Cognitive Psychology Reviewer
Cognitive Psychology Reviewer
Cognitive Psychology Reviewer
Cognitive psychology involves the study of internal mental processes—all of the things that go on
inside your brain, including perception, thinking, memory, attention, language, problem-solving, and
learning. While it is a relatively young branch of psychology, it has quickly grown to become one of the
most popular subfields.
2. Learning more about how people think and process information not only helps researchers gain a
deeper understanding of how the human brain works, but it allows psychologists to develop new ways
of helping people deal with psychological difficulties. For example, by recognizing that attention is both a
selective and limited resource, psychologists are able to come up with solutions that make it easier for
people with attentional difficulties to improve their focus and concentration.
3. Findings from cognitive psychology have also improved our understanding of how people form, store,
and recall memories. By knowing more about how these processes work, psychologists can develop new
ways of helping people improve their memories and combat potential memory problems. For example,
psychologists have found that while your short-term memory is quite short and limited (lasting just 20 to
30 seconds and capable of holding between five and nine items), rehearsal strategies can improve the
chances that information will be transferred to long-term memory, which is much more stable and
durable.
In cognitive psychology, the ways of addressing fundamental issues have changed, but many of the
fundamental questions remain much the same. Ultimately, cognitive psychologists hope to learn how
people think by studying how people have thoughts about thinking.
• A thesis is proposed. A thesis is a statement of belief. For example, some people believe that human
nature governs many aspects of human behavior (e.g., intelligence or personality; Sternberg, 1999).
After a while, however, certain individuals notice apparent flaws in the thesis.
• An antithesis emerges. Eventually, or perhaps even quite soon, an antithesis emerges. An antithesis is
a statement that counters a previous statement of belief. For example, an alternative view is that our
nurture (the environmental contexts in which we are reared) almost entirely determines many aspects
of human behavior.
• A synthesis integrates the viewpoints. Sooner or later, the debate between the thesis and the
antithesis leads to a synthesis. A synthesis integrates the most credible features of each of two (or more)
views. For example, in the debate over nature versus nurture, the interaction between our innate
(inborn) nature and environmental nurture may govern human nature.
A rationalist believes that the route to knowledge is through thinking and logical analysis. That is, a
rationalist does not need any experiments to develop new knowledge. A rationalist who is interested in
cognitive processes would appeal to reason as a source of knowledge or justification. Plato and Rene
Descartes were rationalists.
The empiricists like Aristotle and John Locke believed that we acquire knowledge via empirical
evidence — that is, we obtain evidence through experience and observation. In order to explore how
the human mind works, empiricists would design experiments and conduct studies in which they could
observe the behavior and processes of interest to them. Empiricism therefore leads directly to empirical
investigations of psychology.
We might see the rationalist view of the world as a thesis and the empirical view as an antithesis.
Most psychologists today seek a synthesis of the two. They base empirical observations on theory in
order to explain what they have observed in their experiments. In turn, they use these observations to
revise their theories when they find that the theories cannot account for their real-world observations.
Practical abilities are used to implement the ideas and persuade others of their
value.
3. Cognitive processes interact with each other and with noncognitive processes.
Although cognitive psychologists try to study and often to isolate the functioning of specific
cognitive processes, they know that these processes work together. For example, memory processes
depend on perceptual processes. What you remember depends in part on what you perceive. But
noncognitive processes also interact with cognitive ones. For example, you learn better when you are
motivated to learn. Cognitive psychologists therefore seek to study cognitive processes not only in
isolation but also in their interactions with each other and with noncognitive processes.
5. All basic research in cognitive psychology may lead to applications, and all applied research may lead
to basic understandings.
But the truth is, the distinction between basic and applied research often is not clear at all.
Research that seems like it will be basic often leads to immediate applications. Similarly, research that
seems like it will be applied sometimes leads quickly to basic understandings. For example, a basic
finding from research on memory is that learning is superior when it is spaced out over time rather than
crammed into a short time interval. This basic finding has an immediate application to study strategies.
At the same time, research on eyewitness testimony, which seems on its face to be very applied, has
enhanced our basic understanding of memory systems and of the extent to which humans construct
their own memories.
Week 3 Neuro Science
What are the fundamental structures and processes within the brain?
The nervous system, governed by the brain, is divided into two main parts: the central nervous
system, consisting of the brain and the spinal cord, and the peripheral nervous system, consisting of the
rest of the nervous system (e.g., the nerves in the face, legs, arms, and viscera).
How do researchers study the major structures and processes of the brain?
For centuries scientists have viewed the brain by dissecting it. Modern dissection techniques
include the use of electron microscopes and sophisticated chemical analyses to probe the mysteries of
individual cells of the brain. Additionally, surgical techniques on animals (e.g., the use of selective
lesioning and single-cell recording) often are used. On humans, studies have included electrical analyses
(e.g., electroencephalograms and event-related potentials), studies based on the use of X-ray techniques
(e.g., angiograms and computed tomograms), studies based on computer analyses of magnetic fields
within the brain (magnetic resonance imaging), and studies based on computer analyses of blood flow
and metabolism within the brain (positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance
imaging).
Brain tumors, also called neoplasms, can affect cognitive functioning in very serious ways. Tumors
can occur in either the gray or the white matter of the brain. Tumors of the white matter are more
common (Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, 2009). Two types of brain tumors can occur. Primary brain tumors
start in the brain. Most childhood brain tumors are of this type. Secondary brain tumors start as tumors
somewhere else in the body, such as in the lungs.
Brain tumors can be either benign or malignant. Benign tumors do not contain cancer cells. They
typically can be removed and will not grow back. Cells from benign tumors do not invade surrounding
cells or spread to other parts of the body. However, if they press against sensitive areas of the brain,
they can result in serious cognitive impairments. They also can be life-threatening, unlike benign tumors
in most other parts of the body.
Malignant brain tumors, unlike benign ones, contain cancer cells. They are more serious and
usually threaten the victim’s life. They often grow quickly. They also tend to invade surrounding healthy
brain tissue. In rare instances, malignant cells may break away and cause cancer in other parts of the
body. Following are the most common symptoms of brain tumors (What you need to know about brain
tumors, 2009):
• headaches (usually worse in the morning)
• nausea or vomiting
• changes in speech, vision, or hearing
• problems balancing or walking
• changes in mood, personality, or ability to concentrate
• problems with memory
• muscle jerking or twitching (seizures or convulsions)
• numbness or tingling in the arms or legs
Head injuries result from many causes, such as a car accident, contact with a hard object, or a
bullet wound. Head injuries are of two types. In closed-head injuries, the skull remains intact but there is
damage to the brain, typically from the mechanical force of a blow to the head. Slamming one’s head
against a windshield in a car accident might result in such an injury. In open-head injuries, the skull does
not remain intact but rather is penetrated, for example, by a bullet.
Perceptual constancies (e.g., size and shape constancy) result when our perceptions of objects
tend to remain constant. That is, we see constancies even as the stimuli registered by our
senses change. Some perceptual constancies may be governed by what we know about the
world. For example, we have expectations regarding how rectilinear structures usually appear.
But constancies also are influenced by invariant relationships among objects in their
environmental context.
One reason we can perceive 3-D space is the use of binocular depth cues. Two such cues
are binocular disparity and binocular convergence.
Binocular disparity is based on the fact that each of two eyes receives a slightly
different image of the same object as it is being viewed.
Binocular convergence is based on the degree to which our two eyes must turn
inward toward each other as objects get closer to us.
We also are aided in perceiving depth by monocular depth cues. These cues include
texture gradients, relative size, interposition, linear perspective, aerial perspective, height in the
picture plane, and motion parallax. One of the earliest approaches to form and pattern
perception is the Gestalt approach to form perception. The Gestalt law of Prägnanz has led to
the explication of several principles of form perception. These principles include figure-ground,
proximity, similarity, closure, continuity, and symmetry. They characterize how we perceptually
group together various objects and parts of objects.
3. What happens when people with normal visual sensations cannot perceive visual stimuli?
Agnosias, which are usually associated with brain lesions, are deficits of form and pattern
perception. They cause afflicted people to be insufficiently able to recognize objects that are in
their visual fields, despite normal sensory abilities. People who suffer from visual-object agnosia
can sense all parts of the visual field. But the objects they see do not mean anything to them.
Individuals with simultagnosia are unable to pay attention to more than one object at a time.
People with spatial agnosia have severe difficulty in comprehending and handling the
relationship between their bodies and the spatial configurations of the world around them.
People with prosopagnosia have severe impairment in their ability to recognize human faces,
including their own. These deficits lead to the question of whether specific perceptual
processes are modular—specialized for particular tasks. Color blindness is another type of
perceptual deficit.