Arnal Et Al 2007
Arnal Et Al 2007
Arnal Et Al 2007
net/publication/220028264
CITATIONS READS
24 107
6 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Daniela Fazzio on 24 July 2014.
Despite the strengths of the studies listed above, they have several
limitations. First, although inter-observer reliability checks on the
dependent measure were conducted in all studies, none included
procedural integrity checks to ensure that the training procedures were
carried out as described. Second, training and feedback procedures were
described only briefly, making systematic replications difficult. Third,
the amount of training was considerable, such as 25 hours in the Koegel
et al. (1977) study, and an estimated 30 hours in Ryan and Hemmes
(2005). Given the large number of instructors (parents, educators, and
tutors) needed to provide intensive ABA early intervention for children
with autism, it is important to investigate efficient alternatives for
training them. One possibility is the use of self-instructional training
methods.
Method
Experiment 1
Materials
Before assessing IOA, an observer and the experimenter (the first author)
practiced scoring a videotaped session of an experienced tutor, who
volunteered to apply discrete-trials teaching to a confederate who role-
played a child with autism, until at least a 90% agreement for one 12-trial
practice session was achieved. The experimenter then scored all of the
Table 1.
Checklist for Scoring Discrete-trials Teaching
Trials
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
Before Starting Teaching Trials
1. Determine current targets
2. Gather materials
Provide SDs
4. Secure the child’s attention
6. Present instruction
Table 1 cont’d
17. Praise non-enthusiastically
For all four participants, mean IOR scores were 92% (range = 60-100),
96% (range = 60-100), 91% (range = 80-100), and 96% (range = 80-100),
respectively. IOR scores for the confederate’s script-following behavior
averaged 93% (range = 84-100).
(see Table 2). The participant was then asked to attempt to apply
discrete-trials teaching to the best of his/her ability to teach a confederate
role-playing a child with autism (as described previously), based on the
summary that they had just read, and to record the results on a data
sheet (see Table 3). The confederates’ behaviors were guided by a script
and standardized across participants. This procedure was then repeated
for two other tasks, motor imitation and matching identical pictures,
with a one-page summary of guidelines and a data sheet provided for
each task (similar to the guidelines and data sheet in Tables 2 and 3).
Table 2.
Baseline Instructions for Teaching Pointing to Pictures When Named
For this task you will role-play a tutor who is attempting to teach a child with
autism who has minimal language skills. Do your best at providing what you think
would be appropriate instructions, prompts or cues, and consequences while
attempting to teach the “child”, based on the guidelines listed below.
Here are three pictures. Your task is to teach this person (who will be role-
playing a child with autism) to point to the correct picture after you place the three
pictures on the table and name one of them. Across trials, try to teach the “child” to
point to all 3 pictures when they are named.
After each response by the “child”, record on the attached Data Sheet if the
“child” responded correctly independently, responded correctly with prompts or
cues, or made an error. Place a checkmark like this in the appropriate column.
Summary of Steps
1. Arrange necessary materials.
2. Decide what you will use as consequences for correct and incorrect responses
3. On each trial:
a. Secure the child’s attention.
b. Present the correct materials
c. Present the correct instruction.
d. Provide whatever extra help (i.e., prompts or cues) you think are
necessary for the child to respond correctly.
e. Once the “child” responds, provide what you consider to be an
appropriate feedback or reward for a correct response, or provide an
appropriate reaction for an error
f. Across trials gradually provide less and less prompts or cues (i.e., fade
out the extra prompts)
i. By prompting less
ii. By delaying your prompts
g. Record the results on the data sheet.
Table 3.
Data Sheet for Teaching Pointing to Named Pictures
Date: ___________________
Teacher: ________________
Targets: Banana
Dog
Balloons
Phase 2: Treatment. The participants were asked to read and study a 21-
page self instructional manual on discrete-trials teaching (Fazzio &
Martin, 2006), and to master the answers to the study questions provided
in the manual. Mastery of the study questions was determined by a test
comprised of 40% of the total number of study questions, randomly
selected from a bag. Participants were asked to study the questions until
performance on the mastery test was 100%.
P1 P2
90 90
80 80
70 70
Percent correct
Percent correct
60 60
50 50
40 40
30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
1 2 3 4 5 6 1 2 3 4 5 6
Sessions Sessions
P3 P4
80 80
70 70
Percent correct
Percent correct
60 60
50 50
40 40
30 30
20 20
10 10
0
0
1 2 3 4 5 6
1 2 3 4 5 6
Sessions Sessions
Figure 1.
Mean percent correct performance per session of Participants 1, 2, 3 and 4 while
conducting discrete-trials teaching to teach three tasks: pointing to named pictures ( ● ),
matching ( ■ ), and motor imitation (▲). P stands for participant.
For the purpose of PI, checklists specific to each phase were prepared
describing the steps to be followed by the experimenter. For some of the
sessions, an observer was assigned to monitor the experimenter’s
behavior using the checklist. The percentage of steps recorded by the
observer as accurately followed by the experimenter for a session yielded
a PI score for that session. PI scores were obtained for at least 25% of the
sessions across all participants, and PI was always 100%.
Social Validity
To assess the social validity of the study, the participants were given a
questionnaire to answer and complete anonymously. The questionnaire
evaluated the participant’s view of the extent to which the training and
goals of the research were important, the procedures used were
acceptable, and the training procedures used were effective.
Results
Experiment 2
Materials
Following the treatment phase, that included scoring of the video of the
tutor teaching the pointing-to-named-pictures task to the confederate,
each participant once again attempted to apply discrete trials teaching to
the three tasks to a confederate who role-played a child with autism, as
described for the Baseline phase. However, instead of reading a one-
page summary of the teaching procedures such as that shown in Table 2,
participants were allowed to use the one-page summary of discrete trials
teaching outlined in Chapter 7 of the self-instructional manual, which
contained the 19 items in Table 1.
Social Validity
Results
P5
P6
P
7
Figure 2.
Mean percent correct performance per session of Participants 5, 6, and 7 while
conducting discrete-trials teaching to teach three tasks: pointing to named pictures ( ● ),
matching ( ■ ), and motor imitation (▲). P stands for participant.
the matching task, and 92% while teaching motor imitation. Participants
6 and 7 both showed clear improvement in comparison to their Baseline
performance (see Figure 2). However, unlike Participant 5, they did not
achieve mastery level performance for any of the tasks following
implementation of the training package. Following treatment, while
teaching the pointing-to-named-pictures task, the matching task, and the
motor imitation task, Participant 6 averaged 85%, 70% and 80%
respectively, and Participant 7 averaged 76%, 70% and 70% respectively.
Discussion
References
Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I. (1997). Intensive
behavior treatment for preschoolers with severe mental retardation
and pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal on Mental
Retardation, 102, 238-249.
Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000). Randomized trial of
intensive early intervention for children with pervasive
developmental disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 105,
269-285.
Author Note
This research was supported by the St. Amant Research Program and the
Psychology Department of the University of Manitoba. This manuscript
was submitted by the first author in partial fulfillment for the
requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts Honors in Psychology.
For further information regarding this study, write to Garry L. Martin,
University of Manitoba, 129 St. Paul’s College, 430 Dysart Road,
Winnipeg, MB, Canada, R3T 2M6, gmartin@cc.umanitoba.ca or D. Yu, St
Amant Centre, 440 River Road, Winnipeg, MB, Canada, R2M 3Z9,
yu@stamant.mb.ca.