"Repressive-defensiveness" predicts basal cortisol levels. "Denial and repressive style" predicts information gain and rehabilitation outcomes. The studies of James Gross concerning the intentional inhibition of emotional expression can be found in]. Gross and R. Levenson.
"Repressive-defensiveness" predicts basal cortisol levels. "Denial and repressive style" predicts information gain and rehabilitation outcomes. The studies of James Gross concerning the intentional inhibition of emotional expression can be found in]. Gross and R. Levenson.
"Repressive-defensiveness" predicts basal cortisol levels. "Denial and repressive style" predicts information gain and rehabilitation outcomes. The studies of James Gross concerning the intentional inhibition of emotional expression can be found in]. Gross and R. Levenson.
"Repressive-defensiveness" predicts basal cortisol levels. "Denial and repressive style" predicts information gain and rehabilitation outcomes. The studies of James Gross concerning the intentional inhibition of emotional expression can be found in]. Gross and R. Levenson.
Copyright:
Attribution Non-Commercial (BY-NC)
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online from Scribd
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 7
ROBERT M SAPOLSKY
“Developmental and Personal ry Correlates of Adrenocortical Activity as
Indexed by Salivary Cortisol: Observations in the Age Range of 35 c0 65
Years” Journal of Psycbasomatic Raearch 35 (1991): 173. This isthe study of
Hellhammer and colleagues.
L. Brown, A. Tomarken, D. Orch, P Loosen, N. Kalin, and R, Davide
son, "Individual Differences in Repressive-Defensiveness Predict Basal
Salivary Corcsol Level.” Jourwal af Personality and Sucial Pecholopy 70
(1996): 362.
R. Shaw, F. Cohen, R.Fishrran-Rosen, M. Murphy, . Scerzer, D. Cll,
and K. Myler, "Psychologie Predictors of Psychosocial and Medical Outcomes
in Patients Undergoing Coronary Angioplasty,” Prychaomaric Medicine 48
(1986): 582.
R. Shaw; F. Cohen, B. Doyle, and J Palesky, “The Impact of Denial and
Repressive Style on Information Gain and Rchabilitacion Outcomes in
‘Myocardial Infarction Patients,” Prchasmatic Medicine 47 (1985): 262.
‘A. Tomarken and R. Davidkon, “Frontal Brain Activation in Repressors
and Nonrepressors,” Journal of Abnormal Payebulegy 103 (1994): 339,
D. Weinberger, G. Schwara, and R. Davidson, *“Low-Anxious, High
Anxious, and Repressive Coping Styles: Psychometric Patterns and Behav-
‘ral and Physiological Responses co Sttes,” Journal of Abnormal Prychology
88 (1979): 369,
‘The studies of James Gross concerning the intentional inhibition of
‘emotional expression can be found in J. Gross and R. Levenson,
tional Suppression: Physiology, SelReport, and Expressive Behavior,”
Jonrnal of Pronalty and Social Prychaogy 64 (1993): 870. See aso: J. Gross
‘and R. Levenson, "Hiding Feelings: The Acute Effects of Inhibiting Neg-
ative and Positive Emotion," Journal of Abnormal Payholey (in ptes)
146
The Trouble with Testosterone
Wilt. Boys JusT BE Boys?
6809
le Eu, Heath Thnogh Sport
Th Man Cation, Howton
“7about certain minorities, The stereotypes are typically
pejorative and usually false. But every now and then, they
are true, I writ
F« it, we all do ic. Weall believe in certain stereotypes
c apologeticelly as a member of a minority
about which the stereotypes are indeed true. I am male, We
‘males accoune for less than 50 percent of the population, yee we
generate an incredibly disproportionate percentage of the vio-
lence. Whether ic is something as primal as having an ax fight
in an Amazonian clearing or as detached as using computer-
guided aircraft co strafe a village, someching as condemned as
assauleing a cripple or as glorified as killing someone wearing the
‘wrong uniform, ifit is violene, males excel atic.
Why should thac be? We all chink we know the answer. A
dozen millennia ago or so, an adventurous soul managed to
lop off a surly bull's testicles and thus invented behavioral
endocrinology. It is unclear from the historical records
whether this individual received either a grant or tenure as a
result of this experiment, bur ir certainly generated an influ
‘ential finding—something or other comes out of the testes
that helps co make males such aggressive pains in the ass.
That something or other is testosterone.” The hormone
“Testosterone is one ofa family of related hormones, collectively known,
1s “androgens” or “anabolic steroids” They all are sereted from the estes
449,ROBERT M. SAPOLSKY
binds to specialized receptors in muscles and causes those cells,
to enlarge. It binds to similar receptors in laryngeal cells and
zives rise co operatic basses. It causes other secondary sexual
characteristics, makes for relatively unhealthy blood vessels,
alters biochemical events in the liver too dizzying co even con-
template, has @ profound impact, no doubsr, on the workings
of cells in big toes. And it seeps into the brain, where it binds
to those same “androgen” receptors and influences behavior in
way highly relevant co understanding aggression.
‘What evidence links testosterone with aggression? Some
pretty obvious stuff: Males zend to have higher testosterone
levels in their circulation than do females (one wild exception
to chat will be discussed later) and eo be more aggressive.
Times of life when males ace swimming in escosterone (for
example, after reaching puberty) correspond co when aggres-
sion peaks. Among numerous species, cesces are mothballed
most of the year, kicking into action and pouring out testos-
terone only during a very circumscribed mating season—pre-
cisely the time when male-male aggeession soars.
Impressive, but these are only correlative data, eestosterone
repeatedly being on the scene with no alibi when some
aggression has occurred. The proof comes with the knife, the
performance of what is euphemistically known as a "subtrac-
tion” experiment. Remove the source of testosterone in
species after species and levels of aggression typically plum-
met. Reinstare normal testosterone levels afterward with
injections of synthetic testosterone, and aggression recurns.
To an endocrinologist, the subtraction and replacement
for are the resule of a modification of testosterone, they all have a similar
chemical structure, and they all do roughly similar things. Nonetheless,
androgen mavens spend entire carers seudying the important differences
in the actions of differen androgens. Lam going to throw that subtlety ro
the wind and, for the sake of siplification chae will hortiy many, will
refer throughout to all ofthese related hormones as “ceststeron
150.
:
|
1
/
:
|
|
i
|
|
'
THE TROUBLE WITH TESTOSTERONE
paradigm represents preety damaing. proof: this hormone is
involved. “Normal testosterone levels appear to be a prerequi-
site for notmative levels of aggressive behavior” is the sore of
catchy, hummable phrase that the eextbooks would use. ‘That
probably explains why you shouldn't mess wich a bull moose
duting rutting season. But that’s not why a lot of people wane
to understand this sliver of scierce. Does the action of this
hormone tell us anything about individual differences in levels
of aggression, anything about why some males, some human,
males, are exceptionally violenc? Among. an array of males—
human or otherwise—are the highest eestosterone levels
found in the most aggressive individuals?
Generate some extreme differences and that is precisely
what you see. Castrate some of che well-paid seudy subjects,
inject others with enough testosterone to quadruple the normal
human levels, and che high-testosterone males are over-
whelmingly likely to be the more aggressive ones. However, that
loesn't tell us much about the real world. Now do something,
more subtle by studying the normative vaciabilicy ia cestos-
tetone—in other words, don’t manipulate anything, just see
what everyone's natural levels are like—and high levels of
testosterone and high levels of aggression still cend co go
together. This would seem eo seal che case—interindividual dif
ferences in levels of aggression among normal individuals are
probably driven by differences in levels of testosterone. But this,
turns out to be wrong.
Okay, suppose you note a co:relation becween levels of
aggression and levels of testosterone among these normal
‘males. This could be because (a) testosterone elevates aggres-
sion; (L) aggression elevates testosterone secretion; (c) neither
causes the other. There's a huge bias to assume option 4, while
‘is the answer. Study after scudy has shown that when you
examine testosterone levels when males are first placed
cogether in the social group, testosterone levels predice noth
151
: a3 so
FensROBERT M, SAPOLSKY
ing about who is going to be aggressive. The subsequent
behavioral differences drive che hormonal changes, racher
than the other way around.
Because of a strong bias among certain scientists, it has
taken forever co convince them of this point. Behavioral
cendocrinologists study what behavior and hormones have to do
‘with each other. How do you study behavior? You get yourself
a notebook and a stopwatch and a pair of binoculars. How do
you measure the hormones? You need a gazillion-dollar
machine, you muck around with radiation and chemicals,
wear a lab coat, maybe even goggles—the whole nine yards.
Which coys would you ra:her get for Christmas? Which facet
of science are you going to believe in more? Because che
endocrine aspects of the business are more high-tech, more
reductive, there is the bias to think thae it is somehow
more scientific, more powerful. This is a classic case of what is
often called physics envy, the disease among scientists where
the behavioral biologists fear their discipline lacks the rigor of
physiology, the physiologists wish for the techniques of the bio-
chemists, the biochemists covet the clarity of che answers
revealed by the molecula” biologists, all the way down until
you get co the physicists, who confer only with God.” Hor-
‘mones seem to many co be more real, more substantive, chan
the ephemera of behavier, so when a correlation occurs, it
“An example of physics ervy in action. Recently, a zoologist friend
had obtained blood samples from the carnivores thae he studies and
syanted some hormones in thesample assays in my lab, Alehough inexpe=
rienced with the technique, he olfered to help in any way possible. I ele
hesicant asking him co do anything cedious bat, so long as he had offered,
reneativly sad, “Well if you don’e mind some unspeakable drudgery, you
‘ould number aboue a chousand assay vials.” And chis scientist, whose
superb work has graced the mest prestigious science journals in the world,
cheerfully answered, "Thar's ay, how often do I get to do rea! science,
working with tse cubes?”
THE TROUBLE WITH TESTOSTERONE
must be because hormones regulate behavior, not che ocher
way around.
As I said, ie eakes a lot of work to cure people of chat
physics envy, and to see that interindividual differences in
testosterone levels don’e predict subsequent differences in
aggressive behavior among individuals. Similarly, fluctuations
in cescosterone levels within one individual over time do not
predict subsequent changes in the levels of aggression in that
one individual—gee a hiccup in testosterone secretion one
afternoon and that's not when the guy goes postal
Look at our confusing state: normal levels of testosterone
are a prerequisite for normal levels of aggression, yet chang-
ing the amoune of testosterone in someone's bloodscream
within the normal range doesn't alter his subsequent levels of
aggressive behavior. This is where, like clockwork, the stu-
dents suddenly stare coming ec office hours in a panic, asking,
whether they missed something in their leccure notes.
s going to be on the final, and it’s one of the more
subtle points in endocrinology—whar is referred to as a hor-
mone having a “permissive effect.” Remove someone's testes
and, as noted, the frequency of aggressive behavior is likely to
plummet. Reinseate precastration levels of testosterone by
injecting that hormone, and precastration levels of aggression
typically return. Fair enough. Now this time, castrate an indi-
vidual and rescore testosterone levels to only 20 percent of
normal and. . . amazingly, normal precastration levels of
aggression come back. Castrate and now generate twice che
cestosterone levels from before castration—and the same level
of aggressive behavior returns. You need some testosterone
around for normal aggressive behavior—zero levels after cas-
tration, and down it usually goes; quadruple it (che sore of
range generated in weighe lifters abusing anabolic steroids),
and aggression typically increases. Buc anywhere from
roughly 20 percent of normal co twice normal and it's all theROBERT M SAPOLSKY
same; the brain can’t distinguish among this wide range of
basically normal values.
‘We seem to have figured out a couple of things by now. First,
knowing the differences in che levels of eestosterone in the cir-
culation of a bunch of males will nor help you much in figue-
ing out who is going to be aggressive. Second, the subtraction
and reinstarement data seem to indicate that, nevertheless, in
a broad sort of way, testosterone causes aggressive behavior.
But chae euens out not cobe erue either, and the implications of
this are lost on most peosle the first thirty times you tell chem
about it. Which is why you'd better cell them abouc ie thirty-
cone times, because it is tae most important point ofthis piece.
Round up some male monkeys. Put them in a group cogether,
and give them plenty of time to sore oue where they stand wich
each other—affiliative fiiendships, grudges and dislikes. Give
them enough time to form a dominance hierarchy, a linear rank-
ing system of numbers I through 5. This is the hierarchical sore
of system where number 3, for example, can pass his day
throwing around his weight with numbers 4 and 5, ripping off
their monkey chow, forcing them co relinquish the best spots ¢o
sie in, but, atthe same time, remembering to deal with num-
bers 1 and 2 with shit-eating obsequiousness.
Hierarchy in place, it’s time to do your experiment. ‘Take
thac third-ranking monkey and give him some testosterone.
None of this within-the-normal-range stuff. Inject a ton of it
into him, way higher ehan what you normally see in a rhesus
monkey; give him enough testosterone to grow antlers and a
beard on every ncuron in his brain. And, no surprise, when
you then check the behavioral data, it curns oue that he will
probably be participating in more aguressive interactions
than before.
So even though small fluctuations in che levels of the hor-
mone don't seem to matter much, testosterone still causes
THE
ROUBLE WITH TESTOSTERONE
aggression. Bue chat would be wrong. Check out number 3
more closely. Is he now raining aggressive terror on any and all
in the group, frothing in an androgenic glaze of indiscriminate
violence? Not at all. He's still jdiciously koweowing co num-
bers 1 and 2, but has simply become a total bastard to num-
bers 4 and 5. This is critical: testosterone isn’t causing
aggression, it's exaggenting che aggression that's alzeady there.
‘Another example just co show we're serious. There's a part
of your brain thae probably has lots to do with aggression, a
region called che amygdala.” Sitting righe near ic is che Grand
‘Central Seation of emotion-related activity in your brain, the
hypothalamus. The amygdala communicates wich the hypo-
thalamus by way of a cable of neuro:
stria terminalis, No more jargon, I promise. The amygdala has
its influence on aggression via that pathway, wich bursts of
electrical excication called action potentials thac ripple down
the stria terminalis, putcing the hypothalamus in a pissy mood.
‘Once again, do your hormonal intervention; flood the area
with cestosterone. You can do that by injecting che hormone
into the bloodstream, where it eventually makes its way t0
this part of the brain. Or you can be elegant and surgically
icroinjece the stuff directly into this brain region. Six of one,
half-a dozen of the other. The key thing is what doesn’t hap-
pen next. Does testosterone now cause there to be action
potentials surging down the seria terminalis? Does it turn on.
hae pathway? Not at all. Ifand only if the amygdala is ready
sending an aggression-provoking volley of action potentials
down the stria terminalis, testosterone increases the rate of
Ul connections called the
such action potentials by shortening the resting cine between
“And no one has shown thae differences in che size or shape of the
amygdala, or eliferences in the numbers of neurons in i, can begin co p
dice differences in normal levels of aggression. Same punch line as wichROBERT M SAPOLSKY
chem, It's not curning on the pathway, it's increasing che vol-
ume of signaling if it is already curned on. Ie's not causing
aggression, it’s exaggerating the preexisting pattern of it,
exaggerating che response co environmental criggers of
aggression.
This cranscends issues of testosterone and aggression. In
every generation, ic is the duty of behavioral biologists to uy
to teach this critical point, one that seems a maddening clic
once you get it. You take thac hoary old dichotomy between
nature and nurture, between biological influences and envi-
ronmencal influences, between intrinsic factors and extrinsic
ones, and, che vast majority of the time, regardless of which
behavior you are thinking about and what underlying biology
you are studying, the dichotomy is a sham. No biology. No
environment. Just the interaction between the ewo.
Do you want to know how important environment and
experience are in understanding testosterone and aggression?
Look back at how che effects of castration were discussed ear-
lier. There were statements like “Remove the source of testos-
terone in species after species and levels of aggression typically
plummer.” Nor “Remove the source . .. and aggression
always goes co zero.” On the average it declines, but rarely co
zero, and not at all in some individuals, And the more social
experience an individual hid being aggressive prior to castra-
tion, the more likely chat Sehavior persists sans wnjones. Social
conditioning can more than make up for the hormone.
Another example, one from one of the stranger comers of
the animal kingdom: If yo1 want your assumptions about the
nacure of boy beasts and girl beasts challenged, check out
the spotted hyena. These animals are fast becoming the dar-
lings of endocrinologists, sociobiologists, gynecologists, and
tabloid writers. Why? Because they have a wild sex-reversal
system—females are more muscular and more aggressive
than males and are socially dominant over them, rare traits in
156
THE TROUBLE WITH TESTOSTERONE,
the mammalian world. And get this: females secrete more of
‘certain cestosterone-relaced hormones than the males do, pro-
ducing the muscles, che aggression (and, as a reason for much
of the gawking interese in these animals, wildly masculinized
private parts that make it supremely difficule eo tell the sex of
a hyena), So chis appears to bea strong vote for the causative
powers of high ancliogen levels in aggression and social domi-
nance. Bue that's not the whole answer. High up in che
above the University of California at Berkeley is the world’s
largest colony of spotted hyenas, massive bone-crunching
beasts who fight with each other for che chance to have their
cars scratched by Laurence Frank, che zoologist who brought,
them over as infants from Kenya. Various scientists are seudy-
ing their sex-reversal system. The female hyenas are bigger
and more muscular than the males and have the same weirdo
genitals and elevated androgen levels chat their female
cousins do back in the savannah. Everything is in place except
the social system is completely different from chat in che
wild. Despiee being stoked on androgens, there is a very sig-
nificant delay in the time it takes for the females to begin
socially dominating the males—they’re growing up without
the established social system to learn from
When people first grasp the extent to which biology has
something to do wich behavior, even subtle, complex, human
behavior, there is often an initial evangelical enthusiasm of the
convert, a massive placing of faith in che biological compo-
nents of the story. And this enthusiasm is typically of a fairly
reductive type—because of physics envy, because reduction-
ism is so impressive, because it would be so nice if there were a
single gene ot hormone or neurotransmitcer or part of the
brain chat was it, che cause, the explanation of everything.
And the trouble with testosterone is that people tend to think.
this way in an arena chat really matters.
‘This is no mere academic concern, We are a fine species
157ROBERT M. SAPOLSKY
with some potential. Yee we are racked by sickening amounts
of violence. Unless we are hermies, we feel the threat of it,
often as a daily shadow. And regardless of where we hide,
should our leaders push the bueton, we will all be lose in a
final global violence. Buc as we try to understand and wrestle
with this feature of our sociality, i i critical co remember the
limits of the biology. Testcsterone ie never going co tell us
much about the suburban teenager who, in his after-school
cchess club, has developed a particularly aggressive style with
his bishops. And ie certainly isn’e going co cell us much about
the teenager in some inner-city hellhole who has taken to
mugging people. “Testosterone equals aggression” is inade-
quate for those who would offer a simple solution to the vio-
lent male—just decrease levels of chose pesky steroids. And
“testosterone equals aggression” is certainly inadequate for
those who would offer a simple excuse: Boys will be boys and
certain things in nature are inevitable. Violence is more com-
plex than a single hormone. This is endocrinology for the
bleeding heart liberal—ovr behavioral biology is usually
meaningless outside the context of the social faccors and envi-
ronmenc in which ie occurs.
FURTHER READING
Fora good general review ofthe subject, sce B. Monaghan and S. Glick
‘man, “Hormones and Aggressive Behavior” in J. Becker, M. Breedlove,
and D. Crews, eds., Behavional Endocrinology (Cambridge, Mass.: MIT
Press, 1992), 261. This also has sa overview of the hyena social syscem, as
Glickman heads the study of the Berkeley hyenas. For eeclnical papers on
the acquisition of the female dominance in hyenas, see $. Jenks, M.
Weldele, L Frank, and S. Glickman, “Acquisition of Macrilineal Rank in
‘Captive Spotted Hyenas: Emergence of a Natural Social System i Pece-
Reared Animals and Theie Offspring," Animal Belvier 50 (1995): 8935
and L. Frank, S. Glickman, and C. Zabel, “Oneogeny of Female Domi
158,
THE TROUBLE WITH TESTOSTERONE
nance in the Spotted Hyaena: Perspectives from Nature and Captivity” in
P Jewell and G, Maloy, eds., “The Biology of Large African Mammals in
‘Theie Environment,” Syuporium ofthe Zeobgicl Society of London 61 (1989)
127.
have emphasized thae while testosterone levels in che normal range do
fot have much to do with aggression, a massive elevation of exposure, as
would be seen in anabolic steroid abusers, does usually increase aggression.
For a tecent seuly in which even clovacng into char range (approximately
five times normal level) still had no eect on mood or behavior, see
S. Bhasin, T. Scorer, N, Berman, and celleagues, “The Effects of Supra-
physiologic Doses of Testosterone on Muscle Size and Strengeh in Normal
Men,” New England Jauraa of Medicine 335 (1996):
“The study showing chat raising testosterone levels in che middle-
ranking monkey exaggerates preexisting patterns of aggression can be
found in A. Dixson and J. Hesbert, "Testosterone, Aggressive Behavior
and Dominance Rank in Captive Adule Male Talapoin Monkeys (Mfopithe-
«us talapin)," Physiology and Bebaaior 18 (1977: 539. For the demonstra~
tion thac testosterone shorcens the cesting period between action
potentials in neurons, sce K. Kendrick and R, Drewett, “Testosterone
[Reduces Refractory Period of Seria Terminalis Neutons in the Rat Brain,”
Seinas 204 (1979): 877,
159
Reach Your Optimal Testosterone Levels (Naturally without Side-Effect): How To Naturally Increase Your Testosterone Levels Without Drugs Or Side Effects And Get More Muscle, Less Belly Fat, Higher Sex Drive and Improve Your Mood and Be Happier.