A Longitudinal Investigation of Integration/ Multiculturalism Policies and Attitudes Towards Immigrants in European Countries
A Longitudinal Investigation of Integration/ Multiculturalism Policies and Attitudes Towards Immigrants in European Countries
To cite this article: David Bartram & Erika Jarochova (2021): A longitudinal investigation of
integration/multiculturalism policies and attitudes towards immigrants in European countries,
Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, DOI: 10.1080/1369183X.2021.1922273
Introduction
Recent research in migration studies includes a number of articles offering support for an
attractive finding: policies facilitating the integration of immigrants appear to have a
salutary effect on attitudes towards immigrants among the broader population. Inte-
gration policies don’t just enhance immigrant integration – they reduce the extent of
‘threat perception’ or hostility towards immigrants more broadly (Schlueter, Meuleman,
and Davidov 2013; Schlueter, Masso, and Davidov 2020; Callens and Meuleman 2017;
Green et al. 2020; Hooghe and de Vroome 2015).
This paper reconsiders that finding, using two related methodological reflections that
will likely be relevant to a broader set of research questions relevant to migration studies.
The findings noted above (and discussed further below) are grounded in a common
analytical approach: the researchers take cross-sectional survey data drawn from
Previous research
The widely noted increase in migration to Europe over recent decades forms the context
for sustained research interest in attitudes towards immigrants and immigration. Among
the reasons to consider attitudes, a political angle is especially salient: populist political
movements in a wide range of European countries have sought to gain votes in part
by demonising immigrants – and these efforts have achieved significant success (e.g.
Mudde 2013; Yılmaz 2012). The consequent political and social turmoil has sometimes
become violent; even where violence is not part of the picture, the populist turn has argu-
ably distorted national politics and governance across much of Europe.
To interpret these patterns, researchers draw on and develop two perspectives: group
threat theory and inter-group contact theory. Group threat theories hold that attitudes
towards immigrants become more negative when there is competition over resources,
which are inevitably limited (Davidov and Semyonov 2017; McLaren 2003;
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 3
Scheepers et al. 2002; Quillian 1995). Members of the majority population are said to
rationally assess the (mostly economic) competition between them and immigrants,
especially for jobs. Attitudes towards immigrants become more negative in countries
with worsening economic conditions (e.g. rising unemployment rates). Inter-group
relations are thus more important source of negative attitudes towards outgroups than
individual psychological conditions (Blumer 1958; Sherif, White, and Harvey 1955).
But the inter-group contact theory proposes that more frequent contact across group
boundaries can overcome these tendencies, fostering more positive attitudes and redu-
cing prejudice (Allport 1954; Pettigrew and Tropp 2006; McLaren 2003; Schneider
2007). Even when the proportion of immigrants is rising, negative attitudes are not inevi-
table as long as segregation is mitigated.
In contexts of this sort, researchers have asked: what are the possibilities for policy-
based efforts to mitigate the xenophobia? Several contributions perceive a basis for opti-
mism. Callens and Meuleman (2017) find that ‘respondents living in a country with
more-inclusive integration policies … display lower perceptions of economic threat
[from immigrants]’ (367). That finding emerges from research using data from the
2008 European Values Survey (EVS), matched to country-level data from the 2007
version of the MIPEX. The paper reports however that higher MIPEX values are not
associated with lower perceptions of ‘cultural threat’. Another study (Schlueter, Meule-
man, and Davidov 2013) using the same data aggregates these different forms of threat
and finds that ‘immigrant integration policies that are more permissive are associated
with decreased perceptions of group threat from immigrants’ (670). For the authors,
the policy implications are clear: ‘these findings suggest that immigrant integration pol-
icies are of key importance in improving majority members’ attitudes regarding immi-
grants … ’ (Schlueter, Meuleman, and Davidov 2013, 670).
Schlueter, Masso, and Davidov (2020) investigate attitudes in European countries
specifically in relation to Muslims (many of whom would be immigrants or have
recent immigration family background). They find that ‘more liberal immigrant inte-
gration policies … are associated with lower levels of majority members’ negative atti-
tudes towards Muslim immigration … ’ (649). Their analysis uses data from Round 7
of the European Social Survey (2015), matched to MIPEX data from the same year. A
similar pattern is found also in work by Green et al. (2020), who use the same data
(ESS7 and MIPEX 2014): a higher MIPEX score is associated with lower perceptions
of ‘symbolic’ and ‘realistic’ threat. Additionally, a more tolerant policy frame work (i.e.
a higher MIPEX score) enhances the impact of everyday contact with immigrants in
reducing threat perceptions.
A fifth investigation along these lines (Hooghe and de Vroome 2015) uses the MIPEX
measure and the MCP index, matched to data from Round 5 of the ESS. The MCP Index
is not associated with anti-immigrant sentiment. The MIPEX measure, however, ‘is sig-
nificantly, negatively related to anti-immigrant sentiments … : the more a country offers
inclusive integration policies, the lower the levels of anti-immigrant sentiment are’
(Hooghe and de Vroome 2015, 757).
What we see, then, is a set of studies all supporting the conclusion that more ‘liberal’ or
‘tolerant’ integration policies have important impacts on attitudes towards immigrants.
Integration policies in the first instance affect immigrants, and substantial research
efforts have been directed to the impact of policies on the immigrants themselves (e.g.
4 D. BARTRAM AND E. JAROCHOVA
Bloemraad and Wright 2014). But those policies also appear to mitigate xenophobia
among the broader population. In addition to the shared substantive findings, these
papers also have some key methodological features in common. In particular, they all
involve MLM of cross-sectional data. This decision appears to emerge from the view
that estimating policies→attitudes requires controlling for a broad range of individual-
level variables. We now turn to a close consideration of this approach, raising two key
questions. One pertains to the selection of control variables in general, on which a
great deal seems to be taken for granted. The other pertains to the cross-sectional
nature of the analysis, which leaves findings of this sort open to some significant
vulnerabilities.
would hide part of that impact. Becoming unemployed usually decreases one’s income,
and the decrease in income will then result in lower life satisfaction. If we control for
income, our estimate excludes this ‘indirect effect’ of unemployment on life satisfaction.
It is essential to have clarity on the relationship between X and W. In some instances the
nature of that relationship might not be as clear as it is in this example; perhaps the
relationship is closer to X↔W. But that possibility should not lead us to a practice of
ignoring how X and W are related.
In the present context, where X is a policy (or set of policies), the question of what
controls are needed appears in a different light. In an MLM framework, policy (e.g.
MIPEX or MCP) is a ‘level-2’ variable. When X is a level-2 variable, do we need any indi-
vidual-level (‘level-1’) controls? It seems far from obvious that we do, especially in this
context. The idea that we do need level-1 controls is sometimes expressed with reference
to ‘compositional effects’ – the possibility that higher-level units (e.g. countries) might
differ in the composition of their populations, e.g. age structure, sex balance, educational
attainment, religiosity, etc. For some questions, compositional effects might be impor-
tant. What matters, however, is not just whether individual-level factors affect the depen-
dent variable – for compositional effects to be important, individual-level factors must
also affect the focal independent variable. In this context, it seems unlikely: whether a
society/government adopts a more liberal/tolerant integration policy seems very unlikely
to depend on how old individuals are, or what their sex is, or even how educated they are.
By the same token, living in a country with a higher MIPEX score is very unlikely to affect
the selected individual-level variables. Among the three scenarios articulated above, we
are likely to find that the level-1 controls are irrelevant.
Table 1 confirms that intuition. The table functions first as a replication of the analysis
in Schlueter, Masso, and Davidov (2020). The first column presents their results as
reported in their own paper. The second column is the replication. The key result of
interest is the coefficient for the ‘immigrant integration policies’ (MIPEX) variable; the
value is identical in the first two columns (confirming the overall success of the replica-
tion). In the third column, the model removes all of the level-1 controls. Here the MIPEX
coefficient is very slightly different (0.010, up from 0.009) – but not to the extent that the
substantive conclusions would be different. The level-1 controls are indeed irrelevant.
The same pattern is apparent in replications of two other papers (Schlueter, Meuleman,
and Davidov 2013; Callens and Meuleman 2017), available on request. The issue receives
additional attention further below, in the results section.
Moving forward
Having considered these two methodological points in combination, this paper offers
a contribution taking us beyond the limitations of existing research on this topic, by
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 7
The EVS was conducted in 1990, 1999, and 2008 – so, for the second analysis, we will
draw values from the MCP from 1989, 1998, and 2007. The combination of the EVS and
the MCP facilitates an investigation covering a longer time-frame (relative to what is
available via use of data from the ESS). This pairing accommodates the possibility that
it might take more time for policies to affect attitudes. The set of countries where data
are available on both measures for at least two of those year-pairs is: Austria, Belgium,
Germany, Denmark, Spain, Finland, France, the UK, Greece, Ireland, Italy, Netherlands,
Norway, Portugal, and Sweden.1 For this analysis, it is not possible to exclude immigrants
from the sample.
The EVS/MCP analysis considers four dependent variables that give information
about respondents’ attitudes towards immigrants. These variables do not have the con-
sistent structure of the three ESS questions, so they are not aggregated. For all three waves
of the EVS, respondents are asked whether they would not want to have ‘immigrants/
foreign workers’ as neighbours. The response for individuals is dichotomous, but
again the responses will be aggregated to country-level means, a measure that will be
treated as a continuous dependent variable (with higher levels denoting more xenopho-
bic attitudes). In the same three rounds, respondents are also asked whether employers
should give priority to nationals over immigrants when jobs are scarce. Respondents can
agree or disagree; if they spontaneously say ‘neither’, we assign an intermediate value of
0.5 (in between 1 for agree and 0 for disagree). As before, the aggregation to country-level
averages produces a continuous dependent variable.
For the second and third waves (1990 and 1999), two additional questions are posed.
Respondents are asked about their preferred policy towards immigration, with four cat-
egories for response: (1) let anyone come who wants to; (2) come as long as jobs are avail-
able; (3) strict limits on the number of foreigners; and (4) prohibit people coming here
from other countries. Respondents are also asked about the extent to which they feel con-
cerned about immigrants, with five options for response: very much, much, to a certain
extent, not so much, and not at all. For this variable, responses are reversed so that higher
numbers represent more concern, in line with the patterns for the other dependent
variables.
The independent variable here is the MCP Index. This index indicates whether a
country has policies fostering multiculturalism for immigrants in eight respects: (1) con-
stitutional/legislative/parliamentary affirmation of multiculturalism (together with an
institutional basis for implementation): (2) adoption of multiculturalism in the school
curriculum; (3) inclusion of ethnic representation/sensitivity in mandates for public
media; (4) exemptions from dress codes; (5) accommodation of dual citizenship; (6)
public/state funding for ethnic-group organizations/activities; (7) public/state funding
for bilingual/mother-tongue instruction; and (8) affirmative action for disadvantaged
immigrant groups. Values for each can be 0 for absent, 1 for present and effective, and
0.5 for present but limited or only partially effective. The ‘total’ value of the index is
then additive.
The analyses below consist of ‘fixed-effects’ models exploiting the panel structure of
the data. (The term ‘fixed effects’ can seem opaque; it is likely more enlightening to
refer to these models as ‘within’ models, evoking a contrast to ‘between’ models
drawing on information between countries in a mode akin to cross-sectional analysis.
Bell, Fairbrother, and Jones (2019) are effective on this point.) The coefficients derive
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 9
from change over time in departures from each country’s mean values on the dependent
and independent variables. The coefficients tell us whether anti-immigrant attitudes
increase or decrease (or neither) as integration or multicultural policies are adopted
(or rescinded). In contrast to cross-sectional analyses, however, that question is answered
in a way that controls for any time-constant differences between countries (Brüderl and
Ludwig 2015).
An analysis of repeated cross-sectional data in this mode departs in certain respects
from a conventional longitudinal analysis of panel data where the individual respon-
dents are constant across waves. The main difference is that the population of the
countries changes over time, via the basic demographic processes of birth, death,
and migration. The samples selected for each round of the ESS/EVS can be taken as
representative of the corresponding populations at each point in time, but the popu-
lations themselves do change. We can consider: do the ‘replacement’ processes
amount to confounders in this context, such that it is necessary to adjust our estimates
for those processes in some way? That question is addressed below subsequent to the
main EVS/MCP analysis.
We now return to the question of what (time-varying) country-level control variables
are needed to give an unbiased estimate of the impact of policies on attitudes. There is no
need to consider time-constant differences across societies (e.g. pertaining to geography/
location or fixed aspects of a country’s history). In effect, we need to identify the time-
varying determinants of X (i.e. W→X): why do some countries adopt integration or mul-
ticultural policies (while others do not)? From answers to that question, we can then con-
sider: do any of those factors also affect people’s attitudes towards immigrants? If there
are variables meeting these conditions, then they must be included as controls. But we
should also ensure that we don’t control for determinants of attitudes that are likely
affected by the policies themselves (X→W).
One possibility is per-capita GDP (Quillian 1995; Schneider 2007): perhaps in weal-
thier countries people feel more secure about immigrants (W→Y) and governments are
thus more willing to adopt policies that accommodate immigrants in general and their
differences in a multicultural mode (W→X). It seems unlikely that the relationship
works the other way around (X→W): more immigration might enhance GDP
growth (e.g. Portes 2019), but it is less obvious that integration or multicultural policies
specifically have the same impact. A variable for per-capita GDP (in constant dollars,
adjusted for purchasing-power parity), in thousands, is thus included in models below
(the data are taken from the ‘Gapminder’ website, which draws on the World Bank and
the Penn World Tables – see gapminder.org, the documentation for V26).
Another potential control pertains to the proportion of immigrants in a country’s
population (Schneider 2007; Semyonov, Raijman, and Gorodzeisky 2006). The impact
of such a variable on attitudes towards immigrants could go in either direction.
Intuitively, it might seem that people are likely to be more concerned about immi-
gration when there are more immigrants. On the other hand, the ‘contact hypothesis’
(Allport 1954; McLaren 2003) suggests the reverse possibility: once people encounter
actual immigrants (as opposed to the bogeyman of hypothetical immigrants presented
by populist politicians), they see real human beings and perhaps develop a sense of
empathy. An increased presence of immigrants could also lead countries to adopt
more accommodating policies (W→X); here as well it seems less likely that more
10 D. BARTRAM AND E. JAROCHOVA
Results: ESS/MIPEX
Some comments about interpretation are required before proceeding to the ESS/MIPEX
results given in Table 3. The regression tables include standard errors despite misgivings
(some readers might want this information to gauge the ‘precision’ of the coefficients as
estimates), and the use of asterisks to denote thresholds of statistical significance is not
contemplated. (It is tempting to suggest that some social researchers who do quantitative
work have an ‘asterisk fetish’ that would be worth overcoming.) Statistical significance is
sometimes used as a basis for determining whether an effect is ‘significant’ in some
broader sense (call it ‘substantive’ significance) – but it is better to say that this is a misuse.
Statistical significance might be useful for gauging whether we can be confident in
generalising results from a sample to a larger population. But that usage requires a
sample that is representative of a larger population. We do not have data of that sort
here. The data are national averages from a group of 19 countries in Europe. We do
not have grounds for suggesting that these 19 countries are representative of some
larger group of countries. Of course, ‘Europe’ consists of a larger set of countries, but
it is far from evident that the countries that are included in the analysis are a represen-
tative sample of the larger set that includes countries that are not analysed here. Gener-
alisation is therefore not a relevant idea in this context – so, statistical significance is
likewise irrelevant. That perspective could inform a view about whether statistical signifi-
cance is generally relevant to ‘level-2’ variables in analyses that use MLM; in many
instances it is likely not relevant, because the level-2 entities are not usually representative
of some larger population in the way level-1 respondents in a sample are (Lucas 2014). In
any event, the statistical significance will not help us gauge whether the results in Table 3
are ‘significant’ in some other way. Instead, we will consider effect size.2
The analysis in Table 3 indicates that an increase in the MIPEX leads to a slight
increase in anti-immigrant sentiment. That conclusion is evident in the fixed-effects
models; the inclusion of the specified controls makes little difference (Model 2, vs. Model
1). The coefficients here are positive (denoting higher levels of anti-immigrant senti-
ment) – but they are also very small, at 0.020 and 0.019. The ‘within’ standard deviation
of the dependent variable here is 0.31. By comparison, an effect of 0.02 is very small –
perhaps small enough to be described as negligible. The largest increase (from 2008 to
2018) in the value of the MIPEX in this set of countries is 10 (for Estonia and
Poland). Even with that level of change in the MIPEX, the predicted change in anti-immi-
grant sentiment would be 0.2. The range of the dependent variable runs from approxi-
mately 3.5 to 6.5 – and a change of 0.2 in that context does not seem at all substantial.3
Table 3 also includes models from a ‘pooled’ specification (Models 3 and 4). This spe-
cification is cross-sectional with time addressed only via dummy variables for survey
round; in that respect it is closer to what is used in existing research on this topic
(especially Green et al. 2020, who also match the MIPEX and the ESS). In these
models, higher values of the MIPEX are associated with lower levels of anti-immigrant
sentiment – in line with the broad shape of existing findings. Comparing the coefficients
to their standard errors (in parentheses), we could also anticipate finding that these
results are ‘statistically significant’ (though again that idea is not relevant in this context).
But there are clear reasons to prefer the results from the fixed-effects models – in par-
ticular, the fact that these results are not biased from omission of time-constant between-
country differences. The contrast between the two sets of results leads to what seems like
an important insight: existing research, using a cross-sectional approach, gives findings
that very likely reflect ‘pre-existing’ differences among countries that in fact need to be
controlled as confounders. The FE models, which are more successful in that regard,
indicate that the impact of integration policies (as captured by the MIPEX) is the oppo-
site of what is given in existing/previous research (compare Kragten and Rözer (2017) for
a similar pattern of cross-sectional vs. longitudinal results, pertaining to the relationship
between inequality and health). More conservatively, our core finding is that integration
policies do not mitigate anti-immigrant sentiments – a formulation that still stands as a
contrast to current understandings.
Similar to the replication of Schlueter, Masso, and Davidov (2020) presented above,
we can consider whether individual-level controls are consequential for the analysis in
Table 3. In a supplementary analysis (available in an online appendix, Table A2), we
explored models using individual-level controls via a ‘within-between’ specification
(Bell, Fairbrother, and Jones 2019). Sometimes described as a ‘Mundlak’ model
(Mundlak 1978), this approach facilitates inclusion of individual-level variables in a
‘between’ mode, in the context of a panel model where macro-level independent variables
(e.g. the independent variables used in Table 3) are decomposed into within and between
components. These models enable us to consider whether ‘compositional effects’ are con-
sequential for the estimation of the within estimates that are our main interest here. The
answer is that the individual-level factors are not consequential for that purpose: the
coefficient for the MIPEX variable does not change at all (at three decimal points)
when the individual-level controls are added. This robustness check reinforces the
logic of using a purely longitudinal/within approach as in Table 3, which thus forms
the basis for our main results.
What about reverse causation (attitudes→policies)? We constructed an exploratory
random-intercepts cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM, Hamaker, Kuiper, and
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 13
Grasman 2015) to evaluate the idea that failure to account for the possibility of reciprocal
causal links might be suppressing evidence of the causal effect we are evaluating (policie-
s→attitudes). This model (available via online supplement, Table A3) did not offer any
evidence supporting a conclusion different from what is evident in Table 3. This
model likely has limited value: with only 19 ‘observations’ (countries) there is not a
great deal of leverage for estimating the number of parameters in a typical RI-CLPM.
This topic deserves further exploration; it is entirely plausible that ‘attitudes→policies’
captures an important pattern, with xenophobic attitudes perhaps impeding the adoption
of more accommodating policies towards immigrants. In any event, our own exploration
does not suggest that our conclusions above (about policies→attitudes) are incorrect.
Results: EVS/MCP
An inspection of the bottom portion of Table 2 suggests that the attitudes towards immi-
grants measured in the EVS, considered via country-level averages, do not change a great
deal over the time periods investigated here. The exception pertains to views about
whether employers should give preference to nationals over immigrants in hiring
decisions: here we see a decrease of one-tenth of a point from the first to the third
wave. That decrease is more than half of the aggregated standard deviation of this vari-
able (0.18) and larger than the within standard deviation (0.072). For the other variables,
however, any change over time appears to be very small. In contrast, there is a steady
increase in the adoption of multicultural policies over the same time period. At first
glance, then, it does not appear that these policy developments contribute to change in
attitudes.
The models presented in Table 4 help us evaluate whether this surface-level view
changes when the relationship is investigated in a more formal way, with results adjusted
for the identified confounders. Intuitively, all the coefficients for the MCP in Table 4
seem small. Only for the ‘jobs’ and ‘policy’ analyses does a 1-unit increase in the MCP
index result in a change in the dependent variable amounting to more than one one-hun-
dredth of a point (−0.016 and 0.029, respectively, for models where control variables are
included). If we compare those numbers to the within standard deviations for the
respective variables, on the other hand, the effects for those two variables now appear to
be non-trivial. For the jobs variable, the coefficient amounts to slightly more than one-
tenth of the standard deviation (−0.016/0.072 = −0.22). For the policy variable, the
effect is larger (0.029/0.069 = 0.42). The jobs result, then, exceeds the conventional
threshold associated with Cohen’s D for a ‘small’ effect (0.20); the policy result is not
far from the threshold for a ‘medium’ effect (0.50).
For the other two variables (neighbours and concern), any effect seems negligible. A
bivariate model for ‘concern’ suggests that an increase in the MCP leads to a decline in
concern about immigration that is perhaps substantial (−0.028) – but the addition of
controls indicates that that apparent decline more plausibly stems from changes in p/c
GDP and the relative size of the immigration population. Where effects are not negligible
(jobs and policy), they go in opposite directions. An increased adoption of multicultural
policies leads to a decrease in agreement with the idea that employers should favour
nationals over immigrants in hiring decisions. But it leads more strongly to an increase
in preferences for restrictive immigration policies. Those two trends (which are evident
in Table 2 as well, without connection to the MCP) are not necessarily in conflict: people
can support fair/equal treatment of immigrants already ‘here’ while also favouring a
reduction in (new) immigration.
Even so, it seems reasonable to conclude that there is only weak evidence for the idea
that adopting multicultural policies leads to a reduction in hostility to immigrants and
immigration. The only result that supports that conclusion pertains to hiring preferences.
For the other measures, the effect either forms a contrast (as with the policy measure) or
is negligible, at least as gauged via the comparison of the coefficient to standard deviation.
We could press the issue of effect size by asking about the likely impact of a more
radical adoption of multicultural policies. The country that travelled the furthest distance
in these terms was Finland: in 1989 Finland’s index value was 0 and by 2007 it had
increased to 6. For other countries, the increase was smaller – and in two instances
(Denmark and the Netherlands) the value of the index declined, as previously adopted
policies were rescinded. We might then ask (in a counterfactual mode): what if
Denmark and Finland had been more like Finland?
That question (like all counterfactual questions) cannot be answered directly via data. In
a speculative mode, the answer might appear obvious: if Denmark and the Netherlands had
been more like Finland, they would (‘obviously’?) have seen a similar evolution of attitudes.
Via multiplication, the predicted change in Finnish attitudes regarding hiring decisions
(‘jobs’) would be −0.096 (=−0.016 × 6), and the predicted change in ‘policy’ preferences
would be 0.174 (=0.029 × 6). (The actual jobs figure in Finland declined from 0.768 to
0.654, a difference almost equal to the predicted change; the policy figure increased
from 2.470 to 2.490 – here as well, almost equal to the predicted change.)
Would Denmark and the Netherlands have actually experienced similar evolution of
attitudes if they had adopted multiculturalist policies with a zeal similar to Finland’s, ‘all
else equal’? This question leads us back to the risks associated with drawing conclusions
via comparison of countries to one another (as against using ‘within’ information taken
from change over time). If Denmark and the Netherlands had been more like Finland,
they would have been less like themselves; a change of that sort would have involved
those countries being/becoming quite different from what they were in reality. It is
difficult to know what ‘all else equal’ would mean, from this angle.
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 15
The extent of risk involved in drawing conclusions in this mode can again be gauged
by comparing to a ‘pooled’ specification (having more in common with a cross-sectional
analysis) of models equivalent to those in Table 4. These results are given in an appendix
(Table A1); here the effects are all more ‘negative’, i.e. they offer stronger support for the
idea that adoption of multicultural policies leads to less hostility towards immigrants and
immigration. This analysis does not adjust for the (unmeasured) ways in which these
various countries are different from one another; as suggested above for the ESS/
MIPEX analysis, it is less effective (relative to a FE specification) in controlling for
those differences and in particular for the way they might lead to different tendencies
to adopt multicultural policies. Again, we see that any conclusion suggesting that adop-
tion of multicultural policies generally reduces hostility to immigrants would likely
reflect ‘pre-existing’ differences among countries that in fact need to be controlled as
confounders.
We now turn to the question of whether failing to account for cohort replacement pro-
cesses might be distorting the conclusions derived from Table 4. As noted, that possibility
pertains to the fact that the repeated cross-sections represent changing populations over
time. People who were already relatively old in 1990 are less likely to be present, in the
sample and the population alike, via death, refusal, or residence in unsampled units (e.g.
care homes). By the same token, people who were relatively young in 2008 would not
have been part of the sample (nor the relevant population) in earlier rounds. This repla-
cement process might affect the evolution of attitudes towards immigrants, and an analy-
sis that does not adjust for it might produce results that do not accurately represent the
effect of multicultural policies.
Table 5 explores that possibility by presenting results from analyses specified to isolate
changes that might be taking place within cohorts. As per the row labels, each row starts
with an age-specific subsample in 1990 (the first year of measurement used from the
EVS); for subsequent rounds, the values for the attitude variables are drawn from the cor-
responding cohorts at that later point in time. (So: for the first row, which takes respon-
dents in their 20s in 1990, the attitude measures for 1999 are drawn from respondents
aged 29–38, and the measures for 2008 are drawn from respondents aged 38–47.
Table 5. Coefficients for MCP effect on attitudes towards immigrants, exploration of cohorts.
Restrictive
Immigrants not immigration
wanted as Employers should policy Concern about
neighbours prefer nationals preferences immigration
Age 20–29 in 1990 0.001 0.000 −0.017 −0.018 0.057 0.049 −0.056 −0.024
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
Age 30–39 in 1990 −0.010 −0.012 −0.015 −0.018 0.009 −0.002 −0.034 0.008
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
Age 40–49 in 1990 0.000 −0.002 −0.012 −0.014 0.024 0.016 −0.005 0.031
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
Age 50–59 in 1990 −0.008 −0.010 −0.022 −0.022 0.019 0.006 0.027 0.068
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
Age 60–69 in 1990 −0.010 −0.012 −0.030 −0.032 0.029 0.021 0.036 0.076
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
Average across cohorts −0.005 −0.007 −0.019 −0.021 0.028 0.018 −0.006 0.032
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
From Table 3 −0.002 −0.004 −0.015 −0.016 0.039 0.029 −0.028 0.008
Controls? No Yes No Yes No Yes No Yes
16 D. BARTRAM AND E. JAROCHOVA
Equivalent selections are made for people who in 1990 were in their 30s, 40s, 50s and
60s.) The cohort-specific coefficients are averaged in the penultimate row (of
numbers). The final row (of numbers) in the table duplicates the MCP coefficients
given in Table 3, to facilitate comparison.
The question is whether the cohort-specific results depart substantially from the
results given in Table 4. For the most part, the differences are not large, especially in
the first two pairs of columns (each pair corresponding to models without and with con-
trols). For ‘restrictive immigration policy preferences’ and ‘concern about immigration’,
we can perhaps discern differences that merit further attention. For our purposes, what
matters is that the cohort-specific results do not lead us towards a conclusion that the
adoption of multicultural policies reduces xenophobia (i.e. the finding of previous
research). On the contrary, the cohort-specific analysis takes us further away from that
conclusion, especially in connection with the ‘concern’ variable. In the final column,
the MCP coefficient in a model with controls is now 0.032, as against 0.008 in the corre-
sponding model from Table 4 (so, the policies appear to reinforce xenophobia in this
sense, rather than mitigating it).
There are of course more elaborate methods that could be used to analyse repeated
cross-sectional data (Lebo and Weber 2015). But those methods are intended to
address issues that do not amount to problems here, including incorrect standard
errors, bias from autocorrelation in long time-series, and exploring possibly dynamic
effects of level-1 variables. Taking these in turn: (1) standard errors are irrelevant here
– the level-2 units (countries) do not form a sample and extrapolation via hypothesis-
testing is unnecessary (and indeed inappropriate). (2) The time-series formed by the
data used here (considered as the number of measurements/time periods) is relatively
short (Lebo and Weber’s technique is directed primarily at situations where T > 50).
(3) The question of level-1 effects is irrelevant to the estimation of policies→attitudes.
In general, when variables are selected as controls (W) for the estimation of X→Y, the
coefficients for the controls cannot be treated as estimates of effects denoted by W→Y;
interpretation along these lines amounts to the ‘Table 2 fallacy’ (Westreich and Green-
land 2013).
Conclusion
Adoption of integration and/or multicultural policies, then, does not generally lead to
reduction of hostility towards immigrants and immigration in the specific European
countries investigated here. The optimistic findings of previous research are not
confirmed in a longitudinal investigation; if anything, the findings offered here are
more in line with the pessimistic conclusions of Citrin, Levy, and Wright (2014), who
find that multiculturalism policies intensify the connection between hostility towards
immigration and dissatisfaction with politics. Multicultural policies appear to contribute
to a slightly stronger sense of fairness towards immigrants when it comes to hiring
decisions. But it leads more strongly to an increased preference for restrictive immigra-
tion policies – though even for this measure the apparent impact is still small in the terms
discussed above. In the big picture, any effect of adopting integration/multicultural pol-
icies on attitudes towards immigrants and immigration (policies→attitudes) seems gen-
erally weak.
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 17
This conclusion emerges from a methodological position holding that the question is
best addressed by considering change over time ‘within’ countries, as against comparison
‘between’ countries at one point in time. As with longitudinal analysis more generally,
this approach is more effective at mitigating the extent of omitted variable bias, in par-
ticular with regard to time-constant between-country differences (Brüderl and Ludwig
2015).
This research of course has certain limitations. The article considers the potential
effects of policy on attitudes only via country-level averages. Effects are likely to differ
for individuals with different characteristics; that possibility is clearly established at
least for cross-sectional associations (e.g. Ziller 2020, exploring differences rooted in
whether people hold egalitarian values; see also Citrin, Levy, and Wright 2014). Most
of the cross-sectional research discussed in the ‘Previous research’ section above explores
average effects, and this article is addressed in response to findings constructed in that
way. But future research should indeed explore heterogeneity, as a way of further demon-
strating the value of adopting a longitudinal approach.
Another potential limitation has to do with the fact that the extent of within-
country over time change in the variables explored here is reasonably viewed as
small (via Table 2); in these circumstances, we might wonder whether a longitudinal
analysis is sensible. The answer depends on our research goals. If the goal is simply to
identify an association (asking whether attitudes are different in countries with
different policy frameworks) then perhaps the answer is no. But the value of identify-
ing an association is not clear. An association is surely more interesting and valuable
if it is generated via a causal relationship. In a context where xenophobia has dee-
pened (Mudde 2013; Yılmaz 2012), it is important to know whether the adoption
of policies that are more accommodating to immigrants is an effective response.
The researchers who conducted the cross-sectional studies discussed above are
careful to avoid stating their conclusions in those terms, but the question surely
has intrinsic value. The answer offered in this article is that attitudes do not appear
to change all that much, even when policies do (compare Kustov, Kaaker, and
Reller 2020).
The research presented here suggests in general the desirability of considering policie-
s→attitudes using a longitudinal approach. For future research along these lines, a key
message arising from this paper is that a longitudinal approach is not only desirable
but it is also sometimes possible – especially if we look beyond the idea that MLM of
cross-sectional survey data is the ‘obvious’ way of doing the research. To consider the
impact of policies – a country-level variable – it is more productive to ignore ‘level 1’
and focus on other variables conceived at country level, for which repeated measures
are sometimes available. That approach is relevant beyond the question of how policies
might affect attitudes – it is likely useful for investigation of the way policies might affect
any outcome conceived in the first instance at the level of individuals.
Notes
1. Not every country in the list above offers data for every variable. Nothing is available for
Norway in 1999 and for Greece in 1990. The implication is that the analysis of the policy
and concern variables cannot use Norway (as it would provide data from only one wave).
18 D. BARTRAM AND E. JAROCHOVA
2. This approach also helps avoid the potential objection that “Type 2 errors” (failure to reject a
false null hypothesis) might follow from the way a small sample contributes to larger stan-
dard errors.
3. This analysis includes data from Estonia and Latvia – and so a consideration from Gorod-
zeisky and Leykin (2020) merits attention. In those countries data on “immigrants” include a
good many people of Russian origin who did not themselves migrate to Estonia and Latvia –
rather, they became “foreigners” after the fall of the USSR. (For many this foreign status was
assigned despite their having been born locally.) In a supplementary analysis (available on
request), we have substituted what from this angle would be considered more reasonable
estimates of the immigrant stocks (%) variable. The impact is minimal; the overall con-
clusions remain unchanged.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Elmar Schlueter for helpful feedback on an earlier draft and to Ben Aviani-
Bartram for assistance with data.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).
Data availability
Data used in this paper are available directly from the sources indicated in the text and
references.
Notes on contributors
David Bartram is Associate Professor in Sociology at the University of Leicester. He is co-editor of
the Journal of Happiness Studies and President of RC31, the section on the Sociology of Migration
of the International Sociological Association. He has held grants from the UK Economic and Social
Research Council, Leverhulme, and the Nuffield Foundation. He gained a PhD from the University
of Wisconsin–Madison and a BA from Kenyon College.
Erika Jarochova is a PhD student in Sociology at the University of Leicester. She gained under-
graduate and master’s degrees from Charles University in the Czech Republic.
ORCID
David Bartram http://orcid.org/0000-0002-7278-2270
References
Allport, G. 1954. The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge: Addison-Wesley.
Bartram, D. 2021. “Cross-Sectional Model-Building for Research on Subjective Well-Being:
Gaining Clarity on Control Variables.” Social Indicators Research, doi:10.1007/s11205-020-
02586-3.
Bell, A., M. Fairbrother, and K. Jones. 2019. “Fixed and Random Effects Models: Making an
Informed Choice.” Quality & Quantity 53: 1051–1074. doi:10.1007/s11135-018-0802-x.
JOURNAL OF ETHNIC AND MIGRATION STUDIES 19
Berning, C. C., and E. Schlueter. 2016. “The Dynamics of Radical Right-Wing Populist Party
Preferences and Perceived Group Threat.” Social Science Research 55: 83–93. doi:10.1016/j.
ssresearch.2015.09.003.
Bloemraad, I., and M. Wright. 2014. ““Utter Failure” or Unity out of Diversity? Debating and
Evaluating Policies of Multiculturalism.” International Migration Review 48 (1_suppl): 292–
334. doi:10.1111/imre.12135.
Blumer, H. 1958. “Race Prejudice as a Sense of Group Position.” The Pacific Sociological Review 1
(1): 3–7. doi:10.2307/1388607.
Brüderl, J., and V. Ludwig. 2015. “Fixed-Effects Panel Regression.” In The SAGE Handbook of
Regression Analysis and Causal Inference, edited by H. Best, and C. Wolf, 327–357. Los
Angeles: SAGE Reference.
Callens, M.-S., and B. Meuleman. 2017. “Do Integration Policies Relate to Economic and Cultural
Threat Perceptions? A Comparative Study in Europe.” International Journal of Comparative
Sociology 58 (5): 367–391. doi:10.1177/0020715216665437.
Citrin, J., M. Levy, and M. Wright. 2014. “Multicultural Policy and Political Support in European
Democracies.” Comparative Political Studies 47 (11): 1531–1557. doi:10.1177/
0010414013512604.
Davidov, E., and M. Semyonov. 2017. “Attitudes Toward Immigrants in European Societies.”
International Journal of Comparative Sociology 58 (5): 359–366. doi:10.1177/0020715217732
183.
European Social Survey ERIC (ESS ERIC). 2015. European Social Survey, Round 7 - 2014.
Norwegian Centre for Research Data. doi:10.21338/NSD-ESS7-2014.
EVS. 2011. “European Values Study Longitudinal Data File 1981-2008 (EVS 1981-2008).” GESIS
Data Archive, doi:10.4232/1.11005.
Gangl, M. 2010. “Causal Inference in Sociological Research.” Annual Review of Sociology 36 (1):
21–47. doi:10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102702.
Gorodzeisky, A., and I. Leykin. 2020. “When Borders Migrate: Reconstructing the Category of
‘International Migrant’.” Sociology 54 (1): 142–158. doi:10.1177/0038038519860403.
Green, E. G. T., E. P. Visintin, O. Sarrasin, and M. Hewstone. 2020. “When integration policies
shape the impact of intergroup contact on threat perceptions: A multilevel study across 20
European countries.” Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 46 (3): 631–648. http://doi.org/
10.1080/1369183X.2018.1550159.
Hamaker, E. L., R. M. Kuiper, and R. P. P. P. Grasman. 2015. “A Critique of the Cross-Lagged
Panel Model.” Psychological Methods 20 (1): 102–116. doi:10.1037/a0038889.
Hooghe, M., and T. de Vroome. 2015. “ How Does the Majority Public React to Multiculturalist
Policies? A Comparative Analysis of European Countries.” American Behavioral Scientist 59 (6):
747–768. doi:10.1177/0002764214566499.
Jæger, M. M. 2013. “The Effect of Macroeconomic and Social Conditions on the Demand for
Redistribution: A Pseudo Panel Approach.” Journal of European Social Policy 23 (2): 149–
163. doi:10.1177/0958928712471225.
Jowell, R. 2007. European Social Survey, Technical Report. London: Centre for Comparative Social
Surveys, City University.
Kragten, N., and J. Rözer. 2017. “The Income Inequality Hypothesis Revisited: Assessing the
Hypothesis Using Four Methodological Approaches.” Social Indicators Research 131 (3):
1015–1033. doi:10.1007/s11205-016-1283-8.
Kustov, A., D. Kaaker, and C. Reller. 2020. “The Stability of Immigration Attitudes: Evidence and
Implications.” Journal of Politics.
Lebo, M. J., and C. Weber. 2015. “An Effective Approach to the Repeated Cross-Sectional Design.”
American Journal of Political Science 59 (1): 242–258. doi:10.1111/ajps.12095.
Lucas, S. R. 2014. “An Inconvenient Dataset: Bias and Inappropriate Inference with the Multilevel
Model.” Quality & Quantity 48 (3): 1619–1649. doi:10.1007/s11135-013-9865-x.
McLaren, L. M. 2003. “Anti-Immigrant Prejudice in Europe: Contact, Threat Perception, and
Preferences for the Exclusion of Migrants.” Social Forces 81 (3): 909–936. doi:10.1353/sof.
2003.0038.
20 D. BARTRAM AND E. JAROCHOVA
Mudde, C. 2013. “Three Decades of Populist Radical Right Parties in Western Europe: So What?”
European Journal of Political Research 52 (1): 1–19. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6765.2012.02065.x.
Mundlak, Y. 1978. “On the Pooling of Time Series and Cross Section Data.” Econometrica 46 (1):
69–85.
Niessen, J., et al. 2007. Migrant Integration Policy Index. London: British Council.
Olivera, J. 2015. “Changes in Inequality and Generalized Trust in Europe.” Social Indicators
Research 124 (1): 21–41. doi:10.1007/s11205-014-0777-5.
Pearl, J., and D. Mackenzie. 2018. The Book of Why: The New Science of Cause and Effect. London:
Allen Lane.
Pettigrew, T. F., and L. R. Tropp. 2006. “A Meta-Analytic Test of Intergroup Contact Theory.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90 (5): 751–783. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751.
Portes, J. 2019. “The Economics of Migration.” Contexts 18 (2): 12–17. doi:10.1177/
1536504219854712.
Quillian, L. 1995. “Prejudice as a Response to Perceived Group Threat.” American Sociological
Review 60 (4): 586. doi:10.2307/2096296.
Salt, J. 2001. Current Trends in International Migration in Europe. Council of Europe. https://www.
coe.int/t/dg3/migration/archives/Documentation/Migration management/2001_Salt_report_
en.pdf.
Scheepers, P., M. Gijsberts, and M. Coenders 2002. “Ethnic Exclusionism in European Countries:
Public Opposition to Civil Rights for Legal Migrants as a Response to Perceived Ethnic Threat.”
European Sociological Review 18 (1): 17–34. doi:10.1093/esr/18.1.17.
Schlueter, E., A. Masso, and E. Davidov. 2020. “What Factors Explain Anti-Muslim Prejudice?”
Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 46 (3): 649–664. doi:10.1080/1369183X.2018.1550160.
Schlueter, E., B. Meuleman, and E. Davidov. 2013. “Immigrant Integration Policies and Perceived
Group Threat: A Multilevel Study of 27 Western and Eastern European Countries.” Social
Science Research 42 (3): 670–682. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2012.12.001.
Schneider, S. L. 2007. “Anti-Immigrant Attitudes in Europe: Outgroup Size and Perceived Ethnic
Threat.” European Sociological Review 24 (1): 53–67. doi:10.1093/esr/jcm034.
Semyonov, M., R. Raijman, and A. Gorodzeisky. 2006. “The Rise of Anti-Foreigner Sentiment in
European Societies, 1988-2000.” American Sociological Review 71 (3): 426–449. doi:10.1177/
000312240607100304.
Sherif, M., B. J. White, and O. J. Harvey. 1955. “Status in Experimentally Produced Groups.”
American Journal of Sociology 60 (4): 370–379. doi:10.1086/221569.
Westreich, D., and S. Greenland. 2013. “The Table 2 Fallacy: Presenting and Interpreting
Confounder and Modifier Coefficients.” American Journal of Epidemiology 177 (4): 292–298.
doi:10.1093/aje/kws412.
Yılmaz, F. 2012. “Right-wing Hegemony and Immigration.” Current Sociology 60 (3): 368–381.
doi:10.1177/0011392111426192.
Ziller, C. 2020. “Established and Excluded? Immigrants’ Economic Progress, Attitudes Toward
Immigrants, and the Conditioning Role of Egalitarianism and Intergroup Contact.” Political
Studies, doi:10.1177/0032321720953561.