Effectiveness of Cognitive-Functional (Cog-Fun) Intervention With Children With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Pilot Study
Effectiveness of Cognitive-Functional (Cog-Fun) Intervention With Children With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Pilot Study
Effectiveness of Cognitive-Functional (Cog-Fun) Intervention With Children With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Pilot Study
KEY WORDS The executive function (EF) deficits of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) hinder
their performance of complex daily functions. Despite the existing evidence-based pharmacological inter-
Hahn-Markowitz, J., Manor, I., & Maeir, A. (2011). Effectiveness of cognitive–functional (Cog–Fun) intervention with
children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A pilot study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 65,
384–392. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2011.000901
A
Jeri Hahn-Markowitz, MSc, OTR, is Director of ttention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurobiological disorder
Continuing Education and Doctoral Student, School of
that affects the emotions, behavior, and cognitive state of 4%–7% of children
Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, Hadassah and
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, PO Box 20246, worldwide (Spencer, Biederman, & Mick, 2007). Symptoms include inattention,
Mount Scopus, Jerusalem 91240 Israel; impulsivity, and hyperactivity, and they often persist into adulthood. The long-
jerihahnmarkowitz@gmail.com term emotional, social, educational, and occupational implications of ADHD
Iris Manor, MD, is Medical Director, ADHD Clinic, Geha
are profound and well documented (Cermak, 2005).
Mental Health Center, Petah Tikva, Israel. Executive functions (EFs) consist of higher order cognitive abilities, including
working memory, planning, and emotional regulation (Barkley, 2004), which
Adina Maeir, PhD, OT, is Senior Lecturer, School of are crucial for complex and dynamic activities of daily living (ADLs; Katz &
Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Medicine, Hadassah and
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel.
Hartman-Maeir, 2005). Executive dysfunction is a main deficit in ADHD;
therefore, people with ADHD are at risk for significant limitations in occu-
pational functioning (Brown, 2009).
Medications such as methylphenidate are widely used with children with
ADHD, and they have been shown to be effective in reducing symptoms and
increasing academic productivity (Biederman et al., 2004). However, residual
executive dysfunction has been found in medicated children (Safren, 2006). In
addition, a percentage of children with ADHD do not respond to pharmaco-
logical intervention (O’Connell, Bellgrove, Dockree, & Robertson, 2006).
We designed a cognitive–functional intervention program (Cog–Fun)
targeting EF in occupation to improve attainment of occupational goals. This
ecological intervention has a protocol tailored to the unique executive dys-
function of children with ADHD. The Cog–Fun program focuses on acqui-
sition and transfer of cognitive strategies to enable occupational performance in
the child’s natural environments. It is based on the theoretical foundations of
Data Collection
The first author (Hahn-Markowitz) administered the
TOLDX to the children before and after intervention and
at follow-up. She also completed the children’s COPM
and interviewed participants after each goal was agreed
on, at the end of the intervention, and at follow-up. Pa-
Figure 1. Occupational goals chosen by children. rents completed the COPM and BRIEF independently at
The significant improvements with medium to large and after receiving methylphenidate twice daily for 16 wk
effect sizes found on the BRIEF parent and teacher reports and found no improvement on the TOLDX. This compar-
are encouraging, suggesting a possible intervention effect on ison may represent an advantage for cognitive–functional
EF in daily life. The teachers reported better EF in school intervention on planning abilities. An exhaustive literature
than the parents reported for home, concurring with the search did not generate articles on nonpharmacological
findings of Drechsler et al. (2007), who used the BRIEF studies using the TOLDX as an outcome measure; therefore,
when evaluating neurofeedback training for children with this premise could not be further explored.
ADHD. Several explanations are possible for the discrep- We found a strong positive change in the parents’
ancy between parent and teacher reports on children’s EF. and children’s rating of occupational performance on
The fact that some children were on medication while in trained and untrained goals, as reflected in the COPM
school ( Jarratt et al., 2005) and that the classroom envi- preintervention–postintervention scores. Previous studies
ronment is structured (Mares, McLuckie, Schwartz, & have shown that a change of ³2 points on the COPM is
Saini, 2007) may account for teachers’ witnessing more considered a moderate to large change and a clinically
organized and controlled behavior than parents witnessed important difference as judged by clients and their fam-
at home. In other studies, teachers reported higher ilies (Phipps & Richardson, 2007). According to Toglia
(worse) BRIEF scores than parents ( Jarratt et al., 2005; (2005), multitask activities in a variety of settings should
McCandless & O’Laughlin, 2007; Mares et al., 2007). be included in therapy to facilitate the acquisition and
Differences between informants may be the result of transfer of strategies in a multitude of contexts. In this
cross-situational discrepancies in expectations and per- study, the transfer goals chosen were not addressed di-
ceptions. Future studies should examine the source of rectly in the therapeutic process. The improved COPM
these differences with the aid of an independent infor- scores on the transfer goals could reflect the success of the
mant who would observe the child in both environments program in facilitating transfer of strategy use.
(Mares et al., 2007). Despite this study’s positive results, caution is needed
On the TOLDX, the children completed the tasks in in accounting for the effect of potential human bias on
less time, with fewer moves and fewer rule violations, after the part of respondents on rating scales (Draper, 2002).
intervention, with medium to large effects. This finding The actual size of effects will need to be determined in
could reflect more efficient strategic planning, similar to further controlled, blinded studies.
that reported on the BRIEF. The established test–retest
reliability supports this interpretation and reduces the Mechanisms of Change in Intervention
likelihood of a practice effect. These results are unlike The positive results of this study raise questions as to what
those of Yang, Chung, Chen, and Chen (2004), who com- contributed to change. Notwithstanding the possibility
pared 6- to 12-yr-old Taiwanese children with ADHD before of bias, we hypothesized that the strategies acquired in