roach (2009) says that foreign learners do not need to learn to produce
elisions, but it is important for them to be aware that “when native speakers of English
talk to each other, quite a number of phonemes that the foreigner might expect to
hear are not actually pronounced” (p.113). The objective of the course was not for
the students to start using elision and other linking features immediately, it was,
however, supposed to help improve their receptive skills and possibly give them the
opportunity to start incorporating it in their speech as they become more advanced
and comfortable in all the areas of the English language.
59
Some basic rules for elision as Kelly (2002) lists them are:
„George the Sixth‟s throne‟ [sɪksθ θroʊn] can be simplified to/ˈ[sɪks θroʊn]
In non-rhotic varieties of English, speakers often pronounce the phoneme “r” that
would not normally be articulated to link the final “r” to a word beginning with a vowel.
It can be demonstrated on the example of the word car:
60
In the second example, the “r” is clearly articulated as it is followed by a vowel sound.
This phenomenon is called linking “r”. Similar case is that of intrusive “r”. “BBC
speakers often use “r” in a similar way to link words ending with a vowel, even when
there is no justification from the spelling” (Roach 2009, p.115).
Although these are important connected speech features, they were not included in
the syllabus, mainly because they are specific to some non-rhotic varieties of English
and the teacher-provided model was a rhotic one. Besides, the majority of the course
participants employed rhoticity in their speech.
Teaching Pronunciation
COMMUNICATIVE STRATEGIES
http://cambridgeenglishonline.com/Phonetics_Focus/
62
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Freeman, D. E., & Freeman, Y. S. (2004). Essential linguistics: What you need
to know to teach reading, ESL, spelling, phonics, and grammar. Heinemann
Educational Books.